Boredom Proneness Predicts Self-Assessed Decision Errors in Sports but Is Unrelated to Risk Taking in General
Abstract
:1. Boredom Proneness Predicts Self-Assessed Decision Errors in Sports but Is Unrelated to Risk Taking in General
2. Method
2.1. Design, Participants, and Sample Size Considerations
2.2. Procedure
2.3. Material
2.3.1. Boredom Proneness
2.3.2. Risk-Taking Behavior: Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART)
2.3.3. Self-Reported Risk-Taking Behavior and Decision Making
3. Results
3.1. Boredom Proneness and Risk Taking
3.2. Boredom Proneness and Sports-Specific Decision-Making
3.3. Exploratory Analyses: Boredom as Dependent Variable
4. Discussion
4.1. Boredom Proneness Is Associated with Noise, Not with Risk
4.2. Boredom Proneness Is Associated with Athletic Level and Sporting Choice
4.3. Further Considerations and Directions for Future Research
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Westgate, E.C. Why Boredom Is Interesting. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2020, 29, 33–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Danckert, J.; Eastwood, J.D. Out of my skull. In The Psychology of Boredom; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- van Tilburg, W.A.P.; Igou, E.R. Boredom begs to differ: Differentiation from other negative emotions. Emotion 2017, 17, 309–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pekrun, R.; Goetz, T.; Daniels, L.M.; Stupnisky, R.H.; Perry, R.P. Boredom in achievement settings: Exploring control–value antecedents and performance outcomes of a neglected emotion. J. Educ. Psychol. 2010, 102, 531–549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martarelli, C.S.; Wolff, W. Too bored to bother? Boredom as a potential threat to the efficacy of pandemic containment measures. Humanit. Soc. Sci. Commun. 2020, 7, 28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pfattheicher, S.; Lazarević, L.B.; Westgate, E.C.; Schindler, S. On the relation of boredom and sadistic aggression. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2021, 121, 573–600. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davies, A.H. Discussion on the physical and mental effects of monotony in modern industry. Br. Med. J. 1926, 2, 472–479. [Google Scholar]
- Wolff, W.; Bieleke, M.; Martarelli, C.S.; Danckert, J. A Primer on the Role of Boredom in Self-Controlled Sports and Exercise Behavior. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 637839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wolff, W.; Bieleke, M.; Stähler, J.; Schüler, J. Too bored for sports? Adaptive and less-adaptive latent personality profiles for exercise behavior. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2021, 53, 101851. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Velasco, F.; Jorda, R. Portrait of Boredom among Athletes and Its Implications in Sports Management: A Multi-Method Approach. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Elpidorou, A. The good of boredom. Philos. Psychol. 2018, 31, 323–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bieleke, M.; Wolff, W.; Englert, C.; Gollwitzer, P.M. If-then Planning in Sports: A Scoping Review. Zeitschrift für Sportpsychologie 2020, 28, 109–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bench, S.W.; Lench, H.C. On the function of boredom. Behav. Sci. 2013, 3, 459–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bieleke, M.; Wolff, W. It’s not a bug, it’s boredom: Effortful willpower balances exploitation and exploration. Behav. Brain Sci. 2021, 44, e33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Eastwood, J.D.; Frischen, A.; Fenske, M.J.; Smilek, D. The Unengaged Mind: Defining Boredom in Terms of Attention. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2012, 7, 482–495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wolff, W.; Martarelli, C. Bored into depletion? Towards a tentative integration of perceived self-control exertion and boredom as guiding signals for goal-directed behavior. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2020, 15, 1272–1283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tam, K.Y.Y.; van Tilburg, W.A.P.; Chan, C.S. What is boredom proneness? A comparison of three characterizations. J. Personal. 2021, 89, 831–846. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wolff, W.; