Next Article in Journal
Effects of Concurrent Strength and HIIT-Based Endurance Training on Physical Fitness in Trained Team Sports Players: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis
Previous Article in Journal
Changes in Tobacco and Alcohol Consumption in France during the Spring 2020 Lockdown: Results of the Coviprev and Viquop Surveys
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Factors Influencing Cortisol Concentrations in Breastmilk and Its Associations with Breastmilk Composition and Infant Development in the First Six Months of Lactation

by
Monika A. Zielinska-Pukos
1,
Joanna Bryś
2,
Natalia Kucharz
3,
Agnieszka Chrobak
3,
Aleksandra Wesolowska
4,
Iwona Grabowicz-Chądrzyńska
5 and
Jadwiga Hamulka
1,*
1
Department of Human Nutrition, Institute of Human Nutrition Sciences, Warsaw University of Life Sciences—SGGW, Nowoursynowska St. 159, 02-776 Warsaw, Poland
2
Department of Chemistry, Institute of Food Sciences, Warsaw University of Life Sciences—SGGW, Nowoursynowska St. 159, 02-776 Warsaw, Poland
3
Hirszfeld Institute of Immunology and Experimental Therapy, Polish Academy of Sciences, 53-114 Wrocław, Poland
4
Laboratory of Human Milk and Lactation Research at Regional Human Milk Bank in Holy Family Hospital Department of Medical Biology, Medical University of Warsaw, Litewska 14/16 Str., 00-575 Warsaw, Poland
5
Psychological-Pedagogical Counselling Centre No 12, Dzielna St. 1a, 00-162 Warsaw, Poland
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(22), 14809; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192214809
Submission received: 26 October 2022 / Revised: 3 November 2022 / Accepted: 8 November 2022 / Published: 10 November 2022

Abstract

:
Previous studies provided contradictory results regarding the influence of maternal, seasonal, and infant factors on breastmilk cortisol, and its associations with breastmilk composition and infant development. This study aimed to assess breastmilk cortisol levels at the first, third, and sixth months of lactation and evaluate the associations with maternal psychosocial, seasonal, and infant factors, breastmilk composition, and infant anthropometric and psychomotor development and temperament. Cortisol concentrations were assessed by ELISA in 24 h breastmilk samples obtained from 38 healthy mothers. Maternal psychological status was assessed by EPDS and PSS-10 and infant psychomotor development was assessed using the Children’s Development Scale (DSR). Breastmilk cortisol was 11.2 ± 6.2, 11.2 ± 4.3, and 12.7 ± 6.2 ng/mL at the first, third, and sixth months of lactation (p > 0.05), respectively. In the spring-summer season, we observed lower and higher levels of cortisol in the first and sixth months of lactation (p ≤ 0.05), respectively, but no other associations were detected regarding maternal or infant characteristics. In the third month of lactation, cortisol was related to breastmilk crude protein (β = 0.318, 0.007–0.630) and infant BMI z-score before adjustment for infant birthweight and sex (Model 2: β = 0.359, 0.021–0.697), but no other associations with breastmilk composition, infant development, or temperament were confirmed. Our results indicated that breastmilk cortisol is unrelated to maternal and infant factors and has limited influence on breastmilk crude protein, but not on infant anthropometric and psychomotor development.

1. Introduction

Breastmilk is the gold standard in infant nutrition and provides not only all necessary nutrients, but also a variety of bioactive compounds that support healthy infant growth and development [1,2,3]. Among many of the bioactive factors, breastmilk contains glucocorticoids (GCs): cortisol, cortisone (an inactive cortisol metabolite), and corticosterone [4,5,6,7,8,9,10,11]. The predominant breastmilk glucocorticoid is cortisone [6,7], followed by cortisol (highly correlated with cortisone levels) [4,6,7,10], and corticosterone, which occurs in the lowest concentrations [11]. Interestingly, cortisol is the predominant GAs in serum, but it is converted to cortisone before being transferred to the saliva and breastmilk [4,12]. Thanks to lipophilic properties, GCs can cross the mammary gland epithelia through simple diffusion [8], as systemic circulation is probably their main source, as breastmilk cortisol reflects diurnal variations related to maternal HPA axis activity [6,7,8] and are highly correlated with serum [5,13] and saliva [6,13] GCs. Despite those lipophilic properties, milk GCs are mainly associated with the skim fraction, probably due to binding to proteins, e.g., corticosteroid-binding proteins, globulin, and albumin [8,9].
As breastmilk GCs reflect their serum levels, it was hypothesized that breastmilk GCs are an important biochemical signal of environmental conditions from mother to infant. According to the lactational programming hypothesis, they affect metabolic and neurobiological development, and, in consequence, offspring phenotype and behavior [13,14,15,16]. Studies investigated maternal psychological [10,15,17,18,19,20,21,22], socio-demographic [7,10,18,23], or anthropometric [3,10,24] determinants of breastmilk GCs levels provide inconclusive results. Similarly, results regarding the mode of delivery [5,10], preterm birth [6,10,17] or seasonal variations [10,23] were inconsistent.
GCs not only are involved in the stress response but also play a variety of important biological functions, including an improvement of intestinal maturation, microbiome, macronutrients metabolism, growth, body composition, and neurodevelopment [8,25,26]. They may be particularly important during early development, as GCs receptors are found in higher densities in the intestines in infancy and decrease after weaning [13]. Interestingly, it was shown that breastmilk feeding was associated with a 5.5-fold increase in GC expression compared to formula feeding [16]. Moreover, breastfeeding was associated with decreased methylation of the GC receptor gene (NR3C1), and, in turn, was linked to decreased cortisol stress reactivity, which may be responsible for many breastfeeding health benefits [27]. Previous primate and human studies showed that breastmilk cortisol was associated with infant temperament and fear reactivity, but the direction of those associations was inconsistent and sometimes sex-specific [13,15,19,25,26]. Additionally, due to the regulation of macronutrient metabolism and immune system breastmilk, cortisol may affect breastmilk composition [9,21,24,26,28,29], but these results are also unclear, thus further studies are necessary.
The aim of this study was to evaluate breastmilk cortisol levels at 1, 3, and 6 months of lactation, as well as established its associations with: (1) seasonal, maternal socioeconomic, psychosocial, and infant factors; (2) breastmilk macronutrient composition, energy value, and fatty acids profile; (3) infant anthropometric, psychomotor development, and temperament.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Design and Data Collection

This observational prospective study conducted in the central area of Poland between 2015–2017 consisted of three study visits during the first, third, and sixth months of lactation. Individuals who were 19 years or older, who gave birth to healthy, at term, single infants no later than 6 weeks, and planned to exclusively breastfeed for 6 months were eligible to participate. More details about the study design and the methods used have been previously described [30,31]. Briefly, data about maternal nutrition were collected using a 3 day food record at the 3rd and 6th months of lactation [31]. Anthropometric measurements in mothers (body weight and height) were conducted according to the ISAK standards [32] using a professional stadiometer (SECA 799, Hamburg, Germany) and analyzed using body mass index according to the WHO [33]. Maternal psychosocial characteristics were assessed at the first, third, and sixth months of lactation using the Polish versions of the Edinburgh Postpartum Scale (EPDS) [34,35] and the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS-10) [36,37]. In the EPDS, 12 points of the total score were the cut-off point for the risk of postpartum depression. Infant anthropometric measurements (weight and length) were measured at the third and sixth months of life using the neonatal scale (SECA 728) and anthropometric tape (SECA 203) according to WHO recommendations [38]. Further, infant BMI and body mass to age, length to age, and BMI to age z-score were calculated using the WHO Anthro Survey Analyzer Software v3.2.2 [39]. A children’s psychologist assessed infant psychomotor development and temperament at the sixth month of life using the Children’s Development Scale DSR [40]. More details about infant psychomotor development were described previously [30].

2.2. Participants

Fifty-three mother–infant pairs participated in the study; however, 47 pairs completed all three study visits, and 9 pairs were excluded from the analysis. Thus, in this paper, 38 mother–infant pairs were included (Figure 1).
Mothers included in this analysis were well educated, mostly married, and in a good economic situation, with normal BMI (Table 1). Half of them had given birth to one healthy, male infant with normal development during the first six months of life.

2.3. Breastmilk Sample Collection

Mothers who participated in the study collected breastmilk samples at home prior to the study visits at the first, third, and sixth months of lactation according to the received instructions. A sample of 5–10 mL of pre- and post-feeding breastmilk was collected manually or expressed by an electric pump at four time periods (6:00–12:00; 12:00–18:00; 18:00–24:00; 24:00–06:00) into separate polypropylene containers 24 h prior to the study visit. The samples were transported to the laboratory under cooling conditions, and the same amount of each sample was collected and mixed in a Vortex shaker IKA MS2 (IKA Works Inc., Wilmington, NC, USA) for one minute. The pooled sample was stored in 2 mL aliquots in −80 °C for later analysis.

2.4. Breastmilk Chemical Analysis

Breastmilk micronutrients and energy value were analyzed using MIRIS human milk analyzer HMA (Miris, Uppsala, Sweden), whereas breastmilk fatty acid profiles were analyzed using gas chromatograph after Folch extraction with modifications as described previously.
For the cortisol analysis, a 2 mL of breastmilk sample was thawed in water at bathroom temperature and sonicated (3 × 10 s, 20 s pause). Then, a 200 µL milk sample was diluted in 200 µL of saline. Breastmilk cortisol levels were determined by using commercial enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA) kit (Salivary Cortisol ELISA SLV2930, DRG Instruments GmbH, Marburg, Germany) with the sensitivity of 0.09 ng/mL and detection range of 0.09–30.0 ng/mL. This kit has <0.001% cross-reactivity with testosterone, DHEA-S, estrone (E1), estradiol (E2), 1.47% with estriol (E3), 1.97% with androstenedione, 11.39% with DHEA and 23.40% with progesterone. Analysis was conducted in two parallel repetitions at the Hirszfeld Institute of Immunology and Experimental Therapy. Cortisol values were expressed as ng/mL and the average of the two measurements was used for further statistical analysis.

2.5. Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses were performed with STATISTICA ver. 13.3 (TIBCO Software Inc., Paolo Alto, CA, USA). Data about breastmilk composition, results of EPDS, PSS-10, and DSR scale data were log-transformed prior to statistical analysis. All quantitative data were checked for normality of distribution using the Shapiro–Wilk test and expressed as mean ± standard deviation (SD). Differences in cortisol concentrations between the study visits were assessed using ANOVA for repeated measurement tests, whereas differences between two groups (e.g., season of breastmilk collection) using the Student’s t-test. Partial correlations and linear regression analysis were used to assess associations between breastmilk cortisol and maternal and infant factors, as well as breastmilk composition and infant anthropometrics, psychomotor development, and temperament. A p-value below 0.05 was considered significant in all the conducted analyses.

3. Results

3.1. Breastmilk Cortisol Level and Its Determinants

Breastmilk cortisol was stable during the first sixth months of lactation (Table 2). In the first and sixth months of lactation, we observed no consistent significant differences in cortisol according to seasonal variation—in the first month of lactation breastmilk, cortisol levels were higher in the autumn-winter season, whereas they were lower in the sixth month of lactation in the spring-summer season (p ≤ 0.05). No significant differences were found according to parity, maternal overweight and obesity, self-assessment of the economic situation and average income, infant sex, and mode of delivery (Table S1).
Breastmilk cortisol levels within the first sixth months of lactation were not correlated to any maternal and infant factors, including maternal psychological characteristics—perceived stress and risk of postpartum depression (Table 3).

3.2. Breastmilk Cortisol and Breastmilk Macronutrient and FA Profile

Partial correlation analysis, adjusted for the season of breastmilk sample collection, showed a moderate association between cortisol and crude protein in the third month postpartum, and cortisol and total solids in the sixth month postpartum (Table 3).
Linear regression models, adjusted for season of breastmilk collection, infant age, and number of breastfeedings per day, confirmed associations between breastmilk cortisol and crude protein in the third month of lactation (β = 0.395, 0.084–0.705; Table 4). However, associations with total solids were not confirmed in linear regression analysis.
No significant correlations were observed between breastmilk cortisol and FA profile within the first sixth months of lactation (Table S2).

3.3. Breastmilk Cortisol and Infant Anthropometric Development

Partial correlations, adjusted for the season of breastmilk collection, breastmilk energy value, and crude protein, showed that cortisol levels in the third month of lactation were moderately correlated with BMI z-score in the third and sixth months of life in the total group (r = 0.412 and 0.372, p ≤ 0.05, respectively). In a separate analysis according to infant sex, those associations were observed only in the third month of life in girls (Table 5).
Associations between breastmilk cortisol in the third month of lactation and infant BMI z-score in the third and sixth months of life were partially confirmed in the multivariate linear regression analysis (Table 6). In the third month of lactation, results were statistically significant after adjusting for average breastmilk energy value (Model 2: β = 0.359, 0.021–0.697).

3.4. Breastmilk Cortisol Level and Infant Psychomotor Development and Temperament

Partial correlation analysis controlled for infant sex, age, birthweight, maternal age, education, psychological status, and parity (Table 7) showed moderate adverse associations between breastmilk cortisol in the first and sixth months of lactation and infant regularity in the sixth month of life (r = −0.379 and r = 0.421, respectively, p ≤ 0.05). However, separate analyses conducted for boys and girls did not confirm this association. In girls, breastmilk cortisol in the first and sixth months of lactation moderately correlated (r = 0.634 and r = 0.636, respectively, p ≤ 0.05) with the centile of performance scale—which reflects psychomotor development. In boys, we observed significant associations between cortisol in the first month of lactation and sensitivity in the sixth month of life (r = 0.580, respectively, p ≤ 0.05).

4. Discussion

In this prospective study, we showed that breastmilk cortisol levels in the first, third, and sixth months of lactation were unrelated to any maternal or infant factors. We observed that only seasonal variations were associated with cortisol in the breastmilk from the first to six months of lactation, but the observed interactions were inconsistent. Breastmilk cortisol was moderately associated with breastmilk crude protein in the third month of lactation, but associations with other macronutrients and breastmilk FA were not observed. Moreover, in our study, we detected limited associations between breastmilk cortisol in the third month of lactation and infant BMI z-score, but not infant psychomotor development and temperament.

4.1. Factors Associated with Breastmilk Cortisol

The cortisol levels observed in our study varied insignificantly from 11.2 to 12.7 ng/mL from the first to the sixth month of lactation. Those results are in line with other authors who reported that cortisol levels were highest in colostrum and relatively stable in transitional and mature milk [4,18]. In contrast, a study from the Netherlands reported that breastmilk cortisol increased from two to 12 weeks of lactation [20,41]. Cortisol levels observed in our study were higher than those observed in other studies [6,10,15,19,21,22,23,24,41,42], but lower than those reported in some studies [7,20,43]. Moreover, we observed significant differences between cortisol levels between the season of sample collection—higher levels of cortisol were observed in samples collected during the autumn-winter season in the first month of lactation, but in the sixth month of lactation in the spring-summer season. Other studies provide inconclusive results—one reported no variations [10], whereas another from Finland showed higher levels in summer [23]. Previously, seasonal cortisol variations in plasma or saliva were reported in regions with clear seasonal variations in light and climate, with the highest levels in spring [44,45], summer [46], or fall/winter [47]. Discrepancies between studies assessing breastmilk cortisol may be related to variations in cortisol analysis methods, as in immunoassays, other steroids may cross-react and LC-MS/MS is the most specific [8,42]. However, the results observed in our study were lower than the results obtained by both methods [7,20,43]. Another possible explanation for those differences in sample collection was that glucocorticosteroids in breastmilk reflect HPA axis activity with a peak early in the morning [6,7,8,20,22]. Breastmilk samples in our study were collected four times per day to minimize diurnal variations in breastmilk cortisol, but morning samples could be collected between 6:00 a.m. and 12:00 p.m.

4.1.1. Maternal Factors

In our group, breastmilk cortisol across the first six months of lactation was unrelated to maternal age, self-assessed economic situation, and average income per capita—which is in line with previous studies [10,18,23]. Interestingly, previous studies showed that low maternal education was significantly associated with higher cortisol levels [10,23], which confirms that education plays an important role in individual stress response [48]. However, the mothers who participated in our study were very well-educated, so we were unable to explore these associations. According to previous human [23] and primate [26] studies, primiparas compared to multiparas mothers have higher cortisol levels—probably due to differences in the mammary gland physiology [26]. However, our study and studies by Pundir et al. [7,10] did not confirm these results. Moreover, maternal pre-pregnancy BMI was associated with higher cortisol levels [24], and other studies reported higher cortisone—but not cortisol levels—in mothers with normal weight mothers compared to underweight or overweight [10]. However, our results and those by Hinde et al. [26] from rhesus monkeys also did not confirm associations between maternal BMI and breastmilk cortisol.
Many previous studies tried to explore the associations between maternal psychological characteristics—especially depression or stress—on cortisol levels in breastmilk [10,15,18,19,20,21,22,23], saliva [12,28], serum [12,43], hair [49], and urine [12]. It was reported that maternal hostility [18] and breastfeeding satisfaction [17] were positively correlated with breastmilk cortisol. However, the majority of those studies reported no associations between stress or anxiety and breastmilk [10,15,19,21], saliva [22,28], and hair [49] cortisol, which was in line with our observations based on breastmilk cortisol. On the contrary, Aparicio et al. [20] reported higher breastmilk in mothers with increased stress and anxiety but not depressive symptoms. Similarly to Aparicio et al. [20], we and other researchers did not observe a significant association between maternal depression and breastmilk [10,15,19,21,23,43], serum [12,43], saliva [12], hair [49], and urine [12] cortisol. However, Romjin et al. [22] reported that patients in a psychiatry-obstetric-pediatric clinic had a lower milk cortisol area under the curve compared to controls and that breastmilk and saliva cortisol was highly correlated, but no difference in glucocorticosteroid rhythmicity was observed. Those results indicated that maternal stress did not affect cortisol transport into the mammary gland [22]. The predominant lack of any associations between maternal stress or depression and breastmilk cortisol may be related to several factors. Firstly, large variability in the study design (including the characteristics of the study group), the methods used, and outcomes of maternal psychological state may lead to differences in the sensitivity of the method and, in consequence, hamper detecting associations and the comparison of results [12,20,22,23]. Secondly, during acute stress, the HPA axis is triggered, which leads to elevation of glucocorticosteroid levels; however, chronic stress may lead to the exhaustion of the HPA axis related to a decrease in glucocorticosteroid levels and responsiveness to stressors [12,22].

4.1.2. Infant- and Delivery-Related Factors

In our study, we did not observe any associations or differences in breastmilk cortisol in terms of infant sex, birthweight, gestational age, or mode of delivery—which is in line with previous studies. Studies among humans [7,19,23] and rhesus macaques [26] reported no differences according to infant sex, with one exception, where mothers of male rhesus had higher cortisol levels in the milk but not plasma [13]. Results regarding mode of delivery provided contradictory results—in one study, differences were not observed [10], whereas another study reported differences in cortisol changes in the postpartum period and higher correlations to plasma after cesarean delivery [5].

4.2. Breastmilk Cortisol and Breastmilk Composition

Glucocorticosteroids are crucial for lactogenesis and contribute to nutrient metabolism and the regulation of a variety of physiological processes in the mammary gland, including protein synthesis, nutrient flux, prevention of cell death in the mammary gland, and milk yield [7,26,50,51,52,53]. Thus, breastmilk cortisol levels may affect breastmilk composition of macronutrients, the fatty acid profile, or immune factors. Previous studies reported associations with secretory immunoglobin A (sIgA) and breastmilk cortisol [17,18], but recent studies did not confirm associations between salivary [29] or breastmilk cortisol [20] and sIgA, as well as other immunological components of breastmilk. In our study, we observed a positive association between breastmilk cortisol and crude protein at three months of lactation—which was significant after adjustment for the season of breastmilk collection, infant age, and the number of breastfeedings. These results are in line with studies on rhesus monkeys [13,26], but other human studies did not confirm these observations [9,29]. Moreover, primate studies showed positive associations also to breastmilk fat [13,26], and human studies showed positive associations between fat and salivary cortisol [28] and negative associations between lactose and salivary [28] or cord blood [54] cortisol. We also explored associations between breastmilk cortisol and breastmilk FA profile, but we did not observe any significant correlations at any lactation stage. Previous studies reported higher levels of lauric and myristic FA in the breastmilk samples with high breastmilk cortisol levels [24]. In another study, breastmilk long chain fatty acids, monounsaturated fatty acids, and polyunsaturated fatty acids were positively associated with salivary cortisol but not maternal stress, whereas medium chain fatty acids were related only to maternal stress, not cortisol levels [28].
Previous human and dairy animal studies showed that cortisol may affect protein, glucose, and lipid metabolism and, in consequence, increase the availability of glucose and fatty acids, which are an essential precursor for milk production [50,55,56,57,58]. An in vivo study confirmed that cortisol regulates protein metabolism in the mammary epithelial cells [52], which explains the positive association observed in our study. Moreover, this association was time-dependent, as we observed it at the third, but not first and sixth months of lactation. Previous animal studies showed that cortisol’s effect on milk yield may be dependent on the lactation stage [53], but it was not shown in studies regarding milk composition, and the majority of studies were short-term or cross-sectional [9,13,26,28,51]. Our study did not confirm the influence of cortisol on milk lipids, despite that the influence of cortisol on lipolysis, the release of medium and intermediate chain FA, and the synthesis of long-chain FA had been previously shown [24,58,59,60]. A possible explanation for the discrepancies between the studies could be differences in the method of sample collection. We collected fore- and hindmilk samples multiple times per day, whereas in other human studies only foremilk from all feedings during the day [9] or milk from the full breast between the second and third feedings [28] was collected. Moreover, it was shown that the synthesis of different nutrients in the mammary gland is regulated independently and multidirectional [51,54,61], which may explain why we did not detect any associations with lactose and fat. Furthermore, this may indicate that macronutrients and fatty acids are transported into milk independently of cortisol [9].

4.3. Breastmilk Cortisol and Infant Anthropometric Development

We also investigated associations between breastmilk cortisol and infant anthropometric development. We found a positive association between breastmilk cortisol in the third month of lactation and infant BMI z-score in the third and sixth months of lactation; but after adjustment for infant birthweight and sex, those associations were no longer significant. Our results are in line with the study from the Netherlands, where the authors found no associations between diurnal cortisol rhythm in the first month and infant growth and body composition at the third month of lactation [9]. Furthermore, a study from the USA found a negative effect between breastmilk cortisol at the third month of lactation and BMI percentile gain in the first two years of life, and this influence was stronger in girls than boys [62]. However, in this study, only a single breastmilk sample was collected between 11:30 a.m. and 4:00 p.m., which may result in high variability and may be insufficient to analyze cortisol transmission via breastmilk [63]. Interestingly, the primate study by Hinde et al. [26] reported associations between greater daily growth and higher cortisol during lactation peak or lower cortisol and higher available milk energy at early lactation. Possible discrepancies between studies may be a result of differences in sample collection (both number of samples and timing of collection) as well as cortisol analysis methods. A recent meta-analysis of associations found a significant association between hair (but not serum or salivary) cortisol and children and BMI z-score; however, only in studies extracting cortisol via more sensitive and reliable LC-MS/MS, and not ELISA [64]. However, Hollanders et al. [9] used the LC-MS/MS method, but also did not observe significant associations. Probably, the small sample size in the Hollanders et al. [9] and our study (n = 42 and n = 38, respectively), as well as the short duration of observation (3 and 6 months, respectively), was too small to detect significant associations that may be noticeable in further infant development. Animal studies suggest that cortisol improves intestinal development—including the microbiome and intestinal immune system—which in consequence may affect anthropometric development [8,63,65].

4.4. Breastmilk Cortisol and Infant Psychomotor Development and Temperament

Cortisol may also affect the brain and, in consequence, cognitive and behavioral development [13,15,16,18,19,25,26,32,66]. Thus, in our study, we also explored the associations between breastmilk cortisol and infant psychomotor development and temperament in the sixth month of life. In the total group, we observed negative associations between regularity in the biological and behavioral patterns and breastmilk cortisol in the first and sixth months of lactation. Moreover, in girls, we detected a positive association between psychomotor development and breastmilk cortisol in the first and sixth months of lactation; whereas, in boys, we detected positive associations between sensory sensitivity and breastmilk cortisol in the first month of lactation. However, those gender- and time-specific associations were only significant in partial correlations analysis adjusted for infant sex (in the total group) and age, birthweight maternal age, education, psychological status, and parity. Previous human and animal studies showed that breastmilk or plasma cortisol may be associated with negative behavioral outcomes, such as nervous and less confident temperament [26], higher fear or negative reactivity [15,19,25], impulsivity on the cognitive task [16], and some of the studies found this only in girls [15,19]. Some positive associations were also observed between cortisol and infant neurobiological development and temperament. Studies on rhesus macaques found that higher breastmilk cortisol was related to a more confident temperament in sons [13] and frequency of play in girls, and human studies found a positive association with newborn homeostatic adjustments of the nervous system and better capacity to control involuntary responses (changes in skin color, tremors, and startles) [18]. However, in two human studies, no associations with behavior or sleep pattern [66] or infant crying [41] were found. Rodent studies confirmed associations between maternal cortisol and cognitive and behavioral development. However, cortisol also had a positive impact on fear behavior and cognitive and motor development, contrary to human studies [67,68,69,70]. Discrepancies between studies may be related to interspecies differences as well as variability in study design, including the timing of outcomes assessment, such as gender- and time-specific glucocorticosteroid sensitivity [16,25,26,41]. Moreover, different methods of cognitive, behavior, and temperament assessment were used and these may have a different sensitivity, which could hamper the detection of associations [31].

4.5. Strengths and Limitations

Our study has several strengths. First, the study analyzed breastmilk samples obtained from three time points over the first six months of lactation: first, third, and sixth month. Second, the analyzed breastmilk samples contained both fore- and hindmilk in equal proportions. Third, mothers collected four samples per study point (6:00–12:00; 12:00–18:00; 18:00–24:00; 24:00–6:00), which decreased the influence of diurnal variations in the HPA axis [6,7,8]. However, we pooled the same amount of each sample, so the specific analysis of diurnal variations in cortisol and the calculation of an area under the curve was impossible. Fourth, we analyzed breastmilk macronutrients and FA profiles. Fifth, we collected quite extensive data on mother–infant pairs who participated in the study. Sixth, we assessed infant anthropometric parameters at two time points using the WHO standards.
However, our study is not without limitations. First, our study sample was relatively small. Second, our sample was very well educated and with higher socioeconomic status than the national average, as well as breastfeeding for a longer period [71]. This may be related to self-selection bias and results with non-participating mothers with poorer psychological status; thus, some effects could not be detected, and results should be generalized with caution. Third, we assessed only breastmilk cortisol, which is more active than cortisone and has direct links with metabolism and neurodevelopment but occurs in breastmilk in lower concentrations. Fourth, we analyzed cortisol using the ELISA method, which is less specific compared to the LC-tandem MS assay, but our results were comparable to some studies using this method [7,42].

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, this study found limited associations between breastmilk cortisol and seasonal factors, and breastmilk crude protein and infant BMI z-score. On the other hand, we did not confirm associations between breastmilk cortisol and maternal socioeconomic, psychosocial, infant factors, or other breastmilk macronutrients, energy value, fatty acid profile, and infant psychomotor development and temperament. Our results support most previous human studies. Hence, further follow-up studies on a large mother–infant pair population with serial cortisol measurements over a longer period of time are necessary.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ijerph192214809/s1, Table S1: Breastmilk cortisol at 1, 3, and 6 months postpartum according to maternal parity, infant sex, mode of delivery, and season of breastmilk collection. Table S2: Partial correlations between the breastmilk cortisol (log) and breastmilk fatty acid profile (log) adjusted for season of breastmilk collection.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.A.Z.-P. and J.H.; methodology, M.A.Z.-P., J.H., A.W., J.B., I.G.-C. and A.C.; software, M.A.Z.-P., J.H. and J.B.; validation, M.A.Z.-P. and J.H.; formal analysis, M.A.Z.-P. and J.H.; investigation, M.A.Z.-P., J.B., I.G.-C., A.C. and N.K.; resources, J.H., A.W., J.B., I.G.-C. and A.C.; data curation, M.A.Z.-P. and J.H.; writing—original draft preparation, M.A.Z.-P.; writing—review and editing, M.A.Z.-P., J.B., A.C. and J.H.; visualization, M.A.Z.-P.; supervision, J.H.; project administration, M.A.Z.-P. and J.H.; funding acquisition, M.A.Z.-P., J.H. and A.W. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was financed by the Polish Ministry of Education and Science with funds from the Institute of Human Nutrition Sciences of the Warsaw University of Life Sciences (WULS), for scientific research.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Medical University of Warsaw (Resolution No. AKBE/139/15 in 2015).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all of the mothers and infants for participating in the study. Thanks are also extended to the Holy Family Hospital in Warsaw, especially to Elżbieta Łodykowska for her help in participant recruitment.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Victora, C.G.; Bahl, R.; Barros, A.J.D.; França, G.V.A.; Horton, S.; Krasevec, J.; Murch, S.; Sankar, M.J.; Walker, N.; Rollins, N.C. Breastfeeding in the 21st century: Epidemiology, mechanisms, and lifelong effect. Lancet 2016, 387, 475–490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Boix-Amorós, A.; Collado, M.C.; Van’t Land, B.; Calvert, A.; Le Doare, K.; Garssen, J.; Hanna, H.; Khaleva, E.; Peroni, D.G.; Geddes, D.T.; et al. Reviewing the evidence on breast milk composition and immunological outcomes. Nutr. Rev. 2019, 77, 541–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Gregg, B.; Ellsworth, L.; Pavela, G.; Shah, K.; Berger, P.K.; Isganaitis, E.; VanOmen, S.; Demerath, E.W.; Fields, D.A.; David Fields, C.A. Bioactive compounds in mothers milk affecting offspring outcomes: A narrative review. Pediatr. Obes. 2022, 17, e12892. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Kulski, J.K.; Hartmann, P.E. Changes in the concentration of cortisol in milk during different stages of human lactation. Aust. J. Exp. Biol. Med. Sci. 1981, 59, 769–778. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Patacchioli, F.R.; Cigliana, G.; Cilumbriello, A.; Perrone, G.; Capri, O.; Alemà, S.; Zichella, L.; Angelucci, L. Maternal Plasma and Milk Free Cortisol during the First 3 Days of Breast-Feeding following Spontaneous Delivery or Elective Cesarean Section. Gynecol. Obstet. Investig. 1992, 34, 159–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Van der Voorn, B.; de Waard, M.; van Goudoever, J.B.; Rotteveel, J.; Heijboer, A.C.; Finken, M.J. Breast-Milk Cortisol and Cortisone Concentrations Follow the Diurnal Rhythm of Maternal Hypothalamus-Pituitary-Adrenal Axis Activity. J. Nutr. 2016, 146, 2174–2179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Pundir, S.; Wall, C.R.; Mitchell, C.J.; Thorstensen, E.B.; Lai, C.T.; Geddes, D.T.; Cameron-Smith, D. Variation of Human Milk Glucocorticoids over 24 hour Period. J. Mammary Gland Biol. Neoplasia 2017, 22, 85–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Hollanders, J.J.; Heijboer, A.C.; van der Voorn, B.; Rotteveel, J.; Finken, M.J.J. Nutritional programming by glucocorticoids in breast milk: Targets, mechanisms and possible implications. Best Pract. Res. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2017, 31, 397–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Hollanders, J.J.; Kouwenhoven, S.M.P.; van der Voorn, B.; van Goudoever, J.B.; Rotteveel, J.; Finken, M.J.J. The Association between Breastmilk Glucocorticoid Concentrations and Macronutrient Contents throughout the Day. Nutrients 2019, 11, 259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Pundir, S.; Mäkelä, J.; Nuora, A.; Junttila, N.; Wall, C.R.; Linderborg, K.; Cameron-Smith, D.; Lagström, H. Maternal influences on the glucocorticoid concentrations of human milk: The STEPS study. Clin. Nutr. 2019, 38, 1913–1920. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Zietek, M.; Sochaczewska, D.; Swiatkowska-Freund, M.; Celewicz, Z.; Szczuko, M. The possible role of corticosterone in regulating sodium and potassium concentrations in human milk. Ginekol. Pol. 2021, 92, 812–817. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Seth, S.; Lewis, A.J.; Galbally, M. Perinatal maternal depression and cortisol function in pregnancy and the postpartum period: A systematic literature review. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth 2016, 16, 124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Sullivan, E.C.; Hinde, K.; Mendoza, S.P.; Capitanio, J.P. Cortisol concentrations in the milk of rhesus monkey mothers are associated with confident temperament in sons, but not daughters. Dev. Psychobiol. 2011, 53, 96–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Hinde, K. Lactational programming of infant behavioral phenotype. In Building Babies; Developments in Primatology: Progress and Prospects; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2013; pp. 187–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Nolvi, S.; Uusitupa, H.M.; Bridgett, D.J.; Pesonen, H.; Aatsinki, A.K.; Kataja, E.L.; Korja, R.; Karlsson, H.; Karlsson, L. Human milk cortisol concentration predicts experimentally induced infant fear reactivity: Moderation by infant sex. Dev. Sci. 2017, 21, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Dettmer, A.M.; Murphy, A.M.; Guitarra, D.; Slonecker, E.; Suomi, S.J.; Rosenberg, K.L.; Novak, M.A.; Meyer, J.S.; Hinde, K. Cortisol in Neonatal Mother’s Milk Predicts Later Infant Social and Cognitive Functioning in Rhesus Monkeys. Child Dev. 2018, 89, 525–538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Groer, M.W.; Humenick, S.; Hill, P.D. Characterizations and psychoneuroimmunologic implications of secretory immunoglobulin A and cortisol in preterm and term breast milk. J. Perinat. Neonatal Nurs. 1994, 7, 42–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Hart, S.; Boylan, L.M.; Border, B.; Carroll, S.R.; McGunegle, D.; Lampe, R.M. Breast milk levels of cortisol and Secretory Immunoglobulin A (SIgA) differ with maternal mood and infant neuro-behavioral functioning. Infant Behav. Dev. 2004, 27, 101–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Grey, K.R.; Davis, E.P.; Sandman, C.A.; Glynn, L.M. Human milk cortisol is associated with infant temperament. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2013, 38, 1178–1185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Aparicio, M.; Browne, P.D.; Hechler, C.; Beijers, R.; Rodríguez, J.M.; de Weerth, C.; Fernández, L. Human milk cortisol and immune factors over the first three postnatal months: Relations to maternal psychosocial distress. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0233554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Kortesniemi, M.; Slupsky, C.M.; Aatsinki, A.K.; Sinkkonen, J.; Karlsson, L.; Linderborg, K.M.; Yang, B.; Karlsson, H.; Kailanto, H.M. Human milk metabolome is associated with symptoms of maternal psychological distress and milk cortisol. Food Chem. 2021, 356, 129628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Romijn, M.; van Tilburg, L.J.L.; Hollanders, J.J.; van der Voorn, B.; de Goede, P.; Dolman, K.M.; Heijboer, A.C.; Broekman, B.F.P.; Rotteveel, J.; Finken, M.J.J. The Association between Maternal Stress and Glucocorticoid Rhythmicity in Human Milk. Nutrients 2021, 13, 1608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Lindberg, M.; Nolvi, S.; Härkönen, J.; Aatsinki, A.-K.; Karlsson, L.; Karlsson, H.; Uusitupa, H.-M. Associations between maternal socioeconomic, psychosocial and seasonal factors, infant characteristics and human milk cortisol concentrations. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 2021, 33, e23561. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Linderborg, K.M.; Kortesniemi, M.; Aatsinki, A.-K.; Karlsson, L.; Karlsson, H.; Yang, B.; Uusitupa, H.-M. Interactions between cortisol and lipids in human milk. Int. Breastfeed. J. 2020, 15, 66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Glynn, L.M.; Davis, E.P.; Schetter, C.D.; Chicz-DeMet, A.; Hobel, C.J.; Sandman, C.A. Postnatal maternal cortisol levels predict temperament in healthy breastfed infants. Early Hum. Dev. 2007, 83, 675–681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Hinde, K.; Skibiel, A.L.; Foster, A.B.; Del Rosso, L.; Mendoza, S.P.; Capitanio, J.P. Cortisol in mother’s milk across lactation reflects maternal life history and predicts infant temperament. Behav. Ecol. 2015, 26, 269–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Lester, B.M.; Conradt, E.; Lagasse, L.L.; Tronick, E.Z.; Padbury, J.F.; Marsit, C.J. Epigenetic Programming by Maternal Behavior in the Human Infant. Pediatrics 2018, 142, 20171890. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Ziomkiewicz, A.; Babiszewska, M.; Apanasewicz, A.; Piosek, M.; Wychowaniec, P.; Cierniak, A.; Barbarska, O.; Szołtysik, M.; Danel, D.; Wichary, S. Psychosocial stress and cortisol stress reactivity predict breast milk composition. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 11576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Ziomkiewicz, A.; Apanasewicz, A.; Danel, D.P.; Babiszewska, M.; Piosek, M.; Orczyk-Pawiłowicz, M. Maternal Distress and Social Support Are Linked to Human Milk Immune Properties. Nutrients 2021, 13, 1857. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Zielinska, M.; Hamulka, J.; Grabowicz-Chądrzyńska, I.; Bryś, J.; Wesolowska, A. Association between Breastmilk LC PUFA, Carotenoids and Psychomotor Development of Exclusively Breastfed Infants. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 1144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Zielinska, M.; Hamulka, J.; Wesolowska, A. Carotenoid Content in Breastmilk in the 3rd and 6th Month of Lactation and Its Associations with Maternal Dietary Intake and Anthropometric Characteristics. Nutrients 2019, 11, 193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. ISAK. International Standards for Anthropometric Assessment; International Society for the Advancement of Kinanthropometry: Potchefstroom, South Africa, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  33. World Health Organization. Obesity: Preventing and Managing the Global Epidemic; WHO Technical Report Series 894; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2000; 252p. [Google Scholar]
  34. Cox, J.L.; Holden, J.M.; Sagovsky, R. Detection of Postnatal Depression: Development of the 10-item Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. Br. J. Psychiatry 1987, 150, 782–786. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Bielawska-Batorowicz, E. Determinanty Spostrzegania Dziecka Przez Rodziców w Okresie Poporodowym; Wydawnictwo Uniwersytetu Łódzkiego: Łódź, Poland, 1995. [Google Scholar]
  36. Cohen, S.; Kamarck, T.; Mermelstein, R. A global measure of perceived stress. J. Health Soc. Behav. 1983, 24, 385–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Juczyński, Z.; Ogińska-Bulik, N. PSS-10–Perceived Stress Scale; Psychological Test Laboratory of the Polish Psychological Association: Warsaw, Poland, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  38. WHO. Training Course of Child Growth Assessment. WHO Child Growth Standards; Department of Nutrition for Health and Development: Geneva, Switzerland, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  39. WHO. WHO Anthro Survey Analyser and Other Tools. Available online: https://www.who.int/tools/child-growth-standards/software (accessed on 23 February 2022).
  40. Matczak, A.; Jaworowska, A.; Ciechanowicz, A.; Fecenec, D.; Stańczak, J.; Zalewska, E. DSR—Children Development Scale; Psychological Test Laboratory of the Polish Psychological Association: Warsaw, Poland, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  41. Hechler, C.; Beijers, R.; Riksen-Walraven, J.M.; de Weerth, C. Are cortisol concentrations in human breast milk associated with infant crying? Dev. Psychobiol. 2018, 60, 639–650. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Van der Voorn, B.; Martens, F.; Peppelman, N.S.; Rotteveel, J.; Blankenstein, M.A.; Finken, M.J.J.; Heijboer, A.C. Determination of cortisol and cortisone in human mother’s milk. Clin. Chim. Acta 2015, 444, 154–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Dombrowska-Pali, A.; Chrustek, A.; Gebuza, G.; Kaźmierczak, M. Analysis of cortisol levels in breast milk and blood serum in women with symptoms of postpartum depression. Med. Ogólna Nauk. Zdrowiu 2022, 28, 165–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Persson, R.; Garde, A.H.; Hansen, Å.M.; Österberg, K.; Larsson, B.; Ørbæk, P.; Karlson, B. Seasonal variation in human salivary cortisol concentration. Chronobiol. Int. 2008, 25, 923–937. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Miller, R.; Stalder, T.; Jarczok, M.; Almeida, D.M.; Badrick, E.; Bartels, M.; Boomsma, D.I.; Coe, C.L.; Dekker, M.C.J.; Donzella, B.; et al. The CIRCORT database: Reference ranges and seasonal changes in diurnal salivary cortisol derived from a meta-dataset comprised of 15 field studies. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2016, 73, 16–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Kanikowska, D.; Roszak, M.; Rutkowski, R.; Sato, M.; Sikorska, D.; Orzechowska, Z.; Breborowicz, A.; Witowski, J. Seasonal differences in rhythmicity of salivary cortisol in healthy adults. J. Appl. Physiol. 2018, 126, 764–770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Weitzman, E.D.; DeGraaf, A.S.; Sassin, J.F.; Hansen, T.; Godtlibsen, O.B.; Perlow, M.; Hellman, L. Seasonal patterns of sleep stages and secretion of cortisol and growth hormone during 24 hour periods in northern Norway. Acta Endocrinol. 1975, 78, 65–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Braig, S.; Grabher, F.; Ntomchukwu, C.; Reister, F.; Stalder, T.; Kirschbaum, C.; Genuneit, J.; Rothenbacher, D. Determinants of maternal hair cortisol concentrations at delivery reflecting the last trimester of pregnancy. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2015, 52, 289–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Galbally, M.; van Rossum, E.F.C.; Watson, S.J.; de Kloet, E.R.; Lewis, A.J. Trans-generational stress regulation: Mother-infant cortisol and maternal mental health across the perinatal period. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2019, 109, 104374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Stewart, H.J.; Thompson, G.E. Adrenocorticotrophic hormone stimulation of mammary secretion in lactating goats independent of increased mammary uptake of glucose. J. Endocrinol. 1984, 101, 203–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Motil, K.J.; Thotathuchery, M.; Montandon, C.M.; Hachey, D.L.; Boutton, T.W.; Klein, P.D.; Garza, C. Insulin, cortisol and thyroid hormones modulate maternal protein status and milk production and composition in humans. J. Nutr. 1994, 124, 1248–1257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Lei, J.; Feng, D.; Zhang, Y.; Dahanayaka, S.; Li, X.; Yao, K.; Wang, J.; Wu, Z.; Dai, Z.; Wu, G. Hormonal regulation of leucine catabolism in mammary epithelial cells. Amino Acids 2013, 45, 531–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Bomfim, G.F.; Merighe, G.K.F.; de Oliveira, S.A.; Negrao, J.A. Acute and chronic effects of cortisol on milk yield, the expression of key receptors, and apoptosis of mammary epithelial cells in Saanen goats. J. Dairy Sci. 2022, 105, 818–830. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Chen, D.C.; Nommsen-Rivers, L.; Dewey, K.G.; Lönnerdal, B. Stress during labor and delivery and early lactation performance. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1998, 68, 335–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Plat, L.; Byrne, M.M.; Sturis, J.; Polonsky, K.S.; Mockel, J.; Féry, F.; van Cauter, E. Effects of morning cortisol elevation on insulin secretion and glucose regulation in humans. Am. J. Physiol.-Endocrinol. Metab. 1996, 270, E36–E42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Huzzey, J.M.; Nydam, D.V.; Grant, R.J.; Overton, T.R. The effects of overstocking Holstein dairy cattle during the dry period on cortisol secretion and energy metabolism. J. Dairy Sci. 2012, 95, 4421–4433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Shamay, A.; Shapiro, F.; Barash, H.; Bruckental, I.; Silanikove, N. Effect of dexamethasone on milk yield and composition in dairy cows. Ann. Zootech. 2000, 49, 343–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Djurhuus, C.B.; Gravholt, C.H.; Nielsen, S.; Mengel, A.; Christiansen, J.S.; Schmitz, O.E.; Møller, N. Effects of cortisol on lipolysis and regional interstitial glycerol levels in humans. Am. J. Physiol.-Endocrinol. Metab. 2002, 283, E172–E177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Forsyth, I.A.; Strong, C.R.; Dils, R. Interactions of insulin, corticosterone and prolactin in promoting milk-fat synthesis by mammary explants from pregnant rabbits. Biochem. J. 1972, 129, 929–935. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Djurhuus, C.B.; Gravholt, C.H.; Nielsen, S.; Pedersen, S.B.; Møller, N.; Schmitz, O. Additive effects of cortisol and growth hormone on regional and systemic lipolysis in humans. Am. J. Physiol.-Endocrinol. Metab. 2004, 286, 488–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Chowanadisai, W.; Kelleher, S.L.; Nemeth, J.F.; Yachetti, S.; Kuhlman, C.F.; Jackson, J.G.; Davis, A.M.; Lien, E.L.; Lönnerdal, B. Detection of a single nucleotide polymorphism in the human α-lactalbumin gene: Implications for human milk proteins. J. Nutr. Biochem. 2005, 16, 272–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Hahn-Holbrook, J.; Le, T.B.; Chung, A.; Davis, E.P.; Glynn, L.M. Cortisol in human milk predicts child BMI. Obesity 2016, 24, 2471–2474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Finken, M.J.J.; van der Voorn, B.; Hollanders, J.J.; Dijkstra, L.R.; Toorop, A.A.; Rotteveel, J. Cortisol in human milk: The good, the bad, or the ugly? Obesity 2017, 25, 1153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Ma, L.; Liu, X.; Yan, N.; Gan, Y.; Wu, Y.; Li, Y.; Chu, M.; Chiu, D.T.; Ma, L. Associations Between Different Cortisol Measures and Adiposity in Children: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Front. Nutr. 2022, 9, 879256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Dillon, E.L.; Wu, G. Cortisol enhances citrulline synthesis from proline in enterocytes of suckling piglets. Amino Acids 2021, 53, 1957–1966. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Toorop, A.A.; van der Voorn, B.; Hollanders, J.J.; Dijkstra, L.R.; Dolman, K.M.; Heijboer, A.C.; Rotteveel, J.; Honig, A.; Finken, M.J.J. Diurnal rhythmicity in breast-milk glucocorticoids, and infant behavior and sleep at age 3 months. Endocrine 2020, 68, 660–668. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Catalani, A.; Marinelli, M.; Scaccianoce, S.; Nicolai, R.; Muscolo, L.A.A.; Porcu, A.; Korányi, L.; Piazza, P.V.; Angelucci, L. Progeny of mothers drinking corticosterone during lactation has lower stress-induced corticosterone secretion and better cognitive performance. Brain Res. 1993, 624, 209–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Catalani, A.; Casolini, P.; Scaccianoce, S.; Patacchioli, F.R.; Spinozzi, P.; Angelucci, L. Maternal corticosterone during lactation permanently affects brain corticosteroid receptors, stress response and behaviour in rat progeny. Neuroscience 2000, 100, 319–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Catalani, A.; Casolini, P.; Cigliana, G.; Scaccianoce, S.; Consoli, C.; Cinque, C.; Zuena, A.R.; Angelucci, L. Maternal corticosterone influences behavior, stress response and corticosteroid receptors in the female rat. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2002, 73, 105–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Hernandez, M.; Harlé, G.; Lalonde, R.; Strazielle, C. Effects of corticosterone injections in mid-to-late mouse postnatal development on adult motor activity and coordination. Neurosci. Res. 2021, 164, 22–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Królak-Olejnik, B.; Błasiak, I.; Szczygieł, A. Promotion of breastfeeding in Poland: The current situation. J. Int. Med. Res. 2017, 45, 1976–1984. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. A flowchart of the study sample collection.
Figure 1. A flowchart of the study sample collection.
Ijerph 19 14809 g001
Table 1. Characteristics of mother–infant pairs participating in the study.
Table 1. Characteristics of mother–infant pairs participating in the study.
VariableMean ± SDMin–Maxn (%)
Maternal sociodemographic factors
Maternal age (years)30.9 ± 3.722.7–40.0
Parity, primiparous 19 (50)
University education 38 (100)
Marital status, married 32 (84)
Self-assessment of the economic situation, very good 14 (37)
Average monthly income, >1500 PLN per capita 25 (66)
Maternal biological and psychological factors
Gestational weight gain (kg)13.5 ± 4.40.7–22.0
BMI, pre-pregnancy (kg/m2); overweight or obesity22.7 ± 3.718.4–38.65 (13)
BMI, 1st visit (kg/m2); overweight or obesity24.0 ± 3.918.4–39.79 (24)
BMI, 2nd visit (kg/m2); overweight or obesity23.5 ± 4.118.1–40.86 (16)
BMI, 3rd visit (kg/m2); overweight or obesity23.1 ± 4.319.1–42.67 (18)
EPDS, 1st visit (points); depression risk5.9 ± 4.00–183 (8)
EPDS, 3rd visit (points); depression risk4.7 ± 3.70–182 (5)
EPDS, 6th visit postpartum (points); depression risk5.2 ± 4.10–193 (8)
PSS-10, 1st visit (points); high stress21.0 ± 5.14–2823 (61)
PSS-10, 2nd visit (points); high stress20.4 ± 3.813–2822 (58)
PSS-10, 3rd visit (points); high stress21.5 ± 4.014–2923 (61)
Infant and breastfeeding factors
Infant gender, boys 19 (50)
Infant age, 1st visit (weeks)4.8 ± 0.83.4–6.9
Infant age, 2nd visit (weeks)13.4 ± 1.79.6–20.1
Infant age, 3rd visit (weeks)26.9 ± 1.224–29.6
Gestational age (Hbd)39.3 ± 1.337–42
Mode of delivery, C section 16 (42)
Birthweight (g)3413.9 ± 373.72870–4240
BMI z-score, 2nd visit−1.15 ± 1.48−4.17–3.82
BMI z-score, 3rd visit−1.31 ± 1.32−4.17–1.24
Children Development Scale (DSR) Results
Performance Scale, Manipulation (points); average6.4 ± 1.13–728 (74)
Performance Scale, Perception (points)4.9 ± 1.12–7
Performance Scale, Memory (points)0.8 ± 0.40–1
Performance Scale, Speech and language (points)2.1 ± 1.01–5
Performance Scale, Social behavior (points); high6.7 ± 1.04–93 (8)
Performance Scale, Motor skill (points); high4.1 ± 1.12–615 (39)
Performance Scale, total result (centiles); elevated40.4 ± 13.816–742 (5)
Observational Scale, Vigor (points); high10.6 ± 1.29–133 (8)
Observational Scale, Adaptability (points); high13.7 ± 1.87–1714 (36)
Observational Scale, Regularity (points); high3.3 ± 0.81–72 (5)
Observational Scale, Sensitivity (points); high3.4 ± 0.53–413 (34)
BMI—body mass index; EPDS—Edinburgh Postpartum Depression Scale; Hbd—hebdomas; weeks of gestation; PSS-10—Perceived Stress Scale.
Table 2. Breastmilk cortisol at 1, 3, and 6 months postpartum according to the season of breastmilk collection.
Table 2. Breastmilk cortisol at 1, 3, and 6 months postpartum according to the season of breastmilk collection.
Breastmilk Cortisol [ng/mL]
Mean ± SD
Min–Max
1 Month Postpartum3 Months Postpartum6 Months Postpartum
Total group (n = 38)11.2 ± 6.211.2 ± 4.312.7 ± 6.2
5.0–32.04.8–23.64.4–29.4
p-value 10.154
Season of breastmilk sample collection
spring-summern = 19n = 24n = 21
9.0 ± 3.611.9 ± 5.014.3 ± 5.9
5.0–16.44.8–23.65.0–29.4
n = 19n = 14n = 17
autumn-winter13.5 ± 7.59.9 ± 2.610.8 ± 6.1
5.8–32.05.0–14.24.4–26.0
p-value 20.0200.3320.041
1 ANOVA for repeated measurements test; 2 Student’s t-test. Tests were conducted on log-transformed cortisol data.
Table 3. Partial correlations between breastmilk cortisol (log), maternal and infant factors, and breastmilk macronutrient composition, adjusted for season of breastmilk sample collection.
Table 3. Partial correlations between breastmilk cortisol (log), maternal and infant factors, and breastmilk macronutrient composition, adjusted for season of breastmilk sample collection.
VariableBreastmilk Cortisol (Log)
1 Month Postpartum3 Months Postpartum6 Months Postpartum
Maternal
factors
Maternal age0.0280.008−0.305
Maternal BMI−0.0910.040−0.081
EPDS score0.2620.090−0.055
PSS-10 score0.2990.026−0.038
Infant factorsInfant age−0.182−0.0010.030
Gestational age0.1860.0630.258
Birthweight−0.1070.1110.077
Number of breastfeedings0.147−0.1660.086
Breastmilk
composition
Fat0.005−0.0160.212
Protein0.0810.2810.151
Carbohydrates0.0810.0130.258
Total solids−0.092−0.0010.358 *
Crude protein−0.0800.366 *0.126
Energy−0.0890.0110.282
BMI—body mass index; EPDS—Edinburgh Postpartum Depression Score; PSS-10—Perceived Stress Scale. * p ≤ 0.05.
Table 4. Linear regression models between breastmilk cortisol(log) and breastmilk crude protein in the first sixth months lactation.
Table 4. Linear regression models between breastmilk cortisol(log) and breastmilk crude protein in the first sixth months lactation.
Breastmilk Crude ProteinBreastmilk Cortisol(log)
Model 1Model 2Model 3
1 month lactationβ = −0.013 (−0.351–0.325)
R2= −0.03; p = 0.940
β = 0.022 (−0.331–0.376)
R2= 0.07; p = 0.143
β = −0.011 (−0.434–0.412)
R2 = −0.02; p = 0.518
3 months lactationβ = 0.395 (0.084–0.705) **
R2 = 0.13; p = 0.014
β = 0.311 (0.021–0.601) *
R2= 0.27; p = 0.002
β = 0.318 (0.007–0.630) *
R2 = 0.19; p = 0.030
6 months lactationβ = 0.150 (−0.184–0.484)
R2 = −0.01; p = 0.369
β = 0.133 (−0.232–0.498)
R2 = −0.06; p = 0.830
β = 0.108 (−0.273–0.489)
R2 = −0.08; p = 0.882
Model 1: unadjusted; Model 2: adjusted for the season of breastmilk collection and infant age; Model 3: model 2 adjusted for the number of breastfeedings. * p ≤ 0.05; ** p ≤ 0.01.
Table 5. Partial correlations between breastmilk cortisol(log) and infant BMI z-score at the 3rd and 6th months adjusted for the season of breastmilk collection and breastmilk energy value and crude protein.
Table 5. Partial correlations between breastmilk cortisol(log) and infant BMI z-score at the 3rd and 6th months adjusted for the season of breastmilk collection and breastmilk energy value and crude protein.
VariableBreastmilk Cortisol (Log)
1 Month Postpartum3 Months Postpartum6 Months Postpartum
Total3 months−0.0520.412 *−0.077
6 months0.0940.372 *−0.004
∆ 3–6 months−0.187−0.0790.097
Girls3 months0.0050.541 *-
6 months−0.1210.3880.021
∆ 3–6 months−0.165−0.204−0.385
Boys3 months−0.0020.371-
6 months0.1410.2880.088
∆ 3–6 months0.184−0.1830.395
∆ 3–6 months—changes in the BMI z-score between the 6th and 3rd months of life. * p ≤ 0.05.
Table 6. Linear regression models between breastmilk cortisol(log) and infant anthropometric development.
Table 6. Linear regression models between breastmilk cortisol(log) and infant anthropometric development.
BMI
z-Score
Month
Model 1Model 2Model 3Model 4
cortisollog
1 month
3β = 0.061β = 0.081β = −0.081β = −0.071
(−0.403–0.282)(−0.429–0.268)(−0.388–0.277)(−0.395–0.253)
R2 = −0.02R2 = −0.02R2 = 0.21 *R2 = 0.18 *
6β = 0.094β = 0.104β = 0.096β = 0.018
(−0.253–0.441)(−0.260–0.468)(−0.262–0.454)(−0.346–0.382)
R2 = −0.02R2 = −0.05R2 = 0.02R2 = 0.02
cortisollog
3 month
3β = 0.338β = 0.359β = 0.265β = 0.286
(0.015–0.661) *(0.021–0.697) *(−0.046–0.577)(−0.034–0.607)
R2 = 0.09 *R2 = 0.10 *R 2 = 0.27 **R2 = 0.26 **
6β = 0.337β = 0.451β = 0.405β = 0.334
(0.009–0.665) *(0.095–0.808) **(0.029–0.780) *(−0.065–0.733)
R2 = 0.09 *R2 = 0.11R2 = 0.11R2 = 0.11
cortisollog
6 months
6β = 0.032β = 0.002β = 0.00β = −0.006
(−0.380–0.317)(−0.363–0.367)(−0.359–0.359)(−0.355–0.343)
R2 = −0.03R2 = −0.07R2 = −0.03R2 = 0.03
BMI—body mass index. Model 1: unadjusted; Model 2: adjusted for average breastmilk energy value; Model 3: model 2 adjusted for birthweight; Model 4: model 3 adjusted for infant sex. * p ≤ 0.05; ** p ≤ 0.01.
Table 7. Partial correlations between breastmilk cortisol (log) and infant psychomotor development and temperament at the 6th month of life adjusted for infant sex (in the total group) and age, birthweight maternal age, education, psychological status, and parity.
Table 7. Partial correlations between breastmilk cortisol (log) and infant psychomotor development and temperament at the 6th month of life adjusted for infant sex (in the total group) and age, birthweight maternal age, education, psychological status, and parity.
DSR ScaleBreastmilk Cortisol (Log)
1 Month3 Months6 Months
TotalManipulation 0.2960.2690.184
Perception0.169−0.132−0.160
Memory−0.213−0.144−0.013
Speech0.0570.0890.005
Social behavior−0.0490.162−0.085
Motor−0.2190.053−0.057
Performance scale score0.0690.144−0.035
Performance scale centile0.2300.2820.117
Vigor−0.0110.101−0.011
Adaptability−0.2350.138−0.214
Regularity−0.379 *0.038−0.421 *
Sensitivity0.169−0.0160.071
GirlsManipulation 0.3410.0900.111
Perception0.385−0.448−0.109
Memory−0.025−0.0790.425
Speech0.4680.2290.425
Social behavior−0.370−0.054−0.139
Motor0.0170.5380.284
Performance scale score0.4030.1440.369
Performance scale centile0.634 *0.4280.636 *
Vigor0.0600.419−0.148
Adaptability−0.0170.299−0.231
Regularity−0.3530.088−0.276
Sensitivity−0.121−0.3440.047
BoysManipulation −0.0840.1000.139
Perception−0.309−0.197−0.351
Memory−0.313−0.030−0.555
Speech0.2530.219−0.227
Social behavior−0.0700.434−0.348
Motor−0.2750.119−0.363
Performance scale score−0.2910.256−0.325
Performance scale centile−0.2370.369−0.399
Vigor0.2440.167−0.046
Adaptability−0.1270.486−0.241
Regularity−0.4280.207−0.173
Sensitivity0.580 *−0.016−0.058
DSR—Children’s Developmental Scale. * p ≤ 0.05.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Zielinska-Pukos, M.A.; Bryś, J.; Kucharz, N.; Chrobak, A.; Wesolowska, A.; Grabowicz-Chądrzyńska, I.; Hamulka, J. Factors Influencing Cortisol Concentrations in Breastmilk and Its Associations with Breastmilk Composition and Infant Development in the First Six Months of Lactation. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 14809. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192214809

AMA Style

Zielinska-Pukos MA, Bryś J, Kucharz N, Chrobak A, Wesolowska A, Grabowicz-Chądrzyńska I, Hamulka J. Factors Influencing Cortisol Concentrations in Breastmilk and Its Associations with Breastmilk Composition and Infant Development in the First Six Months of Lactation. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(22):14809. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192214809

Chicago/Turabian Style

Zielinska-Pukos, Monika A., Joanna Bryś, Natalia Kucharz, Agnieszka Chrobak, Aleksandra Wesolowska, Iwona Grabowicz-Chądrzyńska, and Jadwiga Hamulka. 2022. "Factors Influencing Cortisol Concentrations in Breastmilk and Its Associations with Breastmilk Composition and Infant Development in the First Six Months of Lactation" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 22: 14809. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192214809

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop