Next Article in Journal
Management for Caries Prevention in ADHD Children
Previous Article in Journal
Toward a Theory of the Underpinnings and Vulnerabilities of Structural Racism: Looking Upstream from Disease Inequities among People Who Use Drugs
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Effects of Indoor Plants on Human Functions: A Systematic Review with Meta-Analyses

Department of Landscape Architecture, National Chin-Yi University of Technology, Taichung 411030, Taiwan
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(12), 7454; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19127454
Submission received: 11 May 2022 / Revised: 12 June 2022 / Accepted: 15 June 2022 / Published: 17 June 2022

Abstract

:
The influences of indoor plants on people have been examined by only three systematic reviews and no meta-analyses. The objective of this study was therefore to investigate the effects of indoor plants on individuals’ physiological, cognitive, health-related, and behavioral functions by conducting a systematic review with meta-analyses to fill the research gap. The eligibility criteria of this study were (1) any type of participants, (2) any type of indoor plants, (3) comparators without any plants or with other elements, (4) any type of objective human function outcomes, (5) any type of study design, and (6) publications in either English or Chinese. Records were extracted from the Web of Science (1990–), Scopus (1970–), WANFANG DATA (1980–), and Taiwan Periodical Literature (1970–). Therefore, at least two databases were searched in English and in Chinese—two of the most common languages in the world. The last search date of all four databases was on 18 February 2021. We used a quality appraisal system to evaluate the included records. A total of 42 records was included for the systematic review, which concluded that indoor plants affect participants’ functions positively, particularly those of relaxed physiology and enhanced cognition. Separate meta-analyses were then conducted for the effects of the absence or presence of indoor plants on human functions. The meta-analyses comprised only 16 records. The evidence synthesis showed that indoor plants can significantly benefit participants’ diastolic blood pressure (−2.526, 95% CI −4.142, −0.909) and academic achievement (0.534, 95% CI 0.167, 0.901), whereas indoor plants also affected participants’ electroencephalography (EEG) α and β waves, attention, and response time, though not significantly. The major limitations of this study were that we did not include the grey literature and used only two or three records for the meta-analysis of each function. In brief, to achieve the healthy city for people’s health and effective functioning, not only are green spaces needed in cities, but also plants are needed in buildings.

1. Introduction

Throughout history, humans have valued the health benefits of contact with nature [1]. Theoretically, an evolutionary perspective suggests that evolutionary processes enable humans to respond adaptively and positively to nature [2], whereas a cultural perspective contends that culture affects people’s relations with the natural environment [3]. In line with the evolutionary perspective, the concept of biophilia claims that humans are born with emotional connections with nature and/or other living organisms [4]. This emotional predisposition is deeply embedded in the biological nature of humans and does not disappear even after people leave the natural environment to live a modern urban life [5]. Moreover, the Stress Reduction Theory (SRT; [3]) emphasizes that stress is “the process by which an individual responds psychologically, physiologically, and often with behaviors, to a situation that challenges or threatens well-being” ([6], p. 202), and the natural environment is helpful for recovery from stress, whereas the Attention Restoration Theory (ART; [7]) emphasizes that the natural environment is beneficial to the restoration of directed attention for people’s effective functioning. Empirically, an increasing number of studies on human interaction with nature have demonstrated that contact with nature is favorable to human emotions, physiological functioning, attention restoration, behavior, and health [8,9,10]. Scholars have also conducted systematic reviews (e.g., [11,12,13,14,15] and meta-analyses on related topics (e.g., [16,17,18,19,20,21]). In total, by 2022, more than 60 reviews and meta-analyses regarding nature and health and well-being had been conducted (cf. [22]).
Despite long interests in the theoretical and empirical value of nature to humans, at present, 55% of the world population lives in cities, and the urban population worldwide is expected to increase by 68% by 2050 [23]. For this reason, the World Health Organization Regional Office for Europe in 2016 [24] published a report titled “Urban Green Spaces and Health—A Review of Evidence” to address the importance of nature and green spaces for urban living. The World Health Organization also advocates the healthy city, defined as “one that continually creates and improves its physical and social environments and expands the community resources that enable people to mutually support each other in performing all the functions of life and developing to their maximum potential” [25]. Individuals in contemporary society nevertheless spend most of their time indoors [26], with urban dwellers spending more than 80% of their life indoors [27,28,29]. Moreover, urbanities often do not have ready access to nature [30]. Consequently, urbanites have few opportunities to maintain contact with nature. Although nature includes many elements, plants are the most representative symbol of nature [31,32]. Similarly, “green space” refers to open, underdeveloped, naturally planted land [33], land with grass or trees, or other vegetation region [34]. Studies of indoor nature also tend to focus on plants [35,36]. The exploration of the physical and psychological benefits of indoor plants on people, therefore, merits more attention [37,38]. Interior environments and indoor plants could be important elements of the healthy city. The Rural Development Administration of South Korea suggests placing one small potted plant and one large potted plant per 6 m2 floor area in a room to improve the indoor quality [39]. Systematic reviews and/or meta-analyses of the effects of indoor plants on people, however, are far less common than those on natural environments and/or green spaces.
There are only three narrative reviews on the influences of indoor plants related to people. Bringslimark et al. [31] reviewed 21 articles of the experimental research focusing on the benefits of indoor plants on people, which identified benefits such as stress reduction and pain tolerance enhancement. This study was a great stepping stone for later research, particularly with respect to experimental design, measurement, analysis, and reporting. Given that this narrative review was published more than a decade ago, updates are necessary, as is a further distinction of the benefits identified as either self-reported perceptions or objectively measured outcomes using devices or tasks. Deng and Deng [40] reviewed the importance of indoor plants to human health with respect to photosynthesis, transpiration, psychological effects, and air purification, indicating the influence of indoor plants on task performance, health, and stress. Moya et al. [41] reviewed 104 articles published in specific journals between 1984 and 2017 on the influence of vegetation on indoor environmental quality, finding that indoor plants improved people’s comfort, satisfaction, and happiness but presented no strong evidence of improvements in performance and productivity. The inconsistent findings on the influences of indoor plants on participant’s performance [40,41] await further clarification. Further, these three narrative reviews did not follow the rigorous Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA; [42]), which may have resulted in subjectivity and a lack of transparency and comprehensiveness [43]. They also did not cover studies published in widely used languages, such as Chinese.
Recently, three systematic reviews were conducted to address the gaps left by previous narrative reviews of research on indoor plants and human responses. One followed the Centre for Reviews and Dissemination (CRD) guidelines [44], and two followed the PRISMA guidelines. The first systematic review [45] following the PRISMA covered studies published in English and Chinese—two of the most common languages in the world—focused on self-reported perceptions and included 50 empirical studies, which concluded that the primary beneficial effects of indoor plants were an increase in positive emotions and a reduction in negative feelings, while secondary benefits included a reduction in physical discomfort. The second systematic review [36] following the CRD covered 26 studies of the health and well-being impacts of indoor nature (actual and simulated plants and aquariums) on the elderly and concluded that higher-quality studies showed that indoor gardening programs were helpful for cognition, psychological well-being, social outcomes, and life satisfaction. The third review [35] following the PRISMA covered 37 studies published in English and Dutch on the influences of indoor and outdoor nature on adolescents, which found associations between outdoor campus green space and enhanced quality of life and perceived restoration. The common findings of the two systematic reviews are that indoor plants benefit psychological well-being [36,45]. Self-reported psychological responses, however, may be different from actual human functions (cf. [46]). Moreover, Yeo et al. [36] researched only older adults and did not specifically focus on indoor plants. Van den Bogerd et al. [35] researched only adolescents and did not specifically focus on indoor plants. Furthermore, none of these three reviews conducted meta-analyses to provide quantitatively synthesized evidence of the effects of indoor plants on humans. This may be because of the heterogeneity of the outcomes.
Given the above-mentioned factors, the purpose of the present study was to perform a systematic review with meta-analyses of Chinese and English empirical quantitative research on the influences of indoor plants on human functions, in order to address the current research gap of the lack of meta-analyses on this subject and to respond to the fulfillment of the daily functions of urbanites as advocated by the promotion of the healthy city. Specifically, the objective of this study was to examine if the presence of indoor plants of any type serving as an intervention has any objectively measured effects, such as using devices, tasks, examinations, or performance records, on human functions. We reviewed all research with any study design that assessed all human functions exposed to indoor plants against those exposed to no indoor plants or other elements. Accordingly, the review question was whether indoor plants have any effects on human functions. The present systematic review may provide more comprehensive information associated with the aforementioned effects and serve as a reference for future research. This study identifies what empirical and quantitative studies of human functions have been performed in relation to indoor plants, particularly regarding research validity (cf. [31]), such as plant quantity measurements (construct validity: number, size, volume percentage, and green coverage ratio), potential effect modifiers (confounder: exposure duration, distance to plants, room climate, and room size), funding (conflict of interest), and what and how further research could be performed. The meta-analyses further examined the overall results of studies exploring the effects of indoor plants on human functions, rather than reviewing studies individually. Systemic reviews and meta-analyses are effective in providing optimal synthesis evidence, and the constantly updated data may serve as a basis for policy making [43], such as regarding the healthy city or effective human functions [25]. The systematic review and meta-analyses conducted in this study are the first to provide a synthesis of the quantitative evidence regarding the specific effects of indoor plants on human functions.

2. Methods

This study followed the PRISMA guidelines, particularly their specific checklist items and item orders, although PRISMA focuses on evaluating interventions in the field of medical care [42]. The conduct of the review involved no significant deviations from the protocol, except that we searched two more databases than the protocol.

2.1. Eligibility Criteria

The eligibility criteria for inclusion of a study in this research were as follows: (1) participants of any type were recruited; (2) no criteria were set for the type of indoor plant to be used in interventions; (3) the comparator was participants in an indoor environment without any plants or with other elements; (4) the outcome included any type of objectively measured human function, such as use of devices, performance tasks, examinations, or records, rather than self-reports; (5) all types of study design were included; and (6) the language was either English or Chinese. Because Chinese and English are the most commonly used languages in the world, studies written in these two languages were selected to decrease the risk of language bias [43]. Other languages were not included because of limited resources.

2.2. Information Sources

The information sources included four electronic databases, of which the Core Collections hosted by Web of Science (1988–) and Scopus (1970–) are English-language databases, while the Journal Collections hosted by WANFANG DATA (1980–) and Taiwan Periodical Literature (1970–) are Chinese-language databases. At least two databases, therefore, were searched for each of the two languages. The final search on WANFANG DATA was performed on 14 August 2019, while that on the Web of Science was performed on 11 November 2019. The search on Taiwan Periodical Literature was performed on 21 October 2020, and the search on Scopus was performed on 13 November 2020. We searched WANFANG DATA and Web of Science in the first round and Taiwan Periodical Literature and Scopus in the second. About one year thus elapsed between the searches of the two rounds. The follow-up searches of all four databases were completed on 18 February 2021. The coverage cutoff date was 31 December 2020. We decided that the coverage ended at the end of 2020 rather than in the middle of the year, so later updated searches could continue at the start of 2021. Moreover, two supplementary approaches to identifying studies were applied: one was that the related studies were identified by reference searches of the included studies, while The other was contact with the authors of the included studies to seek missing information, particularly regarding the results.

2.3. Search

The search terms included the following: “indoor”, “interior”, “architecture”, “building”, “plant”, “vegetation”, “greening”, “greenery”, “green”, “greenness”, “perception”, “psychology”, “emotion”, “physiology”, “cognition”, “restoration”, “behavior”, “health”, and “performance”, as found in previous studies [8,45,47] and in peer reviewers’ suggestions. In the Boolean search, only “AND” was adopted as the operator, as in (1) indoor “AND” plant “AND” perception, or (2) architecture “AND” greening “AND” psychology. Except for the eligibility criteria and the search coverage, we did not have any restrictions such as topics, keywords, or dates. The full search strings applicable to all four databases are listed in Supplementary Material Table S1.

2.4. Study Selection

The present systematic review included only quantitative empirical studies published in journals, primarily because of their relatively easy accessibility. Technical reports, proceedings, books, and unpublished theses or dissertations (i.e., grey literature) therefore were not considered. Empirical studies using plants in a room or building as the intervention, irrespective of how many plants, what sizes, what types, foliage or floral, actual or virtual, duration of presence, or distance from the participants, were included in accordance with the eligibility criteria. “Empirical research” refers to the analysis of real data, and quantitative research uses computation, mathematics, and statistics to explore the target phenomenon [48]. Regarding the causal relationship between variables in quantitative research, randomized controlled experiments, in which participants are randomly assigned to experimental and control groups (also referred to as “RCT” in clinical professions), provide higher-quality results than nonrandomized controlled quasi-experiments, in which participants are not randomly assigned to experimental and control groups (also referred to as “non-RCT”), and quasi-experiments outperform surveys [49]. Field experiments conducted in real-world environments, however, exhibit more favorable ecological validity than laboratory experiments [50]. If surveys were used to collect objective outcomes such as health indicators, they were also included.

2.5. Data Collection Process

L.-W.R. performed searches with the abovementioned terms in the databases and reviewed study titles and abstracts that met the eligibility criteria. L.-S.L. independently performed searches with the same terms in the same databases and reviewed study titles and abstracts that met the eligibility criteria. As a result, L.-W.R. and L.-S.L. had an agreement rate of 99.9% on both Web of Science and WANFANG DATA, respectively, and L.-S.L. and K.-T.H. also had an agreement rate of 99.9% on Taiwan Periodical Literature. In cases where the title and abstract were insufficient to determine the study’s eligibility, L.-W.R. or L.-S.L. proceeded to read the full text. All the studies of each included paper were reviewed. Then, K.-T.H. reviewed the extracted full-text studies that met the eligibility criteria. K.-T.H. and L.-W.R. conducted data extraction and quality appraisal. Initial disagreement regarding a study’s opinion was resolved by discussion between the two reviewers.

2.6. Data Items

The following 14 data items were extracted from the reviewed records: sources, participants, interventions, comparator, exposure duration, distance to plants, room climate, room size, study design, functions, function categories, outcomes, funding, and languages.

2.7. Risk of Bias in Individual Studies

The included studies were analyzed in accordance with the quality appraisal system proposed by Ohly et al. [19]. This appraisal system comprises 19 appraisal items, including quality indicators from the CRD [44], critical appraisal checklists from the Critical Appraisal Skills Program [51], and quality assessment tool for quantitative studies from the Effective Public Health Practice Project [52]. We adopted the quality appraisal system because it was more comprehensive and current than other appraisal systems. This appraisal system, which has an option of criterion inapplicable to this study design, was applied to both RCTs and nonrandomized studies [19,20].

2.8. Summary Measures

The summary measures of this study included data measured in empirical research using devices, tasks, academic achievement scoring, and actual health indicators. These data were reviewed to examine the influences of indoor plants on participants’ functions.

2.9. Planned Methods of Analysis

Where sufficient studies (at least two studies) using comparable outcome measures allowed us to conduct meta-analyses, the present meta-analyses reported the means and standard deviations (SDs) of each function category. Comprehensive Meta-Analysis version 3 (Biostat, Englewood, NJ, USA) was applied to conduct Cochran’s Q tests and draw forest plots as well as to analyze pooled effect sizes, sensitivities, and publication biases. Because the measurement of functions in similar categories varied between the records, the measurement outcomes of these categories were processed using the standardized mean difference (SMD), thereby providing an indicator enabling the comparison and synthesis of function outcomes in these categories (cf. [43]). For records using the same methods to measure function in the same categories, outcomes were, in general, directly compared and synthesized to determine the mean differences (MDs). For studies with more than one experimental group [53], each experimental group and control group was separately analyzed [43]. Specifically, Cochran’s Q test was performed to examine whether the classification outcomes of function in each category were heterogeneous or homogenous. Additionally, forest plots were adopted to present the relative importance and research outcome directions among the studies visually. Subsequently, the pooled effect size was computed using fixed-effect or random-effect models.

2.10. Risk of Bias across Studies

The meta-analyses used funnel plots and Egger’s regressions to identify potential publication bias on the basis of the research results of each function category.

2.11. Additional Analyses

The meta-analyses included the sensitivity analysis of the results of the records for each function category, which examined if the pooled effect sizes changed notably when any of the records was removed—an indication of the stability of the results.

3. Results

3.1. Study Selection

The abovementioned terms were used to search the four databases separately. The search yielded 30,887 records from the Web of Science, 4323 from WANFANG DATA, 30,203 from Scopus, and 4105 from Taiwan Periodical Literature. Repeated records were excluded. Moreover, 11 records were identified after searching the references of the searched papers (which were considered as other sources), resulting in 31,728 journal articles in total. Papers with titles and abstracts meeting the eligibility criteria were identified, resulting in 63 preliminarily qualified papers. The full texts of these 63 papers were extracted for further scrutiny, and 21 studies that failed to meet the criteria were excluded. As a result, 42 qualifying records were included (Figure 1). The major reasons for excluding records were that the research was not empirical and quantitative, human functions were not objectively measured, and plant functions were measured (Supplementary Material Table S2).

3.2. Study Characteristics

Among the 42 journal articles included in the systematic review, 5 (11.9%) and 37 (88.1%) were written in Chinese and English, respectively. The earliest paper was published in 1996, and the latest in 2020 (the terminus of the search coverage). The 25 year coverage period was divided into 5 year intervals to analyze the number of publications during each interval. The number of published papers increased relatively steadily (Table 1).
In terms of geographical distribution, most of the studies were from China (10; 23.8%), followed by the United States (8; 19.0%), Japan (6; 14.3%), South Korea (5; 11.9%), and Taiwan (4; 9.5%). Asia was thus the leading continent, followed by America, Europe, and Africa. Most studies were from the Global North, followed by the Global South and the Equatorial region (Table 2). Detailed statistics could not be compiled because not every record provided the participants’ socioeconomic backgrounds. The majority of the participants, however, were college students. Only six studies recruited office workers [54,55,56,57,58,59], five studies recruited patients [60,61,62,63,64], two studies recruited junior high school students [65,66], and one study recruited high school students as participants [67] (Table 2).
The records generally did not focus on only one measure of human functions. The reviewers found that they examined 52 functions. Most of the records were related to physiology (27; 51.9%), followed by cognition (15; 28.8%), health, (seven; 13.5%), and behavior (three; 5.8%). Concerning study design, most of the records adopted experimental methods (26; 61.9%). These were followed by those conducting field experiments (seven; 16.7%), field quasi-experiments (five; 11.9%), and surveys (four; 9.5%). Among the 42 records, 20 reported the specific number of indoor potted plants as the intervention; the highest number of potted plants was 34 [66], and the lowest number of potted plants was one [56,59,60,68,69,70,71]. Three papers reported the green coverage ratio, with the highest at 10% and the lowest at 3% [57]. Two papers also indicated the volume of indoor plants as a percentage of the total experimental space, where the largest volume was 17.9% [53] and the smallest was 5% [67] (Table 3). In addition, three records used photographs or slides as surrogates for indoor plants [72,73,74], and one paper employed virtual-reality plants [75] (Table 4).
A total of 34 papers recorded the time during which participants were exposed to indoor plants. Among these, the longest exposure time was one year [76] and the shortest was 15 s [72]. Thirty-three papers reported the room size. The experiment room used by Toyoda et al. [59] was the largest in terms of its floor area (1260 m2), and that used by Genjo et al. [57] was the largest in terms of its volume (675 m3). By contrast, the room used by Kim et al. [77] was the smallest in both floor area (7.26 m2) and volume (14.52 m3). Among the records, only 13 reported the participant–plant distance in a room, with the greatest distance being 3 m [72] and the smallest being 0.38 m [60] (Table 3).
Some records also provided data on the ambient environment in which the plants were placed. Specifically, 19 papers recorded the room temperature, with the highest being 27 °C [78] and the lowest 20 °C [59]. Humidity was reported in 13 papers, with the highest value at 70% [79] and the lowest 34% [80]. Only one record measured wind speed (0.2 m·s−1; [81]). Twelve records indicated lighting, of which only one adopted the quantum as the lighting unit (10.6 μmol m−2·s−1; [69]), whereas the remaining 11 used illuminance as the unit. The most intense lighting was 1365.5 lux [82], while the least intense lighting was 300 lux [56] (Table 3).
Among the 42 records, only 18 indicated their funding sources. Most of the funding sources were in governmental sectors, while only two may be from stakeholders ([83], American Horticultural Therapy Association; [68], The Swedish Flower Corporations) (Table 4). Funding from stakeholders might cause a conflict of interest.
Table 4. Summary of the study characteristics of the records.
Table 4. Summary of the study characteristics of the records.
SourceParticipantInterventionsComparatorExposure
Duration
Distance
to Plants
Room
Size
Room
Climate
Study
Design
FunctionsFunction
Category
FundingPublication
Language
[78]96 US adults (48 males and 48 females, 80 of whom were college students), age: 18 to 46 Presence or absence of 17 potted plants in a computer labControl 13.5 × 7.3 × 2.6 m27 °C, 38% RH, 420 luxField experiment
(RCT)
SBP, reaction timePhysiology, cognition English
[53]81 US adults10 potted plants (accounting 7.16% of the space), 22 potted plants (accounting 17.88% of the space), or no plants in an officeControl15–20 min 12.08 m2, 31.3 m3 Field experiment
(RCT)
A sorting task, a productivity taskCognition English
[84]814 Chinese participants (347 males and 467 females), ethnicity: AsianA building with or without indoor greening Survey
(non-RCT)
Neurobehavioral Functioning Evaluation System TestingCognitionSciences and Technology Commission of ShanghaiChinese
[80]198 US adults (71 males and 127 females), 176 of whom were college students5 potted plants, nonplant objects, no plants in a roomNonplant objects, controlabout 17 min 3.5 × 6 × 2.4 m23 °C, 34% RH, 703 luxExperiment
(RCT)
Skin temperature, blood pressure, pain tolerance Physiology, behavior English
[83]150 US college students (75 males and 75 females), mean age: 19.69 potted red-flowering geraniums, 9 potted non-flowering geraniums, no plants in a labNon-flowering plants, control5 min1.8 m 22.4 °CExperiment
(RCT)
EEG, EDA, finger skin temperaturePhysiologyAmerican Horticultural Therapy AssociationEnglish
[70]146 Japanese college students (83 males and 63 females), ethnicity: Asian1 potted 1-m-tall plant placed in front of the participant, the same plant placed on the right-hand side of the participant, no plants in a roomControl15 min2.345 m in front of and 1.75 m at the side of the participants5.81 × 2.78 × 2.35 m Experiment
(RCT)
An association task, a sorting taskCognition English
[69]66 US college students (32 males and 34 females), age: 91% from 18 to 241 potted flower arrangement (45 × 45 × 45 cm), lavender fragrance, flower and fragrance, or no plants and no fragrance in a labControl30 min 3.5 × 2.7 × 2.4 m21 °C, 10.6 μmol·m−2·s−1Experiment
(RCT)
EEG, EDA, skin temperaturePhysiology English
[85]90 US college female students, mean age: 18.9Foliage and flowing plants, flowing plants, or no plants in a labControl5 min maximum1.4 m3.9 × 2.3 × 2.7 m21.7 °C, 904 luxExperiment
(RCT)
Pain tolerance, EEG, EDA, finger skin temperatureBehavior, physiology English
[71]90 Japanese college students (35 males and 55 females), ethnicity: Asian1 potted 1.5-m-tall plant, a magazine rack put at the same location, or no plants and no magazine racks in a roomA magazine rack, control15 minAbout 2.9 m in front of the participant2.78 × 5.81 × 2.35 m Experiment
(RCT)
An association taskCognition English
[72]38 Taiwanese college students (10 males and 28 females), ethnicity: AsianPresentation of 6 slides (office without a window view nor indoor plants, office without a window view but with indoor plants, office with a city window view but without indoor plants, office with a city window view and with indoor plants, office with a nature window view but without indoor plants, and office with a nature window view and with indoor plants) in a labControl15 s for each slide3 m7 × 5 m25 °CExperiment
(non-RCT)
EEG, EMG, BVPPhysiology English
[55]364 Norwegian office workers, mean age: 43.1Presence or absence of potted plants on desks or shelves in an office Survey
(non-RCT)
Sick leaveHealth English
[68]50 healthy Swedish people (23 males and 27 females), mean age: 39.21 potted flowering begonias (Begonia Elatior) approximately 22 cm high (control plant irrigated with ordinary local tap water; experiment plant irrigated with vortex-rotated local tap water) in an officePlant irrigated with ordinary local tap water10 min for each plant 5.6 × 3.0 × 2.4 m23–24 °C, 36–38% RH, 570–650 luxExperiment
(RCT)
Heart rate, heart rate variability, power spectral densityPhysiologyThe Swedish Flower CorporationsEnglish
[63]90 South Korean patients who had received appendectomy (52 males and 38 females), mean age: 37.6, ethnicity: AsianPresence or absence of 12 potted flowering plants in a wardControl Field experiment
(RCT)
Pain killer consumption, blood pressure, body temperature, heart rate, respiratory rateHealth, physiology English
[67]140 South Korean female high school students, ethnicity: AsianPresence or absence of plants in 2 classrooms (accounting for 5% of the space) Control14 weeks of school time Field quasi-experiment
(non-RCT, pre-post design)
Cortisol level, healthPhysiology, health English
[86]89 US sophomoresPresence or absence of plants in a classroomControl1 semester of class time Field quasi-experiment
(non-RCT)
Course gradeCognition English
[65]76 Taiwanese junior high school students (58 males and 18 females), mean age: 13.55, ethnicity: AsianPresence or absence of 6 potted plants (about 135 × 80 cm, having a green coverage ratio of 6%) in a classroomControl12 weeks of school time Field quasi-experiment
(non-RCT)
Sick leave, misconductHealth, behavior English
[64]80 South Korean female patients who had received thyroidectomy, mean age: 36.2, ethnicity: AsianPresence or absence of 12 potted flowering plants in a wardControl Field experiment
(RCT)
Pain killer consumption, hospitalization daysHealth English
[87]34 Norwegian college students (12 males and 22 females), mean age: 24.15Presence or absence of 4 potted plants (2 flowering pink Phalaenopsis, 1 30-cm-tall Aglaonema commutatum, and 1 120-cm-tall Schefflera arboricola) in an officeControl60 min 3.9 × 2.1 × 3.6 m Experiment
(RCT)
The Reading Span TaskCognition English
[66]36 Taiwanese junior high school students (18 males and 18 females), mean age: 12.41, ethnicity: AsianTaking care of 34 potted plants inside and outside a classroom (with a green coverage ratio of 6.3% indoors)Control18 weeks of school time Field experiment
(RCT)
Examination scoreCognition Chinese
[73]30 Chinese college students (15 males and 15 females), ethnicity: AsianPresentation of 5 photos of vegetation landscapes and a blank in a room Control2 min0.5 m7 × 4 × 3 m25 °C, 40% RHExperiment
(RCT)
ECG, blood pressure, heart rate, GSR, fingertip pulsePhysiology English
[74]30 Chinese college students (15 males and 15 females), age: 18 to 24, ethnicity: AsianPresentation of 12 photos of flowers and a blank in a roomControl2 min0.5 m7 × 4 × 3 m25 °C, 40% RHExperiment
(RCT)
Blood pressure, heart rate, GSR, fingertip plusPhysiologyNational Key Technology Research
and Development Program in China
English
[88]29 Japanese college students (14 males and 15 females), age: 19 to 24, ethnicity: AsianPotted Hedera helix L. (60 × 40 cm) of 5 different colors on a table in a roomDifferent colors of the plant1 min for each plant color0.5 m Experiment
(RCT)
Brain activity, eye movementCognitionEgyptian Ministry of Higher EducationEnglish
[89]28 Japanese undergraduate and graduate students (14 males and 14 females), mean age: 21.42, ethnicity: AsianPlacement of 1 potted plant of 3 different colors on a table in a roomDifferent colors of plants1 min for each plant color1.5 m59.4 m223 °C, 55% RH, 700 luxExperiment
(RCT)
Eye movement, brain activityCognitionEgyptian Ministry of Higher EducationEnglish
[79]30 South Korean college students (15 males and 15 females), mean age: 23.5, ethnicity: AsianPlacement of potted plants (60 × 40 cm) of 5 different colors on a box in a classroomDifferent colors of plants3 min for each plant color1 m7 × 4.5 × 2.8 m25 °C, 70% RH, 700 luxExperiment
(RCT)
EEGPhysiology English
[58]Study 3: 33 British adult office workers (16 males and 17 females), mean age: 28Study 3: presence or absence of 8 potted plants (average height 90 cm) in an officeControl Study 3: Field experiment
(RCT)
An information management and processing task, a vigilance taskCognition English
[81]16 Chinese college students (8 males and 8 females), mean age: 23.5, ethnicity: AsianPresence of potted plants of the combinations of 3 colors, 3 scents, and 3 sizes on a table in an officeCombinations of plant colors, scents, and sizes10–15 min 22 °C, 41.65% RH, 0.2 ms−1 wind velocityExperiment
(RCT)
EEG, ECG, oxyhaemoglobin saturation, fingertip blood flow, skin resistance, respiration ratePhysiologySciences and Technology Commission of ShanghaiEnglish
[82]24 South Korean male adults, mean age: 24.9, ethnicity: AsianA plant transplanting task, a computer operation task on a table in a greenhouse roomA computer task15 min 20.8 °C, 57.7% RH, 1365.5 luxExperiment
(RCT)
Heart rate variability, blood pressure, pulse ratePhysiology English
[54]565 Norwegian office workersOutdoor nature contact, indoor nature contact, and outdoor view through windows Survey
(non-RCT)
Sick leaveHealth English
[61]270 Pakistani surgical patients, ethnicity: AsianPresence or absence of foliage plants and flower arrangements in a wardControl Field experiment
(RCT)
Blood pressure, heart rate, respirationrate, body temperature, hospitalization days, analgesics consumptionPhysiology, healthThe University of Agriculture Peshawar in PakistanEnglish
[90]30 Egyptian male college students, age: 22 to 37, ethnicity: AfricanPotted Hedera helix L. (60 × 40 cm) of 5 different colors on a table in a roomDifferent colors of the plant1 min for each plant color0.5 m59.4 m221 °C, 55% RHExperiment
(RCT)
Eye movements, brain activityCognition, physiologyEgyptian Ministry of Higher EducationEnglish
[91]5 Indonesians, ethnicity: AsianA room with 5 potted plants and a room without plantsControl30 min Experiment
(non-RCT)
Heart rate, blood pressurePhysiologyMinistry of National Education in IndonesiaEnglish
[77]66 Hong Kongese college students (40 males and 26 females), mean age: 25.6, ethnicity: AsianA basement room with plants, with a fake window, with plants and a fake window, and without plants nor a windowControlAt least 8 min 3.3 × 2.2 × 2 m24 °CExperiment
(non-RCT)
EDA, a response time taskPhysiology, cognitionHong Kong Polytechnic UniversityEnglish
[75]28 US adults (12 males and 16 females), age: 23 to 42Presence or absence of plants in an actual environment and a virtual oneControl5 min Experiment
(RCT)
Heart rate, EDA, blood pressure, a visual reaction time task, The Stroop task, a visual backward digit span taskPhysiology, cognitionCampus Sustainability
Innovation Fund, Harvard University Office for
Sustainability
English
[57]36–41 Japanese office workers, mean age: 33.95, ethnicity: Asian Presence (3–10% green coverage ratio) or absence of plants in 2 officesControl16 weeks of working hours 132 m2 (321 m3), 270 m2 (675 m3) Field quasi-experiment
(non-RCT)
Heart rate, salivary amylase activity, critical flicker fusion frequency, fingertip pulse wavePhysiologyGrant-in-Aid for Scientific Research, Japan Society for the Promotion of ScienceEnglish
[60]50 Chinese female elders with hypertension, mean age: 79.2, ethnicity: AsianPresence or absence of 1 potted plant on a table in a roomControl5 min0.38 m 23 °C, 40% RH, 500 lux,Experiment
(RCT)
Blood pressure, EEGPhysiology English
[76]100 Taiwanese elders, age: >65, ethnicity: AsianPresence or absence of plants in houses 1 year Survey
(non-RCT)
Blood pressure, heart ratePhysiologyMinistry of Science and Technology in TaiwanEnglish
[59]63 adult Japanese office workers (33 males and 30 females), mean age: 40.15, ethnicity: AsianPresence or absence of 1 potted plant (15–20 cm tall, 7–10 cm wide) on the desk in an officeControl3 min 1260 m220–24 °C, 40–50% RH, 500–700 luxField experiment
(non-RCT, pre-post design)
Pulse ratePhysiology English
[56]30 Chinese female office workers, mean age: 29.42, ethnicity: AsianPresence or absence of 1 potted plant with blue or purple flowers on a desk in an officeControl3 min0.4 m 21 °C, 50% RH, 300 luxField quasi-experiment
(non-RCT, pre-post design)
EEG, heart rate variability, skin conductancePhysiologyNational Nature Science Foundation of ChinaEnglish
[92]33 Chinese elders, age: 65 to 99, ethnicity: AsianCombination of potted succulents (3–10 cm tall, 3 cm wide) or flower arrangement (50–60 cm tall, 5–18 cm wide) performed indoorsFlower arrangement25 min Experiment
(RCT)
Salivary cortisolPhysiologyNational Nature Science Foundation of ChinaChinese
[62]34 Chinese elders with dementia (13 males and 21 females), ethnicity: AsianWith or without a treatment course of indoor horticultural activities (sowing, transplanting seedlings, succulents potting, and herbal flower potting)Control30 min Experiment
(non-RCT)
Blood pressure, heart rate, ECGPhysiologyNational Nature Science Foundation of China, Beijing Science and Technology Project FoundationChinese
[93]44 Chinese elders living alone, ethnicity: AsianFour kinds of indoor horticultural activities (sowing, transplanting seedlings, succulents potting, and herbal flower potting)Within- participants, between-participants30 min Experiment
(non-RCT)
Blood pressure, heart rate, ECGPhysiologyBeijing Science and Technology Commission Green Communication FoundationChinese
[94]Study 1: 120 South Africans, mean age: 33.72, ethnicity: AfricanPresence of 3 potted plants, 6 plant pictures on 3 walls (80 × 80 cm), and no potted plants and plant pictures in an officeControl35 min 3 × 3 m21 °C, 510 luxExperiment
(RCT)
A card-sorting task, a reading taskCognition English
RH: relative humidity; SBP: systolic blood pressure; DBP: diastolic blood pressure; EEG: electroencephalography; EDA: electrodermal activity; EMG: electromyography; BVP: blood volume pulse; ECG: electrocardiography; GSR: galvanic skin response; RCT: randomized controlled trial; non-RCT: not randomized controlled trial.

3.3. Risk of Bias within Studies

Most of the included studies (90.5%) applied quasi-experimental or experimental methods. Control and experimental groups were therefore involved. In quasi-experimental research, particularly field research (11.9%), researchers were unable to assign interventions randomly to participants as is the case in clinical trials. Surveys, field quasi-experiments, and quasi-experiments, therefore, could not achieve sequence generation, which reduces the risk of bias. In addition, concealing the intervention assignment from participants was difficult because indoor plants were easily noticed in a room, resulting in lower allocation concealment ability. Similarly, blinding participants concerning their intervention was also challenging. Furthermore, the risk of incomplete data on outcomes caused by participant attrition and exclusion might exist because the included studies seldom mentioned participant attrition or exclusion.
The mean quality appraisal score of the 42 records was 17.2 points out of a possible 38, i.e., 45.3% (17.2/38 = 45.3%) of the total, indicating moderate research quality (high: 67–100%, moderate: 34–66%, low: 0–33%; [19]). The five items (of a total of 19) in the quality appraisal system for which the records included scored lowest are discussed next. First, none of the 42 papers complied with the intention-to-treat (ITT) analysis (0%) (i.e., all data were included after allocation). Additionally, the participants were not sufficiently representative because most were students (17 studies included college students, 1 study included high school students, and 2 studies included junior high school students). Only 5 papers involved general adult participants, whereas the remaining papers involved patients or office workers. The second lowest score regarding the quality appraisal system was found in the only 1 paper (2%) in which the outcome assessors were completely unaware of participant allocation. The third lowest scores were observed in the following two items of the quality appraisal system: only 2 papers (5%) reported statistical power and randomization procedure, respectively (Table 5 and Table 6 respectively). The records exhibited desirable quality in the following items of the appraisal system: (1) all the papers (100%) included individual level analyses, (2) data collection in 39 studies (95%) was consistent, (3) a total of 37 studies (90%) provided a clear description of interventions and control, and (4) 32 papers (78%) accounted for all participants and applied statistical analysis methods appropriate for study design.

3.4. Results of Individual Studies

The research outcomes of each study for the systematic review are summarized in Table 7. In brief, the systematic review concluded that indoor plants, in general, affect participants’ functions positively, particularly their physiology and cognition. Regarding physiological functions, participants exhibited greater benefits in a room with plants than in a room without plants in relation to lower blood pressure [60,61,63,76,78,82], lower electrodermal activity (EDA) [69,83,85], lower electroencephalography (EEG) α and β waves [56,69,72,81,83], lower heart rate [59,61,62,63,68,76,91,93], and lower respiration rate and body temperature [61].
Regarding cognitive functions, when indoor plants were present, participants exhibited higher academic achievement [66,86] and better performance in various cognitive tasks [58,71,75,77,78,84,87,94]. In health-related functions, with exposure to indoor plants, participants less frequently took sick leave [54,55,65,67], consumed fewer pain killers [61,63,64], and had fewer hospitalization days [64] than participants in environments where indoor plants were absent. In behavioral functions, participants presented greater pain tolerance of putting hands in cold water [80,85] and less misconduct [65] when indoor plants were in the room than when indoor plants were not in the room.

3.5. Synthesis of Results

The data for the meta-analyses included only the participants’ physiological functions (i.e., diastolic blood pressure (DBP), EEG α and β waves) and cognitive functions (i.e., attention, academic achievement, and response time) because at least two studies are needed to conduct the meta-analyses. Given that the number of the records of each of the function categories was small, randomized control trials and non-randomized studies of interventions were included for the meta-analyses. Moreover, various interventions of indoor plants regardless of species, type, quantity, exposure time, and distance to participants were dichotomized as groups with plants and groups without plants.

3.6. DBP

Three papers examining the influence of indoor plants on DBP, which was measured by sphygmomanometers measured in mmHg, were included for the meta-analysis (Table 8). In total, 248 participants were evenly exposed to conditions either with plants or without plants. Lee et al. [82] recruited only male adults in South Korea, whereas Hassan et al. [60] recruited only female older adults with high blood pressure in China. Chen et al. [76] surveyed male and female elders in Taiwan six times over one year. Both Lee et al. [82] and Hassan et al. [60] randomly assigned their participants to different groups, while Chen et al. [76] did not. All three papers were appraised as having moderate research quality.
The heterogeneity test of the three studies focusing on DBP revealed a significant difference (p < 0.05) with I2 = 97.554%, confirming high heterogeneity among the studies. A random-effect model was therefore applied. Given that the standard deviation (SD) of one study was much smaller than that of the other two, SMD, rather than MD, was adopted here. The pooled effect size (SMD) was −2.526 with a 95% confidence interval ranging between −4.142 and −0.909. The results indicated that the group with plants had significantly (p = 0.002) lower DBP values than the group without plants (Table 9). The relative weight of both the Hassan et al. [60] and Chen et al. [76] studies was about 37.00%, and that of Lee et al. [82] was 25.29% (Figure 2).

3.7. EEG α Waves

Three papers examining the influence of indoor plants on EEG α waves, which was measured by brain activity instruments with Hertz as the unit of measurement, were included for the meta-analysis (Table 10). The studies had a total of 200 participants. Among them, 85 were in the control group (without plants) and 115 in the experimental group (with plants). Chang and Chen [72] recruited college students in Taiwan and Qin et al. [81] recruited college students in China, whereas Elasdek and Liu [56] recruited only female office workers in China. Chang and Chen [72] and Elasdek and Liu [56] did not randomly assign their participants to different groups, while Qin et al. [81] did. These three papers were appraised as having moderate research quality.
The heterogeneity test of the three studies investigating the influence of indoor plants on EEG α waves revealed a significant difference (p < 0.05), with I2 = 94.488%, confirming high heterogeneity among the studies. A random-effect model was therefore adopted. The pooled effect size (MD) was 1.140, and the 95% confidence interval ranged from −0.260 to 2.540. The results indicated that the group with plants had greater EEG α waves than the group without plants, but the difference was nonsignificant (p = 0.110) (Table 11). The relative weight of both the Chang and Chen [72] and Elasdek and Liu [56] studies was about 34.6%, and that of Qin et al. [81] was 30.72% (Figure 3).

3.8. EEG β Waves

Only two papers examining the influence of indoor plants on EEG β waves, which was measured in Hertz by brain activity instruments, were included in this meta-analysis (Table 12). In total, 110 participants were evenly assigned to groups either with plants or without plants. Chang and Chen [72] recruited college students in Taiwan and Qin et al. [81] recruited college students in China. Chang and Chen [72] did not randomly assign their participants to different groups, while Qin et al. [81] did. Both papers were appraised as having moderate research quality.
The heterogeneity test of the two studies investigating the influence of indoor plants on EEG β waves revealed a significant difference (p < 0.05), with I2 = 97.133%, confirming high heterogeneity between the studies. A random-effect model was therefore adopted. The pooled effect size (MD) was 1.455, and the 95% confidence interval ranged from −1.799 to 4.709. Though the results indicated that the group with plants had greater EEG β waves than the group without plants, the difference was not significant (p = 0.381) (Table 13). The relative weight of both the Chang and Chen [72] and Qin et al. [81] studies was about equal, at 50.95% and 49.05%, respectively (Figure 4).

3.9. Attention

Three papers examining the influence of indoor plants on attention, which was measured by various cognitive tasks with the unit of measurement as performance scores, were included for the meta-analysis (Table 14). In total, 177 participants were randomly assigned to different groups. Because Larsen et al. [53] divided the participants into two experimental groups (with a high or moderate number of plants) and one control group (without plants), there were 76 participants and 101 participants in the control and experimental groups, respectively. Larsen et al. [53] recruited participants in the United States, Yin et al. [75] recruited adults in the United States, and Shibata and Suzuki [71] recruited college students in Japan. All three papers were appraised as having moderate research quality.
The heterogeneity test of the three studies (one with two experimental groups) investigating the influence of indoor plants on attention revealed a significant difference (p < 0.05), with I2 = 82.088%, confirming high heterogeneity among the studies. A random-effect model was therefore adopted. The pooled effect size (SMD) was −0.005, and the 95% confidence interval ranged from −0.671 to 0.661. The results indicated that the group with plants had lower attention than the group without plants. The difference, however, was not significant (p = 0.988) (Table 15). The relative weight of the three studies was relatively similar, ranging from 25.85% to 23.32% (Figure 5).

3.10. Academic Achievement

Only two papers examining the influence of indoor plants on academic achievement, which was measured by course grades and examination scores, were included for the meta-analysis (Table 16). The studies had a total of 119 participants. Among these, 58 were in the control group (without plants) and 61 in the experimental group (with plants). Doxey et al. [86] recruited sophomores in the United States, who were not randomly assigned to groups. Han and Hung [66] recruited students from a junior high school in Taiwan, who were randomly assigned to groups. The study of Doxey et al. [86] was appraised as having low research quality, while that of Han and Hung [66] was appraised as having moderate research quality.
The heterogeneity test of the two studies investigating the influence of indoor plants on academic achievement revealed no significant difference (p > 0.05), with I2 = 0%, confirming low heterogeneity between the studies. A fixed-effect model was therefore applied. The pooled effect size (SMD) was 0.534, and the 95% confidence interval ranged from 0.167 to 0.901. The results indicated that the group with plants had significantly higher academic achievement (p = 0.004) than the group without plants (Table 17). The relative weight of Doxey et al. [86] was 68.95% and that of Han and Hung [66] was 31.05% (Figure 6).

3.11. Response Time

Three papers examining the influence of indoor plants on response time, which was measured by various tasks with the unit of measurement as seconds or milliseconds, were included for the meta-analysis (Table 18). These studies had a total of 749 participants. Among them, 374 participants were in the control group (without plants) and 375 in the experimental group (with plants). Nieuwenhuis et al. [58] recruited adult office workers in the United Kingdom, Kim et al. [77] recruited college students in Hong Kong, and Thatcher et al. [94] recruited adults in South Africa. Nieuwenhuis et al. [58] and Thatcher et al. [94] randomly assigned their participants to different groups, while Kim et al. [77] did not. All three papers were appraised as having moderate research quality.
The heterogeneity test of the three studies investigating the influence of indoor plants on response time revealed a significant difference (p < 0.05), with I2 = 96.144%, confirming high heterogeneity among the studies. A random-effect model was therefore adopted. Given that great differences existed between the original data, SMD, rather than MD, was adopted. The pooled effect size (SMD) was −0.939, and the 95% confidence interval ranged from −2.208 to 0.401. The results indicated that the group with plants had less response time than did the group without plants. However, the difference was not significant (p = 0.170) (Table 19). The relative weight of the three studies was relatively similar, ranging from 34.89% to 32.02% (Figure 7).

3.12. Risk of Bias across Studies

Because at least three records are required for the evaluation of publication bias, only the studies investigating the effects on DBP, EEG α waves, attention, and response time were suitable for testing the risk of bias across records in the meta-analyses. All funnel plots of these studies (Figure 8) revealed a symmetric funnel, confirming the absence of publication bias. Furthermore, the linear Egger’s regressions all indicated no evidence of publication bias (p > 0.374) (Table 20).

3.13. Additional Analysis

Sensitivity analysis was separately performed on records investigating the effects of indoor plants on physiological functions, including DBP and EEG α waves, and those on cognitive functions, including attention and response time, because at least three records are required. None of the pooled effect sizes changed notably when any of the studies was removed (Figure 9). In summary, none of the pooled effect size values in the forest plots exceeded the 95% confidence interval of overall pooled effect size [95]. The results of the aforementioned four meta-analyses were therefore not sensitive; i.e., the results were stable and did not lead to a different conclusion if any of the included studies was deleted.

4. Discussion

The 42 records in the present systematic review provide a comprehensive perspective on the topic under investigation. Overall, the review suggests that indoor plants exerted a positive effect on objective functions in participants. Since 90.5% of the records are experiments, the above findings generally support a cause-and-effect relationship [49]. The findings on such matters, such as improved stress-reduction, increased task performance, and improved health, are in accordance with those of the previous reviews [31,36,40,45]. These various reviews together provide converging evidence that indoor plants are beneficial to humans, even though some reviews focused on self-reports, some on objective functions, and some did not distinguish subjective or objective responses. These findings, however, contrast with findings of no improvements in performance and productivity [41] and of no influences of indoor nature on adolescents [35]. This may be because of the differences in the measured outcomes of performance and/or functions and in the ages of the participants (cf. [17]). More studies of the effects of indoor plants on people are needed because only three systematic reviews and one meta-analysis are insufficient to draw conclusive evidence. Moreover, there are some overlapping studies between these reviews, which is not uncommon in reviews. This is also because some studies collected data on both self-reported perceptions and objectively measured responses.

4.1. Summary of Evidence

The meta-analyses covered only 16 records, consequently providing a more limited perspective than the systematic review. Nevertheless, it should be noted that synthesis findings are likely to be more reliable than those of single studies. Regarding the physiological functions, the meta-analyses further provided evidence synthesis that (1) participants exposed to indoor plants had significantly lower DBP values, which is related to excitement and arousal [96], than their counterparts exposed to no indoor plants; (2) participants exposed to indoor plants had greater EEG α waves, which is related to relaxation [97], than their counterparts, though the difference was not significant; and (3) participants exposed to indoor plants had greater EEG β waves, which is related to anxiety [69] and attention [98], than their counterparts. This difference was also not significant. It should be noted that whether the EEG wave patterns is a beneficial or an adverse function depends on the context [99]. Regarding cognitive functions, the meta-analyses further provided evidence synthesis that (1) participants exposed to indoor plants had lower attention than their counterparts, though the difference was not significant; (2) participants exposed to indoor plants had significantly higher academic achievement than their counterparts; and (3) participants exposed to indoor plants responded more quickly than their counterparts, though the difference was not significant.
Given that the pooled effect sizes of the records of DBP, EEG α waves, attention, and response time did not change notably when any record was deleted, the meta-analyses had high stability results; i.e., the removal of no study led to a different conclusion. Furthermore, the perfect homogeneity of the two studies on academic achievement provided reliable meta-analysis results (cf. [20]), although one of the included studies had low research quality, which was associated with risks of bias. Some of the results of the meta-analyses, however, were inconsistent, regardless of whether they reached significance. These included greater EEG α and β waves, and lower attention but higher academic achievement and quicker responses when exposed to indoor plants. Since there are three kinds of attention—working memory, cognitive flexibility, and attentional control—researchers should reach a consensus on which task to use to measure attention in order to have a more reliable evidence synthesis [19,20]. More studies of these subjects are needed.
The evidence synthesis of the relaxed physiology, as indicated by significantly lower DBP values when the participants were exposed to indoor plants than their counterparts, provided partial support to the SRT, which proposes that natural environment is helpful for recovery from stress [3], while that of the enhanced cognition, as indicated by significantly higher academic achievement when the participants exposed to indoor plants than their counterparts, provided partial support to the ART, which claims that the natural environment is beneficial to the restoration of directed attention [7]. Although different cultures may influence people’s perceptions of plants and even their functions in relation to plants (cf. [3]), there appears to be no research on these issues. Nevertheless, the evidence synthesis regarding human functions of this study seems not to be influenced by cultures. The evidence synthesis of relaxed physiology comes from the studies recruiting participants in the South Korea [82], China [60], and Taiwan [76]. Although these three studies had a significant heterogeneity (I2 = 97.554%), the removal of any study did not change the results. Moreover, the evidence synthesis of enhanced cognition comes from the studies recruiting participants in the US [86] and Taiwan [66] but had a perfect homogeneity (I2 = 0%). Nevertheless, more studies are needed to explore the influences of different cultures on peoples’ perceptions and functions with respect to plants.
As mentioned in the previous section concerning the risk of bias within studies, the records suffered, in general, five major risks of bias. First, noncompliance with an ITT analysis might result in unduly liberal estimate of the treatment effect [100]. Second, results obtained from unrepresentative participants might prevent observed effects from being generalizable to a larger population [49], but generalizability is improved more by many heterogeneous small experiments than by only a few large experiments [50]. Third, when outcome assessors were aware of participant allocation, outcomes might be assessed differently [101]. Fourth, statistical power not being reported might increase Type II errors: the acceptance of a null hypothesis that is actually false [102]. Fifth, the lack of appropriate randomization procedures and random allocation to groups might introduce bias [103].
Moreover, the records on the physiological and cognitive functions for the meta-analyses were susceptible to other risks of bias. Inclusion and/or exclusion criteria of participants not being reported [53,58,66,71,77,86,94] might miss the target population and/or might bias the research results [104]. Baseline measures not taken before the intervention [58,72,76,81,86,94] lacked a point of reference to gauge how effective the intervention is [49]. Inappropriate statistical analysis methods for study design, such as repeated-measures or within-subjects design not using repeated-measures or dependent-sample analyses [72,81], led to incorrect results. Not blinding participants to research questions [82] might affect their responses [105]. Lack of individual level allocation [58,81,86], in which each participant did not have an equal opportunity of being assigned to groups, might result in incomparable groups before intervention [50]. Inconsistency of intervention (within and between groups) was an issue in several studies as the intervention included more than one treatment [53,56,58,66,71,72,77,81,94], such as various plant colors.
Some studies found gender differences regarding physiological mobilization [57,62,69,83] and cognitive functions [70,71]. Such findings suggest that taking gender into consideration when investigating the effects of indoor plants is important, since males and females may have differing physiological and psychological responses (cf. [24]). Some of the records also showed different effects of plants with flowers and without flowers on physiology [57,69,83] and behavior [85]. Taking flowers and their colors and even leaf colors into account, therefore, when examining the effects of indoor plants is necessary. Moreover, most of the studies investigated the effect of only single exposure to indoor plants. Although a few studies examined the long-term effects [57,65,66,67,76,86], they did not scrutinize the specific effect of exposure time and/or frequency, nor did they include studies considering the influence of distance between plant and participant.

4.2. Limitations

Only journal articles were included in the review and meta-analyses, whereas grey literature was excluded. Therefore, some publication bias may have been involved [43]. There was a chance of positive [86] and/or small [75,81,82] studies being overrepresented, thus biasing the evidence synthesis. In general, studies with negative findings are less published than positive findings [106], which may give a distorted image of what is really known about a subject [107,108]. However, the results of DBP, EEG α waves, attention, and response time all indicated no evidence of publication bias. Furthermore, only a few records were included for the meta-analyses. Though conducting a meta-analysis with two or three studies is acceptable, it is not ideal. Since some of the 42 papers were published a long time ago, their authors could not locate the original data on means and standard deviations. Some authors could not even be reached. Additionally, five of the six meta-analyses had a very high heterogeneity (I2 > 82%), which is associated with low reliability results [109]. This may be because of the diversity in the recruited participants, applied interventions, measured outcomes, and adopted study designs (cf. [109]). Additionally, because there were only two or three records for each of the meta-analyses, subgroup analyses, meta-regression analyses, moderating factors (gender, plant quantity, exposure duration, distance to plants, room climate, and room size), and further analyses for the risk of bias could not be conducted. Nevertheless, the results of DBP, EEG α waves, attention, and response time showed no publication bias. Moreover, because of the lack of original data on means and standard deviations or the insufficient number of studies, a meta-analysis on the effects of indoor plants on objective functions in behavior (e.g., pain tolerance and misconduct), health (sick leave, pain killer consumption, and hospitalizations), physiology (EDA, heart rate, respiration rate, and body temperature), and cognition (productivity and reaction) could not be performed. Finally, the studies included for the systematic review and those for the meta-analyses, in general, had moderate research quality (45.3% for those in the review, and 48.0% for those in the meta-analyses; [19]). Thus, high-quality research was lacking (Table 5 and Table 6).

4.3. Suggestions

Future studies should recruit more people living in the equatorial area and the Global South in general and Africans in specific, preferably not college students (Table 2). Background information of the participants, such as gender, age, occupation, ethnicity, health status, and number before, during, and after the research, should be provided. Study designs should use more field experiments conducted in real-world indoor environments rather than laboratories in order to improve ecological validity and still maintain sound internal validity [50]. More high-quality research is required, such as research involving experiments that follow the Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT; [110]), nonrandom experiments that follow the Transparent Reporting of Evaluations with Nonrandomized Designs (TREND; [111]), and, in general, the Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association [112].
Given that indoor plants are the intervention itself, indoor plants are associated with the construct validity of the research [31] in which plant quantity, plant–participant distance, exposure time, and exposure frequency all affect the dose–response relationship (cf. [113,114,115]). Accordingly, we suggest that future studies adopt standardized measurements of the plant quantity, such as the volume percentage of the plants in an indoor environment or the visible greenness rate. The volume percentage of the plants has been used in the included studies [53,67], while the visible greenness rate has also been used in previous studies [38]. The visible greenness rate concerns the percentage of the plants seen by human eyes, which is an objective measurement of plants in a three-dimensional space in the field of vision [116]. Consideration of plant quantity, exposure time, frequency, and distance may assist researchers to examine rigorously how exposure to indoor plants in terms of one event or short-term period or multiple events or long-term periods affects the objective functions of individuals by means of a dose–response or exposure–outcome relationship.
In addition to the dose of and/or exposure to the indoor plants, gender difference, flower colors, flower shapes, plant colors, plant shapes [57,62,69,70,71,83,85,88,89], and cultural influences should also be considered. Furthermore, the physiology of plants, including such factors as their roots and microorganisms [117,118], photosynthesis, adsorption, respiration, and evapotranspiration, which are helpful for air quality and microclimate [40,47,119], may need to be considered. Similarly, researchers should also report more detailed data on room climate, room size, light condition (Table 3 and Table 4), and seasonal condition, because air quality, temperature, relative humidity, light, and season also affect human comfort, performance, and health [120]. The mechanisms of and/or pathways to the effects of indoor plants on human functions also await exploration. Furthermore, indoor plants are mostly studied for their individual performances rather than as a combination. The research into the effect of plants usually focuses on the effects of single plants of different species in different conditions. Attention should further be placed on species that can cohabitate together, thus compensating each other’s needs and recreating the basic forms of symbiosis [121].
Finally, if the number of studies remains inadequate during future analyses, various aspects of human functions may be integrated into physiology (with respect to the sympathetic nervous system or parasympathetic nervous system), cognition (regarding participants’ reaction time and accuracy rate), health (in terms of illness and recovery), and behavior (either positive or negative). In that manner, the standardized mean differences (SMDs) of the function data could be adopted to conduct more rigorous meta-analyses, subgroup analyses, meta-regression analyses, and moderating factors. In contrast to self-reported measures, objective outcome measures lead to fewer reliability and validity concerns (cf. [122]) and risk of bias [123]. Nevertheless, compare and contrast of self-reported measures and objective outcome measures can provide interesting results and can be an advantage of such endeavor (c.f. [124,125,126]).

5. Conclusions

The systematic review of 42 records showed that indoor plants affect participants’ objective functions positively, particularly in terms of relaxed physiology and improved cognition. The meta-analyses further provided the evidence synthesis that indoor plants could significantly benefit participants’ SBP and academic achievement, which supported the SRT and ART. The records for the abovementioned meta-analyses, however, were limited, at only three studies for the SBP and two studies for the academic achievement. The evidence synthesis should be interpreted with caution. In brief, the systematic review concluded that, in general, people have better functions with the presence of indoor plants than the absence of indoor plants, and the meta-analyses concluded that, in specific, people have significantly lower SBP and significantly greater academic achievement when indoor plants are present than when indoor plants are not present, though with limited evidence synthesis. Since this study was the first meta-analyses of the effects of indoor plants on people’s functions, however, the findings may help the general public, environmental designers, and planners and policy makers to conduct appropriate assessments and to implement measures to improve psycho-physiological health and productivity (i.e., relaxed physiology and enhanced cognition) of habitants. The estimated productivity decrease caused by sick building syndrome, which is “a medical condition in which people in a building suffer from symptoms of illness or feeling unwell for no apparent reason” [127], in American office workers, for example, was 2%, for an annual cost of roughly 60 billion USD [128]. Furthermore, poor indoor air quality decreases workplace productivity by 10–15% [129]. The integration of plants as a building service is viable. A combination of indoor plants and ventilation technology provides enhanced efficiency and effectiveness of air purification [130,131]. Not only are green spaces needed in cities, but also plants are needed in buildings for people’s health and well-being. For the sake of people’s effective daily functions, indoor plants should be among the important elements of the healthy city, particularly in terms of their easy applicability and accessibility.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ijerph19127454/s1, Table S1: The full search strings; Table S2: Full-text excluded, with reason for exclusion.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, K.-T.H.; methodology, K.-T.H.; validation, K.-T.H.; formal analysis, K.-T.H. and L.-W.R.; investigation, K.-T.H. and L.-W.R.; resources, K.-T.H.; data curation, K.-T.H., L.-W.R. and L.-S.L.; writing—original draft preparation, K.-T.H.; writing—review and editing, K.-T.H.; visualization, L.-W.R. and L.-S.L.; supervision, K.-T.H.; project administration, K.-T.H.; funding acquisition, K.-T.H. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Ministry of Science and Technology, grant number MOST 107-2410-H-167-008-MY2.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Data are available upon reasonable request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Hartig, T.; Mang, M.; Evans, G.W. Restorative effects of natural environment experiences. Environ. Behav. 1991, 23, 3–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Ulrich, R.S.; Parsons, R. Influences of passive experiences with plants on individual well-being and health. In The Role of Horticulture in Human Well-Being and Social Development; Relt, P., Ed.; Timber Press: Portland, OR, USA, 1992; pp. 93–105. [Google Scholar]
  3. Ulrich, R.S. Aesthetic and affective response to natural environment. In Behavior and the Natural Environment; Altman, I., Wohwill, J.F., Eds.; Plenum: New York, NY, USA, 1983; pp. 85–125. [Google Scholar]
  4. Wilson, E.O. Biophilia; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1984. [Google Scholar]
  5. Kellert, S.R.; Wilson, E.O. The Biophilia Hypothesis; Island Press: Washington, DC, USA, 1993. [Google Scholar]
  6. Ulrich, R.S.; Simons, R.F.; Losito, B.D.; Fiorito, E.; Miles, M.A.; Zelson, M. Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 1991, 11, 201–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Kaplan, R.; Kaplan, S. The Experience of Nature: A Psychological Perspective; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1989. [Google Scholar]
  8. Han, K.-T. Restorative Nature: An Overview of the Positive Influences of Natural Landscapes on Humans; Lambert Academic Publishing: Saarbrücken, Germany, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  9. Hartig, T.; Mitchell, R.; de Vries, S.; Frumkin, H. Nature and health. Annu. Rev. Public Health 2014, 35, 207–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  10. van den Bosch, M.; Sang, Å.O. Urban natural environments as nature-based solutions for improved public health-A systematic review of reviews. Environ. Res. 2017, 158, 373–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Bowler, D.E.; Buyung-Ali, L.M.; Knight, T.M.; Pullin, A.S. A systematic review of evidence for added benefits to health of exposure to natural environments. BMC Public Health 2010, 10, 456–465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Britton, E.; Kindermann, G.; Domegan, C.; Carlin, C. Blue care: A systematic review of blue space interventions for health and wellbeing. Health Promot. Int. 2020, 35, 50–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Gascon, M.; Triguero-Mas, M.; Martínez, D.; Dadvand, P.; Rojas-Rueda, D.; Plaséncia, A.; Nieuwenhuijsen, M.J. Residential green spaces and mortality: A systematic review. Environ. Int. 2016, 86, 60–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Lakhani, A.; Norwood, M.; Watling, D.; Zeeman, H.; Kendall, E. Using the natural environment to address the psychosocial impact of neurological disability: A systematic review. Health Place 2019, 55, 188–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Gascon, M.; Triguero-Mas, M.; Martínez, D.; Dadvand, P.; Forns, J.; Plaséncia, A.; Nieuwenhuijsen, M.J. Mental health benefits of long-term exposure to residential green and blue spaces: A systematic review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 4354–4379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Coventry, P.A.; Brown, J.V.E.; Pervin, J.; Brabyn, S.; Pateman, R.; Breedvelt, J.; Gilbody, S.; Stancliffe, R.; McEachan, R.; White, P.C.L. Nature-based outdoor activities for mental and physical health: Systematic review and meta-analysis. SSM Popul. Health 2021, 16, 100934. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. McMahan, E.A.; Estes, D. The effect of contact with natural environments on positive and negative affect: A meta-analysis. J. Posit. Psychol. 2015, 10, 507–519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Menardo, E.; Brondino, M.; Hall, R.; Pasini, M. Restorativeness in natural and urban environments: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Rep. 2021, 124, 417–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Ohly, H.; White, M.P.; Wheeler, B.W.; Bethel, A.; Ukoumunne, O.C.; Nikolaou, V.; Garside, R. Attention Restoration Theory: A systematic review of attention potential of exposure to natural environments. J. Toxicol. Environ. Health Part B 2016, 19, 305–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  20. Stevenson, M.P.; Schilhab, T.; Bentsen, P. Attention Restoration Theory II: A systematic review to clarify attention processes affected by exposure to natural environments. J. Toxicol. Environ. Health Part B 2018, 21, 227–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Twohig-Bennett, C.; Jones, A. The health benefits of the great outdoors: A systematic review and meta-analysis of greenspace exposure and health outcomes. Environ. Res. 2018, 166, 626–637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Yang, B.-Y.; Zhao, T.; Hu, L.-X.; Browning, M.H.E.M.; Heinrich, J.; Dharmage, S.C.; Jalaludin, B.; Knibbs, L.D.; Liu, X.-X.; Luo, Y.-N.; et al. Greenspace and human health: An umbrella review. Innovation 2021, 2, 100164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. United Nations, Department of Economic and Social Affairs, Population Division. 2018 Revision of World Urbanization Prospects; United Nations: New York, NY, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  24. World Health Organization. Urban Green Spaces and Health; WHO Regional Office for Europe: Copenhagen, Denmark, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  25. World Health Organization. European Healthy Cities Network. 2016. Available online: http://www.euro.who.int/en/health-topics/environment-and-health/urban-health/activities/healthy-cities/who-european-healthy-cities-network/what-is-a-healthy-city (accessed on 10 July 2020).
  26. Schweizer, C.; Edwards, R.D.; Bayer-Oglesby, L.; Gauderman, W.J.; Ilacqua, V.; Jantunen, M.J.; Lai, H.K.; Nieuwenhuijsen, M.; Künzli, N. Indoor time-microenvironment–activity patterns in seven regions of Europe. J. Expo. Sci. Environ. Epidemiol. 2007, 17, 170–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Fjeld, T.; Veiersted, B.; Sandvik, L.; Riise, G.; Levy, F. The effect of indoor foliage plants on health and discomfort symptoms among office workers. Indoor Built Environ. 1998, 7, 204–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Klepeis, N.E.; Nelson, W.C.; Ott, W.R.; Robinson, J.P.; Tsang, A.M.; Switzer, P.; Behar, J.V.; Hern, S.C.; Engelmann, W.H. The National Human Activity Pattern Survey (NHAPS): A resource for assessing exposure to environmental pollutants. J. Expo. Anal. Environ. Epidemiol. 2001, 11, 231–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Marć, M.; Śmiełowska, M.; Namiesnik, J.; Zabiegała, B. Indoor air quality of everyday use spaces dedicated to specific purposes—A review. Environ. Sci. Pollut. Res. 2018, 25, 2065–2082. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Beyer, K.; Szabo, A.; Hoormann, K.; Stolley, M. Time spent outdoors, activity levels, and chronic disease among American adults. J. Behav. Med. 2018, 41, 494–503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Bringslimark, T.; Hartig, T.; Patil, G.G. The psychological benefits of indoor plants: A critical review of the experimental literature. J. Environ. Psychol. 2009, 29, 422–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Smardon, R.C. Perception and aesthetics of the urban environments: Review of the role of vegetation. Landsc. Urban Plan. 1988, 15, 85–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Centers for Disease Control. Public Health Terms for Planners & Planning Terms for Public Health Professionals. 2013. Available online: https://www.cdc.gov/healthyplaces/terminology.htm (accessed on 10 July 2020).
  34. Taylor, L.; Hochuli, D. Defining greenspace: Multiple uses across multiple disciplines. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2017, 158, 25–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. van den Bogerd, N.; Dijkstra, S.C.; Koole, S.L.; Seidell, J.C.; de Vries, R.; Maas, J. Nature in the indoor and outdoor study environment and secondary and tertiary education students’ well-being, academic outcomes, and possible mediating pathways: A systematic review with recommendations for science and practice. Health Place 2020, 66, 102403–102418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Yeo, N.L.; Elliott, L.R.; Bethel, A.; White, M.P.; Dean, S.G.; Garside, R. Indoor nature interventions for health and wellbeing of older adults in residential settings: A systematic review. Gerontologist 2020, 60, e184–e199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Han, K.-T. Effects of indoor plants on the physical environment with respect to distance and green coverage ratio. Sustainability 2019, 11, 3679. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Han, K.-T. Effects of visible greenness, quantity and distance of indoor plants on human perceptions and physical parameters. Indoor Build Environ. 2020, 30, 1353–1372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Kim, H.-H.; Lee, J.-Y.; Kim, H.-J.; Lee, Y.-W.; Kim, K.-J.; Park, J.-H.; Shin, D.-C.; Lim, Y.-W. Impact of foliage plant interventions in classrooms on actual air quality and subjective health complaints. J. Am. Soc. Hortic. Sci. 2013, 82, 255–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Deng, L.; Deng, Q. The basic roles of indoor plants in human health and comfort. Environ. Sci. Pollut. Res. 2018, 25, 36087–36101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Moya, T.A.; van den Dobbelsteen, A.; Ottelé, M.; Bluyssen, P.M. A review of green systems within the indoor environment. Indoor Built Environ. 2019, 28, 298–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Moher, D.; Liberati, A.; Tetzla, J.; Altman, D.G.; The PRISMA Group. Preferred reporting items for systemic review and meta-analysis: The PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 2009, 6, e1000097. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Borenstein, M.; Hedges, L.V.; Higgins, J.P.T.; Rothstein, H.R. Introduction to Meta-Analysis; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  44. Centre for Reviews and Dissemination. Systematic Reviews, 3rd ed.; CRD: York, UK, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  45. Han, K.-T.; Ruan, L.-W. Effects of indoor plants on self-reported perceptions: A systemic review. Sustainability 2019, 11, 4506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Stone, A.A.; Bachrach, C.A.; Jobe, J.B.; Kurtzman, H.S.; Cain, V.S. The Science of Self-Report: Implications for Research and Practice; Psychology Press: East Sussex, UK, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  47. Han, K.-T.; Ruan, L.-W. Effects of indoor plants on air quality: A systematic review. Environ. Sci. Pollut. Res. 2020, 27, 16019–16051. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Chiou, H.-J. Quantitative Research and Statistical Analysis in Social and Behavior Sciences; Wunan: Taipei, Taiwan, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  49. Frankfort-Nachmias, C.; Nachmias, D. Research Methods in the Social Sciences; Arnold: London, UK, 1996. [Google Scholar]
  50. Cook, T.C.; Campbell, D.T. Quasi-Experimentation: Design & Analysis Issues for Field Settings; Houghton Mifflin: Boston, MA, USA, 1979. [Google Scholar]
  51. Critical Appraisal Skills Programme. CASP UK Critical Appraisal Checklists; Critical Appraisal Skills Programme: Oxford, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  52. Effective Public Health Practice Project. Quality Assessment Tool for Quantitative Studies. 2013. Available online: http://www.ephpp.ca/tools.html (accessed on 25 March 2019).
  53. Larsen, L.; Adams, J.; Deal, B.; Kweon, B.-S.; Tyler, E. Plants in the workplace: The effects of plant density on productivity, attitudes, and perceptions. Environ. Behav. 1998, 30, 261–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Bjørnstad, S.; Patil, G.G.; Raanaas, R.K. Nature contact and organizational support during office working hours: Benefits relating to stress reduction, subjective health complaints, and sick leave. Work 2016, 53, 9–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Bringslimark, T.; Hartig, T.; Patil, G.G. Psychological benefits of indoor plants in workplaces: Putting experimental results into context. HortScience 2007, 42, 581–587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Elasdek, M.; Liu, B. Effects of viewing flowering plants on employees’ wellbeing in an office-like environment. Indoor Built Environ. 2020, 30, 1429–1440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Genjo, K.; Matsumoto, H.; Ogata, N.; Nakano, T. Feasibility study on mental health-care effects of plant installations in office spaces. Jpn. Archit. Rev. 2019, 2, 376–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Nieuwenhuis, M.; Knight, C.; Postmes, T.; Haslam, S.A. The relative benefits of green versus lean office space: Three field experiments. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 2014, 20, 199–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Toyoda, M.; Yokota, Y.; Barnes, M.; Kaneko, M. Potential of a small indoor plant on the desk for reducing office workers’ stress. HortTechnology 2020, 30, 55–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Hassan, A.; Qibing, C.; Yinggao, L.; Tao, J.; Li, G.; Jiang, M.; Nian, L.; Bing-Yang, L. Psychological and physiological effects of viewing a money plant by older adults. Brain Behav. 2019, 9, e01359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  61. Khan, M.A.; Amin, N.; Khan, A.; Imtiaz, M.; Khan, F.; Ahmad, I.; Ali, A.; Islam, B. Plant therapy: A nonpharmacological and noninvasive treatment approach medically beneficial to the wellbeing of hospital patients. Gesunde Pflanz. 2016, 68, 191–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Li, X.; Huang, Q.; Li, S.; Chen, C. Research on the physical and psychological health effect of the horticultural plant cultivation activity on the elderly with dementia. J. Northwest Univ. (Nat. Sci. Ed.) 2020, 50, 867–880. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Park, S.-H.; Mattson, R.H. Effects of flowering and foliage plants in hospital rooms on patients recovering from abdominal surgery. HortTechnology 2008, 18, 563–568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Park, S.-H.; Mattson, R.H. Therapeutic influences of plants in hospital rooms on surgical recovery. HortScience 2009, 44, 1–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Han, K.-T. Influence of limitedly visible leafy indoor plants on the psychology, behavior, and health of students at a junior high school in Taiwan. Environ. Behav. 2009, 41, 658–692. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  66. Han, K.-T.; Hung, C.-Y. Influences of physical interactions and visual contacts with plants on students’ psycho-physiology, behaviors, academic performance and health. Sci. Agric. 2012, 59, 195–210. [Google Scholar]
  67. Park, S.-Y.; Song, J.-S.; Kim, H.-D.; Yamane, K.; Son, K.C. Effects of interior plantscapes on indoor environments and stress level of high school students. J. Jpn. Soc. Hortic. Sci. 2008, 77, 447–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Johansson, B. Heart rate and heart rate variability response to the Transpiration of Vortex-Water by Begonia Eliator Plants to the air in an office during visual display terminal work. J. Altern. Complementary Med. 2008, 14, 993–1003. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Liu, M.; Kim, E.; Mattson, R.H. Physiological and emotional influences of cut flower arrangements and lavender fragrance on university students. J. Ther. Hortic. 2003, 14, 18–27. [Google Scholar]
  70. Shibata, S.; Suzuki, N. Effects of the foliage plant on task performance and mood. J. Environ. Psychol. 2002, 22, 265–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Shibata, S.; Suzuki, N. Effects of an indoor plant on creative task performance and mood. Scand. J. Psychol. 2004, 45, 373–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Chang, C.-Y.; Chen, P.-K. Human response to window views and indoor plants in the workplace. HortScience 2005, 40, 1354–1359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  73. Li, X.; Zhang, Z.; Gu, M.; Jiang, D.-Y.; Wang, J.; Lv, Y.-M.; Zhang, Q.-X.; Pan, H.-T. Effects of plantscape colors on psycho-physiological responses of university students. J. Food Agric. Environ. 2012, 10, 702–708. [Google Scholar]
  74. Li, X.; Lu, Y.-M.; Zhang, Z.; Wang, J.; Pan, H.-T.; Zhang, Q.-X. The visual effects of flower colors on university students psycho-physiological responses. J. Food Agric. Environ. 2012, 10, 1294–1300. [Google Scholar]
  75. Yin, J.; Zhu, S.; MacNaughton, P.; Allen, J.G.; Spengler, J.D. Physiological and cognitive performance of exposure to biophilic indoor environment. Build. Environ. 2018, 132, 255–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Chen, R.-U.; Ho, K.-F.; Hong, G.-B.; Chuang, K.-J. Houseplant, indoor air pollution, and cardiovascular effects among elderly subjects in Taipei, Taiwan. Sci. Total Environ. 2020, 705, 135770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Kim, J.; Chaa, S.-H.; Koo, C.; Tang, S.-K. The effects of indoor plants and artificial windows in an underground environment. Build. Environ. 2018, 138, 53–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Lohr, V.I.; Pearson-Mims, C.H.; Goodwin, G.K. Interior plants may improve worker productivity and reduce stress in a windowless environment. J. Environ. Hortic. 1996, 14, 97–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Jang, H.S.; Kim, J.; Kim, K.S.; Pak, C.H. Human brain activity and emotional responses to plant color stimuli. Color Res. Appl. 2014, 39, 307–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Lohr, V.I.; Pearson-Mims, C.H. Physical discomfort may be reduced in the presence of interior plants. HortTechnology 2000, 10, 53–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Qin, J.; Sun, C.; Zhou, X.; Leng, H.; Lian, Z. The effect of indoor plants on human comfort. Indoor Built Environ. 2014, 23, 709–723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Lee, M.-S.; Lee, J.; Park, B.-J.; Miyazaki, Y. Interaction with indoor plants may reduce psychological and physiological stress by suppressing autonomic nervous system activity in young adults: A randomized crossover study. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 2015, 34, 21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  83. Kim, E.; Mattson, R.H. Stress recovery effects of viewing red-flowering geraniums. J. Ther. Hortic. 2002, 13, 4–12. [Google Scholar]
  84. Chen, J.-G.; Xiang, C.-Q.; Ruan, S.-U.; Zheng, Y.-Y.; Zhang, S.N. Application of Neurobehavioral Evaluation System Testing in the sealed building study on population Health. J. Labour Med. 2000, 17, 134–137. [Google Scholar]
  85. Park, S.-H.; Mattson, R.H.; Kim, E. Pain tolerance effects of ornamental plants in a simulated hospital patient room. Acta Hortic. 2004, 639, 241–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Doxey, J.S.; Waliczek, T.M. The impact of interior plants in university classrooms on student course performance and on student perceptions of the course and instructor. Hortscience 2009, 44, 384–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Raanaas, R.K.; Evensen, K.H.; Rich, D.; Sjøstrøm, G.; Patil, G. Benefits of indoor plants on attention capacity in an office setting. J. Environ. Psychol. 2011, 31, 99–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Elsadek, M.; Sayaka, S.; Fujii, E.; Koriesh, E.; Moghazy, E.; Elfatah, Y.A. Human emotional and psycho-physiological responses to plant color stimuli. J. Food Agric. Environ. 2013, 11, 1584–1591. [Google Scholar]
  89. Elsadek, M.; Fujii, E. People’s psycho-physiological responses to plantscape colors stimuli: A pilot study. Int. J. Psychol. Behav. Sci. 2014, 4, 70–78. [Google Scholar]
  90. Elsadek, M.; Sun, M.; Fujii, E. Psycho-physiological responses to plant variegation as measured through eye movement, self-reported emotion and cerebral activity. Indoor Built Environ. 2017, 26, 758–770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Sugiono Swara, S.E.; Wijanarko, W.; Sulistyarini, D.H. Investigating the impact of ornamental plants correlated with indoor thermal comfort and eco-energy. Int. Rev. Civ. Eng. 2017, 8, 221–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Huang, Q.; Kang, N.; Li, X.; Li, S. Effects of different horticultural activities on the negative emotions of the elderly. J. Northwest Univ. (Nat. Sci. Ed.) 2020, 50, 887–896. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Wei, Y.; Dong, Z.; Yu, W.; Huang, Q.; Li, S. Study on the physical and psychological effect of the four different horticultural activities on the elderly without family members. J. Northwest Univ. (Nat. Sci. Ed.) 2020, 50, 923–933. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Thatcher, A.; Adamson, K.; Bloch, L.; Kalantzis, A. Do indoor plants improve performance and well-being in offices? Divergent results from laboratory and field studies. J. Environ. Psychol. 2020, 71, 101487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Sutton, A.J.; Abrams, K.R.; Jones, D.R.; Trevor, A.; Sheldon, T.A.; Song, F. Methods for Meta-Analysis in Medical Research; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  96. Hsu, C.-H.; Chang, Y.-C.; Tang, S.-M.; Lee, W.-C.; Hsiao, J.-L.; Chen, Y.-Y. Physiology; New Wun Ching: New Taipei, Taiwan, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  97. Fazio, R.H.; Cooper, J. Arousal in the dissonance process. In Social Psychophysiology: A Sourcebook; Cacioppo, J.T., Petty, R.E., Eds.; Guilford: New York, NY, USA, 1983; pp. 122–152. [Google Scholar]
  98. Gola, M.; Magnuski, M.; Szumska, I.; Wróbel, A. EEG beta band activity is related to attention and attentional deficits in the visual performance of elderly subjects. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2013, 89, 334–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Jena, S.K. Examination stress and its effect on EEG. Int. J. Med. Sci. Public Health 2015, 4, 1493–1497. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  100. Gupta, P.C. Intention-to-treat concept: A review. Perspect. Clin. Res. 2011, 2, 109–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Noseworthy, J.H.; Ebers, G.C.; Vandervoort, M.K.; Farquhar, R.E.; Yetisir, E.; Roberts, R. The impact of blinding on the results of a randomized, placebo-controlled multiple sclerosis clinical trial. Neurology 1994, 44, 16–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences; Erbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
  103. Suresh, K.P. An overview of randomization techniques: An unbiased assessment of outcome in clinical research. J. Hum. Reprod. Sci. 2011, 4, 8–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  104. Patino, C.M.; Ferreira, J.C. Inclusion and exclusion criteria in research studies: Definitions and why they matter. J. Brasialeiro De Pneumol. 2018, 44, 84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  105. Karanicolas, P.J.; Farrokhyar, F.; Bhandari, M. Blinding: Who, what, when, why, how? Can. J. Surg. 2010, 53, 345–348. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  106. Fanelli, D. Negative results are disappearing from most disciplines and countries. Scientometrics 2012, 90, 891–904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Every-Palmer, S.; Howick, J. How evidence-based medicine is failing due to biased trials and selective publication. J. Eval. Clin. Pract. 2014, 20, 908–914. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  108. Lamé., G. Systematic Literature Reviews: An Introduction. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Engineering Design 2019, Delft, The Netherlands, 5–8 August 2019; pp. 1633–1642. [Google Scholar]
  109. Chen, S.-L.; Chen, Y.-M. Health Care Statistics and Meta-Analysis: RevMan 5 Software Operation; Wunan: Taipei, Taiwan, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  110. Schulz, K.F.; Altman, D.G.; Moher, D.; the CONSORT Group. CONSORT 2010 Statement: Updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials. Br. Med. J. 2010, 340, 698–702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Des Jarlais, D.C.; Lyles, C.; Crepaz, N. Improving the reporting quality of nonrandomized evaluations of behavioral and public health interventions: The TREND statement. Am. J. Public Health 2004, 94, 361–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. American Psychological Association. Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association, 7th ed.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  113. Jiang, B.; Larsen, L.; Deal, B.; Sullivan, W.C. A dose-response curve describing the relationship between tree cover density and landscape preference. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2015, 139, 16–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Jiang, B.; Li, D.; Larsen, L.; Sullivan, W.C. A dose-response curve describing the relationship between urban tree cover density and self-reported stress recovery. Environ. Behav. 2016, 48, 607–629. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Shanahan, D.F.; Fuller, R.A.; Bush, R.; Lin, B.B.; Gaston, K.J. The health benefits of urban nature: How much do we need? BioScience 2015, 65, 476–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  116. Hong, X.-M. A study on Restoration Effects of Greening Levels of Plant at Indoor Working Environment. Master’s Thesis, University of Feng Chia, Taichung, Taiwan, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  117. Fjeld, T.; Bonnevie, C. The Effect of Plants and Artificial Day-Light on the Well-Being and Health of Office Workers; School Children and Health Care Personnel, Medicine, Lippincott: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  118. Godish, T.; Guindon, C. An assessment of botanical air purification as a formaldehyde mitigation measure under dynamic laboratory chamber conditions. Environ. Pollut. 1989, 62, 13–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Han, K.-T. Urban Forestry: Theories and Applications; Lamper: Taipei, Taiwan, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  120. Vimalanathan, K.; Ramesh Babu, T. The effect of indoor office environment on the work performance, health and well-being of office workers. J. Environ. Health Sci. Eng. 2014, 12, 113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  121. Persiani, S. Benefits of using plants in indoor environments: Exploring common research gaps. Architecture 2021, 1, 83–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Robins, R.; Fraley, C.; Krueger, R. Handbook of Research Methods in Personality Psychology; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  123. Wood, L.; Egger, M.; Gluud, L.L.; Schulz, K.F.; Jüni, P.; Altman, D.G.; Gluud, C.; Martin, R.M.; Wood AJ, G.; Sterne, J.A.C. Empirical evidence of bias in treatment effect estimates in controlled trials with different interventions and outcomes: Meta-epidemiological study. Br. Med. J. 2008, 336, 601–605. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  124. Obling, K.H.; Hansen, A.L.; Overgaard, K.; Normann, K.; Sandbaek, A.; Maindal, H.T. Association between self-reported and objectively measured physical fitness level in a middle-aged population in primary care. Prev. Med. Rep. 2015, 2, 462–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  125. Orta, O.R.; Barbosa, C.; Velez, J.C.; Gelaye, B.; Chen, X.; Stoner, L.; Williams, M.A. Associations of self-reported and objectively measured sleep disturbances with depression among primary caregivers of children with disabilities. Nat. Sci. Sleep 2016, 8, 181–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  126. Neupane, S.; Karstad, K.; Hallman, D.M.; Rugulies, R.; Holtermann, A. Objectively measured versus self-reported occupational physical activity and multisite musculoskeletal pain: A prospective follow-up study at 20 nursing homes in Denmark. Int. Arch. Occup. Environ. Health 2020, 93, 381–389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  127. World Health Organization. Housing: Sick Building Syndrome (Pamphlet No. 2), Copenhagen, World Health Organization Regional Office for Europe. 2005. Available online: http://www.emro.who.int/ceha/Publication_Details.asp?ID=46 (accessed on 10 July 2020).
  128. Fisk, W. How IEQ affects health, productivity. ASHRAE J. 2002, 44, 56–60. [Google Scholar]
  129. Cincinelli, A.; Martellini, T.; Amore, A.; Dei, L.; Marrazza, G.; Carretti, E.; Belosi, F.; Ravegnani, F.; Leva, P. Measurement of volatile organic compounds (VOCs) in libraries and archives in Florence (Italy). Sci. Total Environ. 2016, 572, 333–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  130. Fleck, R.; Pettit, T.J.; Douglas, A.N.J.; Irga, P.J.; Torpy, F.R. Bio-Based Materials and Biotechnologies for Eco-Efficient Construction; Series in Civil and Structural Engineering; Woodhead Publishing: Sawston, UK, 2020; pp. 305–327. [Google Scholar]
  131. Torpy, F.; Clements, N.; Pollinger, M.; Dengel, A.; Mulvihill, I.; He, C.; Irga, P. Testing the single-pass VOC removal efficiency of an active green wall using methyl ethyl ketone (MEK). Air Qual. Atmos. Health 2018, 11, 163–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Flow chart of the screening process.
Figure 1. Flow chart of the screening process.
Ijerph 19 07454 g001
Figure 2. Forest plot of studies on the influence of indoor plants on DBP [60,76,82].
Figure 2. Forest plot of studies on the influence of indoor plants on DBP [60,76,82].
Ijerph 19 07454 g002
Figure 3. Forest plot of studies on the influence of indoor plants on EEG α waves [56,72,81].
Figure 3. Forest plot of studies on the influence of indoor plants on EEG α waves [56,72,81].
Ijerph 19 07454 g003
Figure 4. Forest plot of studies on the influence of indoor plants on EEG β waves [72,81].
Figure 4. Forest plot of studies on the influence of indoor plants on EEG β waves [72,81].
Ijerph 19 07454 g004
Figure 5. Forest plot of studies on the influence of indoor plants on attention [53,71,75].
Figure 5. Forest plot of studies on the influence of indoor plants on attention [53,71,75].
Ijerph 19 07454 g005
Figure 6. Forest plot of studies on the influence of indoor plants on academic achievement [66,86].
Figure 6. Forest plot of studies on the influence of indoor plants on academic achievement [66,86].
Ijerph 19 07454 g006
Figure 7. Forest plot of studies on the influence of indoor plants on response time [58,77,94].
Figure 7. Forest plot of studies on the influence of indoor plants on response time [58,77,94].
Ijerph 19 07454 g007
Figure 8. Funnel plots. (a) DBP; (b) EEG α waves; (c) attention; (d) response time.
Figure 8. Funnel plots. (a) DBP; (b) EEG α waves; (c) attention; (d) response time.
Ijerph 19 07454 g008
Figure 9. Forest plots of sensitivity analyses. (a) DBP [60,76,82]; (b) EEG α waves [56,72,81]; (c) attention [53,71,75]; (d) response time [58,77,94].
Figure 9. Forest plots of sensitivity analyses. (a) DBP [60,76,82]; (b) EEG α waves [56,72,81]; (c) attention [53,71,75]; (d) response time [58,77,94].
Ijerph 19 07454 g009
Table 1. Statistics of published journal articles in Chinese and English during consecutive 5 year periods.
Table 1. Statistics of published journal articles in Chinese and English during consecutive 5 year periods.
Publication YearPublication LanguageTotal
ChineseEnglish
Number of PapersPercentage (%)Number of PapersPercentage (%)Number of PapersPercentage (%)
1996–200012038.149.5
2001–200500616.2614.3
2006–201000718.9716.7
2011–2015120924.31023.8
2016–20203601232.41535.7
Total5100.037100.042100.0
Table 2. Statistics of geographical distribution of the included studies.
Table 2. Statistics of geographical distribution of the included studies.
Participant LocationNumber of RecordsPercentage (%)
China (Asia, Global North)1023.8
United States (America, Global North)819.0
Japan (Asia, Global North)614.3
South Korea (Asia, Global North)511.9
Taiwan (Asia, Global North)49.5
Norway (Europe, Global North)37.1
United Kingdom (Europe, Global North)12.4
Sweden (Europe, Global North)12.4
Pakistan (Asia, Global North)12.4
Egypt (Africa, Global North)12.4
South Africans (Africa, Global South)12.4
Indonesia (Asia, Equatorial)12.4
Total42100.0
Table 3. Statistics of experimental conditions.
Table 3. Statistics of experimental conditions.
Experimental ConditionMaximumMinimumNumber of Records
ExposureDuration1 year15 s.34
[76][72]
Room SizeFloor area1260 m27.26 m219
[59][77]
Volume675 m314.52 m314
[57][77]
Distance to Plants3 m0.38 m13
[72][60]
Temperature27 °C20 °C19
[78][59]
Relative Humidity70%34%13
[79][80]
Wind Speed0.2 m·s−11
[81]
LightingIlluminance1365.5 lux300 lux11
[82][56]
Quantum10.6 μmol·m−2·s−11
[69]
Table 5. Quality appraisal of records in this study.
Table 5. Quality appraisal of records in this study.
Quality Indicators[78][53][84][80][83][70][69]
Study DesignPower calculation reportedNoNoNoNoNoNoNo
Inclusion/exclusion criteria reportedNoNoNoNoNoNoNo
Individual level allocationNoYesNAYesYesYesYes
Random allocation to groups/condition/orderYesYesNAYesYesYesYes
Randomization procedure appropriateYesUnclearNAUnclearUnclearUnclearUnclear
ConfoundersGroups similar (sociodemographic)UnclearUnclearUnclearYesYesUnclearYes
Group balanced at baselineUnclearUnclearUnclearUnclearUnclearUnclearYes
Participants blind to research questionYesYesUnclearUnclearUnclearUnclearYes
Intervention IntegrityClear description of intervention and controlYesYesNAYesYesYesYes
Consistency of intervention (within and between groups)YesNoNAYesYesYesNo
Data Collection MethodsOutcome assessors blind to group allocationNoNoUnclearUnclearUnclearUnclearYes
Baseline measures taken before the interventionYesUnclearNAYesYesNoYes
Consistency of data collectionYesYesYesYesYesYesYes
AnalysesAll outcomes reported (means and SD/SE)NoYesNoNoNoYesNo
All participants accounted for (i.e., losses/exclusions)YesYesYesYesYesNoYes
ITT analysis conducted (all data included after allocation)UnclearUnclearNAUnclearUnclearNoUnclear
Individual level analysisYesYesYesYesYesYesYes
Statistical analysis methods appropriate for study designYesYesYesYesYesYesYes
External ValiditySample representative of target populationNoNoNoNoNoNoNo
Overall Quality ScoreTotal number of points (out of possible 38)2018820201622
Quality rating as percent52.6 (M)47.4 (M)21.1 (L)52.6 (M)52.6 (M)42.1 (M)57.9 (M)
Responded to query about “uncertain” ratingsYesYesNANoNA Yes
Quality Indicators[85][71][72][55][68][63][67]
Study DesignPower calculation reportedNoNoNoNoNoNoNo
Inclusion/exclusion criteria reportedNoNoYesNoNoYesNo
Individual level allocationYesYesYesNAYesYesNo
Random allocation to groups/condition/orderYesYesUnclearNAYesYesNo
Randomization procedure appropriateUnclearUnclearUnclearNAUnclearUnclearNA
ConfoundersGroups similar (sociodemographic)YesUnclearYesUnclearUnclearUnclearYes
Group balanced at baselineUnclearUnclearYesUnclearUnclearUnclearYes
Participants blind to research questionUnclearYesUnclearYesUnclearYesUnclear
Intervention IntegrityClear description of intervention and controlYesYesYesNAYesYesPartial
Consistency of intervention (within and between groups)NoNoNoNAYesYesNo
Data Collection MethodsOutcome assessors blind to group allocationUnclearNoUnclearNAUnclearUnclearUnclear
Baseline measures taken before the interventionYesYesNoNAYesNoYes
Consistency of data collectionYesYesYesYesYesYesYes
AnalysesAll outcomes reported (means and SD/SE)NoYesYesNoYesNoNo
All participants accounted for (i.e., losses/exclusions)YesYesYesNoYesYesNo
ITT analysis conducted (all data included after allocation)UnclearUnclearUnclearNAUnclearUnclearNo
Individual level analysisYesYesYesYesYesYesYes
Statistical analysis methods appropriate for study designYesYesNoYesYesYesNo
External ValiditySample representative of target populationNoNoNoNoNoNoNo
Overall Quality ScoreTotal number of points (out of possible 38)1820188202011
Quality rating as percent47.4 (M)52.6 (M)47.4 (M)21.1 (L)52.6 (M)52.6 (M)28.9 (L)
Responded to query about “uncertain” ratingsNAYes NoNo
Quality Indicators[86][65][64][87][66][73][74]
Study DesignPower calculation reportedNoNoNoNoNoNoNo
Inclusion/exclusion criteria reportedNoYesYesNoNoYesYes
Individual level allocationNoNoYesYesYesYesYes
Random allocation to groups/condition/orderNoNoYesYesYesYesYes
Randomization procedure appropriateNAUnclearUnclearUnclearUnclearUnclearUnclear
ConfoundersGroups similar (sociodemographic)PartialPartialUnclearUnclearYesYesYes
Group balanced at baselineUnclearUnclearUnclearPartialUnclearYesYes
Participants blind to research questionUnclearYesYesYesYesUnclearUnclear
Intervention IntegrityClear description of intervention and controlYesYesYesYesYesYesYes
Consistency of intervention (within and between groups)YesYesYesYesNoNoNo
Data Collection MethodsOutcome assessors blind to group allocationUnclearNoUnclearUnclearNoUnclearUnclear
Baseline measures taken before the interventionNoYesNoYesYesYesYes
Consistency of data collectionYesYesYesYesYesYesYes
AnalysesAll outcomes reported (means and SD/SE)YesNoNoNoNoYesYes
All participants accounted for (i.e., losses/exclusions)NoYesYesYesYesYesYes
ITT analysis conducted (all data included after allocation)NoUnclearUnclearUnclearUnclearUnclearUnclear
Individual level analysisYesYesYesYesYesYesYes
Statistical analysis methods appropriate for study designYesYesYesYesYesYesYes
External ValiditySample representative of target populationNoNoNoNoNoNoNo
Overall Quality ScoreTotal number of points (out of possible 38)13192021202424
Quality rating as percent34.2 (M)50.0 (M)52.6 (M)55.3 (M)52.6 (M)63.2 (M)63.2 (M)
Responded to query about “uncertain” ratings No NoNo
Quality Indicators[88][89][79][58][81][82][54]
Study DesignPower calculation reportedNoNoNoStudy 3: NoYesNoNo
Inclusion/exclusion criteria reportedYesYesYesStudy 3: NoYesYesYes
Individual level allocationYesYesYesStudy 3: NoNoYesNA
Random allocation to groups/condition/orderYesYesYesStudy 3: YesUnclearYesNA
Randomization procedure appropriateUnclearUnclearUnclearStudy 3: UnclearUnclearUnclearNA
ConfoundersGroups similar (sociodemographic)YesYesYesStudy 3: UnclearYesYesUnclear
Group balanced at baselineYesYesYesStudy 3: UnclearYesYesUnclear
Participants blind to research questionNoUnclearUnclearStudy 3: YesUnclearNoUnclear
Intervention IntegrityClear description of intervention and controlYesYesYesStudy 3: YesYesYesNA
Consistency of intervention (within and between groups)NoNoNoStudy 3: NoNoYesNA
Data Collection MethodsOutcome assessors blind to group allocationNoUnclearUnclearStudy 3: NoUnclearUnclearUnclear
Baseline measures taken before the interventionNoNoNoStudy 3: NoNoYesNA
Consistency of data collectionYesYesYesStudy 3: YesYesYesYes
AnalysesAll outcomes reported (means and SD/SE)NoNoYesStudy 3: NoNoNoNo
All participants accounted for (i.e., losses/exclusions)YesYesYesStudy 3: YesYesYesNo
ITT analysis conducted (all data included after allocation)UnclearUnclearUnclearStudy 3: UnclearUnclearUnclearNA
Individual level analysisYesYesYesStudy 3: YesYesYesYes
Statistical analysis methods appropriate for study designYesYesNoStudy 3: YesNoYesYes
External ValiditySample representative of target populationNoNoNoStudy 3: NoNoNoNo
Overall Quality ScoreTotal number of points (out of possible 38)202020Study 3: 1416248
Quality rating as percent52.6 (M)52.6 (M)52.6 (M)Study 3:
36.8 (M)
42.1 (M)63.2 (M)21.1 (L)
Responded to query about “uncertain” ratingsYesNoNoYes
Quality Indicators[61][90][91][77][75][57][60]
Study DesignPower calculation reportedNoNoNoNoNoNoNo
Inclusion/exclusion criteria reportedYesYesNoNoYesNoYes
Individual level allocationYesYesUnclearYesYesNoYes
Random allocation to groups/condition/orderYesYesUnclearUnclearYesNoYes
Randomization procedure appropriateUnclearYesUnclearUnclearUnclearNAUnclear
ConfoundersGroups similar (sociodemographic)UnclearUnclearYesUnclearYesUnclearUnclear
Group balanced at baselineUnclearUnclearYesUnclearYesUnclearUnclear
Participants blind to research questionUnclearNoUnclearUnclearUnclearUnclearUnclear
Intervention IntegrityClear description of intervention and controlYesYesYesYesYesYesYes
Consistency of intervention (within and between groups)YesNoNoNoYesNoYes
Data Collection MethodsOutcome assessors blind to group allocationUnclearUnclearUnclearUnclearUnclearUnclearUnclear
Baseline measures taken before the interventionNoNoNoYesYesYesPartial
Consistency of data collectionYesYesYesYesYesNoYes
AnalysesAll outcomes reported (means and SD/SE)NoNoNoNoNoNoNo
All participants accounted for (i.e., losses/exclusions)YesYesYesYesNoNoYes
ITT analysis conducted (all data included after allocation)UnclearUnclearUnclearUnclearNoUnclearUnclear
Individual level analysisYesYes UnclearYesYesYesYes
Statistical analysis methods appropriate for study designNoNoUnclearYesYesNoYes
External ValiditySample representative of target populationNoNoNoNoNoNoNo
Overall Quality ScoreTotal number of points (out of possible 38)1616101422619
Quality rating as percent42.1 (M)42.1 (M)26.3 (L)36.8 (M)58.9 (M)15.8 (L)50.0 (M)
Responded to query about “uncertain” ratings
Quality Indicators[76][59][56][92][62][93][94]
Study DesignPower calculation reportedNoNoNoNoNoNoYes
Inclusion/exclusion criteria reportedYesNoYesYesNoYesNo
Individual level allocationNANoYesNoUnclearUnclearYes
Random allocation to groups/condition/orderNANoNoYesUnclearUnclearYes
Randomization procedure appropriateNANANAUnclearUnclearUnclearUnclear
ConfoundersGroups similar (sociodemographic)UnclearYesYesUnclearYesUnclearUnclear
Group balanced at baselineUnclearYesYesUnclearYesUnclearUnclear
Participants blind to research questionUnclearUnclearUnclearUnclearUnclearUnclearYes
Intervention IntegrityClear description of intervention and controlYesYesYesYesYesYesYes
Consistency of intervention (within and between groups)YesNoNoYesNoNoNo
Data Collection MethodsOutcome assessors blind to group allocationUnclearUnclearUnclearUnclearUnclearUnclearUnclear
Baseline measures taken before the interventionNoYesYesYesYesYesNo
Consistency of data collectionYesNoYesYesYesYesYes
AnalysesAll outcomes reported (means and SD/SE)YesNoNoYesYesYesYes
All participants accounted for (i.e., losses/exclusions)YesYesYesNoYesNoYes
ITT analysis conducted (all data included after allocation)NAUnclearUnclearNoUnclearNoUnclear
Individual level analysisYesYesYesYesYesYesYes
Statistical analysis methods appropriate for study designYesNoYesYesYesYesYes
External ValiditySample representative of target populationNoNoNoNoNoNoNo
Overall Quality ScoreTotal number of points (out of possible 38)16122018181420
Quality rating as percent42.1 (M)31.6 (L)52.6 (M)47.4 (M)47.4 (M)36.8 (M)52.6 (M)
Responded to query about “uncertain” ratings
ITT: intention to treatment; Yes = 2; Partial (Pa.) = 1; No = 0; Unclear (Un) = 0; NA = criterion inapplicable to this study design; any changes made after consultation with study authors are highlighted in boldface. Appraisal quality: High (H): 67–100%, Moderate (M): 34–66%, Low (L): 0–33% [19].
Table 6. Statistics of quality appraisal of records in this study.
Table 6. Statistics of quality appraisal of records in this study.
YesPartialNoUnclearNA
Frequency(%)Frequency(%)Frequency(%)Frequency(%)Frequency(%)
Power Calculation Reported250039950000
Inclusion/exclusion Criteria Reported20490021510000
Individual Level Allocation26630082037410
Random Allocation to Groups/Condition/Order256100615615410
Randomization Procedure Appropriate2500003073922
Groups Similar (Sociodemographic)19462500204900
Group Balanced at Baseline15371200256100
Participants Blind to Research Question11270037276600
Clear Description of Intervention and Control379012000037
Consistency of Intervention (within and between groups)16390022540037
Outcome Assessors Blind to Group Allocation1200615338012
Baseline Measures Taken before the Intervention22541214341237
Consistency of Data Collection399500250000
All Outcomes Reported (Means and SD/SE)14340027660000
All Participants Accounted for (i.e., losses/exclusions)3278009220000
ITT Analysis Conducted (all data included after allocation)00006153176410
Individual Level Analysis4010000000000
Statistical Analysis Methods Appropriate for Study Design3278008201200
Sample Representative of Target Population0000411000000
Table 7. Summary of the outcomes of the records.
Table 7. Summary of the outcomes of the records.
SourceOutcomes
[78]When conducting a computer task, participants had a smaller SBP increase with the presence of plants than without plants. After accomplishing the task, the participants also exhibited a faster SBP decrease when plants were present than when plants were absent. Participants’ reaction time was 12% faster when plants were present than when they were absent.
[53]Participants had the lowest productivity when the office was furnished with 22 potted plants, whereas the highest productivity was observed when no plants were present.
[84]Participants had a significantly lower search error rate with indoor greening than without indoor greening.
[80]The percentage of participants putting their hands in ice water for more than 5 min was higher with the presence of plants than without plants.
[83]Female participants’ decreases in EEG β waves and EDA were significantly faster when red-flowering geraniums were present than when flowerless geraniums were present and when plants were absent.
[70]Male participants had a lower score in the association task than their female counterparts when plants were absent, whereas female participants had higher scores on the sorting task regardless of the presence or absence of plants.
[69]Female participants’ EEG β waves and EDA were significantly lower when flower arrangements were present than when flower arrangements were absent.
[85]Participants’ time of hand immersion in ice water was significantly longer when green-leaf and flowering plants were simultaneously present than when only green-leaf plants or flowering plants were in the room and when plants were not in the room. Participants’ EDA was significantly lower when the plants were in the room than when the plants were not in the room.
[71]Female participants showed significantly higher scores of the association task than male participants in the three interventions. Female participants had significantly higher scores of the association task when plants were present than when the magazine-rack was present.
[72]Participants had the greatest effect of EEG β waves when viewing the slide of the office with a nature window view and indoor plants than other slides.
[55]A weak but significant correlation was observed between the number of potted plants and sick leave days in the workplace.
[68]The increased humidity of the indoor potted plants improved the vagus-induced sympathovagal balance of the heart of the participant.
[63]Participants’ frequency of pain killer consumption, SBP, and heart rate were significantly lower when plants were in the room than when plants were not in the room.
[67]Participants’ frequency of visiting the school infirmary was significantly lower when plants were in the room than when plants were not in the room.
[86]Participants’ grade point averages wer significantly higher when plants were present than when plants were absent.
[65]Participants’ sick leave hours and misconduct were significantly less when plants were present than when plants were absent.
[64]Participants’ frequency of pain killer use and hospitalization days were significantly lower when plants were in the room than when plants were not in the room.
[87]Participants’ attention improved significantly from the baseline to after the proofreading task was completed when plants were present, whereas no improvement was noted when plants were absent.
[66]Participants who took care of plants had greater academic achievement than those who did not.
[73]Red, yellow, and green plants significantly reduced participants’ DBP and fingertip pulse. Red, purple, and yellow plants significantly reduced participants’ fingertip pulse. Changes in fingertip pulse were more significant in male participants than in female participants.
[74]Except for yellow African daisies, the other flowers significantly reduced participants’ SBP. Pink and white African daisies, pink and white carnations, and pink and white roses significantly reduced participants’ DBP.
[88]Male participants spent significantly more time looking at white Hedera helix L. than at the dark green variety. Female participants had a greater frequency of looking at yellow-green plants than looking at dark green and green-white plants.
[89]Male participants spent significantly more time looking at green plants than at red-green ones. The number of fixings at red–green plants was greater than at green and white–green plants. Female participants spent significantly more time looking at green and red–green plants and with greater frequency than green–white plants.
[79]Relative to green plants with white, yellow, pink, and red flowers, green-leaf plants resulted in a greater increase in participants’ relative slow α power, relative fast α power, relative low β power, and relative moderate β power spectra. By contrast, green-leaf plants with yellow flowers increased participants’ relative θ power spectrum.
[58]Participants spent less time completing the vigilance and information processing tasks when plants were present than when plants were absent.
[81]Participants had a significantly higher δ waves and significantly lower α and β waves when plants were present than when plants were absent.
[82]After transplanting plants, participants had a significantly lower DBP than their counterparts did after a computer operation task.
[54]The indoor nature contact during work was significantly negatively correlated with sick leave days.
[61]The percentage of patients with stable blood pressure, heart rate, respiration rate, and body temperature was significantly higher in the ward with plants than in the one without plants. These patients also received a significantly lower dose of pain killers and had significantly shorter hospitalization.
[90]Yellow–green Hedera helix L. received more attention than did the plants of other colors.
[91]Participants had lower heart rate in the room when the plants were present than when the plants were not present.
[77]Participants had a significantly faster reaction rate when plants were present than when plants were absent.
[75]In both the actual and virtual environments with plants, participants exhibited greater changes in SBP, DBP, and EDA than in the plantless environment. They also had greater performance in the visual backward digit span task in the plant setting.
[57]Participants had the least flicker fusion frequency (eye fatigue) when flowering plants were provided than with other plants and controls.
[60]Participants had significantly lower SBP and a significant increase in the amplitude of high β waves when plants were present than when plants were absent.
[76]Participants without houseplants had significantly higher SBP and heart rate than those with houseplants.
[59]Participants had a significantly greater proportion of significantly decreased pulse rate when the plant was present than when the plant was absent.
[56]Participants had a significant increase in α relative waves in the prefrontal and occipital lobes and in parasympathetic nervous activity when the plant was present than when the plant was absent.
[92]There were significant differences between the two horticultural activities and between the pretest and the posttest.
[62]There were significant differences between the experimental and the control groups in heart rate variability (standard deviation of the NN intervals, root mean square of the successive differences, low frequency, high frequency, and low frequency/high frequency). Within the treatment, male participants’ standard deviation of the NN intervals was significantly different between sowing and transplanting seedlings.
[93]Participants had a significantly lower heart rate after sowing, transplanting seedlings, and potting succulents. Among the four kinds of horticultural activities, sowing yielded the greatest heart rate reduction while herbal flower potting was the worst.
[94]Participants had significantly fewer errors and faster time of task completion when the plants and pictures were present than when they were absent.
SBP: systolic blood pressure; DBP: diastolic blood pressure; EEG: electroencephalography; EDA: electrodermal activity.
Table 8. Original data of the studies examining the influence of indoor plants on DBP.
Table 8. Original data of the studies examining the influence of indoor plants on DBP.
StudyStudy DesignAppraisal QualityWithout PlantWith Plant
nMeanSDnMeanSD
[82]Experiment (RCT)Moderate2471.750.782465.260.69
[60]Experiment (RCT)Moderate5068.25.775067.39.05
[76]Survey (non-RCT)Moderate30074.206.2030070.106.00
Table 9. Heterogeneity test results of studies on the influence of indoor plants on DBP.
Table 9. Heterogeneity test results of studies on the influence of indoor plants on DBP.
ModelNumber of StudiesPooled Effect SizeHeterogeneity
Effect SizeStandard Errorp-ValueQ-Valuedf (Q)p-ValueI-Squared
Fixed3−0.6440.077<0.00181.7822<0.00197.554
Random3−2.5260.8250.002
Table 10. Original data of the studies examining the influence of indoor plants on EEG α waves.
Table 10. Original data of the studies examining the influence of indoor plants on EEG α waves.
StudyStudy
Design
Appraisal QualityWithout PlantWith Plant
nMeanSDnMeanSD
[72]Experiment
(non-RCT)
Moderate380.1300.210380.0900.170
[81]Experiment
(RCT)
Moderate170.0430.020170.1120.027
[56]Field quasi-
experiment
(non-RCT)
Moderate300.1600.054600.2100.054
Table 11. Heterogeneity test results of studies on the influence of indoor plants on EEG α waves.
Table 11. Heterogeneity test results of studies on the influence of indoor plants on EEG α waves.
ModelNumber of StudiesPooled Effect SizeHeterogeneity
Effect SizeStandard Errorp-ValueQ-Valuedf (Q)p-ValueI-Squared
Fixed30.6050.156<0.00136.2852<0.00194.488
Random31.1400.7140.110
Table 12. Original data of the studies examining the influence of indoor plants on EEG β waves.
Table 12. Original data of the studies examining the influence of indoor plants on EEG β waves.
StudyStudy
Design
Appraisal QualityWithout PlantWith Plant
nMeanSDnMeanSD
[72]Experiment
(non-RCT)
Moderate380.1600.240380.1200.220
[81]Experiment
(RCT)
Moderate170.0510.046170.2140.057
Table 13. Heterogeneity test results of studies on the influence of indoor plants on EEG β waves.
Table 13. Heterogeneity test results of studies on the influence of indoor plants on EEG β waves.
ModelNumber of StudiesPooled Effect SizeHeterogeneity
Effect SizeStandard Errorp-ValueQ-Valuedf (Q)p-ValueI-Squared
Fixed20.3810.2100.06934.8851<0.00197.133
Random21.4551.6600.381
Table 14. Original data of the studies examining the influence of indoor plants on attention.
Table 14. Original data of the studies examining the influence of indoor plants on attention.
StudyStudyDesignAppraisal QualityWithout PlantWith Plant
nMeanSDnMeanSD
[53]_1Experiment (RCT)Moderate2843.556.762740.286.94
[53]_2Experiment (RCT)Moderate2843.556.762638.248.64
[71]Experiment (RCT)Moderate1864.6720.081878.7721.89
[75]Experiment (RCT)Moderate304.691.18305.291.13
Table 15. Heterogeneity test results of studies on the influence of indoor plants on attention.
Table 15. Heterogeneity test results of studies on the influence of indoor plants on attention.
ModelNumber of StudiesPooled Effect SizeHeterogeneity
Effect SizeStandard Errorp-ValueQ-Valuedf (Q)p-ValueI-Squared
Fixed4−0.0380.1430.78916.74930.00182.088
Random4−0.0050.3400.988
Table 16. Original data of the studies examining the influence of indoor plants on academic achievement.
Table 16. Original data of the studies examining the influence of indoor plants on academic achievement.
StudyStudy
Design
Appraisal QualityWithout PlantWith Plant
nMeanSDnMeanSD
[86]Field quasi-experiment (non-RCT)Low392.620.847443.140.795
[66]Field experiment (RCT)Moderate190.1330.009170.1540.098
Table 17. Heterogeneity test results of studies on the influence of indoor plants on academic achievement.
Table 17. Heterogeneity test results of studies on the influence of indoor plants on academic achievement.
ModelNumber
of Studies
Pooled Effect SizeHeterogeneity
Effect SizeStandard Errorp-ValueQ-Valuedf (Q)p-ValueI-Squared
Fixed20.5340.1870.0040.63910.4240.000
Random20.5340.1870.004
Table 18. Original data of the studies examining the influence of indoor plants on response time.
Table 18. Original data of the studies examining the influence of indoor plants on response time.
StudyStudy
Design
Appraisal QualityWithout PlantWith Plant
nMeanSDnMeanSD
[58]Field experiment
(RCT)
Moderate1720.3905.8701617.3903.850
[77]Experiment
(non-RCT)
Moderate317289.90051.115319286.10040.377
[94]Experiment
(RCT)
Moderate401228.000258.72040738.650186.180
Table 19. Heterogeneity test results of studies on the influence of indoor plants on response time.
Table 19. Heterogeneity test results of studies on the influence of indoor plants on response time.
ModelNumber
of Studies
Pooled Effect SizeHeterogeneity
Effect SizeStandard Errorp-ValueQ-Valuedf (Q)p-ValueI-Squared
Fixed3−0.2520.0750.00151.8722<0.00196.144
Random3−0.9390.6840.170
Table 20. Results of linear Egger’s regressions test.
Table 20. Results of linear Egger’s regressions test.
Egger’s Regression Test
EffectInterceptp-Value
DBP−5.8920.527
EEG α waves10.0050.374
attention7.2510.656
response time−5.6790.424
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Han, K.-T.; Ruan, L.-W.; Liao, L.-S. Effects of Indoor Plants on Human Functions: A Systematic Review with Meta-Analyses. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 7454. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19127454

AMA Style

Han K-T, Ruan L-W, Liao L-S. Effects of Indoor Plants on Human Functions: A Systematic Review with Meta-Analyses. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(12):7454. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19127454

Chicago/Turabian Style

Han, Ke-Tsung, Li-Wen Ruan, and Li-Shih Liao. 2022. "Effects of Indoor Plants on Human Functions: A Systematic Review with Meta-Analyses" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 12: 7454. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19127454

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop