Next Article in Journal
Diabetes Prevalence and Associated Risk Factors among Women in a Rural District of Nepal Using HbA1c as a Diagnostic Tool: A Population-Based Study
Next Article in Special Issue
Loneliness and Emancipation: A Multilevel Analysis of the Connection between Gender Inequality, Loneliness, and Social Isolation in the ISSP 2017
Previous Article in Journal
Relation between Noise Pollution and Life Satisfaction Based on the 2019 Chinese Social Survey
Previous Article in Special Issue
From Physical Distancing to Social Loneliness among Gay Men and Other Men Having Sex with Men in Belgium: Examining the Disruption of the Social Network and Social Support Structures
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Lonely in Different Relationships: Bidirectional Effects between Parent- and Peer-Related Loneliness in Adolescence

1
School Psychology and Development in Context, KU Leuven, 3000 Leuven, Belgium
2
Department of Developmental, Personality and Social Psychology, Ghent University, 9000 Ghent, Belgium
3
Youth Studies, Utrecht University, 3584 CH Utrecht, The Netherlands
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(12), 7014; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19127014
Submission received: 6 April 2022 / Revised: 17 May 2022 / Accepted: 18 May 2022 / Published: 8 June 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Loneliness: An Issue for Personal Well-Being and Public Health)

Abstract

:
Although it is assumed that loneliness in one relationship might put one at risk of experiencing loneliness in another relationship, this association has rarely been examined as such. In this longitudinal study, we examined the associations between peer- and parent-related loneliness in a sample of 3391 adolescents across three waves (Mage Wave 1 = 14.53; 59.3% female). Using random intercept cross-lagged panel models, parent- and peer-related loneliness were found to be stable over time and were concurrently related to each other. Moreover, the state of peer-related loneliness predicted the state of parent-related loneliness one year later. Thereby, the current study provides limited evidence of a carry-over effect between relation-specific types of loneliness.

1. Introduction

Adolescence is a developmental period in which individuals are particularly vulnerable to developing psychosocial problems, partly due to substantial changes in the social relationships of adolescents [1]. More specifically, in comparison with children, adolescents expect greater intimacy and self-disclosure from their social relationships; they engage in different and more diverse social situations and their main socializing partners change from parents to peers [2]. Hence, initiating and maintaining satisfying social relationships is seen as an important developmental task during adolescence [1]. Failing to fulfill this task may result in feelings of loneliness, which is a painful experience that has been found to be detrimentally related to the well-being of adolescents. For example, adolescents who feel lonely also experience more psychosocial problems, such as anxiety and depressive symptoms, and more physical problems, such as sleep problems and cardiovascular incidents [3,4,5]. As loneliness arises from unfulfilled relationship needs, people can experience loneliness in one relationship context, but not another [6]. Two important relationship contexts for adolescents are the peer context and the parent context. It has been suggested that relationship problems in one context may carry over to another context [7], but empirical evidence is largely lacking. If such carry-over effects exist, this would mean that loneliness feelings in one relationship could function as a barrier to build and maintain meaningful relationships with other people. In that case, this would also mean that clinicians should be aware that problems with relationships in one domain could lead to problems with relationships in other domains so that they can monitor and target this when needed. Although researchers increasingly acknowledge the importance of distinguishing loneliness types, the focus is still on general loneliness in the majority of studies. When associations between loneliness types are revealed over time, this would add to the literature, highlighting the importance of tackling loneliness in different relationships. In this study, we aimed to examine how parent-related loneliness is associated with peer-related loneliness over time in adolescence.

1.1. Parent- and Peer-Related Loneliness

Loneliness is defined as a negative emotional reaction to the experienced discrepancy between the perceived and desired quantity or quality of one’s social relationships [8]. Although several researchers have suggested that loneliness is a unidimensional phenomenon that varies in intensity [9], researchers have increasingly suggested that loneliness should be considered as a multidimensional phenomenon [10,11]. The multidimensional approach to loneliness is based on the premise that different relationships fulfill different social needs [11]. For example, parents are assumed to particularly provide nurturance and guidance whereas peers particularly provide a sense of integration [12]. When a certain type of relationship does not fulfill a particular social need, adolescents might experience loneliness within this relationship, but not in another relationship context.
One commonly made distinction of loneliness types is between emotional and social loneliness. Emotional loneliness is experienced when close emotional attachments are absent or lacking in quality. Social loneliness is experienced when a network of social relationships is absent or lacking in quality. Within each of these loneliness types, further subtypes can be identified depending on the relationship in which loneliness is experienced [13]. For instance, emotional loneliness can be experienced in relation to a parent or a best friend and social loneliness can be experienced in relation to the broader family or a group of friends. Previous research has indicated that relationship-specific experiences of loneliness are related, but are separate constructs [14]. Moreover, a recent study showed that parent- and peer-related loneliness develop in opposite directions during adolescence; that is, parent-related loneliness increases throughout adolescence whereas peer-related loneliness decreases [15]. In addition, research attempting to identify different subgroups of adolescents based on loneliness experiences in different relationships found separate groups, with one group of adolescents experiencing loneliness in the relationship with their parents without experiencing loneliness in relation to their peers and vice versa for another group [6]. These relation-specific loneliness experiences have also been found to be differentially related to various adjustment outcomes (e.g., [16]). For instance, peer-related loneliness was found to be related to social phobia whereas parent-related loneliness was related to self-harm [16]. These findings highlight the importance of distinguishing between loneliness experiences in various relationships.

1.2. Association between Parent- and Peer-Related Loneliness

Even though there is increasing evidence that different relation-specific types of loneliness can be distinguished, it is not fully understood how loneliness in one relationship relates to loneliness in other relationships. Based on the need–threat/need–fortification framework [17], two contrasting hypotheses can be proposed. On the one hand, it could be that higher levels of loneliness in one relationship type lead to lower levels of loneliness in another relationship type. When experiencing a thwarted need to belong (i.e., loneliness), individuals may act to fortify their relational needs, leading to prosocial thoughts and behaviors. In other words, when adolescents experience that one type of relationship is not fulfilling their relational needs, they might increase their efforts to establish another (type of) relationship that does fulfill their relational needs.
On the other hand, it can also be hypothesized that higher levels of loneliness in one relationship type lead to higher levels of loneliness in another relationship type. For example, it could be that experiencing ostracism, or experiencing being ignored or excluded, threatens the need of an individual for control and meaningful existence, leading to attempts to fortify their needs of control and recognition that might be dealt with through antisocial thoughts and behaviors [17]. Such thoughts and behaviors would then hinder the formation of fulfilling relationships with other people, leading to higher levels of loneliness in other relationship types as well.
This idea is in line with the socio-cognitive loneliness model [18], which suggests that loneliness experiences activate a series of cognitions to ensure self-preservation (e.g., hypervigilance for social threats). Paradoxically, these activated cognitions are biased in a way that social interactions are perceived as more negative and less positive, resulting in confirmatory behaviors that do not promote reconnection with others, but rather elicit negative reactions from others. These confirmatory behaviors in turn reinforce the initial cognitive bias. In this way, the loneliness levels are assumed to endure over time and could be generalized from one relationship to another [2,19]. For example, lonely individuals might withdraw from all social relationships as a result of previous negative interactions in one relationship, which might increase their feelings of loneliness as they become more socially isolated.
Empirical evidence testing these hypotheses is scarce. Based on cross-sectional work, a meta-analysis indicated that parent- and peer-related loneliness are positively correlated (r = 0.22; [19]). This positive correlation could suggest that experiencing parent-related loneliness puts one at risk of experiencing peer-related loneliness or, vice versa, that experiencing peer-related loneliness puts one at risk of experiencing parent-related loneliness. It is also possible that peer- and parent-related loneliness reciprocally influence one another, as suggested by transactional models of development [7], creating a vicious cycle. However, longitudinal studies including both peer- and parent-related loneliness are lacking. As a result, it remains unclear whether an association between different relation-specific types of loneliness can be found to hold over time and whether the associations are unidirectional or bidirectional.

1.3. The Current Study

In the current study, we adopted a transactional approach to loneliness as experienced in different types of social relationships. Specifically, we aimed to examine whether the cross-sectional association between parent- and peer-related loneliness found in previous studies held across time and within individuals. We examined the association between peer- and parent-related loneliness in regard to state-like fluctuations over time (i.e., within-person effects) above and beyond trait-like tendencies (i.e., between-person effects; [20]); that is, within-person processes were separated from stable between-person differences [20]. We hypothesized that experiencing parent-related loneliness at Time N would be related to experiencing more peer-related loneliness at Time N + 1 within individuals, and vice versa.

2. Method

2.1. Procedure and Participants

In this study, we combined data from three independent samples from the Dutch-speaking part of Belgium [21,22,23,24]. In each of the three studies, the same procedure was followed; that is, passive consent was obtained from parents and active consent was obtained from adolescents. The design of the three longitudinal studies was also comparable with a one-year interval between assessments. In each study, during three subsequent years, adolescents completed questionnaires during regular school hours.
The first sample, which was part of another larger longitudinal dataset [23], consisted of 551 adolescents (62.80% female) aged between 12 and 17 years old at the first assessment (M = 14.82, SD = 0.79). In this sample, 21.80% of the adolescents did not participate in Wave 2 and 24.90% did not participate in Wave 3. The MCAR Test of Little [25] revealed a normed χ2 of 1.16, which indicated that the data were randomly missing (i.e., the normed χ2 was below 3; [26]).
The second sample consisted of 1371 adolescents (53.20% female) aged between 11 and 17 years old at the first assessment (M = 13.21, SD = 0.79) who participated in a larger longitudinal study [21,22]. Of these 1371 adolescents, 25.50% did not participate in Wave 2, and 38.80% did not participate in Wave 3. The MCAR Test if Little [25], using an expectation maximization estimation, revealed a normed χ2 of 1.30, which indicated that the data were randomly missing (i.e., the normed χ2 was below 3; [26]).
For the third sample, data from 1469 adolescents (63.80% female) aged between 11 and 20 years old (M = 15.64, SD = 1.33) were used from a larger longitudinal study [24]. Of this sample, 65.40% were enrolled from the first assessment onwards, of which 17.77% did not participate in the second assessment and 38.97% did not participate in the third assessment. In addition, 23.23% enrolled at the second assessment, of which 39.00% did not participate in the third assessment. The remaining adolescents (11.37%) of the total sample enrolled at the third assessment. The MCAR Test of Little [25] revealed a normed χ2 of 1.08, which indicated that the data were randomly missing (i.e., the normed χ2 was below 3; [26]).
Combining these three samples resulted in a sample of 3391 adolescents (59.4% female) aged between 11 and 20 years old at the first assessment (M = 14.52, SD = 1.67). The MCAR test of Little [25] on this combined sample, using an expectation maximization, revealed a normed χ2 of 1.23, which indicated that the data were randomly missing (i.e., the normed χ2 was below 3; [26]). Given the variation in age at the first assessment both within and across the studies, age was included as a covariate in our analyses.

2.2. Measures

Peer- and parent-related loneliness were measured with the Loneliness and Aloneness Scale for Children and Adolescents (LACA; [10]). Both subscales consist of 12 items. Example items from the peer-related loneliness subscale are “I think I have fewer friends than others” and “I feel excluded by my classmates”. Example items from the parent-related loneliness subscale are “I feel left out by my parents” and “I find it hard to talk to my parents”. The items were rated on a four-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (never) to 4 (often). For each participant, an individual mean score was calculated, resulting in mean scores ranging from 1 (low loneliness) to 4 (high loneliness). The questionnaire has been shown to be a valid [10] and reliable measure of loneliness in various countries [27].

2.3. Statistical Analysis

A random intercept cross-lagged panel model (RI-CLPM) was estimated with Mplus Version 8.2 [28]. This type of model allowed us to examine the longitudinal associations between peer- and parent-related loneliness whilst taking into account the stability of the constructs and their within-time correlations. Given that the data were randomly missing, a full information maximum likelihood estimation (FIML) was used to account for the missing data. A robust maximum likelihood estimation (MLR) was used to account for the non-normality of the data.
In an RI-CLPM, the observed scores are decomposed into two parts. The within-person processes are separated from the stable between-person differences [20]. First, all observed scores of peer-related loneliness are regressed on a single latent factor and all observed scores of parent-related loneliness are regressed on another single latent factor [20]; that is, a random intercept for peer-related loneliness and a random intercept for parent-related loneliness are specified. These two random intercepts represent reliable time invariant differences between individuals and can, therefore, be interpreted as trait-like factors. Second, each observed loneliness score (i.e., one peer-related loneliness score and one parent-related loneliness score for each individual from each assessment wave) is regressed on its own latent factor [20]. Across the three waves, this results in three peer-related loneliness latent factors and three parent-related loneliness latent factors. These six latent factors represent situation-dependent fluctuations in loneliness within an individual and can, therefore, be interpreted as state-like factors.
Correlations, stability, and cross-lagged paths can then be specified between these state-like latent factors. First, we estimated an unconstrained model in which all of these aforementioned paths were freely estimated (i.e., Model 1). Second, more parsimonious models were estimated and compared with the first unconstrained model; that is, the stability coefficients of peer-related loneliness (i.e., from peer-related loneliness at Wave N to peer-related loneliness at Wave N + 1) were constrained to be equal to one another in Model 2. In Model 3, the stability coefficients of parent-related loneliness (i.e., from parent-related loneliness at Wave N to parent-related loneliness at Wave N + 1) were constrained to be equal. In Model 4, the within-time correlations between the state-like factors of both peer- and parent-related loneliness at Wave 2 and Wave 3 were constrained to be equal to one another. In Model 5, the cross-lagged paths from parent-related loneliness to peer-related loneliness were constrained to be equal to one another. In Model 6, the cross-lagged paths from peer-related loneliness to parent-related loneliness were constrained to be equal. The path coefficients of the best fitting model were interpreted. In all models ‘age’ was added as a predictor to the observed scores for both peer- and parent-related loneliness at each measurement occasion in order to control for the variability in age at the different measurement waves. This path was set to be equal for each measurement occasion in all models as we did not expect this effect to change across the measurement waves [29]. The syntax of the different models are provided on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/k43r2/?view_only=d00105049a964c64a6090c98ff693541 accessed on 17 May 2022).
The model fit was considered to be satisfactory if the Satorra–Bentler (SB)-scaled chi2 was non-significant [30]. In addition, the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) was designated as 0.06 or smaller, the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) was designated as 0.08 or smaller, and the comparative fit index (CFI) exceeded 0.95 [31]. When comparing the model fit of the different models, the model with the lowest RMSEA and SRMR as well as the highest CFI was preferred [32]. Additionally, a significant chi-squared difference test indicated that the models differed from one another; the more complex model (i.e., the model with the least constraints) was preferred [30]. The paths in the preferred model were investigated and path coefficients with p-values below 0.05 were considered to be statistically significant.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Statistics

Table 1 summarizes the correlations, means, standard deviations, and Cronbach’s alphas for peer- and parent-related loneliness across the three measurement waves. Cronbach’s alphas showed a good reliability for both subscales at each measurement occasion. High positive correlations were found for parent-related loneliness on three measurement occasions, suggesting the relative stability of parent-related loneliness across time. Similarly, high positive correlations were found for peer-related loneliness on various measurement occasions. There were weak to moderate positive correlations between parent- and peer-related loneliness on different measurement occasions. The correlations between parent- and peer-related loneliness were in line with the reported estimated mean correlation in a meta-analysis on this regard (i.e., r = 0.22; [27]).

