Next Article in Journal
The Effect of Perceived Social Support on the Mental Health of Homosexuals: The Mediating Role of Self-Efficacy
Next Article in Special Issue
Accessibility of Psychological Treatments for Bulimia Nervosa: A Review of Efficacy and Engagement in Online Self-Help Treatments
Previous Article in Journal
Ecological Risk Assessment and Source Apportionment of Heavy Metals in the Soil of an Opencast Mine in Xinjiang
Previous Article in Special Issue
Relationship between Attention to Body Shape, Social Physique Anxiety, and Personal Characteristics of Brazilians: A Structural Equation Model
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Brief Report

Body Dissatisfaction and Binge Eating: The Moderating Roles of Sweet Taste Reward Sensitivity and Dietary Restraint among Tobacco Product Users

by
Tyler B. Mason
1,2,*,
Anna Dolgon-Krutolow
1,
Kathryn E. Smith
2,3 and
Adam M. Leventhal
1,2
1
Department of Population and Public Health Sciences, University of Southern California Keck School of Medicine, Los Angeles, CA 90032, USA
2
Institute for Addiction Science, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA 90032, USA
3
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA 90032, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(23), 15523; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192315523
Submission received: 28 September 2022 / Revised: 12 November 2022 / Accepted: 20 November 2022 / Published: 23 November 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Eating Disorders: Challenges, Advances and Public Health Insight)

Abstract

:
Body dissatisfaction is a key predictor of binge eating, yet less is known about factors that may potentiate the association between body dissatisfaction and binge eating. This study examined self-reported dietary restraint and sweet taste reward sensitivity as candidate moderators of the association between body dissatisfaction and binge eating in adults. A convenience sample of 221 tobacco product users completed measures of eating disorder pathology and sweet taste reward sensitivity. Results revealed that elevated sweet taste reward sensitivity strengthened the positive association between higher body dissatisfaction and binge eating. However, there was no main effect, or moderation effect, of dietary restraint on binge eating. The findings of this study demonstrate the key role of sweet taste reward sensitivity in potentiating the association between body dissatisfaction and binge eating. Sweet taste reward sensitivity may serve as a key dispositional factor for uncontrolled eating.

1. Introduction

Binge eating symptoms occur across eating disorders and involve eating large amounts of food in a short period of time and feelings of a subjective sense of loss of control and overeating [1]. Of critical importance, binge-eating symptoms are associated with poor psychosocial and physical health outcomes, notably psychiatric disorders, obesity, and poor quality of life and social functioning [2,3,4]. While binge-eating symptoms may be elevated among those with obesity, binge-eating symptoms affect individuals of all body sizes and weights [5].
Body dissatisfaction is a central risk and maintenance factor included in most theoretical models of binge eating [6], and empirical research has found positive cross-sectional and prospective associations between elevated body dissatisfaction and binge eating [7,8,9]. In fact, regardless of weight status, body image concerns were a key factor that distinguished between women with and without a binge-eating disorder [10,11]. Yet, body dissatisfaction is relatively common in today’s society, often termed “normative discontent” [12]. As such, more research is needed to understand factors that may potentiate, or moderate, the association between body dissatisfaction and binge eating to better understand the dynamics of the association between body dissatisfaction and binge eating. Two key candidate moderators that have been argued to be important drivers of binge eating in the eating disorders and food addiction literature are dietary restraint and sweet taste reward sensitivity [13,14].
The restraint model of binge eating suggests that individuals who exhibit elevated dietary restraint, or attempts to control or limit their dietary intake for the purpose of weight control, are more likely to engage in binge eating [15]. Some empirical tests of the restraint model have shown positive associations between dietary restraint and binge eating [16]. However, Spoor and colleagues [17] did not find a prospective association between dietary restraint and later binge eating, and other research found that in a multivariable model with restraint and body dissatisfaction, only body dissatisfaction was significantly associated with bulimic symptoms [18]. Nevertheless, several studies indicate that dietary restraint moderates the association between psychosocial factors and binge eating [19,20,21]. For example, Woods and colleagues [21] found that dietary restraint strengthened the positive association of daily and life stressors on binge eating symptoms. These studies support the possibility of dietary restraint as a moderator of the association between body dissatisfaction and binge eating, such that individuals with greater body dissatisfaction who engage in dietary restraint as a means of altering their shape/weight may be at greatest risk for subsequent binge eating.
In addition to restraint, reward-related processes have been implicated in binge eating, including food liking, wanting, and expectancies [22,23,24]. For some people, sweet-tasting food may be particularly rewarding [25]. In addition, animal studies have found responsiveness to sweet taste to be particularly important to binge eating in rats [13,26]. While less research has studied sweet taste reward sensitivity in relation to binge eating in humans, there have been several studies examining sweet taste reward-related measures in individuals with binge-eating spectrum disorders [27,28]. One recent study of adults with binge-eating disorder found that elevated sweet taste preference was associated with higher binge-eating and overeating frequencies [28]. Another study showed that women with bulimia nervosa reported elevated sweet taste pleasantness compared to healthy controls [27]. Furthermore, food addiction and food responsiveness measures have shown positive associations with increased binge eating [29,30,31]. This available research suggests that sweet taste reward sensitivity may play a role in binge eating, but the moderating role of sweet taste reward sensitivity in the association between psychological factors, including body dissatisfaction, and binge eating has not been examined. Similar to restraint, it is possible that individuals with greater body dissatisfaction who report higher sweet taste reward sensitivity are at the greatest risk for subsequent binge eating.
To better understand the psychological processes that potentiate the association between body dissatisfaction and binge eating, a secondary analysis of an existing dataset was conducted. Research questions in the current study were developed post hoc to the original aims of the parent study. The current study examined the moderating roles of self-reported dietary restraint and sweet taste reward sensitivity in the association between body dissatisfaction and binge eating. It was hypothesized that elevated sweet taste reward sensitivity and dietary restraint would separately strengthen the positive association between body dissatisfaction and binge eating.

2. Method

2.1. Participants

The current paper used a convenience sample of United States (U.S.) adults who currently used tobacco products. Data were collected from 221 adults who either currently used e-cigarettes or only used combustible cigarettes but were interested in trying e-cigarettes. Participants were recruited through internet advertisements in the U.S. Procedures were reviewed and approved by the University of Southern California Institutional Review Board. Eligible and interested participants (assessed via phone screen) completed a virtual study visit, where they provided written informed consent. Participants completed baseline survey questionnaires and an experimental e-cigarette product appeal paradigm. The questionnaires were completed during breaks from the e-cigarette product testing. Only self-report questionnaires were used in the present analyses.

2.2. Measures

Eating Pathology Symptoms Inventory (EPSI) [32]. The EPSI was used to assess several facets of eating disorder psychopathology. The current study used Binge Eating (e.g., “I stuffed myself with food to the point of feeling sick”), Body Dissatisfaction (e.g., “I wished the shape of my body was different”), and Dietary Restraint (e.g., “I tried to exclude “unhealthy” foods from my diet”) subscales. Participants were asked to report how frequently they experienced each item during the past four weeks on a 5-point scale, from 0 (never) to 4 (very often). Scores were generated by averaging the responses across items for each of the three subscales. The EPSI has shown excellent psychometric properties in previous research across gender [32,33]. The Cronbach’s alphas in the current study were 0.89, 0.73, and 0.83 for body dissatisfaction, dietary restraint, and binge eating, respectively.
Sweet Taste Questionnaire (STQ) [34]. The STQ is a 12-item self-report measure that assesses sweet taste reward sensitivity with items assessing behaviors and attitudes. Items from the STQ measure sensitivity to the mood-altering effects of sugary foods, as well as impaired control over the consumption of sweet foods. Sample items include, “I often have an urge for something sweet”, “I have problems controlling how much sweet food I eat”, and “I am less irritable if I have something sweet to eat.” Participants responded to each item using a 7-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Kampov-Polevoy et al. [34] reported good psychometric properties of the STQ. The Cronbach’s alpha in the current study was 0.88.

2.3. Statistical Analyses

Analyses were run in SPSS v 28.0 (IBM; Armonk, NY, USA). Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations were calculated for the study variables. Next, Q-Q plots, skewness, and kurtosis statistics were examined to test for normality [35]. Linear multiple regression was run within the SPSS PROCESS Macro [36], which is appropriate for examining interactions (i.e., moderation) between continuous independent variables. A two independent moderators model was conducted, which included examining the main effects of body dissatisfaction, dietary restraint, and sweet taste reward sensitivity and two-way interactions between body dissatisfaction and dietary restraint and body dissatisfaction and sweet taste reward sensitivity in relation to binge eating. All independent variables used to create interaction terms were centered so that the mean was 0, which helps avoid issues with multicollinearity. Variance inflation factor (VIF) statistics were examined to test for multicollinearity, with VIF > 10 indicating potential multicollinearity [37]. Covariates included gender, age, and BMI. Significance testing was conducted using bootstrap confidence intervals (CIs). If the bootstrap CI did not include 0, then the estimate was significant. To probe significant interactions, interactions were plotted, and conditional effects were calculated.

3. Results

The gender makeup of the sample was 45.7% Female (n = 101), 53.3% Male (n = 118), and 0.9% Other (n = 2). The race/ethnicity of the sample was 67.1% White, 13.2% Black, 4.6% Asian or Pacific Islander, 0.9% American Indian or Alaskan Native, 5.5% Hispanic, 0.9% Other, and 7.8% Multiracial. Bivariate correlations and descriptive statistics are reported in Table 1. Age was significantly positively correlated with higher BMI and lower binge eating, and BMI was significantly positively correlated with greater body dissatisfaction. Body dissatisfaction was significantly positively correlated with greater dietary restraint, sweet taste reward sensitivity, and binge eating. Dietary restraint was not significantly correlated with sweet taste reward sensitivity or binge eating, but sweet taste reward sensitivity was significantly positively correlated with higher levels of binge eating. Q-Q plots and skewness and kurtosis statistics indicated normality for body dissatisfaction, dietary restraint, sweet taste reward sensitivity, and binge eating.
The linear multiple regression is reported in Table 2. VIF values were all below 1.5, indicating no issues with multicollinearity. Younger age and male compared to female gender were significantly associated with greater binge eating; BMI was unrelated to binge eating. With regard to the main effects, greater body dissatisfaction and sweet taste reward sensitivity were significantly associated with higher binge eating. There was no significant main effect of dietary restraint in relation to binge eating. There was a significant interaction between body dissatisfaction and sweet taste reward sensitivity in relation to binge eating, but no significant interaction between body dissatisfaction and dietary restraint with binge eating. The model explained 31% of the variance in binge eating (R2 = 0.31).
The interaction of body dissatisfaction and sweet taste reward sensitivity is displayed in Figure 1. As shown, higher sweet taste reward sensitivity strengthened the positive association between body dissatisfaction and binge eating. Table 3 displays the conditional effects of body dissatisfaction on binge eating at centered values of sweet taste reward sensitivity and dietary restraint. Regardless of levels of dietary restraint, conditional effects analyses revealed that there was a positive association between body dissatisfaction and binge eating at mean and +1 SD levels of sweet taste reward sensitivity. There was no significant association at −1 SD of sweet taste reward sensitivity.

4. Discussion

This study examined the moderating roles of dietary restraint and sweet taste reward sensitivity in the association between body dissatisfaction and binge eating. Consistent with theoretical and empirical work [6,7,8,9], higher body dissatisfaction was associated with greater levels of binge eating. Dietary restraint was not associated with binge eating and did not moderate the association between body dissatisfaction and binge eating; which is contrary to the restraint model of binge eating [15]. Yet, sweet taste reward sensitivity was positively associated with binge eating and strengthened the association between body dissatisfaction and binge eating. These results suggest that among a non-clinical sample of adult tobacco product users, over-responsiveness to sweet taste appears to be more important in relation to binge eating compared to dietary restraint.
Sweet taste reward sensitivity strengthened the association between body dissatisfaction and binge eating. As such, the experience of elevated body dissatisfaction and sweet taste reward sensitivity represents a psychological profile associated with the risk of more binge-eating symptoms. This supports the notion of the combined importance of eating-disorder-related risk factors (e.g., body dissatisfaction) and food addiction-related risk factors (e.g., responsiveness to sweet taste) in predicting risk for elevated binge-eating symptoms [14]. Studies show that binge-eating episodes often involve sweet foods, and thus, individuals with elevated sweet taste reward sensitivity may lose control when eating sweet types of food [38,39,40]. Moreover, individuals with elevated body dissatisfaction may experience maladaptive intrusive food-related thoughts and preoccupations [7,41,42]. Given the results of the current study, intrusive food-related thoughts may be exacerbated by elevated reward sensitivity to sweet food, which could be associated with a greater risk of binge eating.
The lack of a moderating role of dietary restraint is inconsistent with prior research, as several studies have shown dietary restraint to be a moderator of non-specific eating disorder risk factors and binge eating, such as daily stress and negative affect [19,20,21]. Given the relationship between body dissatisfaction and dietary restraint, these findings could be explained by body dissatisfaction being a confounding variable. That is, body dissatisfaction may be the key moderator associated with binge eating, rather than dietary restraint, which is supported by a previous study [9]. Furthermore, the association between restraint and binge eating appears to be quite complex, with studies finding nuanced associations between restraint and binge eating when examining within- and between-subject associations [43,44]. Additionally, there are a host of conceptualizations and measures of restraint, which include both adaptive and maladaptive components. As such, various conceptualizations and measures of dietary restraint might have differential relationships with binge eating [45]. It is also possible that this finding is related to the sample composition of tobacco product users. Given the overlap between substance use and food addiction, tobacco product users may be more likely to be influenced by addiction-related mechanisms, such as reward sensitivity, rather than eating disorder-specific mechanisms, such as dietary restraint [46].
Several limitations must be noted. First, this was a cross-sectional study, so causality and directionality cannot be confirmed. Longitudinal research will be needed to understand causal relationships between these variables. Second, this study used a convenience sample of tobacco product users. Thus, further research is necessary to determine whether the current findings generalize to other samples, as well as the differences between tobacco vs. non-tobacco product users. Yet, the current findings are important given the associations between tobacco product use and eating disorder psychopathology, and related behaviors, such as binge eating [47]. Third, all measures were assessed with retrospective self-report questionnaires, which are subject to reporting biases. Future research should utilize other methodologies that limit self-report biases, such as intensive longitudinal and experimental designs. Specifically, assessing responses to actual sweet taste intake is an important measure of sweet taste reward sensitivity to include in future research. Fourth, this study focused on sweet taste reward sensitivity, yet binge-eating episodes may consist of other types of foods, such as savory or high-fat foods, and more research will be needed to understand relationships between responsiveness to other types of foods and binge eating.
In conclusion, individuals with elevated body dissatisfaction reported higher binge-eating symptoms when they also had elevated sweet taste reward sensitivity. Models of binge eating should integrate constructs from the eating disorder and food addiction literature to best understand the risk of engaging in binge eating. In addition, dietary restraint was not related to binge-eating symptoms, and future studies of integrative models of binge eating should clarify the role of restraint in these models.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, T.B.M.; methodology, T.B.M. and A.M.L.; formal analysis, T.B.M.; investigation, T.B.M. and A.M.L.; writing—original draft preparation, T.B.M. and A.D.-K.; writing—review and editing, K.E.S. and A.M.L.; funding acquisition, T.B.M. and A.M.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This project was supported in part by grants K01DK124435 from the National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases Award Number (NIDDK), U54CA180905 from the National Cancer Institute (NCI) and Food and Drug Administration (FDA), and K24DA048160 from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of NIDDK, NCI, FDA, or NIDA. The funding agencies had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board (or Ethics Committee) of the University of Southern California.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data are available from the authors by request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; APA: Arlington, TX, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Agüera, Z.; Lozano-Madrid, M.; Mallorquí-Bagué, N.; Jiménez-Murcia, S.; Menchón, J.M.; Fernández-Aranda, F. A review of binge eating disorder and obesity. Neuropsychiatrie 2021, 35, 57–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Hudson, J.I.; Hiripi, E.; Pope, H.G., Jr.; Kessler, R.C. The prevalence and correlates of eating disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Biol. Psychiatry 2007, 61, 348–358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Mason, T.B.; Heron, K.E. Do depressive symptoms explain associations between binge eating symptoms and later psychosocial adjustment in young adulthood? Eat. Behav. 2016, 23, 126–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Lipson, S.K.; Sonneville, K.R. Eating disorder symptoms among undergraduate and graduate students at 12 US colleges and universities. Eat. Behav. 2017, 24, 81–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Pennesi, J.L.; Wade, T.D. A systematic review of the existing models of disordered eating: Do they inform the development of effective interventions? Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2016, 43, 175–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Mason, T.B.; Lewis, R.J. Assessing the roles of impulsivity, food-related cognitions, BMI, and demographics in the dual pathway model of binge eating among men and women. Eat. Behav. 2015, 18, 151–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Stice, E.; Shaw, H.E. Role of body dissatisfaction in the onset and maintenance of eating pathology: A synthesis of research findings. J. Psychosom. Res. 2002, 53, 985–993. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Wardle, J.; Waller, J.; Rapoport, L. Body dissatisfaction and binge eating in obese women: The role of restraint and depression. Obesity Res. 2001, 778–787. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Ahrberg, M.; Trojca, D.; Nasrawi, N.; Vocks, S. Body image disturbance in binge eating disorder: A review. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 2011, 19, 375–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Yiu, A.; Murray, S.M.; Arlt, J.M.; Eneva, K.T.; Chen, E.Y. The importance of body image concerns in overweight and normal weight individuals with binge eating disorder. Body Image 2017, 22, 6–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Tiggemann, M. Sociocultural perspectives on human appearance and body image. In Body Image: A Handbook of Science, Practice, and Prevention; Cash, T.F., Smolak, L., Eds.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2011; pp. 12–19. [Google Scholar]
  13. Avena, N.M. Examining the addictive-like properties of binge eating using an animal model of sugar dependence. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 2007, 15, 481–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Wiss, D.A.; Avena, N.M. Food Addiction, Binge Eating, and the Role of Dietary Restraint: Converging Evidence from Animal and Human Studies. In Binge Eating; Frank, G., Berner, L., Eds.; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Polivy, J.; Herman, C.P. Dieting and binging: A causal analysis. Am. Psychol. 1985, 40, 193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Linardon, J. The relationship between dietary restraint and binge eating: Examining eating-related self-efficacy as a moderator. Appetite 2018, 127, 126–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Spoor, S.T.; Stice, E.; Bekker, M.H.; Van Strien, T.; Croon, M.A.; Van Heck, G.L. Relations between dietary restraint, depressive symptoms, and binge eating: A longitudinal study. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2006, 39, 700–707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  18. Johnson, F.; Wardle, J. Dietary restraint, body dissatisfaction, and psychological distress: A prospective analysis. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2005, 114, 119–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Mason, T.B.; Smith, K.E.; Lavender, J.M.; Lewis, R.J. Independent and interactive associations of negative affect, restraint, and impulsivity in relation to binge eating among women. Appetite 2018, 121, 147–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Stice, E.; Akutagawa, D.; Gaggar, A.; Agras, W.S. Negative affect moderatesthe relation between dieting and binge eating. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2000, 27, 218–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Woods, A.M.; Racine, S.E.; Klump, K.L. Examining the relationship between dietary restraint and binge eating: Differential effects of major and minor stressors. Eat. Behav. 2010, 11, 276–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Davis, C.A.; Levitan, R.D.; Reid, C.; Carter, J.C.; Kaplan, A.S.; Patte, K.A.; Kennedy, J.L. Dopamine for “wanting” and opioids for “liking”: A comparison of obese adults with and without binge eating. Obesity 2009, 17, 1220–1225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. de Carvalho-Ferreira, J.P.; Finlayson, G.; da Cunha, D.T.; Caldas, G.; Bandoni, D.; de Rosso, V.V. Adiposity and binge eating are related to liking and wanting for food in Brazil: A cultural adaptation of the Leeds Food Preference Questionnaire. Appetite 2019, 133, 174–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Smith, K.E.; Mason, T.B.; Peterson, C.B.; Pearson, C.M. Relationships between eating disorder-specific and transdiagnostic risk factors for binge eating: An integrative moderated mediation model of emotion regulation, anticipatory reward, and expectancy. Eat. Behav. 2018, 31, 131–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Han, P.; Mohebbi, M.; Seo, H.S.; Hummel, T. Sensitivity to sweetness correlates to elevated reward brain responses to sweet and high-fat food odors in young healthy volunteers. Neuroimage 2020, 208, 116413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Avena, N.M.; Rada, P.; Hoebel, B.G. Sugar and fat bingeing have notable differences in addictive-like behavior. J. Nutr. 2009, 139, 623–628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. Franko, D.L.; Wolfe, B.E.; Jimerson, D.C. Elevated sweet taste pleasantness ratings in bulimia nervosa. Physiol. Behav. 1994, 56, 969–973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Goodman, E.L.; Breithaupt, L.; Watson, H.J.; Peat, C.M.; Baker, J.H.; Bulik, C.M.; Brownley, K.A. Sweet taste preference in binge-eating disorder: A preliminary investigation. Eat. Behav. 2018, 28, 8–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Carter, J.C.; Van Wijk, M.; Rowsell, M. Symptoms of ‘food addiction’in binge eating disorder using the Yale Food Addiction Scale version 2.0. Appetite 2019, 133, 362–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Davis, C. Compulsive overeating as an addictive behavior: Overlap between food addiction and binge eating disorder. Curr. Obes. Rep. 2013, 2, 171–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Imperatori, C.; Innamorati, M.; Contardi, A.; Continisio, M.; Tamburello, S.; Lamis, D.A.; Tamburello, A.; Fabbricatore, M. The association among food addiction, binge eating severity and psychopathology in obese and overweight patients attending low-energy-diet therapy. Compr. Psychiatry 2014, 55, 1358–1362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Forbush, K.T.; Wildes, J.E.; Pollack, L.O.; Dunbar, D.; Luo, J.; Patterson, K.; Petruzzi, L.; Pollpeter, M.; Miller, H.; Stone, A.; et al. Development and validation of the Eating Pathology Symptoms Inventory (EPSI). Psychol. Assess. 2013, 25, 859–878. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Forbush, K.T.; Wildes, J.E.; Hunt, T.K. Gender norms, psychometric properties, and validity for the Eating Pathology Symptoms Inventory. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2014, 47, 85–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Kampov-Polevoy, A.B.; Alterman, A.; Khalitov, E.; Garbutt, J.C. Sweet preference predicts mood altering effect of and impaired control over eating sweet foods. Eat. Behav. 2006, 7, 181–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Kim, H.Y. Statistical notes for clinical researchers: Assessing normal distribution (2) using skewness and kurtosis. Restor. Dent. Endod. 2013, 38, 52–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Hayes, A.F. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  37. Franke, G.R. Multicollinearity. Wiley International Encyclopedia of Marketing; John Wiley and Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Dalton, M.; Finlayson, G. Psychobiological examination of liking and wanting for fat and sweet taste in trait binge eating females. Physiol. Behav. 2014, 136, 128–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Mason, T.B.; Do, B.; Chu, D.; Belcher, B.R.; Dunton, G.F.; Lopez, N.V. Associations among affect, diet, and activity and binge-eating severity using ecological momentary assessment in a non-clinical sample of middle-aged fathers. Eat. Weight. Disord.-Stud. Anorex. Bulim. Obes. 2022, 27, 543–551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Reslan, S.; Saules, K.K. College students’ definitions of an eating “binge” differ as a function of gender and binge eating disorder status. Eat. Behav. 2011, 12, 225–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Coelho, J.S.; Baeyens, C.; Purdon, C.; Shafran, R.; Roulin, J.L.; Bouvard, M. Assessment of thought–shape fusion: Initial validation of a short version of the trait thought–shape fusion scale. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2013, 46, 77–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Mason, T.B.; Smith, K.E.; Crosby, R.D.; Engel, S.G.; Peterson, C.B.; Wonderlich, S.A.; Jin, H. Multi-state modeling of thought-shape fusion using ecological momentary assessment. Body Image 2021, 39, 139–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Mason, T.B.; Heron, K.E.; Braitman, A.L.; Lewis, R.J. A daily diary study of perceived social isolation, dietary restraint, and negative affect in binge eating. Appetite 2016, 97, 94–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Pearson, C.M.; Mason, T.B.; Cao, L.; Goldschmidt, A.B.; Lavender, J.M.; Crosby, R.D.; Crow, S.J.; Engel, S.G.; Wonderlich, S.A.; Peterson, C.B. A test of a state-based, self-control theory of binge eating in adults with obesity. Eat. Disord. 2018, 26, 26–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Schaumberg, K.; Anderson, D.A.; Anderson, L.M.; Reilly, E.E.; Gorrell, S. Dietary restraint: What’s the harm? A review of the relationship between dietary restraint, weight trajectory and the development of eating pathology. Clin. Obes. 2016, 6, 89–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Zawertailo, L.; Attwells, S.; deRuiter, W.K.; Le, T.L.; Dawson, D.; Selby, P. Food addiction and tobacco use disorder: Common liability and shared mechanisms. Nutrients 2020, 12, 3834. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Mason, T.B.; Tackett, A.P.; Smith, C.E.; Leventhal, A.M. Tobacco product use for weight control as an eating disorder behavior: Recommendations for future clinical and public health research. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2022, 55, 313–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Interaction between body dissatisfaction and sweet taste reward sensitivity in relation to binge eating plotted at one standard deviation below the mean (−1 SD), mean, and one standard deviation above the mean (+1 SD) levels of sweet taste reward sensitivity. The three boxes indicate one deviation below the mean (−0.98), mean (0), and one standard deviation above the mean (0.98) levels of dietary restraint.
Figure 1. Interaction between body dissatisfaction and sweet taste reward sensitivity in relation to binge eating plotted at one standard deviation below the mean (−1 SD), mean, and one standard deviation above the mean (+1 SD) levels of sweet taste reward sensitivity. The three boxes indicate one deviation below the mean (−0.98), mean (0), and one standard deviation above the mean (0.98) levels of dietary restraint.
Ijerph 19 15523 g001
Table 1. Pearson Correlations and Descriptive Statistics of Study Variables.
Table 1. Pearson Correlations and Descriptive Statistics of Study Variables.
123456
1. Age-0.17 *−0.04−0.090.06−0.22 ***
2. Body mass index -0.29 ***0.130.090.001
3. Body dissatisfaction -0.21 **0.34 ***0.32 ***
4. Dietary restraint -0.060.12
5. Sweet taste reward sensitivity -0.40 ***
6. Binge eating -
M34.5326.911.251.422.920.98
SD13.286.110.960.981.390.70
Minimum2116.440.000.001.000.00
Maximum7352.373.864.006.923.71
Skewness0.881.310.580.620.621.10
Kurtosis−0.262.56−0.44−0.12−0.271.44
Note. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Table 2. Linear Regression Model of Binge Eating on Body Dissatisfaction, Dietary Restraint, and Sweet Taste Reward Sensitivity.
Table 2. Linear Regression Model of Binge Eating on Body Dissatisfaction, Dietary Restraint, and Sweet Taste Reward Sensitivity.
ParameterBSEp95% CI
Intercept1.300.22<0.001[0.88, 1.73]
Age−0.010.0030.006[−0.02, −0.003]
BMI−0.0070.0070.33[−0.02, 0.007]
Male vs. female0.270.090.004[0.09, 0.45]
Other vs. female−0.350.430.41[−1.19, 0.49]
Body dissatisfaction0.200.050.002[0.10, 0.30]
Dietary restraint0.040.040.41[−0.05, 0.12]
Sweet taste reward sensitivity0.160.03<0.001[0.10, 0.22]
Body dissatisfaction × dietary restraint−0.040.040.47[−0.11, 0.05]
Body dissatisfaction × sweet taste reward sensitivity0.070.030.02[0.01, 0.13]
Table 3. Conditional Effects of Body Dissatisfaction at Values of Sweet Taste Reward Sensitivity and Dietary Restraint.
Table 3. Conditional Effects of Body Dissatisfaction at Values of Sweet Taste Reward Sensitivity and Dietary Restraint.
Sweet Taste Reward SensitivityDietary RestraintEstimateSEp95% CI
−1 SD−1 SD0.140.080.08[−0.02, 0.29]
−1 SDMean0.100.070.13[−0.03, 0.24]
−1 SD+1 SD0.080.080.36[−0.09, 0.24]
Mean−1 SD0.230.07<0.001[0.10, 0.36]
MeanMean0.200.05<0.001[0.10, 0.30]
Mean+1 SD0.170.070.01[0.04, 0.30]
+1 SD−1 SD0.320.08<0.001[0.17, 0.48]
+1 SDMean0.290.06<0.001[0.17, 0.42]
+1 SD+1 SD0.260.07<0.001[0.12, 0.41]
Note. SD = standard deviation.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Mason, T.B.; Dolgon-Krutolow, A.; Smith, K.E.; Leventhal, A.M. Body Dissatisfaction and Binge Eating: The Moderating Roles of Sweet Taste Reward Sensitivity and Dietary Restraint among Tobacco Product Users. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 15523. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192315523

AMA Style

Mason TB, Dolgon-Krutolow A, Smith KE, Leventhal AM. Body Dissatisfaction and Binge Eating: The Moderating Roles of Sweet Taste Reward Sensitivity and Dietary Restraint among Tobacco Product Users. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(23):15523. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192315523

Chicago/Turabian Style

Mason, Tyler B., Anna Dolgon-Krutolow, Kathryn E. Smith, and Adam M. Leventhal. 2022. "Body Dissatisfaction and Binge Eating: The Moderating Roles of Sweet Taste Reward Sensitivity and Dietary Restraint among Tobacco Product Users" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 23: 15523. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192315523

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop