Next Article in Journal
Eye Movement Desensitization and Reprocessing versus Cognitive Behavior Therapy for Treating Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis
Next Article in Special Issue
Primary Validation of the Submandibular Skinfold as an Anthropometric Measurement of Cardiometabolic Risk in People with Intellectual Disabilities
Previous Article in Journal
Validity and Reliability of Questionnaires That Assess Barriers and Facilitators of Sedentary Behavior in the Pediatric Population: A Systematic Review
Previous Article in Special Issue
Effectiveness of Teaching Mini Handball through Non-Linear Pedagogy in Different Socioeconomic Contexts: A Pilot Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Relationship between Learning and Psychomotor Skills in Early Childhood Education

by
José Manuel Alonso-Vargas
1,
Eduardo Melguizo-Ibáñez
1,*,
Pilar Puertas-Molero
1,
Federico Salvador-Pérez
2 and
José Luis Ubago-Jiménez
1
1
Faculty of Education Sciences, Department of Didactics of Musical, Plastic and Corporal Expression, University of Granada, 18071 Granada, Spain
2
Universidad Internacional de la Rioja (UNIR), 26006 La Rioja, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(24), 16835; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192416835
Submission received: 2 November 2022 / Revised: 5 December 2022 / Accepted: 14 December 2022 / Published: 15 December 2022

Abstract

:
Psychomotor skills are, among others, an aspect particularly valuable for structuring the teaching–learning process of infant schoolchildren. For this reason, a study was carried out with the aim of describing and comparing the socio-demographic, psychomotor, and learning levels of schoolchildren in the second stage of infant education. Ninety-five pupils from the second cycle of infant education in the capital of Granada took part in this study. A sociodemographic questionnaire, the movement assessment battery for children-2 (MABC-2), and the preschool learning behaviour scale (PLBS) were used to collect data. The main results show that manual dexterity appears as the main motor factor and similar figures in the three dimensions of learning behaviours. On the other hand, balance and learning behaviours were higher in 6-year-old schoolchildren. In terms of gender, girls obtained higher values for the level of the learning behaviour variables. A positive correlation was found between the dimensions of learning and motor activity.

1. Introduction

Early childhood education is the most important stage in a person’s life cycle [1]. This period covers the first five years of life, being the phase of greatest intellectual progression of the human being [2,3].
This stage of change, where the child leaves home and enters a school institution, begins to socialise among peers, and to comply with schedules, rules, or regulations, requires a high cognitive effort as the infant will begin to interpret, analyse, and predict among other complex actions [4]. On the other hand, important for the objective of encouraging the child’s learning is the involvement of the teacher and his or her contribution to the overall development of the student beyond the cognitive aspect [5], as well as for the proper development of the child in the centre [6].
The characteristics of this vital period mean that, from the educational environment, the child’s innate abilities can be strengthened to the maximum through experiences with which the child will develop new skills or affinities. Therefore, the care given to this stage will be decisive, in accordance with Alliaume [7]; every protective measure must safeguard integrity, i.e., provide support in the cognitive, physical, and socio-affective aspects, which implement child development. To this must be added the promotion of an academic offer that favours the potentiality of these aspects [8]. On the other hand, neural connections at this stage are another axis to be promoted, as scientific findings confirm that the brain develops and is not born as it is [9]. This evolution occurs even before birth, with an ambiguous interaction between neural connections and experiences and the environment, being the foundation of postnatal learning and memory [10].
Pedagogically, active methodologies are increasingly accepted in the preschool stage [11], in line with Pestalozzi’s approach [12]. The model established by Froebel, based on the child as an active agent, proposes that children should be discoverers and actors in their learning [13]. Piaget, from a psychological and pedagogical approach, emphasises that learning involves sensory-motor skills [14,15]. Likewise, Wallon highlights the motor dimension as a component of the individual [16]. Decroly gives importance to visual, motor, and auditory play among others [17,18]. Research highlights early stimulation as a factor that favours brain plasticity in children [19]. Montessori, in turn, relies on the non-obstruction of the preschooler’s individuality and expressive freedom [20,21,22]. In this sense, the WHO [23] recommends the implementation of active methodologies that favour and encourage the acquisition of physical activity habits that remain over time [24].
At an early age, motor skills are key to the development of psychological functions, and it is through movement that the most basic forms of relationship and communication emerge [25]. Motor acts such as standing and grasping only occur in humans who have thrived in civilization, so that, in addition to inherited abilities, environmental behaviours and learning are necessary for them to develop [26].
Psychomotor practice is also inherently playful, procedural, and enjoyable, moving away from the monotony and sedentary nature of many school activities, which has a positive effect on children’s stimulation and participation [27]. Well-directed and planned physical education will contribute to physical, social, affective, psychological, and emotional development in the preschool stage, as well as being a good way to generate learning in other dimensions of knowledge [1,28]. Likewise, difficulties or problems in cognitive, academic, socio-emotional, or other skills development may be detected [28].
Recent studies have shown that active learning improves psychomotor skills, teamwork, reflection, participation, autonomy, responsibility, and the acceleration of meaningful learning [29,30]. In other words, an increase in physical activity is directly related to an increase in physical, cognitive, emotional, and social capacity [28,31], also confirming the effectiveness of the integrated teaching of physical education with areas such as mathematics [32] or languages [27].
There is a high percentage of fine motor delay in children aged 3 to 6 years [33]. At this early stage, the body and movement are determining factors in the learning and development of individuals [34]. Other aspects to be highlighted are its preventive property against difficulties and pathologies derived from a sedentary lifestyle or its palliative condition in terms of the consequences of some disabilities, as well as adherence to sport and a healthy lifestyle [35]. It should be emphasised that the legislative instruments guiding this period of infant education are committed to the development of these aspects, which are present in Organic Law 3/2020, of 29 December, which modifies Organic Law 2/2006, of 3 May, on education (LOMLOE) [36]. This law reveals the value of physical and healthy activity in the learning and training of schoolchildren.
There is a scarcity of studies and analyses relating to this early stage with respect to others at higher ages. It is important to understand the variables contemplated as aspects of vital importance at this stage, especially those involved in delays in fine motor skills in preschool children, as well as the worrying levels of sedentary lifestyle that currently exist, and their possible influence on learning. This study aims to describe the psychomotor and learning behaviour levels in schoolchildren in infant education and, in turn, to relate these parameters to each other.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Design and Participants

This study presents a descriptive and cross-sectional design that analyses a total sample of 95 early childhood education students, from the same school, in the centre of the city of Granada (Spain), aged between 4 and 6 years, from which, 50.5% (N = 48) were boys and 49.5% (N = 47) girls. The characteristics of the geographical area indicate a medium-high socioeconomic level. The participants belong to six classes of this school with a ratio of approximately twenty-two students per class. The motor activities carried out by these students at school present a lack in terms of dedication time, this being an aspect to consider.

2.2. Instruments and Variables

An ad hoc class questionnaire was used to collect socio-demographic variables such as year of birth, gender, and academic level of the pupil’s parents. The following instruments were used for the analysis of the remaining variables:
  • Movement assessment battery for children 2 (MABC-2), in its Spanish adaptation by Ruiz and Graupera-Sanz [37], intended to collect data on the psychomotor variable. This battery, in its application for the infant stage, is composed of eight tests that assess three main aspects: manual dexterity, aiming and catching, and balance. In this version, a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.81 was obtained for the complete battery.
    -
    Manual dexterity: three tests; inserting coins into the slot of a small box (six coins for 4- and 5-year-olds and twelve coins for 6-year-olds), stringing beads (six beads for 4- and 5-year-olds and twelve beads for 6-year-olds), and drawing lines on paper with a felt-tip pen.
    -
    Aiming and catching: two tests; catching a small beanbag and throwing the sack at a target.
    -
    Balance: three tests; tiptoeing, balancing on one leg, and jumping on mats.
Furthermore, in terms of reliability analysis, Cronbach’s alpha obtained a value of α = 0.856.
  • Preschool learning behaviours Scale (PLBS) [38]. It consists of twenty-nine items, which are intended to measure the behavioural variable of children’s learning in terms of three parameters: their motivation for competence, their attitude towards learning, and their attention or persistence. Each section offers three possible answers, almost always, sometimes, and usually not, with the assigned values being 2, 1, and 0, respectively. Of these 29 items, numbers 1, 4, 11, 20, 25, and 28 are positively phrased, the remaining items being negative and therefore of inverted valence. Finally, the reliability analysis obtained a value of α = 0.855.

2.3. Procedure

Firstly, a collaboration agreement was reached between the school and the research team. In order to collect the data using the instruments described above, once they had been confirmed and prior to filling them in, the preschool teaching staff were informed, being shown the different tests together with a document justifying the study. The school management was also informed, and a letter was sent to the school, with both parties giving their approval for the study to be carried out. Subsequently, we distributed an envelope for each pupil to the different classrooms, which contained an explanatory document and a sheet containing an explanation of the procedure, the sociodemographic test to be filled in by the pupil’s parent or guardian, the MABC-2 battery of activities, and the PLBS test, which, as stated at the end of the page, would be filled in by the research team. Once the families had returned the document with their part completed, we proceeded to contact the tutors of the preschool groups to find a suitable time to carry out the battery of activities with their pupils and, secondly, to ask for their collaboration in the PLBS test. This research was assessed by the ethics committee from the University of Granada (1478/CEIH/2021). The research team was made up of the authors of this manuscript, who had the support and cooperation of teachers from the educational centre where the study was carried out. The entire process strictly followed the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki.

2.4. Data Analysis

The data were processed with the SPSS 25.0 statistical software (SPSS, IBM, SPSS Statistics, v.25.0, Chicago, IL, USA). The normality and homogeneity of variance of the variables were tested using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. For the descriptive analysis, a study of frequencies and basic descriptive variables was carried out. Subsequently, contingency tables, T-Student, ANOVA, and Pearson’s bivariate correlations were used for the comparative analysis. To establish statistically significant differences, the Pearson Chi-Square test was used. In this case, the significance level was set at p < 0.05.

3. Results

Regarding Table 1 the MABC-2, the highest mean value (M = 0.95) is in manual dexterity, followed by balance (M = 0.76), and finally aiming and catching (M = 0.65). For the learning variable, the highest mean value is found in attention/persistence (M = 1.73), followed by attitude towards learning (M = 1.70), and motivation for competence (M = 1.67).
Regarding Table 2 the relationship between the MABC-2 and age, no differences were found in the parameters of manual skills and aiming and catching (p ≥ 0.050). However, in balance, statistically significant differences were found (p = 0.000), due to the fact that students aged 6 years show a better mean (M = 0.96) than those aged 4 years (M = 0.60).
For the relationship between the MABC-2 and the academic level of both parents, no statistically significant differences were found (p ≥ 0.050).
In terms of the relationship (Table 3) between learning and gender, statistically significant differences were found in the parameters of motivation for competence (p = 0.020), and attention/persistence (p = 0.002), due to a higher mean in the female gender, as can be seen in the following table:
Regarding Table 4 the relationship between learning and age, statistically significant differences were found in the parameters of motivation for competence (p = 0.001), attitude towards learning (p = 0.000), and attention/persistence (p = 0.001), generated by a higher mean in 6-year-olds, as shown in the following table:
Regarding the relationship between learning and the father’s and mother’s academic levels, no statistically significant differences were found (p ≥ 0.050).
Regarding the correlations (Table 5), the results indicated that manual skills correlate moderately and positively with the dimensions of learning (E, r = 0.347 **; ACT, r = 0.437 **; AP, r = 0.377 **) in such a way that, as one increases, so do the other dimensions. Likewise, moderate and positive correlations are detected in aiming and catching with the dimensions balance (r = 0.435 **), attitude (r = 0.322 **), and attention/persistence (r = 0.309 **). Similarly, balance correlates with attitude towards learning (r = 0.336 **). Motivation for competence correlates strongly and positively with the dimensions (ACT, r = 0.532 **; AP, r = 0.801 **), just as attitude towards learning correlates with attention and persistence (r = 0.731 **).

4. Discussion

The academic level of the parents of the students is mostly university level, which is consistent with those reported by Álvarez et al. [39], Li et al. [40], and Merino et al. [41]. These data do not coincide with the majority in the average academic level reported by Määttä et al. [42] and Martisone et al. [43], with the understanding that the area of Granada where the school is located is of a medium-high socio-economic level, so that families who live in the area and enrol their children in this educational institution have a higher value in this aspect, which coincides with what is stated by OECD [44] in terms of the comparison between rural and urban areas.
Regarding MABC-2, the highest mean value is in manual skills, which is similar to Hirata et al. [45] and Van der Veer et al. [46], explaining this by the educational models based on manual work over other aspects, which, being also motor, are not so important.
In learning, the highest mean value is found in attention/persistence, and these data correspond to those reported by Angelo [47], which indicates the importance given to this ability within the educational institution, as it is a determining factor both in the day-to-day life of the classroom and in the acquisition of knowledge by the pupils. The female gender presents a higher mean in the three subscales of the test, which is in agreement with what has been reported by Johnson [48] and Valiente et al. [49], who indicate that girls have a better ability to manage learning strategies as well as stress control. Schoolchildren aged 6 years showed a higher motivation for competence, attitude towards learning, and attention/persistence, which is postulated in contrast to the findings of Sáez et al. [27] and Schaefer et al. [50], who indicated in their studies that learning behaviours remain constant with increasing age. Thus, it is understood that this discrepancy is caused by the different methodologies and learning strategies used by schools and specifically by the teaching teams.
In balance, differences were found because the 6-year-old students showed a better average, as Reina and de Haro [51] pointed out, when they indicated that physical abilities evolve with age; therefore, the 6-year-old schoolchildren analysed showed better balance than those of younger ages. Manual skills and balance correlate moderately and positively with the dimensions of learning, so that as one increases, so do the others. Likewise, motivation for competence correlates strongly and positively with motor dimensions, and attitude towards learning correlates with attention and persistence, which is in line with Franco et al. [52] and Pizani et al. [53], who noted that motivation towards learning and motor generated an increase in both cases, so that, in agreement with Tandon et al. [54], a quality motor environment has a positive impact on schoolchildren’s learning.

5. Conclusions

The following findings were obtained from this descriptive, cross-sectional research study:
The students showed, at the motor level, that the highest dimension was manual skills, with no significant differences in terms of gender in any motor variable. In learning, attention/persistence, attitude towards learning, and motivation for competence obtained similar values, with a higher mean in 6-year-old students. Similarly, balance was higher in schoolchildren of this age. In the female gender, higher values were obtained in all three learning subscales. A positive correlation was found between the dimensions of learning and the motor parameters analysed.
Following the results obtained, the need to promote and encourage physical activity in preschoolers through active methodologies is observed. This will allow, according to the results of this study, the levels of learning behaviours from psychomotor work at school to be improved.
A series of limitations have been evidenced based on the characteristics of this research model. Its cross-sectional, descriptive typology has allowed only one measurement to be carried out at a specific point in time and the relationships between the variables to be observed at that time, resulting in the impossibility of establishing cause–effect relationships that some variables apply to others. On the other hand, it should be stressed that the sample in this study focuses on a very specific type of student and a specific geographical area, which prevents the results obtained from becoming generalisable. Finally, it should be noted that several questionnaires intended for the children’s families were not filled in correctly, which reduced the sample of participants to be analysed.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.M.A.-V., P.P.-M. and F.S.-P.; methodology, J.M.A.-V. and E.M.-I.; software, J.M.A.-V.; validation, J.M.A.-V., P.P.-M. and F.S.-P.; formal analysis, J.M.A.-V., J.L.U.-J. and E.M.-I.; investigation, J.M.A.-V.; resources, J.L.U.-J.; data curation, J.M.A.-V.; writing—original draft preparation, J.M.A.-V.; P.P.-M. and F.S.-P.; writing—review and editing, J.M.A.-V., P.P.-M. and J.L.U.-J.; visualization, J.M.A.-V.; supervision, J.L.U.-J. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the University of Granada (1478/CEIH/2021); the approval date was 13 May 2021.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data used to support the findings of the current study are available from the corresponding author upon request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Arufe, V. ¿Cómo debe ser el trabajo de educación Física en educación infantil? Retos 2020, 37, 588–596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Mero, M.; Morán, D.; Espinoza, E.; Cabrera, D.; Sánchez, O. El potenciamiento de las capacidades humanas: Los Centros Infantiles del Buen Vivir en el Ecuador. Espacios 2018, 39, 32. Available online: https://www.revistaespacios.com/a18v39n32/a18v39n32p32.pdf (accessed on 23 October 2022).
  3. Nelson, K. El camino desde la primera infancia a la comunidad de mentes compartidas. Infanc. Aprendiz. 2014, 37, 12–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Duque, C.; Vera, Á. Exploración de la comprensión inferencial de textos narrativos en niños de pre escolar. Rev. Colomb. Psicol. 2010, 19, 21–35. Available online: https://repositorio.unal.edu.co/handle/unal/24397 (accessed on 23 October 2022).
  5. Veraska, N.; Shiyan, O.; Shiyan, I.; Pramling, N.; Pramling, I. La comunicación entre profesor y alumno en la educación infantil: La teoría Vygotskiana y la práctica educativa. Infanc. Aprendiz. 2016, 39, 221–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Zych, I.; Ortega, R.; Sibaja, S. El juego infantil y el desarrollo afectivo afecto, ajuste escolar y aprendizaje en la etapa preescolar. Infanc. Aprendiz. 2016, 39, 390–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Alliaume, J. Educación en la primera infancia: Una cuestión de derechos. Rev. Latinoam. Educ. Infant. 2015, 4, 135–144. Available online: https://revistas.usc.es/index.php/reladei/article/view/4886 (accessed on 22 October 2022).
  8. Ruiz, Á.; Abad, J. Lugares de juego y encuentro para la infancia. Rev. Iberoamérica Educ. 2016, 71, 37–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Shonkoff, J.P.; Garner, A.S.; Committee on Psychosocial Aspects of Child and Family Health; Committee on Early Childhood, Adoption, and Dependent Care; Section on Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics; Siegel, B.S.; Dobbins, M.I.; Earls, M.F.; Garner, A.S.; McGuinn, L.; et al. The lifelong effects of early childhood adversity and toxic stress. Pediatrics 2012, 129, 232–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Zhao, T.; Mishra, V.; Jeon, T.; Ouyang, M.; Peng, Q.; Chalak, L.; Lee, J.; Heyne, R.; Rollins, N.; Shu, N.; et al. Structural network maturation of the preterm human brain. NeuroImage 2019, 185, 699–710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Nielsen, A.; Romance, R.; Dobado, J.C. Teaching Methodologies and School Organization in Early Childhood Education and Its Association with Physical Activity. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 3836. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Aguiar, M.; Rodríguez, J. Pestalozzi, una pedagogía sistémica. Guiniguada. Rev. Inves. Experi. Cienc. Ed. 2017, 26, 7–12. Available online: https://ojsspdc.ulpgc.es/ojs/index.php/ElGuiniguada/article/view/829 (accessed on 16 October 2022).
  13. Chamon, C. A trajetoria profissional de uma educadora: María Guilhermina e a pedagogia Norte-América. Rev. Hist. Educão 2008, 20, 73–99. Available online: https://seer.ufrgs.br/asphe/article/view/29227 (accessed on 22 October 2022).
  14. Cárdenas, A. Piaget: Lenguaje, conocimiento y Educación. Rev. Colomb. Educ. 2011, 60, 71–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Arbués, E.; Ibarrola, S.; Magallón, S. Las competencias básicas en la formación inicial del profesorado de primaria. Una metodología transversal desde las didácticas específicas. Magister 2014, 26, 34–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Picanço, A.; Noronha, T. Ambiente pedagógico na educação infantil e a contribuição da psicologia. Psicol. Esc. Educ. 2016, 20, 229–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Carrillo, D.; Maurandi, A.; Olivares, P. Los juegos decrolyanos matemáticos y los catálogos de material escolar en España (1920–1936). Paedagogica Historica. Int. J. Hist. Educ. 2021, 57, 85–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Decroly, O.; Monchamp, E. El Juego Educativo: Iniciación a La Actividad Intelectual y Motriz; Ediciones Morata: Madrid, Spain, 1983; Available online: https://dokumen.pub/el-juego-educativo-iniciacion-a-la-actividad-intelectual-motriz-8471122162.html (accessed on 23 October 2022).
  19. Sibaja, J.; Sánchez, T.; Rojas, M.; Fornaguera, J. De la neuroplasticidad a las propuestas aplicadas: Estimulación temprana y su implementación en Costa Rica. Rev. Costarric. Psicol. 2016, 35, 159–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Cascella, M. (María Montessori (1870–1952). Emancipación de la mujer, pedagogía y comunicación extra verbal. Rev. Médica Chile 2015, 143, 658–662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Khachatryan, M. Una mirada al programa de inglés preescolar de AUA a través de la lente de la pedagogía Montessori. Procedia-Soc. Behav. Sci. 2015, 197, 304–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Ansari, A.; Winsler, A. Programas de Preescolar de las Escuelas Públicas Montessori y la preparación escolar de niños negros y latinos de bajos ingresos. Rev. Psicol. Educ. 2014, 106, 1066–1079. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. WHO. Global Recommendations on Physical Activity for Health; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  24. Marques, A.; Gómez, F.; Martins, J.; Caunda, R.; Sarmento, H. Association between physical education, school-based physical activity, and academic performance: A systematic review. Retos Nuevas Tend. Educ. Física Deporte Recreación 2017, 31, 316–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Chatzihidiroglou, P.; Chatzopoulos, D.; Lykesas, G.; Doganis, G. Dancing effects on preschoolers’ sensorimotor synchronization, balance, and movement reaction time. Percept. Mot. Ski. 2018, 125, 463–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Wulandari, R.T.; Kustiawan, U. Evaluation study of early formal education teacher competence in early childhood learning dance at kindergartens in Malang, Indonesia. Res. Educ. Media 2018, 10, 3–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Sáez, M.B.; Gil, P.; Martínez, M. Desarrollo psicomotor y su vinculación con la motivación hacia el aprendizaje y el rendimiento académico en Educación Infantil. Rev. Educ. 2021, 392, 177–203. Available online: https://hdl.handle.net/11162/205984 (accessed on 23 October 2022).
  28. Mas, M.; Jimanez, L.; Riera, C. Systematization of the Psychomotor Activity and Cognitive Development. Psicol. Educ. 2018, 24, 38–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Aristizabal, J.; Ramos, A.; Chirino, V. Aprendizaje activo para el desarrollo de la psicomotricidad y el trabajo en equipo. Rev. Electrónica Educ. 2018, 22, 319–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Gómez, E.M.; Arboleda, I.G. Análisis de la psicomotricidad a través de la práctica psicomotriz de Bernard Aucouturier: Estudio de casos. Retos Nuevas Tend. Educ. Física Deporte Recreación 2021, 39, 620–627. Available online: https://recyt.fecyt.es/index.php/retos/article/view/81781/61438 (accessed on 13 October 2022).
  31. Martínez, V.; Antón, A. Efectos de un programa de intervención psicomotriz en preadolescentes con síndrome de Down. Infanc. Aprendiz. 2018, 41, 165–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Hraste, M.; De Giorgio, A.; Jelaska, P.M.; Padulo, J.; Granić, I. When mathematics meets physical activity in the school-aged child: The effect of an integrated motor and cognitive approach to learning geometry. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0196024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Delgado, L.; Montes, R. Perfil y desarrollo psicomotor de los niños españoles entre 3 y 6 años. Sportis 2017, 3, 454–470. Available online: http://hdl.handle.net/2183/22783 (accessed on 23 October 2022). [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Prieto, J.P.; Jiménez, N.G.; Sanz, D.B.; Herrero, D.C. La sala de psicomotricidad para el trabajo de educación física en educación infantil: Un estudio exploratorio. Retos Nuevas Tend. Educ. Física Deporte Recreación 2021, 39, 106–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Heron, M.; Gil, P.; Sáez, M.B. Contribución de la terapia psicomotriz al progreso de niños con discapacidades. Rev. Fac. Med. 2018, 66, 75–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Ley Orgánica 3/2020, de 29 de Diciembre, Por la que se Modifica la Ley Orgánica 2/2006, de 3 de mayo, de Educación; Boletín Oficial del Estado, 340, de 30 de Diciembre de 2020, 122868–122953. Available online: https://boe.es/buscar/pdf/2006/BOE-A-2006-7899-consolidado.pdf (accessed on 23 October 2022).
  37. Ruiz, L.M.; Graupera, J.L. Adaptación española de la batería de evaluación del movimiento para niños-2 (MABC-2): Una herramienta científica para los profesionales de la educación física. En: IV Congreso Internacional de Ciencias del Deporte y la Educación Física. In Proceedings of the VIII Seminario Nacional de Nutrición, Medicina y Rendimiento Deportivo, Pontevedra, Spain, 5 December 2012; ISBN 978-84-939424-2-7. [Google Scholar]
  38. McDermott, P.A.; Green, L.F.; Francis, J.M.; Stott, D.H. PLBS. Preschool Learning Behaviors Scale; Edumetric & Clinical Science: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  39. Álvarez, C.; Guzmán, I.P.; Latorre, P.Á.; Párraga, J.; Palomino, C.; Reyes, F.A.; Paredes, L.; Leal, M.; Obando, I.; Cresp, M.; et al. Association between the sociodemographic characteristics of parents with health-related and lifestyle markers of children in three different Spanish-speaking countries: An inter-continental study at OECD country level. Nutrients 2021, 13, 2672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Li, T.; Bindels, J.G.; Zhang, S.; Bronceado, Z.; Jia, N.; Liu, A.; Zhu, Z.; Dai, Y. A dietary and nutritional status survey among young children in five big cities of China. Asia Pac. J. Clin. Nutr. 2018, 27, 1095–1105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Merino, I.; Mora, J.; Cadenas, C.; Borras, P.A.; Benito, P.J.; Chiva, O.; Torrijos, C.; Samaniego, C.; Quesada, J.J.; Sánchez, A.; et al. Higher socioeconomic status is related to healthier levels of fatness and fitness already at 3 to 5 years of age: The PREFIT project: Relation between socioeconomic status, fatness and fitness in preschoolers. J. Sport Sci. 2019, 37, 1327–1337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Määttä, S.; Kaukonen, R.; Vepsäläinen, H.; Lehto, E.; Ylönen, A.; Ray, C.; Erkkola, M.; Roos, E. The mediating role of the home environment in relation to parental educational level and preschool children’s screen time: A cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health 2017, 17, 688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Martinsone, B.; Supe, I.; Stokenberga, I.; Damberga, I.; Cefai, C.; Camilleri, L.; Bartolo, P.; O’Riordan, M.R.; Grazzani, I. Social Emotional Competence, Learning Outcomes, Emotional and Behavioral Difficulties of Preschool Children: Parent and Teacher Evaluations. Front. Psychol. 2022, 12, 6403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. OECD. PISA 2018 results (Volume II): Where all students can succeed. In PISA; OECD Publishing: Paris, France, 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Hirata, S.; Kita, Y.; Yasunaga, M.; Suzuki, K.; Okumura, Y.; Okuzumi, H.; Hosobuchi, T.; Kokubun, M.; Inagaki, M.; Nakai, A. Aplicabilidad de la batería de evaluación del movimiento para niños (MABC-2) para niños japoneses de 3 a 6 años: Una investigación preliminar que enfatiza la consistencia interna y la validez factorial. Front. Psicol. 2018, 9, 1452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Van der Veer, G.; Kamphorst, E.; Minnaert, A.; Cantell, M.; Kakebeeke, T.H.; Houwen, S. Assessing Motor Performance in Preschool Children: The Zurich Neuromotor Assessment-2 and the Movement Assessment Battery for Children-2. Percept. Mot. Ski. 2021, 128, 2014–2032. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Angelo, L.E. Child-centered examination of preschool learning behavior: A typological investigation. ProQuest 2006. Available online: https://repository.upenn.edu/dissertations/AAI3225426 (accessed on 23 October 2022).
  48. Johnson, C.L. Learning Behaviors and Academic Readiness Among Preschool Children in a Private School. Ph.D. Thesis, Rowan University, Glassboro, NJ, USA, 2017. Available online: https://rdw.rowan.edu/etd/2409 (accessed on 23 October 2022).
  49. Valiente, C.; Martínez, M.; Cabal, P.; Alvarado, J.M. Estrés infantil, estrategias de aprendizaje y motivación académica: Un modelo estructural predictor del rendimiento académico. Rev. Psicol. Educ. 2020, 15, 46–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Schaefer, B.A.; Shur, K.F.; Macri-Summers, M.; MacDonald, S.L. Preschool Children’s Learning Behaviors, Concept Attainment, Social Skills, and Problem Behaviors: Validity Evidence for Preschool Learning Behaviors Scale Scores. J. Psychoeduc. Assessmen 2004, 22, 15–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Reina, L.; de Haro, V.M. Manual de Teoría y Práctica del Acondicionamiento Físico. CV Ciencias del Deporte. 2003. Available online: https://cmapspublic.ihmc.us/rid=1K1NBMTPW-1J7GZ76-55W/metodos%20de%20entrenamiento%20deportivo.pdf (accessed on 23 October 2022).
  52. Franco, E.; Coterón, J.; Martínez, H.A.; Brito, J. Perfiles motivacionales en estudiantes de educación física de tres países y su relación con la actividad física. Suma Psicológica 2016, 24, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Pizani, J.; Barbosa-Rinaldi, I.P.; Monteiro, A.C.; Fiorese, L. (Des) motivação na educação física escolar: Uma análise a partir da teoria da autodeterminação. Ciências Do Esporte 2016, 38, 259–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Tandon, P.; Hassairi, N.; Soderberg, J.; Joseph, G. The relationship of gross motor and physical activity environments in child care settings with early learning outcomes. Early Child Dev. Care 2018, 4, 570–579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Table 1. Descriptive data of the study.
Table 1. Descriptive data of the study.
GenderFather’s Academic LevelMother’s Academic Level
Male50.5% (n = 48)University Education73.7% (n = 70)University Education85.3% (n = 81)
Female49.5% (n = 47)Baccalaureate9.5% (n = 9)Baccalaureate4.2% (n = 4)
AgeSecondary Education5.3% (n = 5)Secondary Education1.1% (n = 1)
4 years old32.6% (n = 31)Primary Education1.1% (n = 1)Primary Education0% (n = 0)
5 years old34.7% (n = 33)Vocational Education8.4% (n = 8)Vocational Education6.3% (n = 6)
6 years old32.6% (n = 31)Others2.1% (n = 2)Others3.2% (n = 3)
Psychomotricity (MABC-2)Learning (PLBS)
Manual skillsM = 0.95MotivationM = 1.67
Aiming and trappingM = 0.65AttitudeM = 1.70
BalanceM = 0.76Attention/persistenceM = 1.73
Table 2. ANOVA of the MABC-2 in relation to the age of the participants.
Table 2. ANOVA of the MABC-2 in relation to the age of the participants.
DimensionsAgeMeanSDFSig.
Manual skills4 years0.920.2051.6880.191
5 years0.930.211
6 years1.000.000
Aiming and trapping4 years0.580.3182.7990.066
5 years0.600.409
6 years0.770.311
Balance4 years0.600.26419.4800.000
5 years0.730.285
6 years0.960.100
Table 3. ANOVA for learning and gender.
Table 3. ANOVA for learning and gender.
DimensionsGenderMeanSDFSig.
Motivation for competenceFemale1.760.3295.6210.020
Male1.580.415
Attitude towards learningFemale1.750.2621.9950.161
Male1.660.315
Attention/persistenceFemale1.870.33510.0450.002
Male1.590.506
Table 4. ANOVA for learning and age of participants.
Table 4. ANOVA for learning and age of participants.
DimensionsAgeMeanSDFSig.
Motivation for competence4 years1.740.2297.6230.001
5 years1.470.516
6 years1.800.244
Attitude towards learning4 years1.790.21712.7210.000
5 years1.520.326
6 years1.820.218
Attention/persistence4 years1.800.3867.2670.001
5 years1.510.542
6 years1.900.292
Table 5. Pearson’s correlation between learning and MABC-2.
Table 5. Pearson’s correlation between learning and MABC-2.
MSACBMOTATLAP
MS1
AC0.1431
B0.347 **0.435 **1
MOT0.267 **0.252 *0.1301
ATL0.437 **0.322 **0.336 **0.532 **1
AP0.377 **0.309 **0.290 **0.801 **0.731 **1
Note. * Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (bilateral); ** Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (bilateral); MS, manual skills; AC, aiming and catching; B, balance; MOT, motivation for competence; ATL, attitude towards learning; AP, attention/persistence.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Alonso-Vargas, J.M.; Melguizo-Ibáñez, E.; Puertas-Molero, P.; Salvador-Pérez, F.; Ubago-Jiménez, J.L. Relationship between Learning and Psychomotor Skills in Early Childhood Education. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 16835. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192416835

AMA Style

Alonso-Vargas JM, Melguizo-Ibáñez E, Puertas-Molero P, Salvador-Pérez F, Ubago-Jiménez JL. Relationship between Learning and Psychomotor Skills in Early Childhood Education. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(24):16835. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192416835

Chicago/Turabian Style

Alonso-Vargas, José Manuel, Eduardo Melguizo-Ibáñez, Pilar Puertas-Molero, Federico Salvador-Pérez, and José Luis Ubago-Jiménez. 2022. "Relationship between Learning and Psychomotor Skills in Early Childhood Education" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 24: 16835. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph192416835

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop