Next Article in Journal
How Does Environmental Regulation Affect the Green Growth of China’s Citrus Industry? The Mediating Role of Technological Innovation
Next Article in Special Issue
mHealth-Supported Hearing Health Training for Early Childhood Development Practitioners: An Intervention Study
Previous Article in Journal
Barriers and Expectations of Adolescents Regarding the Identification and Management of Their Psychoactive Substance Use by Their General Practitioner
Previous Article in Special Issue
Exploring the Factors Associated with Mental Health Attitude in China: A Structural Topic Modeling Approach
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Research on the Influence Mechanisms of the Affective and Cognitive Self-Esteem

1
Department of Industrial Engineering, Tsinghua University, Beijing 100084, China
2
Antai College of Economics and Management, Shanghai Jiao Tong University, Shanghai 200030, China
3
School of Physical Education and Sports Science, Lingnan Normal University, Zhanjiang 524048, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(20), 13232; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192013232
Submission received: 6 September 2022 / Revised: 26 September 2022 / Accepted: 8 October 2022 / Published: 14 October 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Digital Technologies for Public Health Promotion)

Abstract

:
Most prior studies examined the direct relation between social media usage and loneliness. This study tries to reveal the covert mechanisms involved in how different types of SMU affect older adults’ loneliness, which has rarely been an object of attention in the prior literature. A partial least squares structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM) method was used to analyze the data collected from 466 older adults in a field study. This research divided self-esteem into two dimensions: affective self-esteem (AE) and cognitive self-esteem (CE). The study found that changes in CE only stemmed from functional SMU (FSMU), rather than active SMU (ASMU) and passive SMU (PSMU). ASMU and PSMU had a significant effect on FSMU. CE had a significant effect on loneliness. Objective social isolation (OSI) had a positive relationship with loneliness. Moreover, PSMU, FSMU, and CE had a significant effect on ASMU, CE, and AE, respectively. For older adults, the feeling of connecting with others was more valuable than acquiring specific outcomes. The mediation test results showed that FSMU could play a completely mediating role in the relationship between ASMU and PSMU, as well as that between ASMU and CE. CE significantly mediated the relationship between FSMU and loneliness. Finally, the total effect sizes of ASMU and PSMU on FSMU were significant, and those of CE on older adults’ loneliness and AE were significant, while the total effect of AE on older adults’ loneliness was non-significant. AE moderated the relationship between PSMU and OSI, so PSMU was related to higher OSI only for users experiencing high AE. These findings offer a guide for the use of social media to conduct future loneliness interventions for older adults.

1. Introduction

Research has demonstrated that older people experience loneliness more often than young groups do [1,2]. Loneliness refers to a negative emotion experienced when the quality of social relationships is insufficient [3]. There was evidence showing that loneliness is associated with cognitive decline, dementia risk, early mortality risk, and cardiovascular disease for older adults [4]. Living with feelings of loneliness has implications for quality of life, physical and mental health, and mortality [1].
As social media usage [5] has grown exponentially throughout the last decade, older people have also become important participants. Social media has turned into a place for social activities related to conducting loneliness interventions for older adults [6]. Indeed, prior studies have revealed that SMU impacts older adults in many ways, such as depression, mental health, and loneliness [7,8,9,10]. On the one hand, loneliness can drive SMU [11,12,13,14,15,16,17]. On the other hand, SMU has an impact on increasing or decreasing loneliness [13,18,19,20,21,22,23,24]. Compared with their attention toward younger age groups, these studies paid little attention to older adults [25,26,27]. Going beyond the direct association between SMU and loneliness, the covert mechanism connecting the two should be identified, especially for older adults. For example, shopping and paying on social media can decrease cognitive self-esteem, which, in turn, decreases loneliness. In addition, classifying different types of SMU helps with this issue (e.g., active and passive use) [28,29,30,31,32]. To our knowledge, no study has simultaneously investigated how different types of SMU (i.e., active, passive, or functional use) affect loneliness in older adults.
Based on a field study in China, 466 older adults were interviewed. A PLS-SEM method was used to examine how these types of SMU affect older adults’ loneliness. The cognitive self-esteem, affective self-esteem, and objective social isolation of older adults were also included. The research results revealed that SMU had a very different impact on loneliness for older adults than it does for young adults. Since preventing loneliness is more important than curing it, our study offers a guide for the use of social media to conduct future loneliness interventions for older adults [3].

1.1. Types of SMU

This typology is seemingly increasingly used in the literature. Gerson and his colleagues [31] have tried to develop a professional scale for this typology. In fact, active SMU can be classified into two sub-types (e.g., interactive and non-interactive types). This typology is understood according to the information process of older adults. Active SMU refers to information generation, while passive SMU refers to information consumption. However, there are activities on SM that do not (or only slightly) involve processes of emotional information, such as payment. We call these activities as functional SMU. Active and passive SMU refer to activities on SM that involve emotional information processes.
While many studies have measured overall SMU, the most studied taxonomy of SMU is the framework that includes the active and passive types of usage [33]. Different social media activities may have distinguishing effects. Burke and her colleagues divided these activities into directed communication, broadcasting, and passive consumption [34,35]. Then, researchers theoretically developed these terms into active Facebook use and passive Facebook use [31,32]. According to the prior literature, we defined (a) passive SMU as using social media to consume information from others, (b) active SMU as using social media to exchange information with others, and (c) functional SMU as using social media to complete tasks in life with non-emotional involvement in information of others [28,30,31,32].

1.2. Social Media and Loneliness

Loneliness refers to the feelings associated with the absence of an intimate attachment or feelings of emptiness or abandonment [36]. Feeling lonely is not just emotionally painful, but literally deleterious to human health [3]. Loneliness occurs when one’s intimate and social needs are not adequately met. Social media have been identified as affording particular qualities that provide people with both transient and enduring feelings of connectedness [37]. As evidence, Hajek and König found that daily social media users reported lower loneliness scores compared with those with less frequent or no use [26]. A higher degree of SMU also resulted in users’ feeling that they are in the same community as others. The degree of salience—thereby closely imitating real-life social interactions—was effective in attenuating loneliness [22].
In terms of active SMU, users interact with their friends on social media and exchange their information through self-disclosure. Passive SMU permits users to observe overflowing information about others’ life events. Nonetheless, users do not become involved in others’ information when engaging in functional SMU. Additionally, older adults lack the ability to recognize cognitive adjustments, so they tend to trust information from others on social media [38]. Compared with younger people, lonely older adults tend to avoid engaging in social media due to their poor social abilities [39]. Accordingly, we propose the following hypothesis:
Hypothesis H1a:
ASMU directly increases older adults’ FSMU.
Hypothesis H1(b/c):
PSMU decreases older adults’ (b) ASMU and (c) FSMU.
Hypothesis H1d:
PSMU indirectly decreases older adults’ FSMU, mediated by an increase in ASMU.

1.3. Objective Social Isolation

Social isolation refers to an objective state of lack of social contact [40]. Objective social isolation means that an individual lacks social contact in real life. While social isolation is one of the determinants of loneliness, people can feel lonely when not socially isolated [41]. Older adults with depression often have uncontrollable conflicts with their peers stemming from the pain of diseases and affective disorders, all of which can lead to social isolation [42]. Although OSI and loneliness are closely related, socially isolated older adults may report a low level of loneliness [41]. In addition, older adults with high OSI may have a strong motivation to use social media [26]. Social media use was positively correlated with mental health and negatively correlated with social isolation [43]. However, inappropriate use of social media was associated with perceived levels of social isolation [44]. Accordingly, we posited the following hypotheses:
Hypothesis H2a:
ASMU directly decreases older adults’ OSI.
Hypothesis H2(b/c):
(b) PSMU and (c) FSMU directly increase older adults’ OSI.
Hypothesis H3:
AE directly decreases older adults’ OSI.
Hypothesis H4:
OSI directly increases older adults’ loneliness.

1.4. Self-Esteem

Self-esteem mainly refers to the subjective evaluation of an individual’s own value, role, and attitude [45]. Low self-esteem is linked to poor mental health [46]. For example, low self-esteem can lead to apathy, isolation, and passivity [47]. Explicit self-esteem was negatively correlated with depressive symptoms, suicidal ideation, and loneliness [48]. Social media use has a positive impact on self-esteem [49], and it has been found that social media participation is positively correlated with group self-esteem [50]. Instagram and Facebook are regarded as active social media types, and users post positive content to increase the positive responses of subscribers, thereby enhancing their self-esteem [51]. More active TV watching is associated with lower social contact and self-esteem [52]. Passive Facebook use affected women’s internalization of societal beauty ideals, which were inversely correlated with women’s satisfaction with their bodies and self-esteem [53]. It was found that loneliness was negatively correlated with self-esteem and positively correlated with smartphone addiction, while self-esteem was negatively correlated with smartphone addiction [54]. In addition, due to the extensive existence of peer pressure and conformity, appropriate consumption at the right time is very important for self-esteem, social acceptance, and maintenance of social relations of specific groups. Some studies have suggested that the self-esteem of some groups has been commercialized, and there are many adverse consequences of failing to “keep up” with consumption trends, such as social exclusion, negative peer evaluation, and low self-esteem; this requires the strengthening of people’s correct understanding of self-esteem [55]. Moreover, level A of theoretical analysis at the RSES level specifies that the behavioral pattern of subjects in self-evaluations can be divided into cognitive and affective elements [56]. However, no studies have incorporated affective self-esteem and cognitive self-esteem into the framework of active, passive, and functional social media use based on objective social isolation theory. Thus, we proposed the following hypotheses:
Hypothesis H5(a/b):
ASMU directly increases older adults’ (a) AE and (b) CE.
Hypothesis H5c:
ASMU indirectly decreases older adults’ CE, mediated by an increase in FSMU.
Hypothesis H6(a/b):
PSMU decreases older adults’ (a) AE and (b) CE.
Hypothesis H7a:
FSMU directly increases older adults’ AE.
Hypothesis H7b:
FSMU directly decreases older adults’ CE.
Hypothesis H7c:
FSMU indirectly decreases older adults’ AE, mediated by an increase in CE.
Hypothesis H7d:
FSMU indirectly increases older adults’ loneliness, mediated by an increase in CE.
Hypothesis H8:
CE directly increases older adults’ AE.
Hypothesis H9(a/b):
(a) AE and (b) CE directly decrease older adults’ loneliness.
Hypothesis H9c:
PSMU indirectly increases older adults’ CE, mediated by an increase in FSMU.
Hypothesis H10:
For older adults, AE will enhance the effects that PSMU has on OSI.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants and Procedure

The study began with a pilot study involving 32 older adults who lived in a care home. Some questions were originally written in English and then translated into and back-translated from Chinese by two of the researchers who were fluent in both languages. The results demonstrated sufficient validity and reliability. After that, a field study was conducted in another pension institution (hereafter, “the institution”) from May to July in 2019. The institution operated 8 locations located in Anhui Province, China. Since there were well-structured questions in this research, the caretakers were trained to interview the older adults for research efficiency. In order to reduce bias, all eight of the locations took part in the study. A total of 466 older adults were interviewed, of which 52.1 percent (N = 243) reported experience with SMU. The numbers of participants in the different locations ranged from 43 to 70. With regard to age (N = 460; M = 69.84; SD = 8.47), 94.5% were older than 60 years. Participants had predominantly low levels of education (82.7% with less than a high school degree) and were predominantly female (N = 456; 62.5%) and seemingly healthy (94.2%). Additionally, the main social media applications that participants adopted included WeChat, QQ, Weibo, and Toutiao.

2.2. Measures

Type of SMU: We adapted validated scales from prior studies [28,30,31,32,57] to measure passive and active SMU. Four and three items were used to evaluate passive and active SMU, respectively. The measurement for functional SMU was based on two additional items. Participants needed to evaluate how often they engaged in a number of activities while using any social media site, such as “Browsing content posted by friends”, “Commenting”, and “Shopping on social media”. The cutoff points were based on six-point Likert scales anchored at 1 (‘‘never’’) and 6 (‘‘several times one day’’).
Objective social isolation: A measure of objective social isolation from family and friends was used [40]. The frequency of contact with family members (friends) was measured by the question, “How often do you see, write or talk on the telephone with relatives (friends) who do not live with you?”. The cutoff points were based on seven-point Likert scales anchored at 1 (‘‘every day’’) and 7 (‘‘never’’).
Six items modified by Rosenberg were used to evaluate older adults’ self-esteem with respect to the institution [58,59]. The affective self-esteem measure was composed of three items that were related to the perceived affection of the older adults, and cognitive self-esteem measure was composed of three items that were related to the perceived cognition of the older adults.

2.3. Statistical Techniques

This study adopted partial least squares structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM) as a statistical method. The SPSS24 and SmartPLS3 software was used to analyze the responses. Only 201 records (83%) were analyzed with PLS-SEM, since 42 of the social-media-using participants provided incomplete data. The PLS-SEM method was recommended for testing these complex hypotheses for the following reasons: (a) Our hypotheses formed a complex path model with latent variables that had many indicators [60]; (b) the size of our sample was small relatively for SEM analysis [61]; (c) older adults who lived in the same location may have been related to each other such that the distribution of our sample would not have met the requirements of the parametric factor-based SEM approach [62].

3. Results

Common method bias became a potential risk to the PLS-SEM analysis because we adopted the same interview procedure and scales for the participants [63]. Multiple methods were utilized to alleviate and assess the common method bias. As a remedy, the measures across different constructs eliminated common scale properties by changing the response formats across sections [64]. As a statistical assessment, Harman’s single-factor test showed that a single factor only explained 11% of the variance, which indicated a very low likelihood of the threat of common method bias [65].
The results of the PLS-SEM analysis firstly offered a reliable and valid measurement model that provided support for the measurement quality. Each of the constructs used within the PLS-SEM analysis was measured with a reflective indicator. Therefore, the indicator reliability, internal consistency reliability, convergent validity, and discriminant validity were evaluated. CFA loadings above 0.7 can indicate the indicator reliability. Some items of cognitive self-esteem and affective self-esteem were deleted because of their low CFA loadings. Cronbach’s Alpha, RHO_A, and composite reliability [48] values were examined to assess the internal consistency reliability, with all of them having a criterion value above 0.6 [66,67]. Convergent validity was assessed with the average variance extracted (AVE) with a criterion value above 0.5. The discriminant validity assessment involved analyzing the heterotrait–monotrait (HTMT) ratio of the correlations with a criterion value below 0.85 [68]. Table 1 and Table 2 display the reliability and validity of the measurement models after modification.
The structural model of the PLS-SEM analysis for the testing of the hypotheses is displayed in Figure 1. All VIF values among the constructs were below 5, which indicates that the collinearity was not serious. The results revealed that there was a unique pattern of relationships between SMU and loneliness for older adults with respect to that for younger age groups.
In terms of the antecedents of FSMU, ASMU (b = 0.418, t = 5.643, p < 0.001) and PSMU (b = −0.202, t = 3.120, p < 0.01) had a significant effect on FSMU, providing support for H1a and H1c. CE (b = −0.237, t = 3.501, p < 0.001) had a significant effect on loneliness. That is, H9b was supported. Moreover, OSI had a positive relationship with loneliness (b = 0.258, t = 3.320, p < 0.001), which was consistent with the findings of previous studies [41]. That is, H4 was supported. PSMU, FSMU, and CE had significant effects on ASMU (b = −0.449, t = 10.000, p < 0.001), CE (b = −0.271, t = 3.672, p < 0.001), and AE (b = 0.445, t = 5.349, p < 0.001), respectively. That is, H1b, H7b, and H8 were supported. For older adults, AE enhanced the effects that PSMU had on OSI (b = 0.145, t = 2.084, p < 0.05). Thus, H10 was supported.
The SmartPLS 3.3.3 software provides researchers with algorithms that can be used to acquire specific indirect effects in order to test mediation hypotheses [69]. The results of the mediation test showed that FSMU could play a mediating role in the relationships of ASMU (b = −0.113, t = 3.147, p < 0.01) and PSMU (b = 0.055, t = 2.248, p < 0.05) with CE. That is, H5c and H9c were supported. ASMU significantly mediated the relationship between PSMU and FSMU (b = −0.188, t = 4.668, p < 0.001). CE significantly mediated the relationship between FSMU and AE (b = −0.120, t = 3.106, p < 0.01). Moreover, CE significantly mediated the relationship between FSMU and loneliness (b = 0.064, t = 2.364, p < 0.05). That is, H1d, H7c, and H7d were supported, whereas the others were not. Finally, the results also showed that the total effect size of ASMU on FSMU was 0.418 (t = 5.643, p < 0.001), the total effect size of PSMU on FSMU was −0.389 (t = 7.239, p < 0.001), the total effect size of FSMU on CE was −0.271 (t = 3.672, p < 0.001), the total effect size of CE on AE was 0.445 (t = 5.349, p < 0.001), and the total effect size of CE on older adults’ loneliness was −0.249 (t = 4.203, p < 0.001), while the total effect size of AE (t = 0.402) on older adults’ loneliness was non-significant.

4. Discussion and Conclusions

From a motivation perspective, while alleviating loneliness is a motivator for young adults to use social media [16,17], it does not drive older adults. This result also reminds us that when examining the impact of SMU on older adults, we are studying a group of older adults who are less lonely.
Furthermore, our results revealed that SMU has a more limited impact on the loneliness of older adults than it does on the loneliness of young adults. Consistently with a prior study [32], changes in cognitive self-esteem only stem from functional SMU rather than from active and passive SMU. This implies that only more consumption activities on social media can result in cognitive self-esteem. Unlike active and functional SMU, passive SMU is mainly driven by browsing the information of others, leading one to face an enormous amount of information about others [29,70]. In terms of the antecedents of functional SMU, active SMU and passive SMU have a significant effect on functional SMU. Cognitive self-esteem has a significant effect on loneliness. Moreover, often, if older adults realize that their life is more isolated than those of others on social media, their lack of offline social connections will be more fatal. In addition, different types of SMU have an indirect impact on affective self-esteem and objective social isolation.
Objective social isolation has a positive relationship with loneliness, which is consistent with the findings of previous studies [41]. Moreover, passive SMU, functional SMU, and cognitive self-esteem have a significant effect on active SMU, cognitive self-esteem, and affective self-esteem, respectively. For older adults, the feeling of connecting with others is more valuable than acquiring specific outcomes.
Moreover, the results of the mediation test show that functional SMU could play a completely mediating role in the relationships of active SMU and passive SMU with cognitive self-esteem. This implies that improving the functional SMU of older adults can reduce the impact of active SMU on their cognitive self-esteem and increase the impact of passive SMU on their cognitive self-esteem. In this sense, for older adults who actively and passively use social media, in order to change their cognitive self-esteem, they should be encouraged to engage in activities of consumption on social media. Cognitive self-esteem significantly mediates the relationship between functional SMU and loneliness. This suggests that in order to alleviate older adults’ loneliness through cognitive self-esteem, the first step should be to improve their concept of consumption. Finally, the results also showed that the total effect sizes of active SMU and passive SMU on functional SMU were significant; those of cognitive self-esteem on older adults’ loneliness and on affective self-esteem were also significant, while the total effect size of affective self-esteem on older adults’ loneliness was non-significant.This study explores the underlying mechanisms underlying different types of self-esteem. It reveals the internal relationship between cognitive self-esteem, affective self-esteem and mental health of the elderly, and provides theoretical support for the development of different types of self-esteem. This study provides a reference for elderly care institutions and government departments to develop mental and physical health management strategies for the elderly. Social media technology plays an important role in promoting the development of healthy aging and provides convenience for the elderly. Reasonable use of social media is conducive to alleviating the loneliness of the elderly and improving their quality of life.

5. Limitations

Although this study has made some progress, there are still some limitations. First, although a sufficiently representative sample was obtained in this study, the survey’s objects mainly focused on the elderly in smart elderly care institutions, and the scope of the research had certain limitations. Second, due to the limitations of human resources and other factors, this study only collected data at a certain time point. Future research can increase the number of samples, consider longitudinal data factors, and collect data from different community pension institutions so as to make the study more extensive and representative. Third, this study may have overlooked other factors regarding the effects of social media use on loneliness in older adults. In addition, future research could consider other contributing factors.

Author Contributions

S.Y.: Writing—original draft (Preparation of the published work, specifically writing the initial draft.); M.Z.: Writing—review & editing (Critical review, commentary or revision—including pre- or post-publication stages). All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and ethical approval were waived for this study due to this study only conducted questionnaires and other surveys and did not involve experiments.

Informed Consent Statement

This study has been approved by the organizations. The authors also appreciate the staff of organizations such as Anhui Jing’an Health Development.

Data Availability Statement

The data used in this study can be requested from the authors.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Dahlberg, L.; Agahi, N.; Lennartsson, C. Lonelier than ever? Loneliness of older people over two decades. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 2018, 75, 96–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Hanratty, B.; Stow, D.; Collingridge Moore, D.; Valtorta, N.K.; Matthews, F. Loneliness as a risk factor for care home admission in the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Age Ageing 2018, 47, 896–900. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Cattan, M.; White, M.; Bond, J.; Learmouth, A. Preventing social isolation and loneliness among older people: A systematic review of health promotion interventions. Ageing Soc. 2005, 25, 41–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. McHugh Power, J.E.; Steptoe, A.; Kee, F.; Lawlor, B.A. Loneliness and social engagement in older adults: A bivariate dual change score analysis. Psychol. Aging 2019, 34, 152–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. El-Gabalawy, R.; Mackenzie, C.S.; Thibodeau, M.A.; Asmundson, G.J.G.; Sareen, J. Health anxiety disorders in older adults: Conceptualizing complex conditions in late life. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2013, 33, 1096–1105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Chau, M.M.; Burgermaster, M.; Mamykina, L. The use of social media in nutrition interventions for adolescents and young adults-A systematic review. Int. J. Med. Inform. 2018, 120, 77–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Yoon, S.; Kleinman, M.; Mertz, J.; Brannick, M. Is social network site usage related to depression? A meta-analysis of Facebook-depression relations. J. Affect. Disord. 2019, 248, 65–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Frost, R.L.; Rickwood, D.J. A systematic review of the mental health outcomes associated with Facebook use. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2017, 76, 576–600. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Seabrook, E.M.; Kern, M.L.; Rickard, N.S. Social Networking Sites, Depression, and Anxiety: A Systematic Review. JMIR Ment. Health 2016, 3, e50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Baker, D.A.; Algorta, G.P. The Relationship Between Online Social Networking and Depression: A Systematic Review of Quantitative Studies. Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw. 2016, 19, 638–648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Kim, J.-H. Psychological issues and problematic use of smartphone: ADHD’s moderating role in the associations among loneliness, need for social assurance, need for immediate connection, and problematic use of smartphone. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2018, 80, 390–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Scott, G.G.; Boyle, E.A.; Czerniawska, K.; Courtney, A. Posting photos on Facebook: The impact of Narcissism, Social Anxiety, Loneliness, and Shyness. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2018, 133, 67–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Wang, K.; Frison, E.; Eggermont, S.; Vandenbosch, L. Active public Facebook use and adolescents’ feelings of loneliness: Evidence for a curvilinear relationship. J. Adolesc. 2018, 67, 35–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Gentina, E.; Chen, R. Digital natives’ coping with loneliness: Facebook or face-to-face? Inf. Manag. 2019, 56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Reissmann, A.; Hauser, J.; Stollberg, E.; Kaunzinger, I.; Lange, K.W. The role of loneliness in emerging adults’ everyday use of facebook—An experience sampling approach. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2018, 88, 47–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Kim, J.; Kim, J.; Yang, H. Loneliness and the use of social media to follow celebrities: A moderating role of social presence. Soc. Sci. J. 2019, 56, 21–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Blachnio, A.; Przepiorka, A.; Boruch, W.; Balakier, E. Self-presentation styles, privacy, and loneliness as predictors of Facebook use in young people. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2016, 94, 26–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Deters, F.G.; Mehl, M.R. Does Posting Facebook Status Updates Increase or Decrease Loneliness? An Online Social Networking Experiment. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 2013, 4, 579–586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Sutcliffe, A.G.; Binder, J.F.; Dunbar, R.I.M. Activity in social media and intimacy in social relationships. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2018, 85, 227–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Chopik, W.J. The Benefits of Social Technology Use Among Older Adults Are Mediated by Reduced Loneliness. Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw. 2016, 19, 551–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Phu, B.; Gow, A.J. Facebook use and its association with subjective happiness and loneliness. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2019, 92, 151–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Pittman, M.; Reich, B. Social media and loneliness: Why an Instagram picture may be worth more than a thousand Twitter words. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2016, 62, 155–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Baek, Y.M.; Bae, Y.; Jang, H. Social and parasocial relationships on social network sites and their differential relationships with users’ psychological well-being. Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw. 2013, 16, 512–517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Jung, Y.; Song, H.; Vorderer, P. Why do people post and read personal messages in public? The motivation of using personal blogs and its effects on users’ loneliness, belonging, and well-being. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2012, 28, 1626–1633. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. van der Velden, P.G.; Setti, I.; van der Meulen, E.; Das, M. Does social networking sites use predict mental health and sleep problems when prior problems and loneliness are taken into account? A population-based prospective study. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2019, 93, 200–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Hajek, A.; Konig, H.H. The association between use of online social networks sites and perceived social isolation among individuals in the second half of life: Results based on a nationally representative sample in Germany. BMC Public Health 2019, 19, 40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Aarts, S.; Peek, S.T.; Wouters, E.J. The relation between social network site usage and loneliness and mental health in community-dwelling older adults. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2015, 30, 942–949. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Thorisdottir, I.E.; Sigurvinsdottir, R.; Asgeirsdottir, B.B.; Allegrante, J.P.; Sigfusdottir, I.D. Active and Passive Social Media Use and Symptoms of Anxiety and Depressed Mood Among Icelandic Adolescents. Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw. 2019, 22, 535–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Burnell, K.; George, M.J.; Vollet, J.W.; Ehrenreich, S.E.; Underwood, M.K. Passive social networking site use and well-being: The mediating roles of social comparison and the fear of missing out. Cyberpsychology: J. Psychosoc. Res. Cyberspace 2019, 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Escobar-Viera, C.G.; Shensa, A.; Bowman, N.D.; Sidani, J.E.; Knight, J.; James, A.E.; Primack, B.A. Passive and Active Social Media Use and Depressive Symptoms Among United States Adults. Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw. 2018, 21, 437–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Gerson, J.; Plagnol, A.C.; Corr, P.J. Passive and Active Facebook Use Measure (PAUM): Validation and relationship to the Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2017, 117, 81–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Frison, E.; Eggermont, S. Exploring the Relationships Between Different Types of Facebook Use, Perceived Online Social Support, and Adolescents Depressed Mood. Soc. Sci. Comput. Rev. 2015, 34, 153–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Pertegal-Vega, M.Á.; Oliva-Delgado, A.; Rodríguez-Meirinhos, A. Systematic review of the current state of research on Online Social Networks: Taxonomy on experience of use. Comunicar. Media Educ. Res. J. 2019, 27, 81–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Burke, M.; Marlow, C.; Lento, T. Social network activity and social well-being. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, Atlanta, GA, USA, 10–15 April 2010; pp. 1909–1912. [Google Scholar]
  35. Burke, M.; Kraut, R.; Marlow, C. Social capital on facebook. In Proceedings of the 2011 Annual Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems-CHI ‘11, Vancouver, BC, Canada, 7–12 May 2011. [Google Scholar]
  36. de Jong Gierveld, J. A review of loneliness: Concept and definitions, determinants and consequences. Rev. Clin. Gerontol. 1998, 8, 73–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Mahoney, J.; Le Moignan, E.; Long, K.; Wilson, M.; Barnett, J.; Vines, J.; Lawson, S. Feeling alone among 317 million others: Disclosures of loneliness on Twitter. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2019, 98, 20–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Bailis, D.S.; Chipperfield, J.G.; Perry, R.P. Optimistic Social Comparisons of Older Adults Low in Primary Control: A Prospective Analysis of Hospitalization and Mortality. Health Psychol. 2005, 24, 393–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Correa, T.; Hinsley, A.W.; de Zúñiga, H.G. Who interacts on the Web?: The intersection of users’ personality and social media use. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2010, 26, 247–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Chatters, L.M.; Taylor, H.O.; Nicklett, E.J.; Taylor, R.J. Correlates of Objective Social Isolation from Family and Friends among Older Adults. Healthcare 2018, 6, 24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Lee, Y.; Ko, Y.-g. Feeling lonely when not socially isolated: Social isolation moderates the association between loneliness and daily social interaction. J. Soc. Pers. Relatsh. 2017, 35, 1340–1355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Stahl, J. Training social skills in groups of depressive elderly patients: Experiences of a day clinic for gerontopsychiatry and psychotherapy. Gruppenpsychother. Gruppendyn. 2004, 40, 130–147. [Google Scholar]
  43. Choi, D.H.; Noh, G.Y. The influence of social media use on attitude toward suicide through psychological well-being, social isolation, and social support. Info. Commun. Soc. 2020, 23, 1427–1443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Meshi, D.; Cotten, S.R.; Bender, A.R. Problematic Social Media Use and Perceived Social Isolation in Older Adults: A Cross-Sectional Study. Gerontology 2020, 66, 160–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Du, H.F.; Li, X.M.; Chi, P.L.; Zhao, S.R.; Zhao, J.F. Loneliness and self-Esteem in children and adolescents affected by parental HIV: A 3-year longitudinal study. Appl. Psychol. Health Well Being 2019, 11, 3–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  46. Gonzalez-Marin, P.; Puig-Barrachina, V.; Cortes-Franch, I.; Bartoll, X.; Artazcoz, L.; Malmusi, D.; Clotet, E.; Daban, F.; Diez, E.; Cardona, A.; et al. Social and material determinants of health in participants in an active labor market program in Barcelona. Arch. Public Health 2018, 76, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Tapia, A.J.; Barrios, L.M.; Gonzalez-Forteza, C. Self-esteem, depressive symptomatology, and suicidal ideation in adolescents: Results of three studies. Salud Ment. 2007, 30, 20–26. [Google Scholar]
  48. Creemers, D.H.M.; Scholte, R.H.J.; Engels, R.; Prinstein, M.J.; Wiers, R.W. Implicit and explicit self-esteem as concurrent predictors of suicidal ideation, depressive symptoms, and loneliness. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 2012, 43, 638–646. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Al-Ghafri, R.K.; Al-Badi, A.H. The Effect of Social Media on Self-Esteem. In Proceedings of the 27th International Business Information Management Association Conference, Milan, Italy, 4–5 May 2016; pp. 3228–3237. [Google Scholar]
  50. van Eldik, A.K.; Kneer, J.; Jansz, J. Urban & Online: Social Media Use among Adolescents and Sense of Belonging to a Super-Diverse City. Media Commun. 2019, 7, 242–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Djafarova, E.; Trofimenko, O. Exploring the relationships between self-presentation and self-esteem of mothers in social media in Russia. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2017, 73, 20–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Rivadeneyra, R.; Ward, L.M.; Gordon, M. Distorted reflections: Media exposure and Latino adolescents’ conceptions of self. Media Psychol. 2007, 9, 261–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Strubel, J.; Petrie, T.A.; Pookulangara, S. “Like” Me: Shopping, Self-Display, Body Image, and Social Networking Sites. Psychol. Pop. Media Cult. 2018, 7, 328–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Yuh, J. The Relationship between Loneliness and Smartphone Addiction Symptoms among Middle School Students: Testing the Mediating Role of Self-Esteem. Korean J. Youth Stud. 2016, 23, 129–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Isaksen, K.J.; Roper, S. The commodification of self-Esteem: Branding and british teenagers. Psychol. Mark. 2012, 29, 117–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Dancer, L.S. On the multidimensional structure of self-esteem: Facet analysis of Rosenberg’s self-esteem scale. In Facet Theory; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1985; pp. 223–236. [Google Scholar]
  57. Nisar, T.M.; Prabhakar, G.; Ilavarasan, P.V.; Baabdullah, A.M. Facebook usage and mental health: An empirical study of role of non-directional social comparisons in the UK. Int. J. Inf. Manag. 2019, 48, 53–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Jian, W. The Effect of Wording Effects on Factor Structure of Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale. Master’s Thesis, Guizhou Normal University, Guiyang, China, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  59. Kanten, P.; Kanten, S. The antecedents of procrastination behavior: Personality characteristics, self-esteem and self-efficacy. In Proceedings of the Global Business Research Congress (GBRC), Istanbul, Turkey, 26–27 May 2016; pp. 331–341. [Google Scholar]
  60. Fornell, C.G.; Bookstein, F.L. Two structural equation models: LISREL and PLS applied to consumer exit-voice theory. J. Mark. Res. 1982, 19, 440–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Willaby, H.W.; Costa, D.S.J.; Burns, B.D.; MacCann, C.; Roberts, R.D. Testing complex models with small sample sizes: A historical overview and empirical demonstration of what partial least squares (PLS) can offer differential psychology. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2015, 84, 73–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Hair, J.F.; Sarstedt, M.; Ringle, C.M.; Mena, J.A. An assessment of the use of partial least squares structural equation modeling in marketing research. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 2012, 40, 414–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Podsakoff, P.M.; MacKenzie, S.B.; Lee, J.-Y.; Podsakoff, N.P. Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 2003, 88, 879–903. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Podsakoff, P.M.; MacKenzie, S.B.; Podsakoff, N.P. Sources of method bias in social science research and recommendations on how to control it. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2012, 63, 539–569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Malhotra, N.K.; Kim, S.S.; Patil, A. Common method variance in is research: A comparison of alternative approaches and a reanalysis of past research. Manag. Sci. 2006, 52, 1865–1883. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Dijkstra, T.K.; Henseler, J. Consistent partial least squares path modeling. MIS Quart. 2015, 39, 297–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Hair, J.F.; Hult, G.T.M.; Ringle, C.M.; Sarstedt, M. A Primer on Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM), 2nd ed.; Sage: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  68. Henseler, J.; Ringle, C.M.; Sarstedt, M. A new criterion for assessing discriminant validity in variance-based structural equation modeling. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 2015, 43, 115–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  69. Zheng, D.Q.; Huang, L.; Kim, K.J. How ambidextrous social networking service users balance different social capital benefits: An evidence from WeChat. Internet Res. 2021, 31, 479–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Verduyn, P.; Ybarra, O.; Resibois, M.; Jonides, J.; Kross, E. Do Social Network Sites Enhance or Undermine Subjective Well-Being? A Critical Review. Soc. Issues Policy Rev. 2017, 11, 274–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. PLS-SEM results for the relationship between SMU patterns and older adults’ loneliness. Notes: SMU, social media usage; thick solid lines indicate significant effects, and thin dashed lines indicate insignificant effects; significance was assessed with the bootstrapping method (N = 2000); * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, and *** p < 0.001.
Figure 1. PLS-SEM results for the relationship between SMU patterns and older adults’ loneliness. Notes: SMU, social media usage; thick solid lines indicate significant effects, and thin dashed lines indicate insignificant effects; significance was assessed with the bootstrapping method (N = 2000); * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, and *** p < 0.001.
Ijerph 19 13232 g001
Table 1. Reliability and convergent validity.
Table 1. Reliability and convergent validity.
Cronbach’s AlphaRHO_ACRAVE
ASMU0.9180.9180.9600.924
PSMU0.9010.9070.9310.771
FSMU0.6230.6370.8400.725
OSI0.7740.7770.8990.816
CE0.6230.6520.8020.581
AE0.6480.6500.8100.588
LN0.8230.8660.8930.737
Note: ASMU, active SMU; PSMU, passive SMU; FSMU, functional SMU; CE, cognitive self-esteem; AE, affective self-esteem; OSI, objective social isolation; LN, loneliness.
Table 2. HTMT ratio of the correlations.
Table 2. HTMT ratio of the correlations.
ASMUPSMUFSMUOSICEAE
PSMU0.493
FSMU0.6770.496
OSI0.1680.1440.218
CE0.1490.1700.3580.118
AE0.1690.1340.0840.1260.675
LN0.1060.1980.1750.3300.3370.163
Note: ASMU, active SMU; PSMU, passive SMU; FSMU, functional SMU; CE, cognitive self-esteem; AE, affective self-esteem; OSI, objective social isolation; LN, loneliness.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Yang, S.; Zhang, M. Research on the Influence Mechanisms of the Affective and Cognitive Self-Esteem. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 13232. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192013232

AMA Style

Yang S, Zhang M. Research on the Influence Mechanisms of the Affective and Cognitive Self-Esteem. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(20):13232. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192013232

Chicago/Turabian Style

Yang, Shufang, and Mingyao Zhang. 2022. "Research on the Influence Mechanisms of the Affective and Cognitive Self-Esteem" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 20: 13232. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192013232

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop