Next Article in Journal
A Survey of Telemedicine Use by Doctors in District Hospitals in KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa
Next Article in Special Issue
Compassionate Work Environments and Their Role in Teachers’ Life Satisfaction: The Contribution of Perceived Collective School Performance and Burnout
Previous Article in Journal
Burnout and Quality of Work Life among Municipal Workers: Do Motivating and Economic Factors Play a Mediating Role?
Previous Article in Special Issue
Psychometric Properties and Factor Structure of the Slovak Version of the Copenhagen Burnout Inventory
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Holistic Assessment of Factors Associated with Exhaustion, the Main Symptom of Burnout: A Meta-Analysis of Longitudinal Studies

1
Center of Primary Care and Public Health (Unisanté), University of Lausanne, 1066 Epalinges-Lausanne, Switzerland
2
Department of Psychology, Norwegian University of Science and Technology (NTNU), 7491 Trondheim, Norway
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(20), 13037; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192013037
Submission received: 28 June 2022 / Revised: 4 October 2022 / Accepted: 7 October 2022 / Published: 11 October 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Worker Burnout: Evidence and Potential Solutions)

Abstract

:
Background: This meta-analysis summarized longitudinal findings pertaining to exhaustion’s predictors. In so doing, our aim was ultimately to identify target factors for the prevention of burnout. Methods: We searched for studies that (a) examined predictors of exhaustion longitudinally and (b) reported correlation coefficients as an effect estimate. We conducted our literature search in three databases: MEDLINE, PsycINFO, and Embase. We focused on studies published between January 1990 and November 2020. Predictors were grouped into families, subfamilies, and subgroups. A meta-analysis of z-transformed correlation coefficients (rho) was performed. The results were scrutinized in relation to studies’ follow-up length. Results: We included 65 studies assessing 242 predictors of different types captured across different occupations. Our findings highlighted mostly weak associations (rho < 0.30). For six predictors—Job control, Job resources, Interactions at work, Communication and leadership, Job attitudes, and Work-family interface—longer length of follow-up involved weaker associations with exhaustion. The quality of the evidence available was generally low. Conclusions: The evidence available does not point to clear target factors for preventing burnout. The decrease in associations as the follow-up length increases may suggest a relatively short latency period, followed by recovery. Higher-quality cohorts should be conducted to better understand the etiology and course of burnout.

1. Introduction

Occupational burnout is not recognized as a medical condition in either the 11th edition of the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11; [1]) or the 5th edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders [2]. Though burnout remains nosologically and diagnostically uncharacterized, a recent harmonized definition of the entity identifies it with a state of “physical and psychological exhaustion due to prolonged exposure to work-related problems” ([3], p. 95). Exhaustion has long been considered the core symptom of burnout (e.g., [4]) and constitutes the only consensual characteristic of burnout [5,6].
Exhaustion is the best-measured aspect of burnout by the currently available Patient-Reported Outcome Measures (PROMs; see [7]). Some PROMs, such as the Copenhagen Burnout Inventory (CBI; [6]) and Shirom Melamed Burnout Measure (SMBM; [5]), equate burnout with exhaustion. Other PROMs, such as the Maslach Burnout Inventory (MBI; [8]) and Oldenburg Burnout Inventory (OLBI; [9]), consider burnout to involve additional symptoms (e.g., disengagement). These additional symptoms, however, are generally considered secondary. Conceptually, it is believed that exhaustion is the first symptom of burnout to develop in a worker in response to work stressors [10]. Furthermore, exhaustion has been conceived of as a symptom that is both psychological and physical. From a prevention perspective, it is more effective to focus on the early symptoms rather than the more advanced ones [11]. Thus, by preventing exhaustion, we may prevent the worker from developing other symptoms such as depersonalization or disengagement. Additionally, focusing on exhaustion can help increase inclusiveness when performing a meta-analysis by harmonizing the definition of the outcome (i.e., exhaustion).
A large body of literature addresses the determinants of burnout. However, no systematic review with meta-analysis has summarized the evidence derived from these studies in a comprehensive manner. Heterogeneity is a crucial issue in both qualitative and quantitative systematic reviews. This might explain why the meta-analyses performed until now focused on only certain types of determinants or occupations [12,13,14,15,16,17]. A recent meta-analysis of 48 longitudinal studies examining the relationships between job stressors and burnout concluded that unobserved heterogeneity was an important limitation [18]. The meta-analysis in question [18] focused only on job stressors. Its authors concluded that job stressors such as workload have only small effects on burnout, suggesting the existence of many other determinants of burnout. Although it is useful to thoroughly examine certain types of determinants or occupations, we believe it is indispensable to consider the determinants of burnout globally and to pay attention to their respective importance. The work-related predictors are mainly studied in relation to burnout with many theoretical models [19]. On the other hand, non-work-related factors are less studied. Although based on the person-job misfit theory [4], individual dispositions (e.g., personality traits) are expected to play a role in burnout’s etiology (e.g., by contributing to shaping job experiences, including job stress experiences). Moreover, only two systematic reviews focused on the exhaustion symptom of burnout [20,21] and these two studies did not perform a meta-analysis.
Another gap in the literature on burnout’s etiology concerns latency. In occupational epidemiology, latency is defined as the time between the first exposure and the development of the symptoms of interest [22]. However, there are different definitions of latency in the epidemiological literature [23]. For instance, Rothman [24] defines latency as “the period between disease initiation and disease detection”. This review will target studies that assessed the associations between exhaustion and its putative predictors longitudinally. These studies will thus not be able to address latency. However, we will assess the effect of follow-up length as an approximation of latency.
It is noteworthy that estimating the incidence or prevalence of burnout remains challenging given the nosological and diagnostic blur surrounding this entity [25,26]. In such a context, focusing on studies that measure burnout as a continuous (rather than a dichotomous) variable is warranted.
Considering the abovementioned gaps in our knowledge of burnout’s etiology, we decided to re-examine the literature using a holistic approach. Understanding the etiology of a phenomenon is crucial before planning preventive strategies [27]. In other words, to build successful preventive strategies for burnout, we should have knowledge of its determinants and focus on the most impactful ones. Preventing burnout is highly important because it may reduce the quality of life of the affected worker [28]. Indeed, research suggests that burnout can be a predictor of both self-reported and medically certified sickness leaves [29,30]. Moreover, burnout may threaten job performance, with potentially serious consequences (e.g., medical errors among health professionals). All in all, preventing burnout may be beneficial for reducing costs in healthcare and working life.

Aims of the Study

We aimed to address the current gaps in our knowledge of burnout’s etiology in at least five ways. First, by focusing on the only consensual symptom of burnout, exhaustion. Second, by approaching exhaustion as a continuous outcome given the absence of diagnostic standards. Third, by comparing the effect sizes of work-related and work-unrelated predictors. Fourth, by including different occupational activities to generate more generalizable results. Fifth, by investigating the time-dependency of the associations between exhaustion and its predictors.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Protocol and Registration

The protocol of this study is available on the international database PROSPERO with the registration number CRD42021293031 from: https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/display_record.php?ID=CRD42021293031 (accessed on 24 December 2021). We conducted this study following the Conducting Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses of Observational Studies of Etiology (COSMOS-E) [31] and reported the results following the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) statement [32].

2.2. Literature Search

The literature search was conducted in three databases: MEDLINE, PsycINFO, and Embase (Figure 1). We included studies published between January 1990 and November 2020. Only longitudinal studies were considered. We searched for studies reporting correlation coefficients (rho) between a predictor at the first measurement point (baseline) and the outcome at the second measurement point (end of follow-up). We included studies written in English, German, French, Polish, Russian, or Spanish, displaying at least 50 workers per group. We excluded studies when their outcome was not exhaustion, and when the exposures were not relevant (e.g., measured at the same time as the outcome and not before). We also excluded studies based on study design (i.e., not longitudinal), publication type (i.e., conferences papers, letter to editors), and study population (e.g., non-workers). At least two independent reviewers performed the screening for the inclusion of studies in our review independently, and when needed, a third reviewer was consulted. The screening was performed using Rayyan software [33].

2.3. Data Extraction

We extracted the following data from the selected studies:
The Pearson correlations between the predictor at the first measurement point and the outcome (i.e., exhaustion) at the second measurement point, the sample mean age, the gender distribution in the sample, the follow-up length, the number of follow-up measurement points, the type of the occupational activity of the participants included in the sample, and the country where the studies were conducted. Data extraction was conducted by one reviewer and 20% was cross-checked by another.

2.4. Meta-Analysis

First, we grouped the predictors included in the different studies into four main families (Figure 2). Then, we grouped predictors in each family into subfamilies (Figure 2); we had five subfamilies for the first family, two for the second, one for the third and one for the fourth family, yielding nine subfamilies in total. The grouping of the predictors into families and subfamilies was carried out following a recent systematic review [20]. Eventually, we grouped predictors into families first, then subfamilies, then subgroups. In this sense, for some families there were not enough predictors to group into subfamilies, so we created only one subfamily and gave it the same name of the family in order to harmonize it with other subfamilies. We used the theoretical models that were used in the publication [7] to group the predictors. For example, we defined job control in this review following Karasek’s definition: “a ‘composite of two empirical but theoretically distinct constructs-the worker’s authority to make decisions (decision authority) and the breadth of skills used by the worker on the job (skills discretion)” [34]. In this context, the subgroup job control includes decision authority and skills discretion. Another example is the definition of the reward subgroup following Siegrist’s definition, implying esteem, promotion and security [35], and thus the subgroup includes these three elements. The considered subfamilies were: Job demands, Job control, Job resources, Interactions at work, Communication and leadership, Personality characteristics & self-reported health status, Job attitudes, Work-family interface, and Perceived intermediate work consequences. These subfamilies of predictors were then further subdivided into 66 subgroups of predictors to increase homogeneity (Figures S1–S9). The coefficients rho are known to be non-normally distributed, a Fisher’s z-transformation has been thus applied [36]. The meta-analysis was then conducted on such a transformed scale, where the standard error of a transformed estimate from a study is given by se = sqrt(1/(n − 3)), n being the sample size of the study [37], before being back-transformed on the original scale. We modeled the study-group specific z-transformed coefficients using a restricted maximization likelihood model with the study ID as a random effect. To avoid possible dependency issues [38], only one analysis was included from a study that examined the association of exhaustion and different predictor variables (from the same subgroup of predictor variables and involving the same individuals). If a study included more than one predictor variable that was to be grouped in the same subfamily, we included only one predictor variable which was the most comparable to other predictors in the subfamily. Consequently, we chose the most studied predictor variable in the literature. In the case of one study which included repeated measurement points, we chose to include the association representing the longest follow-up length in the analysis. We started to perform a meta-analysis for each subfamily of predictor variables. If the heterogeneity was too high (I2 larger than 60%), we performed a meta-analysis for each subgroup of predictor variables in that subfamily [38]. The strength of association was measured via the summary estimates (rho) and rated following the labels used in the field of psychology (i.e., 0: zero; ±0.1, ±0.3: weak; ±0.4, ±0.6: moderate; ±0.7, ±0.9 strong; and −1, 1 perfect) [39,40]. We assessed the publication bias using funnel plots and the Egger regression test [41,42]. This test is commonly used for testing the asymmetry in the funnel plot [43]. We performed the sensitivity analysis by excluding each study at a time and evaluating the impact by comparing the summary estimates (rho) and the heterogeneity (I2) before and after the exclusion of each study [44]. Moreover, we examined the effect of different follow-up lengths on the associations between predictors and exhaustion. We also performed an additional sensitivity analysis by meta-analyzing the subgroups only for studies conducted in Europe to observe if different countries would have an effect on the association between predictors and exhaustion. An additional sensitivity analysis by meta-analyzing the subgroups only for studies conducted on workers in the medical and health field was also performed. We chose to perform the sensitivity analysis for studies conducted in Europe and workers in the medical and health field because these two categories had the highest percentage among the 65 included studies, and we intended to ensure enough numbers for subgroup analysis. We also calculated prediction intervals for subgroups with at least ten studies as another major of heterogeneity. All statistical analyses were performed using Stata statistical software version 16 [45].

2.5. Risk of Bias and Overall Quality of Evidence

First, we assessed the risk of bias of the included studies individually using the Methodological Evaluation of Observational Research Checklist (MEVORECH) [46]. Secondly, we used the results of the risk of bias in the Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development and Evaluation (GRADE) approach, in which the risk of bias is the first domain considered [47]. The GRADE was used for the overall quality of evidence of subfamilies, and it has five domains: risk of bias, inconsistency, indirectness, imprecision, and publication bias.

3. Results

3.1. Included Studies

Considering that occupational burnout is the result of prolonged exposure to workplace stress [1,3], we decided to exclude three studies that used a follow-up length of fewer than three months [48,49,50]. We therefore included 65 studies which met our inclusion criteria (Figure 1). We analyzed 66 samples included in these studies because one study included two different samples. Out of these 65 studies, 47 were conducted in Europe, 12 in North America, 4 in Asia, and 2 in Australia. These studies were conducted between 2001 and 2020, using different follow-up lengths and different numbers of follow-up measurement points (Supplementary Materials Table S1). The shortest length of follow-up was four months whilst the longest was ten years. The number of follow-up measurement points was two, three, or a maximum of four. Although all of the included studies were longitudinal using a multiple-follow-up measurements panel design, their design did not separate exposed from non-exposed participants and did not follow the participants to capture the onset of burnout. Therefore, they did not allow for capturing the onset of burnout, nor estimating the incidence of burnout or its symptoms and their latency. The panel or repeated cross-sectional study designs rather follow participants to observe the increase or decrease in burnout. Among included studies, 15% focused on nurses, another 15% focused on teachers, 3% on physicians, 17% on health and social service employees, and 18% on participants from different occupational sectors (mixed samples). The remaining studies included workers from other occupations. The mean age of the population included in the meta-analysis was 31 years and 75% of the participants were women. There were 34% of studies with a low risk of bias, 45% with a moderate risk of bias, and 21% with a high risk of bias. The included studies assessed the predictors and exhaustion using self-reported measurement tools only (Supplementary Table S1).

3.2. Meta-Analysis

We started performing the meta-analysis for each of the nine subfamilies of predictor variables (Figure 2), but we found a high heterogeneity for every subfamily (I2 was consistently much than 60% (Table 1 and Supplementary Materials Figures S1–S9)). Therefore, the summary estimates cannot be considered meaningful. Subgroup analysis allowed us to reduce the heterogeneity below 40%, and we found a moderate quality of evidence on the effect of six subgroups of predictors: Physical demands, Workload, Work agreements, Lack of support from coworkers, Stressful interactions with patients/students, and Lack of control (Table 1). However, the associations between each of these subgroups and exhaustion were weak (rho = 0.25, 0.38, −0.25, 0.27, 0.22, and 0.17, respectively).
For three subgroups of predictors (Job demands (overall), Quality of social interactions at work, and Psychological/physical toll), the evidence of association with exhaustion was low. The associations between Job demands (overall), Quality of social interactions and exhaustion were weak (rho = 0.33, −0.27). On the other hand, the association between Psychological/physical toll and burnout was moderate (rho = 0.44). Finally, among five other subgroups (Performance-based self-esteem, Avoidance-motives, Family work facilitation, Value congruency, and Job insecurity) with moderate heterogeneity (I2 values ranged from 40 to 60%), we found a moderate quality of evidence for a weak association for Job insecurity (rho = 0.16) and low to very low evidence for weak associations for the other four (Table 1). For the Job demands subfamily, the strongest association with exhaustion was found for Workload (rho = 0.38), followed by Time pressure and Job demands (rho = 0.35). Five subgroups (Job control, Skill discretion, Autonomy, Decision authority, and Flow experiences) of the Job control subfamily had protective effects against exhaustion whereas one subgroup showed a harmful effect (i.e., Lack of control) on exhaustion. However, the evidence for the associations of these five subgroups with exhaustion was low to very low while the evidence for the association of the Lack of control subgroup was moderate. The association with Job resources; and with Lack of job resources is the same in absolute value but has opposite directions (rho = 0.12). The associations between Reward and exhaustion; and Lack of reward/inequity and exhaustion were of very similar strength but in opposite directions (−0.32 for Reward-exhaustion and 0.35 for Lack of reward-exhaustion). For the Interactions at work subfamily, we found the strongest association between Fairness/justice and exhaustion (rho = −0.35). Indeed, as Fairness/justice increased, exhaustion decreased (protective effect). On the other hand, Conflict and interpersonal problems had the strongest harmful effect on exhaustion (rho = 0.30). When Conflict and interpersonal problems increased, so did exhaustion. For the Communication and leadership subfamily, the strongest association with exhaustion was observed for the Quality of social interactions at work (rho = −0.27). Thus, when the Quality of social interactions at work increased, exhaustion decreased. For the Personality characteristics and self-reported health status subfamily, only four subgroups showed protective effects against exhaustion but the evidence of the effects of three of them is very low despite a moderate risk of bias. For the Job attitudes subfamily, Positive and negative job attitudes have almost the same effect on exhaustion in absolute value (rho = −0.24 for positive job attitudes examined in seven studies, 0.25 for negative job attitudes examined in six studies). It is worth mentioning that despite the overall risk of bias being moderate and despite the number of studies being adequate, the evidence regarding the effects of these factors is very low. For the Work-family interface subfamily, the strongest associations were observed between exhaustion and Work-family conflict (rho = 0.36); and between exhaustion and Value congruency (rho = −0.27). Nevertheless, the evidence of these associations is low and very low, respectively. For the Perceived intermediate work consequences subfamily, Work stressors and satisfaction have the same effect on exhaustion in absolute values (rho = 0.24 for Work stressors and rho = −0.29 for satisfaction).

3.3. Effect of the Follow-Up Length on the Observed Associations

Table 2 and Supplementary Materials Figures S10–S18 show how the associations between subfamilies of predictor variables and exhaustion vary over time when using a longer follow-up length for each of the nine subfamilies. To better understand the Supplementary Figures S10–S18, let’s take an example the Job resources subfamily; Supplementary Figure S12 shows that the effect of Job resources decreased by almost four-fold after 12 months and six-fold after 36 months of follow-up length. In Supplementary Figure S12 the first column on the left is the study details (authors, year, predictor), then the point in the middle of the line represents the effect size (rho) while the line is the 95% confidence interval, the second column is the numeric effect size with 95% confidence interval followed by p-value and the follow-up length (in months). For the Job demands subfamily, the average effect size did not differ over different lengths of follow-up: 3 months (rho = 0.33), 6 months (rho = 0.38), nine months (rho = 0.39), 12 months (rho = 0.36), 18 months (rho = 0.35), 24 months (rho = 0.35), 36 months (rho = 0.33), and 48 months (rho = 0.33). Additionally, for the other two subfamilies (i.e., Personality characteristics & self-reported health status and Perceived intermediate work consequences), we observed no changes in the association between the subfamily of predictor variable and exhaustion when the follow-up length increased. However, for Job control, the average effect was almost two-fold lower after 12 months of follow-up. The effect of Job resources decreased by almost four folds after 12 months and six folds after 36 months of follow-up length. The average effect of the Interactions at work subfamily on exhaustion decreased by almost four folds when the follow-up length increased from six to 12 months. The effect of the Interactions at work subfamily on exhaustion decreased additionally by four folds after 24 months of follow-up length. For the Communication and leadership subfamily, the average effect on exhaustion decreased two-fold after 18 months of follow-up length. The average effect of Job attitudes on exhaustion was reduced by two-fold when the follow-up length increased from three to 12 months. For the Work-family interface, the average effect on exhaustion decreased by three-fold after 12 months of follow-up length.

3.4. Sensitivity Analysis and Additional Analyses

The sensitivity analysis allowed us to reduce heterogeneity for 25 subgroups (Supplementary Materials Table S2). However, the summary estimates after the sensitivity analysis showed weak associations between the subgroup of predictor variables and exhaustion with the exception of Reward, which had a moderate association with occupational burnout (rho = −0.42). We observed that the Self-esteem association (statistically significant) with exhaustion decreased from −0.33 to −0.23 when deleting one study [51]. The association between the Reward and exhaustion (statistically not significant) increased from −0.32 to −0.42 when deleting one study [52]. For the Work-family conflict subgroup, deleting one of the following four studies [53,54,55,56] reduced the heterogeneity from 49 to 0% with no change in the association strength.
The results of the additional sensitivity analysis by performing the meta-analysis of subgroups for studies either conducted in Europe or in workers in medical and health field did not show any significant changes in the results (Supplementary Materials Tables S3 and S4). Additionally, we calculated prediction intervals for three subgroups of predictors where the subgroup included at least 10 studies. These prediction intervals were: (−0.54, 0.19) for social support, (−0.39, 0.02) for self-efficacy and (0.27, 0.45) for work-family conflict. These wide prediction intervals indicate high heterogeneity. We note that there are no correlations higher than 0.60 in absolute value for these three subfamilies since the prediction intervals do not include this value.

3.5. Publication Bias

The funnel plots for all subfamilies showed that the studies were distributed on both sides of the plots (Supplementary Materials Table S5). All the p-values for the Regression-based Egger test for small-study effects per subfamily were not significant (Supplementary Materials Table S5). Consequently, we did not find any evidence for publication bias for these nine subfamilies of predictor variables.

4. Discussion

4.1. Main Findings

This meta-analysis aimed to identify exhaustion’s predictors. The meta-analysis included 65 studies and assessed 242 predictors. Our findings mainly highlighted high heterogeneity, relatively weak associations, and quite a low quality of evidence. Following the holistic approach, it is unsurprising that heterogeneity increased, but this approach helped include virtually all studies and compare different types of predictors, countries, occupational activities, and follow-up lengths. Unfortunately, even subgroup analyses did not yield better results. In some subgroups, the number of included studies was too small and thus the heterogeneity index could not be interpreted with confidence. Nevertheless, the weak associations observed in this meta-analysis are due to the effect size of the associations in the included studies where these associations were predominantly weak. The additional analysis of the effect of follow-up lengths on the associations between predictors and exhaustion suggests a variation in these associations’ strength between six sub-families of predictor variables and exhaustion with the increasing length of follow-up. In this regard, our study suggests that as the follow-up length increases, the effect of six out of nine subfamilies of predictor variables decreases (Job control, Job resources, Interactions at work, Communication and leadership, Job attitudes, and Work-family interface), which is an original and important finding to the best of our knowledge. These results may suggest a short latency period followed by a recovery.

4.2. Defining and Measuring Exhaustion

This meta-analysis identified a diversity of measurement tools for occupational burnout (Supplementary Materials Table S1). In an attempt to reduce this source of heterogeneity, we limited the outcome definition to exhaustion, in line with a recent harmonized definition of occupational burnout [3]. Nonetheless, we encountered a large variability in the measures of exhaustion (Supplementary Materials Table S1). Many researchers mentioned the heterogeneity in the definition and measurement of burnout when trying to combine available studies in a meta-analysis [26,57,58,59]. In addition to the aforementioned heterogeneity, there is the issue of whether burnout should be considered a diagnosis or not [60,61,62]. The diagnosis is difficult because the burnout phenomenon is still “medically unexplained” [63], and there are still no well-established biomarkers of burnout [63,64]. Although researchers started investigating the epigenetic correlates of burnout [63,65,66], these studies define occupational burnout using PROMs. Unfortunately, none of the available PROMs address how exhaustion develops over time in response to stress. Therefore, their usage in further studies would not answer the question of occupational burnout latency. New and more objective outcome measurement methods are necessary.

4.3. Defining and Measuring Predictors of Occupational Burnout

We included four main families of predictors in our study and each family was further divided into nine subfamilies, and the subfamilies into subgroups. We faced a methodological issue, which was the heterogeneity in defining and measuring the predictors. One example refers to Job demands and Job resources, which constitute the Job Demands Resource (JD-R) model that is amongst the most used models in research and practice [67,68]. Concerning the JD-R model, Schaufeli and Taris explained that there is a variation in representing the constructs of Job demands and Job resources because the model is not restricted to well-defined demands or resources but rather follows an open and heuristic approach [69]. Another example is Work-family conflict which can be assessed using many measurement tools developed based on different concepts and dimensions [70]. Self-efficacy was measured in ten studies (Supplementary Materials Table S1 and Figure S6), using ten different measurement tools (or their translated versions). These three examples illustrate the heterogeneity in the definition and measurement of almost all predictor variables of occupational burnout but also other mental health outcomes. Therefore, future research should use the best defined and harmonized definition of each predictor variable and the most valid methods of their measurement to enable the systematic assessment of their effects.

4.4. The Latency of Occupational Burnout

Occupational burnout is often considered a process that develops gradually [71] over years or even decades [72]. However, the time needed for burnout to develop (i.e., its “latency”) is not easy to capture and there is no consensus on the best follow-up length for burnout. Consequently, researchers conducted studies using different follow-up lengths (Supplementary Table S1). Although most of these studies included the same participants at all measurement points (i.e., using the panel study design), capturing the onset of burnout remains difficult because they did not identify the date when exposure started. Moreover, a panel study usually has a discontinuous follow-up, which makes it challenging to identify the length of the exposure until the onset of burnout.
We note that there are differences between subfamilies regarding the results of the effects of the different follow-up lengths on the associations between the predictors and exhaustion. Six subfamilies of predictors showed a change in the association as the follow-up length increased, whereas three subfamilies did not show any change. We could not interpret these results reliably but we suggest three hypotheses. One suggested hypothesis can be the heuristic model [69], which implies that similar interactions do not necessarily exist between all predictors and all outcomes, distinct from the strict cause-and-effect-models.
The “healthy worker survivor effect” could be one of the possible explanations as well. The healthy worker survivor effect describes an ongoing selection of workers such as those who stay employed tend to be healthier than those who leave their job [73]. Consequently, the workers who suffer from work-related stress and early burnout may leave their company and thus cannot be part of a study. Another possible explanation is that workers in the early stages of burnout who were correctly identified and treated develop either resilience or affective coping over time. The literature suggests that resilience protects workers from work-related stress or burnout [74,75,76,77]. One systematic review found that certain coping strategies can reduce stress in health professionals and caregivers [78]. A systematic review of predictor variables associated with return to work in burnout found one study that showed that covert coping has a negative association with return to work [79].
Ahola et al., concluded that the phenomenon of burnout is unstable and diminishes over time and that the majority of burned-out workers keep working [80]. On the other hand, burnout has been linked to a decline in specific cognitive functioning [81], even with long-lasting cognitive impairments in the non-clinical burnout stage (i.e., early stage of burnout) [82]. It is worth noting that the MBI was the most widely used measure of burnout in the studies included in our meta-analysis and the meta-analysis of Ahola et al. [80]. However, the MBI does not assess cognitive functioning. Thus, the long-lasting effects of burnout may not be detected.
To solve the question of burnout latency in a reasonable timeline, the question of the diagnosis of burnout should be solved. After reaching a consensus on how to diagnose burnout, comprehensive studies of burnout cases may be helpful, particularly those using comprehensive retrospective assessment of exposures, ideally using objective data on occupational, personal and organizational factors with their respective duration before burnout onset. A better understanding of burnout development in terms of symptoms and impairments associated with each of its severity degrees would also help in determining which stage corresponds to a clinical form and based on which criteria the latter could be defined or diagnosed. Once such criteria are established, a prospective cohort design would be the best option to identify the new burnout cases and to measure burnout incidence, instead of measuring prevalence in the currently available studies. With consistent data on burnout incidence, it should finally be possible to estimate the latency of burnout development in different exposure conditions and develop effective preventive interventions focusing on the most relevant exposure window.

4.5. Implications of the Findings

This meta-analysis showed that there is a change in the association between six subfamilies (out of nine subfamilies studied) and exhaustion as the follow-up lengths varied. We could observe a decline in the association for the subfamilies (Job control, Job resources, Interactions at work, Communication and leadership, Job attitudes, and Work-family interface) when the follow-up length increased. Consequently, this study highlights the need to capture and quantify the latency of occupational burnout onset which only can be carried out after finding a consensus on the diagnosis of burnout. This enables future studies to use the prospective cohort study design and calculate the incidence of occupational burnout, ultimately estimating the time between exposure to predictor variables and occupational burnout onset. Another implication of this study is showing the importance of targeting the six subgroups of exhaustion predictors (i.e., Physical demands, Workload, Work agreements, Lack of support from coworkers, Stressful interactions with patients/students, and Lack of control) when planning preventive strategies for occupational burnout. Exhaustion is a predictor of stress-related health outcomes, and it is the most studied predictor of burnout compared to its other dimensions [10]. Preventive strategies targeting exhaustion may thus be of great benefit to prevent occupational burnout.
This study confirms that a standardization of the measurement of occupational burnout is urgently needed. In addition, we recommend that potential predictors of burnout be investigated using more objective indicators. Higher-quality cohorts and qualitative studies of burned-out workers should be conducted to better understand the etiology and course of burnout, and improve the overall evidence regarding the new and the less studied predictors.

4.6. Conclusive Remarks

The holistic approach used in this study enabled us to systematically assess which predictors can be moderately associated with exhaustion but for which the evidence is still very low or low. For example, the associations with exhaustion for unvalued trait/characteristics and maladaptive coping were (rho = 0.32, 0.33, respectively) and the quality of evidence was very low. Therefore, this review highlights the gap of knowledge which is the predominance of work-related predictors over work-unrelated predictors. This meta-analysis examined 242 predictor variables; of which 62% were work-related, 9% were from the Work-individual interface and 29% were Individual predictor variables. These results are unsurprising considering that burnout is considered an occupational phenomenon and is assumed to result from work-related stress [1]. Burnout was considered as one of the occupational health outcomes and was mainly studied in the frame of job strain theoretical models such as Karasek’s, Siegrist’s or Demerouti’s models [35,83,84,85]. Therefore, the majority of occupational predictors studied are often limited to those ascribed to these models. In our meta-analysis, 33% of the predictors were from the Job demands, control, and resources subfamilies. However, it remains necessary to thoroughly examine the associations between non-work-related predictors and burnout in future longitudinal studies.
We observed high heterogeneity in defining and measuring exhaustion and the predictors in the included studies. We also observed the use of different follow-up lengths between measurement points. These factors can contribute as a source of heterogeneity in this meta-analysis. However, an additional source of heterogeneity can be the different populations used across the studies that were often implicitly selected. Nevertheless, this meta-analysis summarized the associations between nine subfamilies of predictors and exhaustion in 65 studies. A strength of this meta-analysis is its focus on longitudinal studies. This ensures the measurement of exposure at the beginning of the follow-up before the measurement of the outcome. The inclusion of studies over almost 30 years and the inclusion of both work-related and non-work-related predictors are two additional strengths of this work. Finally, we can mention the rigorous methodology applied and the novel approach centered on exhaustion among the strengths of this study.

5. Conclusions

This meta-analysis summarized longitudinal findings pertaining to exhaustion’s predictors. Our findings revealed mostly weak associations with limited overall evidence, reflecting the limitations of the included studies and the available literature. Unsurprisingly, following a holistic approach increased the heterogeneity but this approach remained beneficial in comparing the associations between different types of predictors and exhaustion. Unfortunately, the evidence available does not point to clear target factors for preventing burnout. The decrease in associations as the follow-up length increases may suggest a short latency period followed by recovery. Future research should benefit from using a prospective cohort study design to answer burnout’s etiological questions along with qualitative studies of burned-out workers.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ijerph192013037/s1, Table S1: Description of the reviewed studies; Figure S1: Job demands forest plot; Figure S2: Job control forest plot; Figure S3: Job resources forest plot; Figure S4: Interactions at work forest plot; Figure S5: Communication and leadership forest plot; Figure S6: Personality characteristics and self-reported health status forest plot; Figure S7: Job attitudes forest plot; Figure S8: Work-family interface forest plot; Figure S9: Perceived intermediate work consequences forest plot; Figure S10: Cumulative meta-analysis of the association between Job demands and occupational burnout by increasing follow-up lengths; Figure S11: Cumulative meta-analysis of the association between Job control and occupational burnout by increasing follow-up lengths; Figure S12: Cumulative meta-analysis of the association between Job resources and occupational burnout by increasing follow-up lengths; Figure S13: Cumulative meta-analysis of the association between Interactions at work and occupational burnout by increasing follow-up lengths; Figure S14: Cumulative meta-analysis of the association between Communication and leadership and occupational burnout by increasing follow-up lengths; Figure S15: Cumulative meta-analysis of the association between Personality characteristics and self-reported health status and occupational burnout by increasing follow-up lengths; Figure S16: Cumulative meta-analysis of the association between Job attitudes and occupational burnout by increasing follow-up lengths; Figure S17: Cumulative meta-analysis of the association between Work-life interface and occupational burnout by increasing follow-up lengths; Figure S18: Cumulative meta-analysis of the association between Perceived intermediate work consequences and occupational burnout by increasing follow-up lengths; Table S2: Results of the sensitivity analysis if deleting one study changes the summary estimate of associations between subgroups of predictor-variables and exhaustion, and the heterogeneity of these summary estimates; Table S3: Comparison of Meta-analysis results per subgroups of studied predictor-variables between overall studies and studies conducted in Europe; Table S4: Comparison of Meta-analysis results per subgroups of studied predictor-variables between overall studies and studies conducted for medical and health workers Table S5: Results of the Regression-based Egger test for small-study effects per subfamily; Figure S19: Job demands funnel plot; Figure S20: Job control funnel plot; Figure S21: Job resources funnel plot; Figure S22: Interactions at work funnel plot; Figure S23: Communication and leadership funnel plot; Figure S24: Personality characteristics and self-reported health status funnel plot; Figure S25: Job attitudes funnel plot; Figure S26: Work-family interface funnel plot; Figure S27: Perceived intermediate work consequences funnel plot. References [51,52,54,55,56,86,87,88,89,90,91,92,93,94,95,96,97,98,99,100,101,102,103,104,105,106,107,108,109,110,111,112,113,114,115,116,117,118,119,120,121,122,123,124,125,126,127,128,129,130,131,132,133,134,135,136,137,138,139,140,141,142,143,144,145] are cited in the supplementary materials.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, I.G.C.; methodology, Y.S., V.R. and I.G.C.; software, Y.S. and V.R.; validation, Y.S., V.R. and I.G.C.; formal analysis, Y.S. and V.R.; investigation, Y.S., V.R. and I.G.C.; resources, Y.S., V.R. and I.G.C.; data curation, Y.S., V.R. and I.G.C.; writing—original draft preparation, Y.S., V.R., R.B. and I.G.C.; writing—review and editing, Y.S., V.R., R.B. and I.G.C.; visualization, Y.S., V.R., R.B. and I.G.C.; supervision, I.G.C.; project administration, I.G.C.; funding acquisition, I.G.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program (under the Marie Skłodowska-Curie grant agreement No. 801076, through the SSPH+ Global PhD Fellowship Program in Public Health Sciences (GlobalP3HS) of the Swiss School of Public Health) partly supported the PhD position of Y.S. Unisanté supported the other part, via the General Directorate of Health of the Canton of Vaud via the grant of the Commission for Health Promotion and the Fight against Addictions Grant No. 8273/3636000000-801.

Data Availability Statement

Data are available online as Supplementary Materials of the present article.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Word Health Organization. Burn-Out an “Occupational Phenomenon”: International Classification of Diseases. Available online: https://www.who.int/news/item/28-05-2019-burn-out-an-occupational-phenomenon-international-classification-of-diseases (accessed on 28 May 2019).
  2. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  3. Guseva Canu, I.; Marca, S.C.; Dell’Oro, F.; Balázs, Á.; Bergamaschi, E.; Besse, C.; Bianchi, R.; Bislimovska, J.; Koscec Bjelajac, A.; Bugge, M.; et al. Harmonized definition of occupational burnout: A systematic review, semantic analysis, and Delphi consensus in 29 countries. Scand. J. Work. Environ. Health 2021, 47, 95–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Maslach, C.; Schaufeli, W.B.; Leiter, M.P. Job burnout. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2001, 52, 397–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Shirom, A.; Melamed, S. A comparison of the construct validity of two burnout measures in two groups of professionals. Int. J. Stress Manag. 2006, 13, 176–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Kristensen, T.S.; Borritz, M.; Villadsen, E.; Christensen, K.B. The Copenhagen Burnout Inventory: A new tool for the assessment of burnout. Work Stress 2005, 19, 192–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Shoman, Y.; Marca, S.C.; Bianchi, R.; Godderis, L.; van der Molen, H.F.; Guseva Canu, I. Psychometric properties of burnout measures: A systematic review. Epidemiol. Psychiatry Sci. 2021, 30, e8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Maslach, C.; Jackson, S.E. The Measurement of Experienced Burnout. J. Occup. Behav. 1981, 2, 99–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Demerouti, E.; Demerouti, E.; Bakker, A.B.; Vardakou, I.; Kantas, A. The Convergent Validity of Two Burnout Instruments. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2003, 19, 12–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Maslach, C.; Leiter, M.P. Understanding the burnout experience: Recent research and its implications for psychiatry. World Psychiatry 2016, 15, 103–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Kisling, L.; Das, J.P. Prevention Strategies; StatPearls Publishing: Treasure Island, FL, USA, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  12. West, C.P.; Dyrbye, L.N.; Erwin, P.J.; Shanafelt, T.D. Interventions to Prevent and Reduce Physician Burnout: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. Obstet. Gynecol. Surv. 2017, 72, 147–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Vargas, C.; Canadas, G.A.; Aguayo, R.; Fernandez, R.; de la Fuente, E.I. Which occupational risk factors are associated with burnout in nursing? A meta-analytic study. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 2014, 14, 28–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Pradas-Hernandez, L.; Ariza, T.; Gomez-Urquiza, J.L.; Albendin-Garcia, L.; De la Fuente, E.I.; Canadas-De la Fuente, G.A. Prevalence of burnout in paediatric nurses: A systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0195039. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. O’Connor, K.; Neff, D.M.; Pitman, S. Burnout in mental health professionals: A systematic review and meta-analysis of prevalence and determinants. Eur. Psychiatry 2018, 53, 74–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Aronsson, G.; Theorell, T.; Grape, T.; Hammarstrom, A.; Hogstedt, C.; Marteinsdottir, I.; Skoog, I.; Traskman-Bendz, L.; Hall, C. A systematic review including meta-analysis of work environment and burnout symptoms. BMC Public Health 2017, 17, 264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  17. Alarcon, G.M. A meta-analysis of burnout with job demands, resources, and attitudes. J. Vocat. Behav. 2011, 79, 549–562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Guthier, C.; Dormann, C.; Voelkle, M.C. Reciprocal Effects Between Job Stressors and Burnout: A Continuous Time Meta-Analysis of Longitudinal Studies. Psychol. Bull. 2020, 146, 1146–1173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Chirico, F. Job stress models for burnout syndrome: A review. Ann. Istituto Super. Sanita 2016, 52, 443–456. [Google Scholar]
  20. Shoman, Y.; El May, E.; Marca, S.; Wild, P.; Bianchi, R.; Bugge, M.; Caglayan, C.; Cheptea, D.; Gnesi, M.; Godderis, L.; et al. Predictors of Occupational Burnout: A Systematic Review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 9188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Seidler, A.; Thinschmidt, M.; Deckert, S.; Then, F.; Hegewald, J.; Nieuwenhuijsen, K.; Riedel-Heller, S.G. The role of psychosocial working conditions on burnout and its core component emotional exhaustion—A systematic review. J. Occup. Med. Toxicol. 2014, 9, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Wang, M.; Liao, X.; Laden, F.; Spiegelman, D. Quantifying risk over the life course—Latency, age-related susceptibility, and other time-varying exposure metrics. Stat. Med. 2016, 35, 2283–2295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Kuh, D.; Ben-Shlomo, Y.; Lynch, J.; Hallqvist, J.; Power, C. Life course epidemiology. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2003, 57, 778–783. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Rothman, K. Epidemiology: An Introduction; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  25. Schaufeli, W.B.; Vandierendonck, D. A Cautionary Note About the Cross-National and Clinical Validity of Cutoff Points for the Maslach Burnout Inventory. Psychol. Rep. 1995, 76, 1083–1090. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Rotenstein, L.S.; Torre, M.; Ramos, M.A.; Rosales, R.C.; Guille, C.; Sen, S.; Mata, D.A. Prevalence of Burnout among Physicians: A Systematic Review. JAMA 2018, 320, 1131–1150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. White, F. Application of Disease Etiology and Natural History to Prevention in Primary Health Care: A Discourse. Med. Princ. Pract. 2020, 29, 501–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Ahola, K.; Gould, R.; Virtanen, M.; Honkonen, T.; Aromaa, A.; Lonnqvist, J. Occupational burnout as a predictor of disability pension: A population-based cohort study. Occup. Environ. Med. 2009, 66, 284–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Toppinen-Tanner, S.; Ojajarvi, A.; Vaananen, A.; Kalimo, R.; Jappinen, P. Burnout as a predictor of medically certified sick-leave absences and their diagnosed causes. Behav. Med. 2005, 31, 18–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Borritz, M.; Rugulies, R.; Christensen, K.B.; Villadsen, E.; Kristensen, T.S. Burnout as a predictor of self-reported sickness absence among human service workers: Prospective findings from three year follow up of the PUMA study. Occup. Environ. Med. 2006, 63, 98–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Dekkers, O.M.; Vandenbroucke, J.P.; Cevallos, M.; Renehan, A.G.; Altman, D.G.; Egger, M. COSMOS-E: Guidance on conducting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of observational studies of etiology. PLoS Med. 2019, 16, e1002742. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Page, M.J.; McKenzie, J.E.; Bossuyt, P.M.; Boutron, I.; Hoffmann, T.C.; Mulrow, C.D.; Shamseer, L.; Tetzlaff, J.M.; Akl, E.A.; Brennan, S.E.; et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 2021, 372, n71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Ouzzani, M.; Hammady, H.; Fedorowicz, Z.; Elmagarmid, A. Rayyan—A web and mobile app for systematic reviews. Syst. Rev. 2016, 5, 210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Karasek, R. Control in the workplace and its health-related aspect. In Job Control and Worker Health; Sauter, S.L., Hurrell, J.J., Cooper, C.L., Eds.; Wiley: Chichester, UK, 1989; pp. 129–160. [Google Scholar]
  35. Siegrist, J. Adverse health effects of high-effort/low-reward conditions. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 1996, 1, 27–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Welz, T.; Doebler, P.; Pauly, M. Fisher transformation based confidence intervals of correlations in fixed- and random-effects meta-analysis. Br. J. Math. Stat. Psychol. 2021, 75, 12224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Bishara, A.J.; Hittner, J.B. Confidence intervals for correlations when data are not normal. Behav. Res. Methods 2017, 49, 294–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Higgins, J.P.; Thomas, J.; Chandler, J.; Cumpston, M.; Li, T.; Page, M.J.; Welch, V.A. (Eds.) Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions, 2nd ed.; John Wiley & Sons: Chichester, UK, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  39. Dancey, C.; Reidy, J. Statistics without Maths for Psychology; Pearson Education: London, UK, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  40. Akoglu, H. User’s guide to correlation coefficients. Turk. J. Emerg. Med. 2018, 18, 91–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Simmonds, M. Quantifying the risk of error when interpreting funnel plots. Syst. Rev. 2015, 4, 24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Light, R.; Pillemer, D. Summing Up: The Science of Reviewing Research; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1984. [Google Scholar]
  43. Pustejovsky, J.E.; Rodgers, M.A. Testing for funnel plot asymmetry of standardized mean differences. Res. Synth. Methods 2019, 10, 57–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  44. Sutton, A.; Abrams, K.; Jones, D.; Sheldon, T.; Song, F. Methods for Meta-Analysis in Medical Research; Wiley: Chichester, UK, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  45. StataCorp. Stata Statistical Software: Release 16; StataCorp LLC: College Station, TX, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  46. Shamliyan, T.; Kane, R.L.; Dickinson, S. A systematic review of tools used to assess the quality of observational studies that examine incidence or prevalence and risk factors for diseases. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 2010, 63, 1061–1070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Guyatt, G.H.; Oxman, A.D.; Vist, G.E.; Kunz, R.; Falck-Ytter, Y.; Alonso-Coello, P.; Schunemann, H.J.; Group, G.W. GRADE: An emerging consensus on rating quality of evidence and strength of recommendations. BMJ 2008, 336, 924–926. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Westman, M.; Etzion, D.; Gattenio, E. International business travels and the work-family interface: A longitudinal study. J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 2008, 81, 459–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Fritz, C.; Sonnentag, S. Recovery, well-being, and performance-related outcomes: The role of workload and vacation experiences. J. Appl. Psychol. 2006, 91, 936–945. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Fritz, C.; Sonnentag, S. Recovery, health, and job performance: Effects of weekend experiences. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 2005, 10, 187–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Spence Laschinger, H.K.; Finegan, J. Situational and dispositional predictors of nurse manager burnout: A time-lagged analysis. J. Nurs. Manag. 2008, 16, 601–607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Gelsema, T.I.; van der Doef, M.; Maes, S.; Janssen, M.; Akerboom, S.; Verhoeven, C. A longitudinal study of job stress in the nursing profession: Causes and consequences. J. Nurs. Manag. 2006, 14, 289–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Travis, D.; Lizano, E.; Mor Barak, M. ‘I’m So Stressed!’: A Longitudinal Model of Stress, Burnout and Engagement among Social Workers in Child Welfare Settings. Br. J. Soc. Work 2015, 46, 1076–1095. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  54. Rubio, C.; Osca, A.; Recio, P.; Urien, B.; Peiro, J.M. Work-family conflict, self-efficacy, and emotional exhaustion: A test of longitudinal effects. J. Work Organ. Psychol. 2015, 31, 147–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Lizano, E.L.; Barak, M.E.M. Workplace demands and resources as antecedents of job burnout among public child welfare workers: A longitudinal study. Child Youth Serv. Rev. 2012, 34, 1769–1776. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Langballe, E.; Innstrand, S.T.; Aasland, O.; Falkum, E. The Predictive Value of Individual Factors, Work-Related Factors, and Work-Home Interaction on Burnout in Female and Male Physicians: A Longitudinal Study. Stress Health 2011, 27, 73–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Sabitova, A.; McGranahan, R.; Altamore, F.; Jovanovic, N.; Windle, E.; Priebe, S. Indicators Associated with Job Morale among Physicians and Dentists in Low-Income and Middle-Income Countries: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. JAMA Netw. Open 2020, 3, e1913202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Koutsimani, P.; Montgomery, A.; Georganta, K. The Relationship between Burnout, Depression, and Anxiety: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Bianchi, R.; Verkuilen, J.; Schonfeld, I.S.; Hakanen, J.J.; Jansson-Frojmark, M.; Manzano-Garcia, G.; Laurent, E.; Meier, L.L. Is Burnout a Depressive Condition? A 14-Sample Meta-Analytic and Bifactor Analytic Study. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2021, 9, 579–597. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Hillert, A.; Albrecht, A.; Voderholzer, U. The Burnout Phenomenon: A Résumé after More than 15,000 Scientific Publications. Front. Psychiatry 2020, 11, 519237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Eurofound. Burnout in the Workplace: A Review of Data and Policy Responses in the EU; Publicatios Office of the European Union: Luxembourg, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  62. Bianchi, R.; Schonfeld, I.S.; Laurent, E. Is it Time to Consider the “Burnout Syndrome” A Distinct Illness? Front. Public Health 2015, 3, 158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Engebretsen, K.M. Suffering without a medical diagnosis. A critical view on the biomedical attitudes towards persons suffering from burnout and the implications for medical care. J. Eval. Clin. Pract. 2018, 24, 1150–1157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Danhof-Pont, M.B.; van Veen, T.; Zitman, F.G. Biomarkers in burnout: A systematic review. J. Psychosom. Res. 2011, 70, 505–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Bakusic, J.; Schaufeli, W.; Claes, S.; Godderis, L. 1496 Epigenetic perspective of burnout. BMJ 2018, 75, A574. [Google Scholar]
  66. Bakusic, J.; Schaufeli, W.; Claes, S.; Godderis, L. Stress, burnout and depression: A systematic review on DNA methylation mechanisms. J. Psychosom. Res. 2017, 92, 34–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Kaiser, S.; Patras, J.; Adolfsen, F.; Richardsen, A.M.; Martinussen, M. Using the Job Demands-Resources Model to Evaluate Work-Related Outcomes Among Norwegian Health Care Workers. Sage Open 2020, 10, 2158244020947436. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Bakker, A.B.; Demerouti, E. Job Demands-Resources Theory: Taking Stock and Looking Forward. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 2017, 22, 273–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Schaufeli, W.B.; Taris, T.W. A critical review of the job demands-resources model: Implications for improving work and health. In Bridging Occupational, Organizational and Public Health: A Transdisciplinary Approach; Springer Science + Business Media: New York, NY, USA, 2014; pp. 43–68. [Google Scholar]
  70. Chen, W.; Zhang, G.; Tian, X.; Wang, L.; Luo, J. Rasch Analysis of Work-Family Conflict Scale Among Chinese Prison Police. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 37005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Schaufeli, W.; Enzmann, D. The Burnout Companion to Study and Practice: A Critical Analysis; Taylor & Francis: London, UK, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  72. Guedes, D.; Gaspar, E. Burnout in a sample of Brazilian Physical Education professional. Rev. Bras. Educ. Fís. Esporte 2016, 30, 1011–1022. [Google Scholar]
  73. Arrighi, H.M.; Hertz-Picciotto, I. The evolving concept of the healthy worker survivor effect. Epidemiology 1994, 5, 189–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Zhang, X.J.; Song, Y.; Jiang, T.; Ding, N.; Shi, T.Y. Interventions to reduce burnout of physicians and nurses: An overview of systematic reviews and meta-analyses. Medicine 2020, 99, e20992. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Romppanen, J.; Haggman-Laitila, A. Interventions for nurses’ well-being at work: A quantitative systematic review. J. Adv. Nurs. 2017, 73, 1555–1569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. McDonald, G.; Jackson, D.; Wilkes, L.; Vickers, M.H. Personal resilience in nurses and midwives: Effects of a work-based educational intervention. Contemp. Nurse 2013, 45, 134–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Guo, Y.F.; Luo, Y.H.; Lam, L.; Cross, W.; Plummer, V.; Zhang, J.P. Burnout and its association with resilience in nurses: A cross-sectional study. J. Clin. Nurs. 2018, 27, 441–449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Hawken, T.; Turner-Cobb, J.; Barnett, J. Coping and adjustment in caregivers: A systematic review. Health Psychol. Open 2018, 5, 055102918810659. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Karkkainen, R.; Saaranen, T.; Hiltunen, S.; Ryynanen, O.P.; Rasanen, K. Systematic review: Factors associated with return to work in burnout. Occup. Med. 2017, 67, 461–468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  80. Ahola, K.; Toppinen-Tanner, S.; Seppänen, J. Interventions to alleviate burnout symptoms and to support return to work among employees with burnout: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Burn. Res. 2017, 4, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Deligkaris, P.; Panagopoulou, E.; Montgomery, A.J.; Masoura, E. Job burnout and cognitive functioning: A systematic review. Work Stress 2014, 28, 107–123. [Google Scholar]
  82. Van Dijk, D.M.; van Rhenen, W.; Murre, J.M.J.; Verwijk, E. Cognitive functioning, sleep quality, and work performance in non-clinical burnout: The role of working memory. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0231906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Karasek, R.A. Job Demands, Job Decision Latitude, and Mental Strain—Implications for Job Redesign. Admin. Sci. Quart. 1979, 24, 285–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Karasek, R.; Theorell, T. Healthy Work, Stress, Productivity, and the Reconstruction of Working Life; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1990. [Google Scholar]
  85. Demerouti, E.; Bakker, A.B.; Nachreiner, F.; Schaufeli, W.B. The job demands-resources model of burnout. J. Appl. Psychol. 2001, 86, 499–512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Zwingmann, I.; Wolf, S.; Richter, P. Every light has its shadow: A longitudinal study of transformational leadership and leaders’ emotional exhaustion. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2016, 46, 19–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Wirtz, N.; Rigotti, T.; Otto, K.; Loeb, C. What about the Leader? Crossover of Emotional Exhaustion and Work Engagement from Followers to Leaders. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 2017, 22, 86–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  88. Welp, A.; Meier, L.; Manser, T. The interplay between teamwork, clinicians’ emotional exhaustion, and clinician-rated patient safety: A longitudinal study. Crit. Care 2016, 20, 110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  89. Vegchel, N.v.; Jonge, J.d.; Söderfeldt, M.; Dormann, C.; Schaufeli, W. Quantitative Versus Emotional Demands Among Swedish Human Service Employees: Moderating Effects of Job Control and Social Support. Int. J. Stress Manag. 2004, 11, 21–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  90. van der Ploeg, E.; Kleber, R.J. Acute and chronic job stressors among ambulance personnel: Predictors of health symptoms. Occup. Environ. Med. 2003, 60, 40–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  91. Van de Ven, B.; van den Tooren, M.; Vlerick, P. Emotional Job Resources and Emotional Support Seeking as Moderators of the Relation Between Emotional Job Demands and Emotional Exhaustion: A Two-Wave Panel Study. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 2013, 18, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  92. Turgut, S.; Michel, A.; Rothenhofer, L.M.; Sonntag, K. Dispositional resistance to change and emotional exhaustion: Moderating effects at the work-unit level. Eur. J. Work Organ. Psychol. 2016, 25, 735–750. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Tonjes, B.; Dickhauser, O. Longitudinal effects of goal orientation on factors of occupational burden in the teacher profession. Z. Entwickl. Padagog. Psychol. 2009, 41, 79–86. [Google Scholar]
  94. Thompson, M.J.; Carlson, D.S.; Kacmar, K.M.; Vogel, R.M. The Cost of Being Ignored: Emotional Exhaustion in the Work and Family Domains. J. Appl. Psychol. 2020, 105, 186–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Taris, T.W.; Peeters, M.C.; Le Blanc, P.M.; Schreurs, P.J.; Schaufeli, W.B. From inequity to burnout: The role of job stress. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 2001, 6, 303–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Taris, T.W.; Kompier, M.A.J.; Geurts, S.A.E.; Houtman, I.L.D.; van den Heuvel, F.F.M. Professional efficacy, exhaustion, and work characteristics among police officers: A longitudinal test of the learning-related predictions of the demand-control model. J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 2010, 83, 455–474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Salanova, M.; Schaufeli, W.; Martínez, I.; Bresó, E. How obstacles and facilitators predict academic performance: The mediating role of study burnout and engagement. Anxiety Stress Coping 2009, 23, 53–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  98. Richter, A.; Schraml, K.; Leineweber, C. Work-family conflict, emotional exhaustion and performance-based self-esteem: Reciprocal relationships. Int. Arch. Occup. Environ. Health 2015, 88, 103–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  99. Ramarajan, L.; Barsade, S.; Burack, O. The influence of organizational respect on emotional exhaustion in the human services. J. Posit. Psychol. 2008, 3, 4–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Raimo, J.; LaVine, S.; Spielmann, K.; Akerman, M.; Friedman, K.A.; Katona, K.; Chaudhry, S. The Correlation of Stress in Residency with Future Stress and Burnout: A 10-Year Prospective Cohort Study. J. Grad. Med. Educ. 2018, 10, 524–531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  101. Pomaki, G.; Karoly, P.; Maes, S. Linking Goal Progress to Subjective Well-Being at Work: The Moderating Role of Goal-Related Self-Efficacy and Attainability. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 2009, 14, 206–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Pisanti, R.; van der Doef, M.; Maes, S.; Meier, L.L.; Lazzari, D.; Violani, C. How Changes in Psychosocial Job Characteristics Impact Burnout in Nurses: A Longitudinal Analysis. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 1082. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  103. Philipp, A.; Schupbach, H. Longitudinal effects of emotional labour on emotional exhaustion and dedication of teachers. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 2010, 15, 494–504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Petrou, P.; Demerouti, E.; Schaufeli, W.B. Job crafting in changing organizations: Antecedents and implications for exhaustion and performance. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 2015, 20, 470–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  105. McManus, I.C.; Winder, B.C.; Gordon, D. The causal links between stress and burnout in a longitudinal study of UK doctors. Lancet 2002, 359, 2089–2090. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Martinez-Inigo, D.; Totterdell, P. The mediating role of distributive justice perceptions in the relationship between emotion regulation and emotional exhaustion in healthcare workers. Work Stress 2016, 30, 26–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  107. Makikangas, A.; Kinnunen, U. Psychosocial work stressors and well-being: Self-esteem and optimism as moderators in a one-year longitudinal sample. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 2003, 35, 537–557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Lu, L.; Lin, H.Y.; Cooper, C.L. Unhealthy and present: Motives and consequences of the act of presenteeism among Taiwanese employees. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 2013, 18, 406–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  109. Leiter, M.P.; Hakanen, J.J.; Ahola, K.; Toppinen-Tanner, S.; Koskinen, A.; Vaananen, A. Organizational predictors and health consequences of changes in burnout: A 12-year cohort study. J. Organ. Behav. 2013, 34, 959–973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Lavigne, G.L.; Forest, J.; Crevier-Braud, L. Passion at work and burnout: A two-study test of the mediating role of flow experiences. Eur. J. Work Organ. Psychol. 2012, 21, 518–546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Lapointe, É.; Vandenberghe, C.; Boudrias, J.-S. Psychological contract breach, affective commitment to organization and supervisor, and newcomer adjustment: A three-wave moderated mediation model. J. Vocat. Behav. 2013, 83, 528–538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Kubicek, B.; Korunka, C.; Tement, S. Too much job control? Two studies on curvilinear relations between job control and eldercare workers’ well-being. Int. J. Nurs. Stud. 2014, 51, 1644–1653. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Kubicek, B.; Korunka, C. Does job complexity mitigate the negative effect of emotion-rule dissonance on employee burnout? Work Stress 2015, 29, 379–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  114. Korunka, C.; Kubicek, B.; Paskvan, M.; Ulferts, H. Changes in work intensification and intensified learning: Challenge or hindrance demands? J. Manag. Psychol. 2015, 30, 786–800. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Konze, A.-K.; Rivkin, W.; Schmidt, K.-H. Is Job Control a Double-Edged Sword? A Cross-Lagged Panel Study on the Interplay of QuantitativeWorkload, Emotional Dissonance, and Job Control on Emotional Exhaustion. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 1608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  116. Koch, A.K.; Adler, M. Emotional exhaustion and innovation in the workplace-a longitudinal study. Ind. Health 2018, 56, 524–538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  117. Knoll, M.; Hall, R.J.; Weigelt, O. A Longitudinal Study of the Relationships Between Four Differentially Motivated Forms of Employee Silence and Burnout. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 2019, 24, 572–589. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  118. Jimenez, P.; Dunkl, A. The Buffering Effect of Workplace Resources on the Relationship between the Areas of Worklife and Burnout. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  119. Jensen, M.; Knudsen, K. A two-wave cross-lagged study of business travel, work–family conflict, emotional exhaustion, and psychological health complaints. Eur. J. Work Organ. Psychol. 2016, 26, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Innstrand, S.T.; Langballe, E.; Falkum, E. The longitudinal effects of individual vulnerability, organisational factors, and work–home interaction on burnout among male church ministers in Norway. Ment. Health Relig. Cult. 2011, 14, 241–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Tone Innstrand, S.; Melbye Langballe, E.; Arild Espnes, G.; Falkum, E.; Gjerl⊘w Aasland, O. Positive and negative work-family interaction and burnout: A longitudinal study of reciprocal relations. Work Stress 2008, 22, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Idris, M.A.; Dollard, M.F.; Yulita. Psychosocial safety climate, emotional demands, burnout, and depression: A longitudinal multilevel study in the Malaysian private sector. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 2014, 19, 291–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  123. Hudek-Knezevic, J.; Maglica, B.K.; Krapic, N. Personality, organizational stress, and attitudes toward work as prospective predictors of professional burnout in hospital nurses. Croat. Med. J. 2011, 52, 538–549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  124. Huang, Y.H.; Chen, C.H.; Du, P.L.; Huang, I.C. The causal relationships between job characteristics, burnout, and psychological health: A two-wave panel study. Int. J. Hum. Resour. Man 2012, 23, 2108–2125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  125. Hornung, S.; Weigl, M.; Glaser, J.; Angerer, P. Is It So Bad or Am I So Tired? Cross-Lagged Relationships Between Job Stressors and Emotional Exhaustion of Hospital Physicians. J. Pers. Psychol. 2013, 12, 124–131. [Google Scholar]
  126. Hochwalder, J. A longitudinal study of the relationship between empowerment and burnout among registered and assistant nurses. Work 2008, 30, 343–352. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  127. Gregersen, S.; Vincent-Höper, S.; Nienhaus, A. The Relation Between Leadership and Perceived Well-Being: What Role Does Occupational Self-Efficacy Play? J. Leadersh. Stud. 2014, 8, 6–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Gonzalez-Morales, M.G.; Rodriguez, I.; Peiro, J.M. A Longitudinal Study of Coping and Gender in a Female-Dominated Occupation: Predicting Teachers’ Burnout. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 2010, 15, 29–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Gonzalez-Morales, M.G.; Peiro, J.M.; Rodriguez, I.; Bliese, P.D. Perceived collective burnout: A multilevel explanation of burnout. Anxiety Stress Coping 2012, 25, 43–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  130. Goddard, R.; O’Brien, P.; Goddard, M. Work environment predictors of beginning teacher burnout. Brit. Educ. Res. J. 2006, 32, 857–874. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  131. Gil-Monte, P.R.; Garcia-Juesas, J.A. Effects of Work Overloading and Self-Efficacy over the Burnout Syndrome: A Longitudinal Survey about Nursing. Rev. Mex. Psicol. 2008, 25, 329–337. [Google Scholar]
  132. Firoozabadi, A.; Uitdewilligen, S.; Zijlstra, F.R.H. Should You Switch Off or Stay Engaged? The Consequences of Thinking About Work on the Trajectory of Psychological Well-Being Over Time. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 2018, 23, 278–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  133. Figueiredo-Ferraz, H.; Grau-Alberola, E.; Gil-Monte, P.R.; Garcia-Juesas, J.A. Burnout and job satisfaction among nursing professionals. Psicothema 2012, 24, 271–276. [Google Scholar]
  134. Fida, R.; Laschinger, H.K.S.; Leiter, M.P. The protective role of self-efficacy against workplace incivility and burnout in nursing: A time-lagged study. Health Care Manag. Rev. 2018, 43, 21–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  135. Feuerhahn, N.; Bellingrath, S.; Kudielka, B.M. The Interplay of Matching and Non-Matching Job Demands and Resources on Emotional Exhaustion among Teachers. Appl. Psychol. Health Well-Being 2013, 5, 171–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  136. Fernet, C.; Lavigne, G.L.; Vallerand, R.J.; Austin, S. Fired up with passion: Investigating how job autonomy and passion predict burnout at career start in teachers. Work Stress 2014, 28, 270–288. [Google Scholar]
  137. Fernet, C.; Gagne, M.; Austin, S. When does quality of relationships with coworkers predict burnout over time? The moderating role of work motivation. J. Organ. Behav. 2010, 31, 1163–1180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  138. Dubois, C.A.; Bentein, K.; Ben Mansour, J.; Gilbert, F.; Bedard, J.L. Why Some Employees Adopt or Resist Reorganization of Work Practices in Health Care: Associations between Perceived Loss of Resources, Burnout, and Attitudes to Change. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2014, 11, 187–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  139. Chrisopoulos, S.; Dollard, M.F.; Winefield, A.H.; Dormann, C. Increasing the probability of finding an interaction in work stress research: A two-wave longitudinal test of the triple-match principle. J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 2010, 83, 17–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  140. Childs, J.H.; Stoeber, J. Do you want me to be perfect? Two longitudinal studies on socially prescribed perfectionism, stress and burnout in the workplace. Work Stress 2012, 26, 347–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  141. Boamah, S.A.; Read, E.A.; Laschinger, H.K.S. Factors influencing new graduate nurse burnout development, job satisfaction and patient care quality: A time-lagged study. J. Adv. Nurs. 2017, 73, 1182–1195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  142. Birkeland, I.K.; Richardsen, A.M.; Dysvik, A. The Role of Passion and Support Perceptions in Changing Burnout: A Johnson-Neyman Approach. Int. J. Stress Manag. 2018, 25, 163–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  143. Basinska, B.A.; Gruszczynska, E. Job-Related Emotions and Job Burnout among Civil Servants: Examining the Shape of the Relationship in Cross-Sectional and Longitudinal Models. Med. Pr. 2019, 70, 201–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  144. Angelo, R.P.; Chambel, M.J. The Reciprocal Relationship between Work Characteristics and Employee Burnout and Engagement: A Longitudinal Study of Firefighters. Stress Health 2015, 31, 106–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  145. Adriaenssens, J.; De Gucht, V.; Maes, S. Causes and consequences of occupational stress in emergency nurses, a longitudinal study. J. Nurs. Manag. 2015, 23, 346–358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Flow-chart of the included studies.
Figure 1. Flow-chart of the included studies.
Ijerph 19 13037 g001
Figure 2. Description of the grouping of predictors in four families and nine subfamilies, shown in italics.
Figure 2. Description of the grouping of predictors in four families and nine subfamilies, shown in italics.
Ijerph 19 13037 g002
Table 1. Meta-analysis results per subgroups of studied predictor-variables.
Table 1. Meta-analysis results per subgroups of studied predictor-variables.
Studied Predictor-Variables
Grouped Per (Sub)Family
Number of StudiesHeterogeneity “I2 Estimate”Summary Estimate of the Association with Exhaustion95% Confidence IntervalOverall Risk of Bias ResultsInconsistencyIndirectnessImprecisionPublication BiasOverall Quality of Evidence 1
Job demands2789.25%0.330.28–0.38
Work and time demands 891.40%0.330.22–0.43LowNoYesYesNoLow
Cognitive demands389.74%0.13−0.05, 0.31ModerateYesNoYesNoVery low
Physical demands20.00%0.250.17, 0.34LowNoNoYesNoModerate
Workload 618.69%0.380.34–0.43ModerateNoNoNoNoModerate
Time pressure592.34%0.350.17–0.53LowNoNoYesNoModerate
Job demands (overall)213.55%0.350.23–0.48ModerateNoNoYesNoLow
Emotional demands831.69%0.340.30–0.39ModerateNoYesYesNoVery Low
Job control2094.14%−0.15−0.21, −0.09
Job control876.78%−0.23−0.30, −0.16LowNoYesYesNoLow
Skill discretion30.00%−0.05−0.08, −0.02HighNoYesYesNoVery low
Autonomy677.82%−0.21−0.21, −0.11ModerateYesYesYesNoVery low
Decision authority581.59%−0.06−0.19, 0.06HighYesYesYesNoVery low
Flow experiences1NA−0.40−0.51, −0.29HighNANANANoVery low
Lack of control238.44%0.170.07, 0.28LowNoNoYesNoModerate
Job resources1197.22%−0.07−0.23, 0.08
Job resources697.75%−0.12−0.47, 0.22LowNoNoYesNoModerate
Lack of job resources473.40%0.120.02, 0.23ModerateNoNoYesNoLow
Reward383.64%−0.32−0.51, −0.12LowNoNoNoNoHigh
Lack of reward/inequity296.27%0.35−0.12, 0.82LowNoNoYesNoModerate
Material resources372.77%−0.27−0.42, −0.13LowNoNoYesNoModerate
Interactions at work2396.57%−0.02−0.10, 0.07
Social support1289.24%−0.18−0.27, −0.08ModerateNoNoYesNoLow
Poor social climate579.37%0.240.12, 0.35LowNoNoYesNoModerate
Support from supervisor391.71%−0.16−0.29, −0.03LowNoYesYesNoVery low
Support from colleagues 30.01%−0.16−0.21, −0,12LowNoYesYesNoVery low
Fairness/justice 20.00%−0.35−0.45, −0.25HighNoNoYesNoVery low
Lack of support from supervisor285.25%0.270.01, 0.52LowNoNoYesNoModerate
Lack of support from coworkers20.01%0.270.20, 0.35LowNoNoYesNoModerate
Conflict & interpersonal problems392.19%0.300.05, 0.55ModerateNoNoNoNoModerate
Communication & leadership1293.09%−0.13−0.24, −0.03
Work agreements20.00%−0.25−0.33, −0.16LowNoNoYesNoModerate
Communication/information flow494.05%−0.09−0.30, 0.12ModerateNoYesYesNoLow
Quality of social interactions at work320.20%−0.27−0.34, −0.19ModerateNoNoNoNoLow
Leadership390.31%−0.07−0.31, 0.17LowYesYesYesNoVery low
Role conflict1NA0.190.09, 0.29ModerateNANAYesNoLow
Personality characteristics & self-reported health status2696.60%−0.02−0.11, 0.07
Unvalued trait/
characteristics
390.29%0.320.07, 0.57HighNoYesYesNoVery low
Valued trait/
characteristics
588.15%−0.24−0.39, −0.09ModerateYesNoYesNoVery low
Extraversion1NA0.13−0.05, 0.31ModerateNANAYesNALow
Conscientiousness1NA−0.01−0.19, 0.17ModerateNANAYesNALow
Openness1NA0.03−0.15, 0.21ModerateNANAYesNALow
Self-efficacy 1070.20%−0.19−0.25, −0.12ModerateNoYesYesNoVery low
Maladaptive coping30.00%0.330.24, 0.42ModerateNoYesYesNoVery low
Adaptive coping473.55%−0.02−0.16, 0.11ModerateYes YesYesNoVery low
Emotion-focused coping287.42%−0.02−0.18, 0.14ModerateYesNoYesNoVery low
Self-esteem 283.22%−0.33−0.53, −0.13LowNoNoYesNoModerate
Performance-based self-esteem345.52%0.240.20, 0.28HighNoNoYesNoLow
Self-reported health status (harmful) 292.14%0.340.13, 0.55ModerateNoNoYesNoVery low
Self-reported health status (protective)1NA−0.33−0.46, −0.20HighNANAYesNAVery low
Job attitudes1895.73%0.05−0.04, 0.13
Positive job attitudes779.71%−0.24−0.33, −0.15ModerateYesYesYesNoVery low
Negative job attitudes679.93%0.250.17, 0.33ModerateYesYesYesNoVery low
Intrinsically motivated behavior886.28%−0.07−0.17, 0.03HighYesYesYesNoVery low
Extrinsically motivated behavior483.30%0.280.05, 0.51ModerateNoNoYesNoLow
Avoidance motives254.33%0.200.03, 0.37HighNoYesYesNoVery low
Acquiescent silence1NA0.220.14, 0.30HighNANAYesNAVery low
Quiescent silence1NA0.260.18, 0.34HighNANAYesNAVery low
Prosocial silence1NA0.01−0.07, 0.09HighNANAYesNAVery low
Opportunistic silence1NA0.130.05, 0.21HighNANAYesNAVery low
Work-family interface1198.35%0.130.02, 0.23
Work-family conflict1049.36%0.360.33, 0.39ModerateNoYesNoNoLow
Family-work conflict30.00%0.200.17, 0.24HighNoYesYesNoVery low
Work-family facilitation 371.24%−0.11−0.19, −0.02HighNoNoYesNoLow
Family-work facilitation357.95%−0.05−0.11, 0.02HighYesNoYesNoLow
Value congruency 354.12%−0.27−0.34,−0.20ModerateYesNoYesNoVery low
Perceived intermediate work consequences 1695.04%0.190.09, 0.29
Work stressors480.55%0.240.13, 0.35Low NoNoYesNoModerate
Stressful interactions with patients/students 20.00%0.220.16, 0.28LowNoNoYesNoModerate
Job insecurity 256.18%0.160.03, 0.30LowNoNoYesNoModerate
Impact of change290.29%0.260.08, 0.44ModerateNoNoYesNoLow
Psychological/physical toll 233.39%0.440.31, 0.56ModerateNoYesNoNoLow
Stress from work393.06%0.260.06, 0.46ModerateNoNoYesNoLow
Satisfaction 375.43%−0.29−0.47, −0.11HighNoNoYesNoVery low
Colleagues/team exhaustion288.04%0.27−0.10, 0.64ModerateNoNoYesNoLow
Note: 1 Based on the GRADE, which takes into account the risk of bias, inconsistency, indirectness, imprecision, and publication bias of all studies for a given predictor.
Table 2. Summary of the effect of different follow-up lengths on the association between subfamilies of predictors and exhaustion.
Table 2. Summary of the effect of different follow-up lengths on the association between subfamilies of predictors and exhaustion.
SubfamilyChange in the Associations between Predictors and ExhaustionChange in Follow-Up Length (Months)
Job demandsNo change3–48
Job control2-fold decrease6–12
Job resources2-fold decrease
6-fold decrease
6–12
12–36
Interactions at work and occupational burnout4-fold decrease
4-fold decrease
6–12
12–24
Communication and leadership2-fold decrease3–18
Personality characteristics and self-reported health statusNo change3–48
Job attitudes2-fold decrease3–12
Work-life interface3-fold decrease6–36
Perceived intermediate work consequencesNo change6–120
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Shoman, Y.; Rousson, V.; Bianchi, R.; Guseva Canu, I. Holistic Assessment of Factors Associated with Exhaustion, the Main Symptom of Burnout: A Meta-Analysis of Longitudinal Studies. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 13037. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192013037

AMA Style

Shoman Y, Rousson V, Bianchi R, Guseva Canu I. Holistic Assessment of Factors Associated with Exhaustion, the Main Symptom of Burnout: A Meta-Analysis of Longitudinal Studies. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(20):13037. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192013037

Chicago/Turabian Style

Shoman, Yara, Valentin Rousson, Renzo Bianchi, and Irina Guseva Canu. 2022. "Holistic Assessment of Factors Associated with Exhaustion, the Main Symptom of Burnout: A Meta-Analysis of Longitudinal Studies" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 20: 13037. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192013037

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop