Developing Metacognition of 5- to 6-Year-Old Children: Evaluating the Effect of a Circling Curriculum Based on Anji Play
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. The Structure and Measurements of Metacognition
1.2. Metacognition Interventions in Early Childhood
1.3. Metacognition Intervention Approaches
1.3.1. Reflective Dialogues
1.3.2. Observation Records
1.3.3. Games
1.4. Anji Play Teaching Model
1.5. The Current Study
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Design of the CCMT
2.2.1. Playfulness
2.2.2. Systematic Applicability
2.2.3. High Flexibility
2.3. Instruments
2.3.1. Knowledge of Cognition
2.3.2. Regulation of Cognition
2.4. Procedure
2.5. Statistical Analyses
3. Results
3.1. Testing Hypothesis
3.2. Testing the Training Effects
4. Discussion
4.1. Effectiveness of the CCMT on Young Children’s Metacognition
4.2. Educational Implications
4.3. Limitations, Future Directions, and Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Phase | Stage | Teaching Objectives | Teaching Content |
---|---|---|---|
Phase One: Enriching the knowledge of cognition | Pre-play |
|
|
In-play |
|
| |
Post-play |
|
| |
Phase Two: Enhancing off-line metacognitive skills | Pre-play |
|
|
In-play |
|
| |
Post-play |
|
| |
Phase Three: Enhancing the regulation of cognition | Pre-play |
|
|
In-play |
|
| |
Post-play |
|
|
Appendix B
Appendix B.1. Talking Activities
Appendix B.2. Teacher Records
Appendix B.3. Painting Activities
Appendix B.4. Sharing Discussion Activities
References
- Magno, C. The role of metacognitive skills in developing critical thinking. Metacognition Learn. 2010, 5, 137–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lai, E.R. Metacognition: A literature review. Always Learn. Pearson Res. Rep. 2011, 24, 1–40. [Google Scholar]
- Cheng, X. Letting Go of Play to Discover Children; East China Normal University Press: Shanghai, China, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- National Center on Early Childhood Quality Assurance, Accommodation for CHILDREN & FAMILIES. Early Learning Standards and Guidelines. Available online: https://childcareta.acf.hhs.gov/resource/state-early-learning-standards-and-guidelines (accessed on 10 September 2022).
- Higgins, S.; Katsipataki, M.; Villanueva Aguilera, A.B.; Dobson, E.; Gascoine, L.; Rajab, T.; Reardon, J.; Stafford, J.; Uwimpuhwe, G. The Teaching and Learning Toolkit: Communicating research evidence to inform decision-making for policy and practice in education. Rev. Educ. 2022, 10, e3327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perry, J.; Lundie, D.; Golder, G. Metacognition in schools: What does the literature suggest about the effectiveness of teaching metacognition in schools? Educ. Rev. 2019, 71, 483–500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Veenman, M.V.; Wilhelm, P.; Beishuizen, J.J. The relation between intellectual and metacognitive skills from a developmental perspective. Learn. Instr. 2004, 14, 89–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Higgins, S.; Katsipataki, M.; Villanueva-Aguilera, A.; Coleman, R.; Henderson, P.; Major, L.; Coe, R.; Mason, D. The Sutton Trust-Education Endowment Foundation Teaching and Learning Toolkit. 2016. Available online: https://educationendowmentfoundation.org.uk/resources/teaching-learning-toolkit (accessed on 3 September 2022).
- Desoete, A. Metacognitive prediction and evaluation skills and mathematical learning in third-grade students. Educ. Res. Eval. 2009, 15, 435–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haller, E.P.; Child, D.A.; Walberg, H.J. Can comprehension be taught? A quantitative synthesis of “metacognitive” studies. Educ. Res. 1988, 17, 5–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Veenman, M.V.; Spaans, M.A. Relation between intellectual and metacognitive skills: Age and task differences. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2005, 15, 159–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schraw, G.; Moshman, D. Metacognitive theories. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 1995, 7, 351–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Flavell, J.H. Metacognitive aspects of problem solving. In The Nature of Intelligence; Lawrence Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1976. [Google Scholar]
- Desoete, A.; Roeyers, H. Off-line metacognition—A domain-specific retardation in young children with learning disabilities? Learn. Disabil. Q. 2002, 25, 123–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Prevost, R.A.; Bronson, M.B.; Casey, M.B. Planning processes in preschool children. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 1995, 16, 505–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pintrich, P.R.; De Groot, E.V. Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom academic performance. J. Educ. Psychol. 1990, 82, 33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schneider, W. Performance prediction in young children: Effects of skill, metacognition and wishful thinking. Dev. Sci. 1998, 1, 291–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Winne, P.H.; Perry, N.E. Measuring self-regulated learning. In Handbook of Self-Regulation; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2000; pp. 531–566. [Google Scholar]
- Sperling, R.A.; Howard, B.C.; Miller, L.A.; Murphy, C. Measures of children’s knowledge and regulation of cognition. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2002, 27, 51–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kramarski, B.; Mevarech, Z.R. Enhancing mathematical reasoning in the classroom: The effects of cooperative learning and metacognitive training. Am. Educ. Res. J. 2003, 40, 281–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Piaget, J. The Grasp of Consciousness (Psychology Revivals): Action and Concept in the Young Child; Psychology Press: Hove, UK, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Dermitzaki, I.; Leondari, A.; Goudas, M. Relations between young students’ strategic behaviours, domain-specific self-concept, and performance in a problem-solving situation. Learn. Instr. 2009, 19, 144–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, Y.; Wang, Y.; Xiao, X. A study on the developmental characteristics of metacognitive monitoring in children aged 3–5 years. Psychol. Behav. Stud. 2006, 5–8. [Google Scholar]
- Zhang, Y. A Study on Metacognitive Development and Intervention in Mathematics Activities of Children Aged 5–6 Years. Ph.D. Thesis, East China Normal University, Shanghai, China, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Whitebread, D.; Bingham, S.; Grau, V.; Pasternak, D.P.; Sangster, C. Development of metacognition and self-regulated learning in young children: Role of collaborative and peer-assisted learning. J. Cogn. Educ. Psychol. 2007, 6, 433–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lyons, K.E.; Ghetti, S. The development of uncertainty monitoring in early childhood. Child Dev. 2011, 82, 1778–1787. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Feurer, E.; Sassu, R.; Cimeli, P.; Roebers, C. Development of meta-representations: Procedural metacognition and the relationship to theory of mind. J. Educ. Dev. Psychol. 2015, 5, 6–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chernokova, T.E. Features of the Metacognition Structure for Pre-school Age Children. Procedia-Soc. Behav. Sci. 2014, 146, 203–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Muijs, D.; Bokhove, C. Metacognition and Self-Regulation: Evidence Review; Education Endowment Foundation: London, UK, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- Larkin, S. Collaborative group work and individual development of metacognition in the early years. Res. Sci. Educ. 2006, 36, 7–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pennequin, V.; Sorel, O.; Nanty, I.; Fontaine, R. Metacognition and low achievement in mathematics: The effect of training in the use of metacognitive skills to solve mathematical word problems. Think. Reason. 2010, 16, 198–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roll, I.; Aleven, V.; McLaren, B.M.; Koedinger, K.R. Designing for metacognition—Applying cognitive tutor principles to the tutoring of help seeking. Metacognition Learn. 2007, 2, 125–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hattie, J. Visible Learning: A Synthesis of Over 800 Meta-Analyses Relating to Achievement; Routledge: London, UK, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Robson, S. Self-regulation and metacognition in young children: Does it matter if adults are present or not? Br. Educ. Res. J. 2016, 42, 185–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hattie, J. Calibration and confidence: Where to next? Learn. Instr. 2013, 24, 62–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hennessey, M.G. Probing the Dimensions of Metacognition: Implications for Conceptual Change Teaching-Learning; ERIC: Washington, DC, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Aras, S.; Erden, F.T. Documentation panels: Supporting young children’s self-regulatory and metacognitive abilities. Int. J. Early Years Educ. 2020, 28, 63–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rinaldi, C. Projected curriculum constructed through documentation—Progettazione. In The hundred languages of children: The Reggio Emilia approach—Advanced reflections; Ablex Publishing Corporation: Greenwich, CT, USA, 1998; pp. 113–125. [Google Scholar]
- Pettersson, K.E. Children’s participation in preschool documentation practices. Childhood 2015, 22, 231–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bodrova, E.; Leong, D.J. Tools of the mind: The Vygotskian-based early childhood program. J. Cogn. Educ. Psychol. 2019, 17, 223–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krafft, K.C.; Berk, L.E. Private speech in two preschools: Significance of open-ended activities and make-believe play for verbal self-regulation. Early Child. Res. Q. 1998, 13, 637–658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tominey, S.L.; McClelland, M.M. Improving behavioral regulation in preschool. HS Dialog: Res. Pract. J. Early Child. Field 2013, 16, 149–154. [Google Scholar]
- Liu, C. The Practice and Inspiration of Teaching Activities Generated by Anji Kindergarten Based on Children’s Spontaneous Play. Master’s Thesis, Fujian Normal University, Fuzhou, China, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Cheng, X. Origins and development:A profound revolution in children’s play. Presch. Educ. 2019, 10–15. [Google Scholar]
- Wang, J.; Lu, W. The construction of professional competence of early childhood teachers based on the analysis of Angie’s game. J. Lanzhou Coll. Arts Sci. (Soc. Sci. Ed.) 2020, 36, 119–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Forum, W.E. Schools of the future: Defining new models of education for the fourth industrial revolution. In Proceedings of the World Economic Forum Annual Meeting, Davos-Klosters, Switzerland, 21–24 January 2020; pp. 1–33. [Google Scholar]
- Feng, X. “Anji’s game” and deep learning—And why we should learn from Anji. Early Child. Educ. 2021, 8–12. [Google Scholar]
- Hanline, M.F.; Milton, S.; Phelps, P.C. The relationship between preschool block play and reading and maths abilities in early elementary school: A longitudinal study of children with and without disabilities. Early Child Dev. Care 2010, 180, 1005–1017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tian, M.; Luo, T.; Cheung, H. The development and measurement of block construction in early childhood: A review. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 2020, 38, 767–782. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sa, T.; Yang, N. The effect of problem-based learning on the metacognitive development of children in large classes. Presch. Educ. Res. 2012, 42–45+52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Garrett, A.J.; Mazzocco, M.M.; Baker, L. Development of the metacognitive skills of prediction and evaluation in children with or without math disability. Learn. Disabil. Res. Pract. 2006, 21, 77–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Monkeviciene, O.; Vildžiuniene, J.; Valinciene, G. The Impact of Teacher-Initiated Activities on Identifying and Verbalizing Ways of Metacognitive Monitoring and Control in Six-Year-Old Children. Res. Soc. Sci. Technol. 2020, 5, 72–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Robson, S. Self-regulation and metacognition in young children’s self-initiated play and reflective dialogue. Int. J. Early Years Educ. 2010, 18, 227–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Larkin, S. Metacognition in Young Children; Routledge: London, UK, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Gandini, L.; Edwards, C.P. Bambini: The Italian Approach to Infant/Toddler Care; Teachers College Press: New York, NY, USA, 2001; Volume 77. [Google Scholar]
- Nielsen, A.M. Forskeres arbejde med oplevelser af børns tegninger som forskningsmetode. Psyke Logos 2012, 33, 18. [Google Scholar]
- Frederiksen, M. Mixed methods-forskning: Fra praksis til teori. In Mixed Methods-Forskning: Principper Og Praksis; Hans Reitzels Forlag: Copenhagen, Denmark, 2014; pp. 9–34. [Google Scholar]
- Dahlberg, G.; Moss, P.; Pence, A.R. Beyond Quality in Early Childhood Education and Care: Postmodern Perspectives; Psychology Press: Hove, UK, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Wo, J.; Lin, C. Research on the development of teenagers’ Self-monitoring Ability. Psychol. Sci. 2000, 10–15+124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fisher, R. Thinking about thinking: Developing metacognition in children. Early Child Dev. Care 1998, 141, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schraw, G. Promoting general metacognitive awareness. Instr. Sci. 1998, 26, 113–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clark, I. Formative assessment: Assessment is for self-regulated learning. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2012, 24, 205–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rinaldi, C. In Dialogue with Reggio Emilia: Listening, Researching and Learning; Routledge: London, UK, 2021. [Google Scholar]
- Miller, J.M.; Miller, H.V. Rethinking program fidelity for criminal justice. Criminol. Public Policy 2015, 14, 339–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Experimental Group (n = 25) | Control Group (n = 22) | t | |
---|---|---|---|
Gender | |||
Boy | 15 | 12 | |
Girl | 10 | 10 | |
Age range (month) | 60–73 | 60–75 | |
Age (month) | 65.92 (3.58) | 66.77 (3.87) | 0.437 |
Stage | Function | Duration |
---|---|---|
Pre-play | Conduct reflective dialogue between teachers and children, help children to enrich their knowledge of cognition, guide them to plan and predict, and provide a metacognitive foundation for the coming play activities | 15 min |
In-play | Observe and record the children’s regulation of cognition behavior during play | 40 min |
Post-play | Use various teaching strategies to help young children to recall and characterize cognitive processes to promote the improvement of evaluation and reflection skills | 30 min |
Experimental | Control | u | p | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | Max | M | SD | Max | |||
Knowledge of cognition | 11.64 | 3.88 | 18 | 12.05 | 2.06 | 16 | 266.5 | 0.86 |
Knowledge of persons | 4.48 | 1.66 | 7 | 4.36 | 1.29 | 6 | 253.5 | 0.63 |
Knowledge of tasks | 4.16 | 1.57 | 6 | 4.36 | 1.46 | 6 | 254.5 | 0.65 |
Knowledge of strategies | 3.00 | 1.44 | 6 | 3.32 | 1.49 | 6 | 240.5 | 0.45 |
On-line metacognition | 33.32 | 10.60 | 53 | 33.00 | 11.30 | 52 | 274.5 | 0.99 |
Planning | 9.36 | 6.40 | 26 | 8.68 | 7.67 | 29 | 234.5 | 0.39 |
Monitoring | 18.12 | 6.59 | 36 | 17.59 | 8.80 | 35 | 272.0 | 0.95 |
Debugging | 5.84 | 2.59 | 10 | 6.73 | 2.35 | 12 | 233.5 | 0.37 |
Off-line metacognition | 2.12 | 1.42 | 5 | 2.00 | 0.87 | 4 | 269.5 | 0.90 |
Prediction | 1.12 | 0.73 | 2 | 1.05 | 0.65 | 2 | 258.0 | 0.69 |
Evaluation | 1.00 | 0.87 | 3 | 0.95 | 0.72 | 2 | 272.0 | 0.95 |
Experimental | Control | u | p | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | Max | M | SD | Max | |||
Knowledge of cognition | 15.56 | 2.26 | 20 | 13.73 | 1.83 | 16 | 121.0 | 0.00 ** |
Knowledge of persons | 5.52 | 1.30 | 8 | 4.82 | 0.96 | 6 | 163.5 | 0.01 * |
Knowledge of tasks | 5.60 | 0.71 | 7 | 4.77 | 1.02 | 6 | 147.5 | 0.00 ** |
Knowledge of strategies | 4.44 | 0.82 | 6 | 4.14 | 1.21 | 6 | 240.0 | 0.43 |
On-line metacognition | 42.68 | 8.99 | 61 | 35.36 | 10.96 | 64 | 156.5 | 0.01 * |
Planning | 13.68 | 5.78 | 25 | 9.41 | 7.38 | 26 | 169.0 | 0.02 * |
Monitoring | 23.3 | 6..27 | 39 | 19.00 | 7.86 | 36 | 173.0 | 0.03 * |
Debugging | 5.68 | 1.49 | 9 | 6.96 | 2.79 | 12 | 206.0 | 0.14 |
Off-line metacognition | 3.40 | 1.00 | 6 | 2.64 | 0.85 | 4 | 160.0 | 0.01 * |
Prediction | 1.76 | 0.66 | 3 | 1.46 | 0.60 | 2 | 215.5 | 0.16 |
Evaluation | 1.64 | 0.70 | 3 | 1.18 | 0.73 | 2 | 189.0 | 0.046 * |
Experimental | Control | u | p | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | Max | M | SD | Max | |||
Knowledge of cognition | 3.92 | 2.33 | 9 | 1.68 | 1.46 | 5 | 112.5 | 0.00 ** |
Knowledge of persons | 1.04 | 0.98 | 3 | 0.45 | 0.91 | 3 | 174.5 | 0.02 * |
Knowledge of tasks | 1.44 | 1.39 | 4 | 0.41 | 1.14 | 3 | 156.5 | 0.01 * |
Knowledge of strategies | 1.44 | 1.29 | 5 | 0.82 | 1.10 | 3 | 198.5 | 0.09 |
On-line metacognition | 9.36 | 4.79 | 21 | 2.36 | 5.43 | 12 | 92.0 | 0.00 ** |
Planning | 4.32 | 3.54 | 11 | 0.73 | 3.03 | 8 | 120.0 | 0.00 ** |
Monitoring | 5.20 | 3.73 | 12 | 1.41 | 3.76 | 10 | 123.5 | 0.00 * |
Debugging | 0.16 | 2.90 | 4 | 0.23 | 2.20 | 3 | 255.0 | 0.67 |
Off-line metacognition | 1.28 | 1.10 | 3 | 0.64 | 0.66 | 2 | 179.5 | 0.03 * |
Prediction | 0.64 | 0.57 | 2 | 0.41 | 0.50 | 1 | 218.0 | 0.17 |
Evaluation | 0.64 | 0.81 | 2 | 0.23 | 0.53 | 1 | 187.0 | 0.04 * |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Chen, C.; Wu, J.; Wu, Y.; Shangguan, X.; Li, H. Developing Metacognition of 5- to 6-Year-Old Children: Evaluating the Effect of a Circling Curriculum Based on Anji Play. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 11803. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191811803
Chen C, Wu J, Wu Y, Shangguan X, Li H. Developing Metacognition of 5- to 6-Year-Old Children: Evaluating the Effect of a Circling Curriculum Based on Anji Play. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(18):11803. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191811803
Chicago/Turabian StyleChen, Chen, Jianfen Wu, Yunpeng Wu, Xiaoyun Shangguan, and Hui Li. 2022. "Developing Metacognition of 5- to 6-Year-Old Children: Evaluating the Effect of a Circling Curriculum Based on Anji Play" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 18: 11803. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191811803
APA StyleChen, C., Wu, J., Wu, Y., Shangguan, X., & Li, H. (2022). Developing Metacognition of 5- to 6-Year-Old Children: Evaluating the Effect of a Circling Curriculum Based on Anji Play. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19(18), 11803. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191811803