3.2. Random Intercept Cross-Lagged Panel Models

The model fit of the different RI-CLPMs as well as the model comparisons are shown in Table 2. All models showed a good fit. The model with the stability paths for peer-related loneliness constrained to be equal (i.e., Model 2) was not preferred over the fully unconstrained model (i.e., Model 1). Model 3, with the stability paths of parent-related loneliness constrained to be equal, was not preferred over Model 1. The model in which the within-time correlations between parent- and peer-related loneliness at Waves 2 and 3 (i.e., Model 4) were constrained to be equal was preferred over Model 1. The model in which the paths from parent-related loneliness to peer-related loneliness one year later were constrained to be equal was also preferred over Model 1. The model in which the paths from peer-related loneliness to parent-related loneliness one year later were constrained to be equal was not preferred over Model 1. As the constraints of Models 4 and 5 were allowed, we compared a final model in which we applied the constraints of Models 4 and 5 simultaneously (i.e., Model 7) to the fully unconstrained model (i.e., Model 1). This comparison showed that Model 7 was preferred over Model 1.
The standardized path coefficients of Model 7 are shown in Figure 1 (for the unstandardized coefficients, standard errors, and p-values, see Table 3). First, the trait-like factors (i.e., the random intercepts) of parent-related loneliness and peer-related loneliness were moderately correlated. This indicated that the adolescents who reported more parent-related loneliness than their peers across the three measurement occasions also reported more peer-related loneliness than their peers across these measurement occasions and vice versa. Second, the moderate stability coefficients indicated that the earlier state of parent- and peer-related loneliness predicted, respectively, the state of parent- and peer-related loneliness one year later. Third, the state of peer-related loneliness at Wave 2 was related to the state of parent-related loneliness at Wave 3. Specifically, the adolescents who reported higher levels of peer-related loneliness at Wave 2 were more likely to report higher levels of parent-related loneliness at Wave 3. Remarkably, this effect was not found between the state of peer-related loneliness at Wave 1 and the state of parent-related loneliness at Wave 2. Fourth, the state of parent-related loneliness was not related to the state of peer-related loneliness one year later. Fifth, cross-sectionally, the state of peer-related loneliness was related more to the state of parent-related loneliness and vice versa. Finally, age was a significant positive predictor for the state of parent-related loneliness at all waves; that is, higher ages were more associated with the state of parent-related loneliness.

4. Discussion

Researchers adhering to a multidimensional approach on loneliness underscore the importance of distinguishing between loneliness experiences in various relationships (e.g., [10,11,16]). The moderate positive correlation between peer- and parent-related loneliness found in a previous work [27] suggests that experiencing loneliness in one relationship might put one at risk of also experiencing loneliness in other relationships. On the other hand, based on the need–threat/need–fortification model [17], a negative association between loneliness in the two relationship types could exist as well. However, studies longitudinally examining this hypothesis are lacking, and it remains unclear whether the associations among different relation-specific types of loneliness are unidirectional or bidirectional. The current study examined how loneliness experienced in the relationship with parents on the one hand and peers on the other hand is related to each over time.
The findings indicated that state-like loneliness, both in relation to parents and in relation to peers, endures over time. More specifically, the stability of peer- and parent-related loneliness in adolescents over time was not fully accounted for by their respective trait-like loneliness components. Similar results were found for general loneliness experiences of adults in a previous study of Lim et al. (2016; [33]) in which the stability of general loneliness was not fully accounted for by the trait-like loneliness component either. These findings suggest that loneliness is susceptible to changes due to momentary external factors and that momentary fluctuations in loneliness might affect future levels of loneliness.
The current study found limited evidence of a carry-over effect between parent- and peer-related loneliness. First, we found an effect of peer-related loneliness on parent-related loneliness one year later; that is, adolescents who experienced more peer-related loneliness were more likely to experience increased parent-related loneliness one year later. However, this cross-lagged effect was not found at all time points and it was very small. Hence, we are hesitant to interpret this effect. Second, a correlated change between parent- and peer-related loneliness was found; that is, changes in the state of parent-related loneliness were associated with changes in peer-related loneliness in the same direction. Our findings seem to suggest that the reciprocal process in which relation-specific loneliness types influence each other does not operate over years, but might occur across a smaller time interval. We encourage future studies to examine this hypothesis.

Strengths and Limitations

The current study had several strengths such as the use of a large longitudinal dataset, the use of a well-established loneliness measure, and the application of state-of-the art statistical techniques. However, this study was not without limitations. First, although the cross-lagged panel models included a temporal dimension, no causal conclusions could be drawn as this was still a correlational method.
Second, we only relied on self-reports to measure parent- and peer-related loneliness. Although this method has been put forward as the most adequate assessment method for a subjective and internal experience such as loneliness [3,34], this approach might have resulted in inflated findings due to common-method variances.
Third, although we examined loneliness in relation to key figures in the social environment of adolescents, examining loneliness in relation to additional significant others in the lives of adolescents is important as well. For instance, romantic relationships become increasingly important in adolescence and are associated with the well-being of adolescents [35]. Hence, a broader examination of the carry-over effects of loneliness across different relationship types in adolescence is necessary.
Fourth, in this study we used a community sample in which loneliness levels were naturally not that high. In line with other research using community samples, the distribution of loneliness was skewed, with the majority of participants scoring below 2 [10]. We urge readers to take this issue into account when interpreting our results. Future research could replicate this study in a clinical sample. Nonetheless, we do not expect the associations between the variables to be substantially different among adolescents with high loneliness scores.
Finally, the sample of the current study varied in terms of age; that is, at the first measurement occasion, adolescents between the ages of 11 and 17 years old participated in this study. Although we corrected for age with regard to the mean levels of both types of loneliness, we could not verify whether the correlations and regression coefficients were different across adolescence. Future research should examine this research question whilst taking the different stages of adolescence into account.

5. Conclusions

The findings of the current study indicated that, in line with previous research, parent- and peer-related loneliness were positively associated with one another at the same point in time. Moreover, a significant longitudinal within-person effect was found, indicating that the state of peer-related loneliness predicted the state of parent-related loneliness one year later. Hence, the current study provides limited evidence of a carry-over effect from peer-related loneliness to parent-related loneliness. Although further research is needed, our findings suggest that high levels of a relation-specific loneliness type could be a risk factor for another relation-specific loneliness type.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.S.; methodology, A.S. and F.G.; formal analyses, A.S. and F.G.; resources, M.B., M.M. and J.V.; data curation, M.B., J.V., A.S. and F.G.; writing—original draft preparation, A.S. and F.G.; writing—review and editing, M.B., J.V. and M.M.; visualization, A.S. and F.G.; supervision, A.S. and F.G.; project administration, A.S. and F.G. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by KU Leuven Research Council, grant number C14/16/040. The APC was funded by KU Leuven Research Council, grant number GOA/12/009.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board of KU Leuven.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Steinberg, L.; Morris, A.S. Adolescent development. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2001, 52, 83–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Qualter, P.; Vanhalst, J.; Harris, R.; Van Roekel, E.; Lodder, G.; Bangee, M.; Maes, M.; Verhagen, M. Loneliness across the life span. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2015, 10, 250–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Heinrich, L.M.; Gullone, E. The clinical significance of loneliness: A literature review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2006, 26, 695–718. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Hawkley, L.C.; Capitanio, J.P. Perceived social isolation, evolutionary fitness and health outcomes: A lifespan approach. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2015, 370, 20140114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Caspi, A.; Harrington, H.; Moffitt, T.E.; Milne, B.J.; Poulton, R. Socially isolated children 20 years later: Risk of cardiovascular disease. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 2006, 160, 805–811. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Maes, M.; Vanhalst, J.; Spithoven, A.W.; Van den Noortgate, W.; Goossens, L. Loneliness and attitudes toward aloneness in adolescence: A person-centered approach. J. Youth Adolesc. 2016, 45, 547–567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Sameroff, A. The transactional model. In The Transactional Model of Development: How Children and Contexts Shape Each Other; Sameroff, A., Ed.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2009; pp. 3–22. [Google Scholar]
  8. Peplau, L.A.; Perlman, D. Perspectives on loneliness. In Loneliness: A Sourcebook of Current Theory, Research and Therapy; Peplau, L.A., Perlman, D., Eds.; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 1982; pp. 1–18. [Google Scholar]
  9. Russell, D.W. UCLA loneliness scale (version 3): Reliability, validity, and factor structure. J. Personal. Assess. 1996, 66, 20–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Goossens, L. (Ed.) Leuvense Eenzaamheidsschaal voor Kinderen en Adolescenten (LEKA): Eenzaamheid Meten bij Jongeren in Vlaanderen en Nederland [Loneliness and Aloneness Scale for Children and Adolescents (LACA): Manual]; Acco: Leuven, Belgium, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  11. Weiss, R.S. Loneliness: The Experience of Emotional and Social Isolation; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1973. [Google Scholar]
  12. DiTommaso, E.; Spinner, B. Social and emotional loneliness: A re-examination of Weiss’ typology of loneliness. Personal. Individ. Differ. 1997, 22, 417–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Maes, M.; Vanhalst, J.; Qualter, P. Loneliness. In The Encyclopedia of Child and Adolescent Development; Hupp, S., Jewell, J., Eds.; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2020; pp. 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Maes, M.; Klimstra, T.; Van den Noortgate, W.; Goossens, L. Factor structure and measurement invariance of a multidimensional loneliness scale: Comparisons across gender and age. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2015, 24, 1829–1837. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Danneel, S.; Maes, M.; Vanhalst, J.; Bijttebier, P.; Goossens, L. Developmental change in loneliness and attitudes toward aloneness in adolescence. J. Youth Adolesc. 2018, 47, 148–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Lasgaard, M.; Goossens, L.; Bramsen, R.H.; Trillingsgaard, T.; Elklit, A. Different sources of loneliness are associated with different forms of psychopathology in adolescence. J. Res. Personal. 2011, 45, 233–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Williams, K.D. Ostracism. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2007, 58, 425–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Cacioppo, J.T.; Hawkley, L.C. Perceived social isolation and cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2009, 13, 447–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  19. Spithoven, A.W.M.; Bijttebier, P.; Goossens, L. It is all in their mind: A review on information processing bias in lonely individuals. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2017, 58, 97–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Hamaker, E.L.; Kuiper, R.M.; Grasman, R.P.P.P. A critique of the cross-lagged panel model. Psychol. Methods 2015, 20, 102–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Bastin, M.; Luyckx, K.; Raes, F.; Bijttebier, P. Co-rumination and depressive symptoms in adolescence: Prospective associations and the mediating role of brooding rumination. J. Youth Adolesc. 2021, 50, 1003–1016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Nelis, S.; Bastin, M.; Raes, F.; Mezulis, A.; Bijttebier, P. Trait affectivity and response styles to positive affect: Negative affectivity relates to dampening and positive affectivity relates to enhancing. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2016, 96, 148–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Teppers, E.; Klimstra, T.A.; Damme, C.V.; Luyckx, K.; Vanhalst, J.; Goossens, L. Personality traits, loneliness, and attitudes toward aloneness in adolescence. J. Soc. Pers. Relatsh. 2013, 30, 1045–1063. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Vanhalst, J.; Luyckx, K.; Goossens, L. Experiencing loneliness in adolescence: A matter of individual characteristics, negative peer experiences, or both? Soc. Dev. 2014, 23, 100–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Little, R.J.A. A test of missing completely at random for multivariate data with missing values. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 1988, 83, 1198–1202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Ullman, J.B. Structural equation modeling. In Using Multivariate Statistics; Tabachnick, B.G., Fidell, L.S., Eds.; Pearson: London, UK, 2013; pp. 681–785. [Google Scholar]
  27. Maes, M.; Van den Noortgate, W.; Goossens, L. A reliability generalization study for a multidimensional loneliness scale. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2015, 31, 294–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Muthén, L.K.; Muthén, B.O. Mplus User’s Guide; Muthén & Muthén: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  29. Mulder, J.D.; Hamaker, E.L. Three extensions of the random intercept cross-lagged panel model. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 2020, 28, 638–648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Satorra, A.; Bentler, P.M. A scaled chi-square test statistic for moment structure analysis. Psychometrika 2001, 66, 507–514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Hu, L.-T.; Bentler, P.M.-T.; Bentler, P.M. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 1999, 6, 1–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Geiser, C. Linear structural equation models. In Data Analysis with Mplus; Little, T.D., Ed.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2010; pp. 24–80. [Google Scholar]
  33. Lim, M.H.; Rodebaugh, T.L.; Zyphur, M.J.; Gleeson, J.F.M. Loneliness over time: The crucial role of social anxiety. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2016, 125, 620–630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Vanhalst, J.; Klimstra, T.A.; Luyckx, K.; Scholte, R.H.; Engels, R.C.; Goossens, L. The interplay of loneliness and depressive symptoms across adolescence: Exploring the role of personality traits. J. Youth Adolesc. 2012, 41, 776–787. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Gómez-López, M.; Viejo, C.; Ortega-Ruiz, R. Well-being and romantic relationships: A systematic review in adolescence and emerging adulthood. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 2415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Random intercept cross-lagged model with standardized path coefficients from the meta-analysis. W: Wave. ** p = 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Figure 1. Random intercept cross-lagged model with standardized path coefficients from the meta-analysis. W: Wave. ** p = 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Ijerph 19 07014 g001
Table 1. Correlations among study variables.
Table 1. Correlations among study variables.
VariableαMSD12345
1. Parent W10.901.700.58-
2. Parent W20.921.670.590.68 ***-
3. Parent W30.921.640.570.61 ***0.72 ***-
4. Peer W10.891.640.550.21 ***0.19 **0.18 *-
5. Peer W20.901.630.550.19 ***0.26 ***0.25 **0.56 ***-
6. Peer W30.901.620.540.18 ***0.22 ***0.31 ***0.49 ***0.63 ***
N = 3391. W1: Wave 1. W2: Wave 2. W3: Wave 3. * p < 0.05. ** p < 0.01. *** p < 0.001.
Table 2. Model fit of the random intercept cross-lagged panel models of peer- and parent-related loneliness.
Table 2. Model fit of the random intercept cross-lagged panel models of peer- and parent-related loneliness.
ModelRMSEASRMRCFIχ2dfΔχ2df
10.0230.0100.99813.842 *5
20.0300.0200.99524.066 ***610.121 **1
30.0250.0150.99618.758 **64.834 *1
4.20.0200.0100.99814.021 *60.2091
5.20.0190.0100.99813.713 *60.2581
6.20.0250.0130.99718.27864.418 *1
70.0170.0100.99813.640 *70.3262
χ2: Satorra–Bentler χ2. N = 3391. Chi2 difference test was always performed in comparison with the preferred model in the step before. * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.
Table 3. Path coefficients of the random intercept cross-lagged panel Model 7 of parent- and peer-related loneliness.
Table 3. Path coefficients of the random intercept cross-lagged panel Model 7 of parent- and peer-related loneliness.
PathBSEβp
Parent W1 à Parent W20.270.080.240.002
Parent W2 à Parent W30.360.060.36<0.001
Peer W1 à Peer W20.190.060.190.001
Peer W2 à Peer W30.330.050.34<0.001
Parent W1 à Peer W20.030.040.030.471
Parent W2 à Peer W30.030.040.030.471
Peer W1 à Parent W20.000.040.000.983
Peer W2 à Parent W30.110.040.110.005
Age à Parent W10.050.010.15<0.001
Age à Parent W20.050.010.15<0.001
Age à Parent W30.050.010.15<0.001
Age à Peer W10.000.010.000.891
Age à Peer W20.000.010.000.891
Age à Peer W30.000.010.000.891
Parent W1—Peer W10.010.010.070.165
Parent W2—Peer W20.030.000.21<0.001
Parent W3—Peer W30.030.000.25<0.001
RI Parent—RI Peer0.060.010.24<0.001
N = 3391. Parent: parent-related loneliness. Peer: peer-related loneliness. W1: Wave 1. W2: Wave 2. W3: Wave 3. RI: random intercept. SE: standard error. à represents regression paths.—represents correlations.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Geukens, F.; Spithoven, A.; Bastin, M.; Vanhalst, J.; Maes, M. Lonely in Different Relationships: Bidirectional Effects between Parent- and Peer-Related Loneliness in Adolescence. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 7014. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19127014

AMA Style

Geukens F, Spithoven A, Bastin M, Vanhalst J, Maes M. Lonely in Different Relationships: Bidirectional Effects between Parent- and Peer-Related Loneliness in Adolescence. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(12):7014. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19127014

Chicago/Turabian Style

Geukens, Flore, Annette Spithoven, Margot Bastin, Janne Vanhalst, and Marlies Maes. 2022. "Lonely in Different Relationships: Bidirectional Effects between Parent- and Peer-Related Loneliness in Adolescence" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 12: 7014. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19127014

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop