Next Article in Journal
Sweet as Sugar—How Shared Social Identities Help Patients in Coping with Diabetes Mellitus
Next Article in Special Issue
Trajectories of Job Burnout among Bus Drivers in China: A Three-Year Follow-Up Study
Previous Article in Journal
Financial Reasons for Working beyond the Statutory Retirement Age: Risk Factors and Associations with Health in Late Life
Previous Article in Special Issue
Social Undermining and Interpersonal Rumination among Employees: The Mediating Role of Being the Subject of Envy and the Moderating Role of Social Support
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Moderator Role of Interpersonal Emotion Regulation on the Associations between Commitment, Intimacy, and Couple Satisfaction

by
Mihaela Jitaru
and
Maria Nicoleta Turliuc
*
Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Alexandru Ioan Cuza University of Iasi, 700554 Iasi, Romania
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(17), 10506; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191710506
Submission received: 11 July 2022 / Revised: 15 August 2022 / Accepted: 20 August 2022 / Published: 23 August 2022

Abstract

:
Couple satisfaction is seen as very important by all those in a romantic relationship; however, there are no recipes for it. Using a dyadic approach, we investigate how commitment and intimacy influence couple satisfaction and the moderator role of interpersonal emotion regulation (affect-improving and -worsening strategies). To achieve the scope of the study, we collected data from 131 couples, which were later analyzed using the actor–partner interdependence model with moderation (APIMoM). The results showed that the actor-effect of both commitment and intimacy on couple satisfaction is significant. We found mixed results for the partner-effect of the two variables. Both partners’ strategies moderated the association between commitment and couple satisfaction. Women’s use of affect-worsening strategies moderated the link between men’s intimacy and women’s couple satisfaction. The impact of the interactions of commitment or intimacy with interpersonal affect-improving and -worsening strategies on couple satisfaction is discussed further, as well as the implications and importance of the results.

1. Introduction

Romantic relationships play an essential role in everyone’s life; therefore, the need to have them be healthy, strong, stable, and durable is understandable. However, not everyone can have such a relationship. An indicator often used to assess how the relationship is going is the general couple satisfaction reported by the partners. Couple satisfaction is a subjective global evaluation of the relationship made by one or both partners [1] or the positive evaluation and attraction to the relationship [2,3]. The previous studies highlighted that a higher level of couple satisfaction was associated with the relationship’s longevity [4], with partners’ overall health [5], satisfaction with life [6], and well-being [7]. Considering all these favorable outcomes, highlighting and researching those factors that can contribute to a higher level of couple satisfaction are essential endeavors. The factors influencing couple satisfaction were intimacy, commitment [8,9,10], emotion regulation, and many others. This study aimed to verify the moderating influence of interpersonal emotion regulation (IER) on the relation between intimacy or commitment and couple satisfaction.

1.1. Commitment and Couple Satisfaction

Relational commitment is commonly understood as an individual’s intention or desire to continue their relationship [11,12]. It is also characterized by a long-term orientation and a strong attachment to the partner and the relationship [13,14]. Therefore, it is often viewed as a cognitive activity that impacts the decisions one is making about their relationship and partner [3,15].
In multiple instances, it was revealed that relational commitment and couple satisfaction hold a strong association [10,16,17,18]. The investment model [2,19] aimed to explain the connections between commitment and couple satisfaction. It explored why some relationships persist in time while others wither and dissolve. It theorized that satisfying experiences will not suffice for a long-term relationship. Other factors, like social pressure to continue, complicated termination procedures, and availability of alternative romantic partners, also had a significant impact on the relationship [20].
The investment model of commitment proposed that satisfaction can be a predictor of commitment; yet, some authors suggested that the inverse relation should be further explored [8,9]. Previous research suggested that high levels of commitment were associated with high levels of marital satisfaction and that commitment was one of the most critical factors for a good relationship [21]. Furthermore, it showed that low levels of commitment often revealed that the partner offered less and obtained only minor benefits from the relationship and thus had low levels of satisfaction [22].

1.2. Intimacy and Couple Satisfaction

Romantic intimacy was defined as a process involving the feeling of closeness to the partner and the wish to share experiences and activities with them [23,24]. It is relevant to highlight the interactional aspect of intimacy, as it is not unidirectional. The action produced by one partner will elicit a responsive reaction from the other [24]. Individuals with higher levels of intimacy tend to be more open, supportive, understanding, caring, and honest in their romantic relationships [25]. In previous research, intimacy emerged as an essential factor in couple satisfaction, as those with a higher level of intimacy had a more satisfactory relationship [26]. Similar to couple satisfaction, intimacy can positively affect both the individual and the relationship. Intimacy was revealed to positively impact physical health [27], well-being [28,29], and couple satisfaction [30,31,32].

1.3. Interpersonal Emotion Regulation and Romantic Relationships

Interpersonal emotion regulation (IER) is a process through which an individual (regulator) aims to change and influence another’s (target) emotional state [33,34,35]. Niven, Totterdell, and Holman’s model distinguished two types of regulating strategies, the interpersonal affect-improving ones and the interpersonal affect-worsening ones. The strategies that improve the affect represent all the behaviors and actions that a regulator employs to elicit, raise the level of positive emotions or lower the level of negative emotions in their target [33,34]. The affect-worsening strategies encompass all the behaviors and actions that a regulator employs intending to elicit negative emotions, lower the level of positive emotions, or raise the level of negative emotions in their target [33,34].
IER, due to its very nature, had an effect on social relationships and, therefore, on romantic relationships [36]. Using IER (such as touch) in romantic dyads helped promote the psychological well-being of the partners [37]. Through IER (down-regulation of negative emotions), partners reduced in each other the level of distress they were experiencing [38]. In turn, this helped maintain a high level of couple satisfaction [39]. Couple satisfaction was also directly and positively influenced by IER (enhancing positive affect) [40]. As previously seen, IER strategies benefited couples’ satisfaction; yet, they needed to be from the affect-improving category, as seen in the previous examples. In opposition, when using strategies meant to worsen the affect, a negative impact on couple satisfaction was most likely to be seen. Hostile criticism [41], ridicule [42], withdrawal, and punitive intent [43] had negative connections to couple satisfaction.

1.4. Commitment and Interpersonal Emotion Regulation

Both commitment and IER are processes specific to interpersonal relationships. Romantic commitment was described as an individual’s desire to continue their relationship and having a strong attachment to both their partner and the relationship [13,14]. IER represents the actions taken by a person to influence another individual’s affective state, in this case, their romantic partner [33,34]. Commitment and interpersonal affect-worsening strategies (withdrawal behaviors, criticism, belittling, disrespect) were negatively connected [44,45,46,47]. In opposition, interpersonal affect-improving strategies (support, allowing the partner to present their emotions) were positively related to commitment [47,48].

1.5. Intimacy and Interpersonal Emotion Regulation

As stated previously, intimacy is an interactive process between the individuals in a romantic relationship [24,49]; thus, making it an interpersonal process. Due to its characteristics, IER is also a relational process [33,34]. Intimacy, more specifically, disclosure, has been established as an indirect socio-affective pathway to emotion regulation [37,50]. In addition, the relationships that one has throughout their life have been highlighted as essential resources in emotional regulation [51,52,53,54,55].

1.6. The Present Study

This research aimed to investigate the relationship between the main study’s variables and, most importantly, show how commitment and intimacy impact romantic relationships and interpersonal emotion regulation moderates those relationships. In addition, all these connections are explored from a dyadic perspective using the Actor–Partner Interdependence Model [56]. This model allows researchers to investigate the associations between an individual’s traits and their outcomes (actor-effect) and partner outcomes (partner-effect). Previous research showed that commitment and intimacy are among the factors that impact the most couple satisfaction [21,26]. IER is a relatively new concept that needs investigation for a better understanding, especially concerning other relationship-relevant factors. However, some behaviors that are part of IER strategies were connected to romantic commitment [44,48], intimacy [50], and couple satisfaction [40,43]. Therefore, this study explored the relations between commitment/intimacy and couple satisfaction with the moderating role of IER in both its forms (affect-improving strategies and affect-worsening strategies). Given what we know till this moment of the connections between previously stated variables, we expected that both commitment and intimacy would have a positive effect (actor and partner) on couple satisfaction. In addition, we expected a positive impact of the use of affect-improving strategies on the relationship between commitment/intimacy and couple satisfaction and a negative one for the use of affect-worsening strategies.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

This study’s data were gathered from 131 heterosexual young couples (N = 262). The inclusion criteria for this study were the following: the participants had to be at least 18 years old and in a romantic relationship for at least six months at the moment they took the survey, and both of the couple partners had to complete all of the questionnaires. The mean age for women was 21.76 years (SD = 3.13), and for men it was 23.60 years (SD = 3.58), and the average relationship length was 26.35 months (SD = 26.68). Of the total of 131 couples, 43 couples were living together, while the rest of the couples were not living in the same household when they took the survey. All the participants gave their informed consent to participate in the study and were informed about the anonymity of their answers and the fact that they could withdraw from the study without any consequences. The respondents’ participation was voluntary, and they were not compensated.

2.2. Measures

The Couple Satisfaction Index−4 (CSI−4) [57] was used to assess the general couple satisfaction. CSI−4 is a four-item scale developed using the item response theory. The total score was calculated by summing the answers given by the respondents to the items (e.g., ‘I have a warm and comfortable relationship with my partner’). The internal consistency in this study’s sample was high, α = 0.90 for women and α = 0.91 for men.
The commitment was assessed with the commitment subscale from the Investment Model Scale (IMS) [58]. This subscale comprises seven items (e.g., ‘I am committed to maintaining my relationship with my partner.’). For each item, the respondents reported their degree of agreement on a scale ranging from 0 (do not agree at all) to 8 (agree completely). The total score was calculated by summing the answers given to each item. The internal consistency coefficients were high both for women and men (αw = 0.88, αm = 0.93).
The Personal Assessment of Intimacy in Relationships (PAIR) [23] was used to assess the respondents’ couple intimacy. The scale consisted of 36 items (e.g., ‘My partner listens to me when I need someone to talk to.’) that measure the self-reported feelings of intimacy along five dimensions (emotional, social, sexual, intellectual, and conventionality). Because the subscales are highly correlated they can be combined to form a composite measure of intimacy [59]. The internal consistency was high both for women (α = 0.85), and men (α = 0.83).
The interpersonal emotion regulation was assessed using the Emotion Regulation of Others and Self (EROS) [60]. For the purpose of this study, only nine items were used, the ones that appertain to the extrinsic subscale. Furthermore, they were modified to fit the aims of this study better. Therefore, we changed the generalist “someone” to the more focused “my partner” in each item. Of the nine items, three of them were for assessing the affect-worsening strategies (e.g., ‘I told my partner about their shortcomings to try to make them feel worse.’), while the other six items assessed the use of affect-improving strategies (e.g., ‘I did something nice with my partner to try to make them feel better.’). The total score for the two subscales was calculated by summing the answers given by the participants for each item on a scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (a great deal). The internal consistencies for the subscales were acceptable for this research (affect-worsening strategies: α = 0.68 for women, α = 0.67 for men; affect-improving strategies: α = 0.80 for women, α = 0.77 for men).
Additionally, the demographic variables we assessed were age, relationship length, and the living situation (whether the couple lived together or in different households). All the items are presented in Appendix A.

2.3. Statistical Analysis

Firstly, we computed paired-sample t-tests to assess the gender differences for the main variables and partial correlations, controlling for age, relationship length, and the living situation (together or in separate households). Secondly, using the Actor–Partner Moderation Model (APIMoM) [61], we computed four different analyses (as observed in the conceptual model from Figure 1). The first analyzed the relationship between commitment and couple satisfaction, moderated by the use of interpersonal affect-improving strategies, while the second changed the moderator to the use of interpersonal affect-worsening strategies. The third model investigated the relationship between intimacy and couple satisfaction, moderated by the interpersonal affect-improving strategies, and the fourth model kept the main variables, changing the moderator to the interpersonal affect-worsening strategies. After analyzing the four models, their terms were graphed following Dawson’s [62] indications for a more straightforward interpretation of the significant interactions. The statistical analyses were computed using IBM SPSS version 20 (SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) and IBM SPSS Amos version 22 (SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).

3. Results

Firstly, we present the preliminary analyses, which comprise the results of the paired-sample t-tests for the gender differences for the main variables and the correlations for the same variables. Secondly, we present the results separately for the four APIMoM models we computed. For each model, we present the fit indices, highlight the significant results, and for the significant interaction effects, we provide the interaction plots. The first model contains the commitment of both partners as predictors, their couple satisfaction separately as outcomes, and their use of interpersonal affect-improving strategies as moderators. The second model follows the precedent’s structure, changing only the moderators, here being the interpersonal affect-worsening strategies. In the third and fourth models, we have the level of intimacy of both partners as predictors and their couple satisfaction, separately, as outcomes. In the third model, the moderator is the interpersonal affect-improving strategies, and the affect-worsening strategies is the moderator in the fourth model. Before starting to present the results of this study, we need to clarify that the couples that completed the surveys were young, with an average age of 23 years old. This means that the results of this study cannot be reliably generalized to older individuals that are in a romantic relationship.

3.1. Preliminary Analysis

Firstly, we conducted a series of paired-sample t-tests to assess the gender differences for the main variables of the study. The results (Table 1) show significant differences between women and men only for the use of interpersonal affect-worsening strategies t (130) = 2.60, p = 0.011. Table 2 displays the correlational matrix for the study’s main variables. Table 2 shows the correlational matrix for the study’s main variables. Both women’s and men’s couple satisfaction were positively and significantly associated with each other and with women’s and men’s commitment, intimacy, and use of affect-improving strategies and negatively associated with the use of interpersonal affect-worsening strategies.

3.2. The Effect of Romantic Commitment on Couple Satisfaction Moderated by the Use of Affect-Improving Strategies

This model showed very good fit indices: χ²(3) = 3.502, p = 0.321, χ²/df = 1.167, CFI = 0.999, RMSEA = 0.036 (0.000–0.156). The results (Table 3) show that women’s and men’s romantic commitment had a positive and significant effect on their couple satisfaction (βwoman = 0.234, βman = 0.265). The partner effects of commitment on couple satisfaction were not significant for either women or men (βwoman = 0.043, βman = 0.070). The women’s use of interpersonal affect-improving strategies significantly affected their couple satisfaction (βwoman = 0.197) but not their partners’ couple satisfaction (βman = 0.028). Men’s use of interpersonal affect-improving strategies had no significant effect on either men’s or women’s couple satisfaction.
The interaction effect between women’s romantic commitment and women’s use of affect-improving strategies was significant for both women’s and men’s couple satisfaction (βwoman = 0.028, βman = 0.019). The interaction plot (Figure 2a) suggests that the slopes for the relationships between women’s commitment and women’s satisfaction were positive for both low and high women’s use of interpersonal affect-improving strategies. However, it can be observed that for the women who used more interpersonal affect-improving strategies and were more committed to their relationships, the effect is more accentuated.
In the second interaction plot (Figure 2b), we noticed that the relationship between women’s commitment level and men’s couple satisfaction varied, but only when women used more interpersonal affect-improving strategies. Therefore, when women had a higher level of commitment and used more interpersonal affect-improving strategies, their partners had a higher couple satisfaction.
The interaction effects between men’s commitment and women’s use of affect-improving strategies were significant for both women’s and men’s couple satisfaction (βwoman = −0.018, βman = −0.023). For the first effect, the interaction plot (Figure 2c) suggests that women’s couple satisfaction did not noticeably vary based on their partners’ level of commitment when the women used high levels of interpersonal affect-improving strategies. On the opposite side, when the women used less interpersonal affect-improving strategies, there was a positive relationship between their partners’ commitment and their own satisfaction.
In the next interaction plot (Figure 2d), we can observe that both slopes indicate positive relations. However, it is important to note that men’s couple satisfaction seems higher when they had a higher commitment and their partners used less interpersonal affect-improving strategies.
The interaction effects between women’s commitment and men’s use of interpersonal affect-improving strategies were significant for men’s couple satisfaction but not for women’s couple satisfaction (βwoman = 0.010, βman = −0.027). The interaction plot (Figure 3a) computed for the significant effect indicates that there was a positive relationship between women’s commitment and men’s satisfaction only when men used less affect-improving strategies.
The interaction effects between men’s romantic commitment and men’s use of interpersonal affect-improving strategies had a significant impact only on men’s couple satisfaction (βwoman = −0.015, βman = 0.039). The computed interaction plot (Figure 3b) suggests that men’s couple satisfaction was the highest when they were committed to their romantic relationship and used more interpersonal affect-improving strategies.

3.3. The Effect of Romantic Commitment on Couple Satisfaction Moderated by the Use of Affect-Worsening Strategies

The model revealed very good fit indices: χ²(1) = 0, p = 0.991, χ²/df = 0, CFI = 1, RMSEA = 0 (0.000–0.000). The results (Table 3) indicate that women’s and men’s romantic commitment significantly and positively affected their couple satisfaction (βwoman = 0.229, βman = 0.170). The partner effect of commitment on couple satisfaction was significant and positive for both women and men (βwoman = 0.066, βman = 0.100). In the results, we can also observe that women’s use of interpersonal affect-worsening strategies did not significantly influence their couple satisfaction (βwoman = −0.123); however, it affected their partners’ satisfaction in a significant and negative way (βman = −0.211). Men’s use of interpersonal affect-worsening strategies had no significant impact on their or their partners’ couple satisfaction (βwoman = −0.099, βman = −0.078).
The results do not show a significant interaction effect of women’s commitment and use of interpersonal affect-worsening strategies on women’s or men’s couple satisfaction (βwoman = −0.012, βman = −0.017). In comparison, the interaction of men’s commitment and women’s use of interpersonal affect-worsening strategies had a significant and positive effect on both women’s and men’s couple satisfaction (βwoman = 0.030, βman = 0.030). The interaction plot (Figure 4a) for women’s couple satisfaction indicates that there was a positive relationship between men’s commitment and women’s satisfaction only when women used more affect-worsening strategies.
In the interaction plot for men’s couple satisfaction (Figure 4b), we can observe that there was a positive relationship between men’s commitment and their satisfaction regardless of the women’s level of affect-worsening strategies. However, the relationship was stronger when women used more affect-worsening strategies. For this model, none of the other interactions significantly affected women’s or men’s couple satisfaction (Table 3).

3.4. The Effect of Intimacy on Couple Satisfaction Moderated by the Use of Affect-Improving Strategies

The analyzed model showed very good fit indices: χ²(1) = 0.025, p = 0.875, χ²/df = 0.025, CFI = 1, RMSEA = 0 (0.000–0.119). The results (Table 4) display a positive and significant effect of intimacy on couple satisfaction for both women and men (βwoman = 0.084, βman = 0.103). The partner effect was significant only for men (βman = 0.047); women’s level of intimacy positively influenced their couple satisfaction. In comparison, women’s couple satisfaction was not affected by men’s relational intimacy (βwoman = 0.024). Women’s use of interpersonal affect-improving strategies had a positive and significant effect on their couple satisfaction (βwoman = 0.180) but no significant impact on their partner’s couple satisfaction (βman = 0.030). Men’s employment of interpersonal affect-improving strategies had no impact on their own or their partner’s couple satisfaction (βwoman = 0.042, βman = 0.082). As can be observed in Table 4, there were no significant interaction effects for this model.

3.5. The Effect of Intimacy on Couple Satisfaction Moderated by the Use of Affect-Worsening Strategies

The model showed a very good fit: χ²(1) = 0.129, p = 0.720, χ²/df = 0.129, CFI = 1, RMSEA = 0 (0.000–0.166). This model’s results (Table 4) indicate that intimacy had a positive and significant effect on couple satisfaction for women and men (βwoman = 0.091, βman = 0.104). The partner effect suggests that only men’s couple satisfaction was significantly affected by women’s level of intimacy (βwoman = 0.023, βman = 0.040). In addition, neither women’s (βwoman = −0.043, βman = −0.091) nor men’s (βwoman = −0.084, βman = −0.067) use of interpersonal affect-worsening strategies had a significant effect on women’s or men’s couple satisfaction. The interaction effects of women’s intimacy and their use of interpersonal affect-worsening strategies on their or their partners’ couple satisfaction were not significant (βwoman = 0.002, βman = −0.001). The interaction of men’s level of intimacy with women’s use of interpersonal affect-worsening strategies had a significant effect on women’s couple satisfaction (βwoman = 0.015) but not on men’s satisfaction (βman = 0.002).
The interaction plot (Figure 4c) for the significant effect shows that men’s intimacy and women’s satisfaction were positively associated only when women used more affect-worsening strategies. The interaction between women’s intimacy and men’s use of interpersonal affect-worsening strategies did not significantly affect women’s or men’s couple satisfaction (βwoman = −0.013, βman = −0.006). Similarly, the interaction of men’s intimacy and their use of interpersonal affect-worsening strategies did not significantly affect women’s or men’s couple satisfaction (βwoman = 0.005, βman = 0.006).

4. Discussion

Couple satisfaction is a characteristic most people in a romantic relationship desire. However, how to reach it is not always clear and there are no general recipes. That makes exploring the possible mechanisms that contribute to the changes in couple satisfaction all the more desirable. Consequently, this research explored the effects of commitment and intimacy on couple satisfaction while adding the moderating effect of IER. This study and its results focused on young couples; therefore, the following explanations and implications need to be understood through this lens.
Prior to investigating the effects mentioned earlier, we verified if there were gender differences in our variables’ scores due to the dyadic character of our investigation process. The only differences observed were for the use of interpersonal affect-worsening strategies. Prior research did not show a clear pattern of connections between gender and the use of interpersonal affect-improving and -worsening strategies. Some studies showed differences between women’s and men’s use of interpersonal affect-worsening strategies, with the latter employing more [60,63]. In contrast, other studies did not find gender differences [64,65]. However, there is a study that obtained similar results, with women using more interpersonal affect-worsening strategies than men [66]. Given these contradictory results, more investigation of this process is necessary.
We observed that the actor-effect of commitment on couple satisfaction was a significant one. These results align with previous studies that showed similar effects [9,21,22]. The partner-effect of commitment on couple satisfaction was significant only in the model that included the use of interpersonal affect-worsening strategies as a moderator, which means that the men’s level of commitment positively affected their partners’ couple satisfaction. Women’s commitment influenced men’s couple satisfaction similarly. Therefore, commitment contributes significantly to couple satisfaction [21]. The factors contributing to commitment, compared to alternatives or investment size [2,19], can explain why it impacts couple satisfaction. Therefore, if an individual, after evaluating the possible alternatives, concludes that their relationship and their partner are the best possible for them, it will increase their couple satisfaction. Furthermore, it is possible, as the present results seem to indicate, that seeing one’s partner’s high level of commitment increases their couple satisfaction.
Intimacy had a significant actor-effect on couple satisfaction. These results again confirm those from previous research [26,30,31,32] on how intimacy positively impacts couple satisfaction. However, only women’s level of intimacy significantly affected their partners’ couple satisfaction, which means that a higher level of intimacy on women’s part will impact in a positive way men’s level of couple satisfaction. Previous research tended to investigate different parts of intimacy, and the results depended on which ones were analyzed. For example, women’s and men’s responsiveness influenced each other’s couple satisfaction, while women’s disclosure influenced only their couple satisfaction [67]. On a similar note, when analyzing the effects of emotional and sexual intimacy, only the first one had a partner effect [30]. Therefore, it is possible that general intimacy does not strongly influence partners as much as specific types of intimacy would.
As for the impact of the use of interpersonal affect-improving strategies, in both models that contained it, a significant and positive effect was observable in women’s cases. Their usage of these strategies had a relevant impact on their couple satisfaction. The use of affect-worsening strategies was significant only when the model contained the commitment variable, and it showed that women’s employment of these strategies negatively impacted their partners’ couple satisfaction. These results partially fit those from previous research, showing a positive impact of affect-improving strategies [37,40] and a negative effect of affect-worsening strategies [41,42,43] on couple satisfaction. In addition, the way women provide social support can explain further these results. Women provided more supportive strategies [68] and were more sensitive to their partners’ emotional needs [69]. Furthermore, this is also expected from them to fulfill their gender role [70].
Regarding the interactions between commitment or intimacy and interpersonal affect-improving or -worsening strategies, some proved significant, while others did not. We observed that the model that contained commitment and couple satisfaction and interpersonal affect-improving strategies as the moderator role had the most interaction effects. The interaction of women’s commitment and use of affect-improving strategies positively impacted women’s and men’s couple satisfaction. Therefore, in this case, we observed that the positive impact of commitment [9,21,22] and affect-improving strategies [37,40] on couple satisfaction remained even when they interact. The higher women’s commitment and use of interpersonal affect-improving strategies were, the higher their and their partners’ couple satisfaction was.
A significant impact was observable on women’s and men’s couple satisfaction from the interaction of men’s commitment and women’s use of interpersonal affect-improving strategies. Interestingly, here we observe that when men had a lower level of commitment, women’s use of more affect-improving strategies helped them have a higher level of couple satisfaction. Therefore, in this case, IER seemed to work as a buffering element between low commitment and couple satisfaction. However, for men’s couple satisfaction, it seems better if they had a high level of commitment and their partners use less affect-improving strategies. We could also observe a similar situation for women’s couple satisfaction; if their partners had a high level of commitment, there were better outcomes if they used less interpersonal affect-improving strategies. These results might seem counterintuitive, as we expected the best outcomes to appear when there was a high level of commitment and more interpersonal affect-improving strategies were used. Nonetheless, a possible explanatory mechanism can be given by the overuse of social support. When it is too much, it can feel overwhelming, become a social strain [71,72], and influence the emotional state in a negative manner [73], which in turn could negatively affect couple satisfaction [74,75]. Therefore, in this case, if men perceive their partners as trying too much to improve their emotional state, it could harm their couple satisfaction. In women’s case, the possible explanation can come from the emotional labor that is considered a significant part of women’s duty in a relationship [76]. As such, if women see their partners as committed and feel they do not need to expend that much emotional effort in their relationship, it increases their couple satisfaction.
When it comes to men’s couple satisfaction and commitment, their using more interpersonal affect-improving strategies will produce the best outcomes. Here, we observed that the effects of the interaction went in a similar direction as the first one presented for this model. Therefore, we can see how couple satisfaction is influenced by commitment [9,21,22] and affect-improving strategies [37,40].
The other models in which we observed significative interaction effects were those with interpersonal affect-worsening strategies as the moderator. In the case of commitment, it seems that both women and men had higher levels of couple satisfaction when men had higher levels of commitment and their partners used more affect-worsening strategies. When the levels of commitment were low, using fewer affect-worsening strategies influenced couple satisfaction positively than when more of these strategies were used. Similar to the model containing commitment, in the one with intimacy, we observed a significant interaction between men’s lower level of intimacy and women’s use of more interpersonal affect-worsening strategies and its positive impact on women’s couple satisfaction. These results may seem counterintuitive, as we would have expected that exhibiting behavior meant to induce or exacerbate negative emotions would negatively influence other relational outcomes. However, the overall tone of those interactions influenced the result. Even if there were many interactions based on criticism between the partners, not all of them necessarily produced bad outcomes for the relationship. Non-hostile criticism seems to have a positive connection for both women’s and men’s couple satisfaction [77]. Similarly, ridicule can contribute to a lower level of couple satisfaction [78]. However, its effects are dependent on how the individual relates themself to them [42]. If the individual enjoys being laughed at, their couple’s satisfaction will not suffer, but if they hate being ridiculed, there will be negative consequences for their relationship outcomes.
The results of this study are important because they show how IER can interact with already established variables, such as commitment and intimacy, to affect couple satisfaction. To our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate how interpersonal affect-improving and -worsening strategies interact with commitment and intimacy to influence couple satisfaction from a dyadic perspective. Using an APIMoM model helped us present a more nuanced view of the relations between the study’s variables. Although the research had its advantages, some limitations need to be addressed. This study’s sample consisted of primarily young college students, and most couples were not cohabiting at the moment of data collection. Therefore, it would be interesting to conduct the same investigation on older participants in different stages of life with different responsibilities. Similarly, conducting this investigation on an all-cohabiting sample may provide a different perspective on the analyzed relations. In addition, causal studies (longitudinal or experimental) can add to the reliability of the results and therefore improve the research approach. Moreover, more of the participants’ background information (such as mental status, family of origin, education level, or financial status) was not addressed in the present study, which constitutes a limit, as it may have influenced the manner in which individuals use IER strategies and their couple satisfaction. Future research should consider and explore the impact of the abovementioned variables on the use of IER strategies and couple satisfaction.

5. Conclusions

This study aimed to explore the effects of commitment and intimacy on couple satisfaction and the moderator effect of interpersonal emotion regulation, in both its forms, as affect-improving and affect-worsening strategies. This research explored all the aforementioned effects on a younger population; as such, the following summary of the results and their implications in research and practice is primarily applicable to younger individuals.
As a plus of the present study, the models we computed (APIMoM) consider the partners as different units. Therefore we could analyze both the effects of their predictors on their outcomes and their partners’.
In the following paragraph we present the summary of this study’s results. The results highlight commitment and intimacy’s impact on couple satisfaction from both the actor’s and the partner’s perspectives. In all the models, commitment and intimacy significantly and positively impacted individuals’ couple satisfaction. The partner effect for commitment was significant only in the presence of interpersonal affect-worsening strategies. As for intimacy, only women’s level of intimacy appeared to affect men’s level of couple satisfaction. In addition, women’s use of interpersonal affect-improving strategies contributed to their couple satisfaction. When considering commitment, women’s use of interpersonal affect-worsening strategies decreased men’s couple satisfaction. Furthermore, this study showed how the interactions between commitment, intimacy, and IER strategies impacted couple satisfaction.
Therefore, these results shed some light on the previously unexplored moderating effect of interpersonal emotion regulation on the relations between commitment, intimacy, and couple satisfaction. As such, this research advances the knowledge of the ways interpersonal emotion regulation interacts with already established variables in the study of romantic relationships to affect couple satisfaction.
Consequently, these results can also offer some relevant information and hints for therapists working with couples. Practitioners can use these results to shape more accurate intervention plans depending on their initial assessment of the couple. As presented above, some of the results show that one of the partners can bring a more significant change to the overall couple satisfaction, and this can help the practitioners create a plan that will better fit the current couple they are working with rather than a general plan that will produce results only in some situations. For example, understanding that putting effort into consolidating the commitment aspect of the relationship produces good results when also working on the use of affect-improving IER strategies, but when developing the intimacy part of the relationship, it is not as important to work on the use of IER strategies, unless they are the affect-worsening type. Differentiating between what is relevant or not to spend time on in counseling or therapy can be extremely important, as it can represent the difference between building and continuing with a healthy relationship or separating because nothing changes.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.J. and M.N.T.; methodology, M.J. and M.N.T.; investigation, M.J. and M.N.T.; data curation, M.J.; formal analysis, M.J.; writing—original draft preparation, M.J. and M.N.T.; writing—review and editing, M.J. and M.N.T.; visualization, M.J. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Alexandru Ioan Cuza University of Iaşi (protocol code 281/27.01.2022).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Appendix A.1. Personal Assessment of Intimacy in Relationships (PAIR)

  • My partner listens to me when I need someone to talk to.
  • We enjoy spending time with other couples.
  • I am satisfied with our sex life.
  • My partner helps me clarify my thoughts.
  • We enjoy the same recreational activities.
  • My partner has all the qualities I’ve ever wanted in a mate.
  • I can state me feelings without him/her getting defensive.
  • We usually “keep to ourselves.”
  • I feel our sexual activity is just routine.
  • When it comes to having a serious discussion it seems that we have little in common.
  • I share very few of my partners’ interests.
  • There are times when I do not feel a great deal of love and affection for my partner.
  • I often feel distant from my partner.
  • We have very few friends in common.
  • I am able to tell my partner when I want sexual intercourse.
  • I feel “put-down” in a serious conversation with my partner.
  • We like playing together.
  • Every new thing that I have learned about my partner has pleased me.
  • My partner can really understand my hurts and joys.
  • Having time together with friends is an important part of our shared activities.
  • I “hold back” my sexual interest because my partner makes me feel uncomfortable.
  • I feel it is useless to discuss some things with my partner.
  • We enjoy the out-of-doors together.
  • My partner and I understand each other completely.
  • I feel neglected at times by my partner.
  • Many of my partner’s closest friends are also my closest friends.
  • Sexual expression is an essential part of our relationship.
  • My partner frequently tries to change my ideas.
  • We seldom find time to do fun things together.
  • I don’t think anyone could possibly be happier than my partner and I when we are with one another.
  • I sometimes feel lonely when we’re together.
  • My partner disapproves of some of my friends.
  • My partner seems disinterested in sex.
  • We have an endless number of things to talk about.
  • I think that we share some of the same interests.
  • I have some needs that are not being met by my relationship.
The response scale is from 1-Does not describe me/my relationship at all to 5-Describes me/my relationship very well.

Appendix A.2. Commitment Level Index (CLI)

  • I want our relationship to last for a very long time (please circle a number).
  • I am committed to maintaining my relationship with my partner.
  • I would not feel very upset if our relationship were to end in the near future.
  • It is likely that I will date someone other than my partner within the next year.
  • I feel very attached to our relationship-very strongly linked to my partner.
  • I want our relationship to last forever.
  • I am oriented toward the long-term future of my relationship (for example, I imagine being with my partner several years from now).
The response scale is from 0-Do not agree at all to 8-Agree Completely.

Appendix A.3. Emotion Regulation of Others and Self-Extrinsic Scale (EROS-ES)

  • I gave my partner helpful advice to try to improve how they felt.
  • I told my partner about their shortcomings to try to make them feel worse
  • I did something nice with my partner to try to make them feel better
  • I acted annoyed towards my partner to try to make them feel worse
  • I explained to my partner how they had hurt myself or others, to try to make them feel worse
  • I discussed my partner’s positive characteristics to try to improve how they felt
  • I made my partner laugh to try to make them feel better
  • I listened to my partner’s problems to try to improve how they felt
  • I spent time with my partner to try to improve how they felt
The response scale is from 1-Not at all to 5-A great deal.

Appendix A.4. Couple Satisfaction Index–4 (CSI−4)

  • Please indicate the degree of happiness, all things considered, of your relationship. (0-Extremely unhappy to 6-Perfect)
  • I have a warm and comfortable relationship with my partner. (0-Not at all true to 5-Completely true)
  • How rewarding is your relationship with your partner? (0-Not at all to 5-Completely)
  • In general, how satisfied are you with your relationship? (0-Not at all to 5-Completely)

References

  1. Keizer, R. Relationship Satisfaction. In Encyclopedia of Quality of Life and Well-Being Research; Michalos, A.C., Ed.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2014; pp. 5437–5443. [Google Scholar]
  2. Rusbult, C.E. Commitment and Satisfaction in Romantic Associations: A Test of the Investment Model. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 1980, 16, 172–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Sternberg, R.J. A Triangular Theory of Love. Psychol. Rev. 1986, 93, 119–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Hand, M.M.; Thomas, D.; Buboltz, W.C.; Deemer, E.D.; Buyanjargal, M. Facebook and Romantic Relationships: Intimacy and Couple Satisfaction Associated with Online Social Network Use. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2013, 16, 8–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Kiecolt-Glaser, J.K.; Newton, T.L. Marriage and Health: His and Hers. Psychol. Bull. 2001, 127, 472–503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. House, J.S.; Landis, K.R.; Umberson, D. Social Relationships and Health. Science 1988, 241, 540–545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Proulx, C.M.; Helms, H.M.; Buehler, C. Marital Quality and Personal Well-Being: A Meta-Analysis. J. Marriage Fam. 2007, 69, 576–593. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Givertz, M.; Segrin, C.; Woszidlo, A. Direct and Indirect Effects of Commitment on Interdependence and Satisfaction in Married Couples. J. Fam. Psychol. 2016, 30, 214–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Hou, Y.; Jiang, F.; Wang, X. Marital Commitment, Communication and Marital Satisfaction: An Analysis Based on Actor-Partner Interdependence Model: Commitment and Marital Satisfaction. Int. J. Psychol. 2019, 54, 369–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Vannier, S.A.; O’Sullivan, L.F. Passion, Connection, and Destiny: How Romantic Expectations Help Predict Satisfaction and Commitment in Young Adults’ Dating Relationships. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 2017, 34, 235–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Johnson, M.P. Personal, Moral, and Structural Commitment to Relationships. In Handbook of Interpersonal Commitment and Relationship Stability; Adams, J.M., Jones, W.H., Eds.; Springer US: Boston, MA, USA, 1999; pp. 73–87. [Google Scholar]
  12. Weigel, D.J.; Davis, B.A.; Woodard, K.C. A Two-Sided Coin: Mapping Perceptions of the Pros and Cons of Relationship Commitment. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 2015, 32, 344–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Arriaga, X.B.; Agnew, C.R. Being Committed: Affective, Cognitive, and Conative Components of Relationship Commitment. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2001, 27, 1190–1203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Wieselquist, J.; Rusbult, C.E.; Foster, C.A.; Agnew, C.R. Commitment, pro-Relationship Behavior, and Trust in Close Relationships. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1999, 77, 942–966. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Sternberg, R.J. Triangulating Love. In The Psychology of Love; Sternberg, R.J., Barnes, M.L., Eds.; Yale University Press: New Haven, CT, USA, 1988; pp. 119–138. [Google Scholar]
  16. Schoebi, D.; Karney, B.R.; Bradbury, T.N. Stability and Change in the First 10 Years of Marriage: Does Commitment Confer Benefits beyond the Effects of Satisfaction? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2012, 102, 729–742. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Webster, G.D.; Laurenceau, J.-P.; Smith, C.V.; Mahaffey, A.L.; Bryan, A.D.; Brunell, A.B. An Investment Model of Sociosexuality, Relationship Satisfaction, and Commitment: Evidence from Dating, Engaged, and Newlywed Couples. J. Res. Pers. 2015, 55, 112–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Yampolsky, M.A.; West, A.L.; Zhou, B.; Muise, A.; Lalonde, R.N. Divided Together: How Marginalization of Intercultural Relationships Is Associated with Identity Integration and Relationship Quality. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 2021, 12, 887–897. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Rusbult, C.E. A Longitudinal Test of the Investment Model: The Development (and Deterioration) of Satisfaction and Commitment in Heterosexual Involvements. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1983, 45, 101–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Rusbult, C.E.; Coolsen, M.K.; Kirchner, J.L.; Clarke, J.A. Commitment. In The Cambridge Handbook of Personal Relationships; Vangelisti, A.L., Perlman, D., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2006; pp. 615–636. [Google Scholar]
  21. Rosen-Grandon, J.R.; Myers, J.E.; Hattie, J.A. The Relationship between Marital Characteristics, Marital Interaction Processes, and Marital Satisfaction. J. Couns. Dev. 2004, 82, 58–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Karney, B.R.; Bradbury, T.N. Assessing Longitudinal Change in Marriage: An Introduction to the Analysis of Growth Curves. J. Marriage Fam. 1995, 57, 1091. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Schaefer, M.T.; Olson, D.H. Assessing Intimacy: The Pair Inventory. J. Marital Fam. Ther. 1981, 7, 47–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Reis, H.T.; Shaver, P. Intimacy as an Interpersonal Process. In Handbook of Personal Relationships: Theory, Relationships and intervention; Duck, S.W., Ed.; Wiley: Chichester, UK, 1988; pp. 367–389. [Google Scholar]
  25. Aron, A.; Westbay, L. Dimensions of the Prototype of Love. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1996, 70, 535–551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Patrick, S.; Sells, J.N.; Giordano, F.G.; Tollerud, T.R. Intimacy, Differentiation, and Personality Variables as Predictors of Marital Satisfaction. Fam. J. 2007, 15, 359–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Hook, M.K.; Gerstein, L.H.; Detterich, L.; Gridley, B. How Close Are We? Measuring Intimacy and Examining Gender Differences. J. Couns. Dev. 2003, 81, 462–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Johnson, H.D.; Kent, A.; Yale, E. Examination of Identity and Romantic Relationship Intimacy Associations with Well-Being in Emerging Adulthood. Identity 2012, 12, 296–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Poucher, J.; Prager, K.J.; Shirvani, F.; Parsons, J.; Patel, J. Intimacy, Attachment to the Partner, and Daily Well-Being in Romantic Relationships. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 2022, 39, 1574–1601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Dandurand, C.; Lafontaine, M.-F. Intimacy and Couple Satisfaction: The Moderating Role of Romantic Attachment. Int. J. Psychol. Stud. 2013, 5, 74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Hassebrauck, M.; Fehr, B. Dimensions of Relationship Quality. Pers. Relatsh. 2002, 9, 253–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Lee, J.; Gillath, O.; Miller, A. Effects of Self- and Partner’s Online Disclosure on Relationship Intimacy and Satisfaction. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0212186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Niven, K. The Four Key Characteristics of Interpersonal Emotion Regulation. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2017, 17, 89–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Niven, K.; Totterdell, P.; Holman, D. A Classification of Controlled Interpersonal Affect Regulation Strategies. Emotion 2009, 9, 498–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Turliuc, M.N.; Jitaru, M. Interpersonal Emotion Regulation—A Concept in Search of Clarification. Rev. Psih. 2019, 65, 281–291. [Google Scholar]
  36. Niven, K.; Macdonald, I.; Holman, D. You Spin Me Right Round: Cross-Relationship Variability in Interpersonal Emotion Regulation. Front. Psychol. 2012, 3, 394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  37. Debrot, A.; Schoebi, D.; Perrez, M.; Horn, A.B. Touch as an Interpersonal Emotion Regulation Process in Couples’ Daily Lives: The Mediating Role of Psychological Intimacy: The Mediating Role of Psychological Intimacy. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2013, 39, 1373–1385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Levy-Gigi, E.; Shamay-Tsoory, S.G. Help Me If You Can: Evaluating the Effectiveness of Interpersonal Compared to Intrapersonal Emotion Regulation in Reducing Distress. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 2017, 55, 33–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Gana, K.; Jakubowska, S. Relationship between Infertility-Related Stress and Emotional Distress and Marital Satisfaction. J. Health Psychol. 2016, 21, 1043–1054. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Florean, I.-S.; Păsărelu, C.-R. Interpersonal Emotion Regulation and Cognitive Empathy as Mediators between Intrapersonal Emotion Regulation Difficulties and Couple Satisfaction. J. Evid.-Based Psychother. 2019, 19, 119–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Klein, S.R.; Renshaw, K.D.; Curby, T.W. Emotion Regulation and Perceptions of Hostile and Constructive Criticism in Romantic Relationships. Behav. Ther. 2016, 47, 143–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Brauer, K.; Proyer, R.T. To Love and Laugh: Testing Actor-, Partner-, and Similarity Effects of Dispositions towards Ridicule and Being Laughed at on Relationship Satisfaction. J. Res. Pers. 2018, 76, 165–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Prager, K.J.; Poucher, J.; Shirvani, F.K.; Parsons, J.A.; Allam, Z. Withdrawal, Attachment Security, and Recovery from Conflict in Couple Relationships. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 2019, 36, 573–598. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Owen, J.; Quirk, K.; Manthos, M. I Get No Respect: The Relationship between Betrayal Trauma and Romantic Relationship Functioning. J. Trauma Dissociation 2012, 13, 175–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Peterson, J.L.; DeHart, T. In Defense of Self-Love: An Observational Study on Narcissists’ Negative Behavior during Romantic Relationship Conflict. Self Identity 2014, 13, 477–490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Stanley, S.M.; Markman, H.J.; Whitton, S.W. Communication, Conflict, and Commitment: Insights on the Foundations of Relationship Success from a National Survey. Fam. Process 2002, 41, 659–675. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Wright, C.N.; Roloff, M.E. Relational Commitment and the Silent Treatment. Commun. Res. Rep. 2009, 26, 12–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Juarez, L.; Pritchard, M. Body Dissatisfaction: Commitment, Support, and Trust in Romantic Relationships. J. Hum. Behav. Soc. Environ. 2012, 22, 188–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Debrot, A.; Cook, W.L.; Perrez, M.; Horn, A.B. Deeds Matter: Daily Enacted Responsiveness and Intimacy in Couples’ Daily Lives. J. Fam. Psychol. 2012, 26, 617–627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Horn, A.B.; Samson, A.C.; Debrot, A.; Perrez, M. Positive Humor in Couples as Interpersonal Emotion Regulation: A Dyadic Study in Everyday Life on the Mediating Role of Psychological Intimacy. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 2019, 36, 2376–2396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Coan, J.A.; Sbarra, D.A. Social Baseline Theory: The Social Regulation of Risk and Effort. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2015, 1, 87–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  52. Francis, L.; Monahan, K.; Berger, C. A Laughing Matter? The Uses of Humor in Medical Interactions. Motiv. Emot. 1999, 23, 155–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Palmateer, T.; Tamminen, K. A Case Study of Interpersonal Emotion Regulation within a Varsity Volleyball Team. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 2018, 30, 321–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Thoits, P.A. Managing the Emotions of Others. Symb. Interact. 1996, 19, 85–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Thompson, R.A.; Meyer, S. The Socialization of Emotion Regulation in the Family. In Handbook of Emotion Regulation; Gross, J., Ed.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2007; pp. 249–268. [Google Scholar]
  56. Kenny, D.A.; Cook, W. Partner Effects in Relationship Research: Conceptual Issues, Analytic Difficulties, and Illustrations. Pers. Relatsh. 1999, 6, 433–448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Funk, J.L.; Rogge, R.D. Testing the Ruler with Item Response Theory: Increasing Precision of Measurement for Relationship Satisfaction with the Couples Satisfaction Index. J. Fam. Psychol. 2007, 21, 572–583. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Rusbult, C.E.; Martz, J.M.; Agnew, C.R. The Investment Model Scale: Measuring Commitment Level, Satisfaction Level, Quality of Alternatives, and Investment Size. Pers. Relatsh. 1998, 5, 357–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Smith LeBeau, L.; Buckingham, J.T. Relationship Social Comparison Tendencies, Insecurity, and Perceived Relationship Quality. J. Soc. Pers. Relatsh. 2008, 25, 71–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Niven, K.; Totterdell, P.; Stride, C.B.; Holman, D. Emotion Regulation of Others and Self (EROS): The Development and Validation of a New Individual Difference Measure. Curr. Psychol. 2011, 30, 53–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Garcia, R.L.; Kenny, D.A.; Ledermann, T. Moderation in the Actor-Partner Interdependence Model: Moderation in the APIM. Pers. Relatsh. 2015, 22, 8–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Dawson, J.F. Moderation in Management Research: What, Why, When, and How. J. Bus. Psychol. 2014, 29, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Barnett, M.D.; Mollenkopf, K.K.; Haygood, A.N. Gender Moderates Relationships between Pathological Narcissism and Intrinsic-Extrinsic Emotion Regulation Strategies. Curr. Psychol. 2022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Niven, K.; Totterdell, P.; Holman, D.; Headley, T. Does Regulating Others’ Feelings Influence People’s Own Affective Well-Being? J. Soc. Psychol. 2012, 152, 246–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Jitaru, M. The Associations between Interpersonal Emotion Regulation and Couple Satisfaction: A Dyadic Perspective. In Paths of Communication in Postmodernity; Boldea, I., Sigmirean, C., Buda, D., Eds.; Arhipelag XXI Press: Tîrgu Mureş, România, 2020; pp. 226–232. [Google Scholar]
  66. Chavira Trujillo, G.; Gallego Tomás, M.; López-Pérez, B. The Link between Cognitive and Affective Empathy and Interpersonal Emotion Regulation Direction and Strategies. Scand. J. Psychol. 2022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Vaillancourt-Morel, M.-P.; Rellini, A.H.; Godbout, N.; Sabourin, S.; Bergeron, S. Intimacy Mediates the Relation between Maltreatment in Childhood and Sexual and Relationship Satisfaction in Adulthood: A Dyadic Longitudinal Analysis. Arch. Sex. Behav. 2019, 48, 803–814. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Liu, D.Y.; Strube, M.J.; Thompson, R.J. Interpersonal Emotion Regulation: An Experience Sampling Study. Affec. Sci. 2021, 2, 273–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Cutrona, C.E.; Shaffer, P.A.; Wesner, K.A.; Gardner, K.A. Optimally Matching Support and Perceived Spousal Sensitivity. J. Fam. Psychol. 2007, 21, 754–758. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Abele, A.E. The Dynamics of Masculine-Agentic and Feminine-Communal Traits: Findings from a Prospective Study. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 85, 768–776. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Lee, H.-Y.; Oh, J.; Kawachi, I.; Heo, J.; Kim, S.; Lee, J.-K.; Kang, D. Positive and Negative Social Support and Depressive Symptoms According to Economic Status among Adults in Korea: Cross-Sectional Results from the Health Examinees-Gem Study. BMJ Open 2019, 9, e023036. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Palant, A.; Himmel, W. Are There Also Negative Effects of Social Support? A Qualitative Study of Patients with Inflammatory Bowel Disease. BMJ Open 2019, 9, e022642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  73. Pierce, K.D.R.; Quiroz, C.S. Who Matters Most? Social Support, Social Strain, and Emotions. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 2019, 36, 3273–3292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Kirchner-Häusler, A.; Boiger, M.; Uchida, Y.; Higuchi, Y.; Uchida, A.; Mesquita, B. Relatively Happy: The Role of the Positive-to-Negative Affect Ratio in Japanese and Belgian Couples. J. Cross. Cult. Psychol. 2022, 53, 66–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Renshaw, K.D.; Blais, R.K.; Smith, T.W. Components of Negative Affectivity and Marital Satisfaction: The Importance of Actor and Partner Anger. J. Res. Pers. 2010, 44, 328–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  76. Cranny-Francis, A.; Waring, W.; Stavropoulos, P.; Kirkby, J. Gender Studies: Terms and Debates; Red Globe Press: London, UK, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  77. Campbell, S.B.; Renshaw, K.D.; Klein, S.R. Sex Differences in Associations of Hostile and Non-Hostile Criticism with Relationship Quality. J. Psychol. 2017, 151, 416–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Brauer, K.; Sendatzki, R.; Proyer, R.T. Testing the Associations between Dispositions toward Ridicule and Being Laughed at and Romantic Jealousy in Couples: An APIM Analysis. J. Pers. 2021, 89, 883–898. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Conceptual moderated actor–partner interdependence model. Commitment/Intimacy—independent variable; Couple satisfaction—dependent variable; Interpersonal affect-improving/ affect-worsening strategies—moderator. a = actor effect; p = partner effect; m = moderator effect; W = women; M = men.
Figure 1. Conceptual moderated actor–partner interdependence model. Commitment/Intimacy—independent variable; Couple satisfaction—dependent variable; Interpersonal affect-improving/ affect-worsening strategies—moderator. a = actor effect; p = partner effect; m = moderator effect; W = women; M = men.
Ijerph 19 10506 g001
Figure 2. Women’s and men’s couple satisfaction with women’s or men’s low and high commitment for women that use less vs. more interpersonal affect-improving strategies: (a) women’s satisfaction with their commitment and use of interpersonal affect-improving strategies; (b) men’s satisfaction with women’s commitment and use of interpersonal affect-improving strategies; (c) women’s satisfaction with men’s commitment and women’s use of interpersonal affect-improving strategies; (d) men’s satisfaction with their commitment and women’s use of interpersonal affect-improving strategies.
Figure 2. Women’s and men’s couple satisfaction with women’s or men’s low and high commitment for women that use less vs. more interpersonal affect-improving strategies: (a) women’s satisfaction with their commitment and use of interpersonal affect-improving strategies; (b) men’s satisfaction with women’s commitment and use of interpersonal affect-improving strategies; (c) women’s satisfaction with men’s commitment and women’s use of interpersonal affect-improving strategies; (d) men’s satisfaction with their commitment and women’s use of interpersonal affect-improving strategies.
Ijerph 19 10506 g002
Figure 3. Men’s couple satisfaction with women’s or men’s low and high commitment for men that use less vs. more interpersonal affect-improving strategies: (a) men’s satisfaction with women’s commitment and men’s use of interpersonal affect-improving strategies; (b) men’s satisfaction with their commitment and use of interpersonal affect-improving strategies.
Figure 3. Men’s couple satisfaction with women’s or men’s low and high commitment for men that use less vs. more interpersonal affect-improving strategies: (a) men’s satisfaction with women’s commitment and men’s use of interpersonal affect-improving strategies; (b) men’s satisfaction with their commitment and use of interpersonal affect-improving strategies.
Ijerph 19 10506 g003
Figure 4. Women and men’s couple satisfaction with men’s low and high commitment (a,b) intimacy (c) for women that use less vs. more interpersonal affect-worsening strategies.
Figure 4. Women and men’s couple satisfaction with men’s low and high commitment (a,b) intimacy (c) for women that use less vs. more interpersonal affect-worsening strategies.
Ijerph 19 10506 g004
Table 1. Descriptive statistics and paired-sample t-tests for this study’s variables.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics and paired-sample t-tests for this study’s variables.
MeanSDPaired Sample CorrelationMean DifferenceSD (Mean Difference)tdf
Commitment 0.509 **1.188.581.58130
Women50.797.07
Men49.69.65
Intimacy 0.559 **1.415.131.06130
Women138.9716.43
Men137.5715.77
Affect-improving strategies 0.425 **−0.183.86−0.52130
Women25.713.76
Men25.893.42
Affect-worsening strategies 0.498 **0.562.492.6 *130
Women5.512.6
Men4.952.35
Couple Satisfaction 0.693 **0.022.620.07130
Women20.843.22
Men20.823.45
Note. ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05.
Table 2. Zero-order correlations for the main variables of the study by gender.
Table 2. Zero-order correlations for the main variables of the study by gender.
Variables123456789
1. Commitment—W1
2. Commitment—M0.488 **1
3. Affect-improving—W0.248 *0.0671
4. Affect-improving—M0.226 *0.377 **0.395 **1
5. Affect-worsening—W–0.338 **–0.224 *–0.187 *−0.1161
6. Affect-worsening—M–0.277 **–0.315 **−0.118−0.1160.499 **1
7. Intimacy—W0.497 **0.311 **0.249 *0.268 *–0.503 **–0.386 **1
8. Intimacy—M0.358 **0.512 **0.120.370 **–0.331 **–0.369 **0.557 **1
9. Satisfaction—W0.618 **0.440 **0.359 **0.318 **–0.351 **–0.300 **0.565 **0.421 **1
10. Satisfaction—M0.505 **0.617 **0.198 *0.354 **–0.348 **–0.343 **0.502 **0.634 **0.679 **
Note. ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05; W = woman variable, M = man variable.
Table 3. Model predicting couple satisfaction from commitment, moderated by women’s and men’s interpersonal affect-improving and affect-worsening strategies.
Table 3. Model predicting couple satisfaction from commitment, moderated by women’s and men’s interpersonal affect-improving and affect-worsening strategies.
Model with Affect-Improving IER StrategiesModel with Affect-Worsening IER Strategies
EffectUnstand. Coeff.Stand. Coeff.SEpUnstand. Coeff.Stand. Coeff.SEp
Actor Effect of Commitment
Woman0.2340.5150.036<0.0010.2290.5030.035<0.001
Man0.2650.7290.038<0.0010.170.4750.027<0.001
Partner Effect of Commitment
Woman0.0430.1280.0350.2210.0660.1990.0250.009
Man0.070.1440.0380.0670.10.2050.0380.008
Woman IER Strategies
Woman0.1970.2330.0610.001−0.123−0.0990.0960.2
Man0.0280.0310.0660.669−0.211−0.1590.1030.04
Man IER Strategies
Woman0.0870.0940.0690.204−0.099−0.0720.1110.373
Man0.080.0810.0730.273−0.078−0.0530.1190.516
Woman Commitment by Woman IER Strategies
Woman0.0280.3250.0090.002−0.012−0.0680.0150.392
Man0.0190.2090.010.049−0.017−0.0870.0160.272
Man Commitment by Woman IER Strategies
Woman−0.018−0.2450.0070.0120.030.2140.0120.015
Man−0.023−0.2970.0070.0020.030.1990.0130.025
Woman Commitment by Man IER Strategies
Woman0.010.0840.0120.437−0.026−0.1650.0150.073
Man−0.027−0.220.0130.0400.0030.0160.975
Man Commitment by Man IER Strategies
Woman−0.015−0.1610.0110.1660.0020.0150.0130.889
Man0.0390.4010.011<0.001−0.002−0.0120.0140.908
Note. IER = Interpersonal Emotion Regulation. The significant effects are bold.
Table 4. Model predicting couple satisfaction from intimacy, moderated by both women’s and men’s interpersonal affect-improving and affect-worsening strategies.
Table 4. Model predicting couple satisfaction from intimacy, moderated by both women’s and men’s interpersonal affect-improving and affect-worsening strategies.
Model with Affect-Improving IER StrategiesModel with Affect-Worsening IER Strategies
EffectUnstand. Coeff.Stand. Coeff.SEpUnstand. Coeff.Stand. Coeff.SEp
Actor Effect of Intimacy
Woman0.0840.4310.016<0.0010.0910.4650.018<0.001
Man0.1030.4710.017<0.0010.1040.4740.018<0.001
Partner Effect of Intimacy
Woman0.0240.1180.0170.150.0230.1120.0180.204
Man0.0470.2230.0160.0040.040.190.0180.031
Woman IER Strategies
Woman0.180.210.0650.006−0.043−0.0340.1190.72
Man0.030.0320.0670.661−0.091−0.0680.1210.452
Man IER Strategies
Woman0.0420.0450.0760.577−0.084−0.0610.1370.54
Man0.0820.0810.0790.301−0.067−0.0460.1390.631
Woman Intimacy by Woman IER Strategies
Woman−0.009−0.1370.0050.0930.0020.0350.0060.698
Man−0.01−0.1470.0050.063−0.001−0.0090.0060.912
Man Intimacy by Woman IER Strategies
Woman0.0030.0550.0050.4980.0150.1810.0070.035
Man−0.007−0.1110.0050.1570.0020.0250.0070.756
Woman Intimacy by Man IER Strategies
Woman−0.008−0.1170.0060.182−0.013−0.1920.0090.143
Man0.0010.0080.0060.923−0.006−0.0790.0090.525
Man Intimacy by Man IER Strategies
Woman−0.008−0.1330.0060.1350.0050.0630.010.633
Man−0.004−0.0570.0060.5060.0060.0770.010.54
Note. IER = Interpersonal Emotion Regulation. The significant effects are bold.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Jitaru, M.; Turliuc, M.N. The Moderator Role of Interpersonal Emotion Regulation on the Associations between Commitment, Intimacy, and Couple Satisfaction. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 10506. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191710506

AMA Style

Jitaru M, Turliuc MN. The Moderator Role of Interpersonal Emotion Regulation on the Associations between Commitment, Intimacy, and Couple Satisfaction. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(17):10506. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191710506

Chicago/Turabian Style

Jitaru, Mihaela, and Maria Nicoleta Turliuc. 2022. "The Moderator Role of Interpersonal Emotion Regulation on the Associations between Commitment, Intimacy, and Couple Satisfaction" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 17: 10506. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191710506

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop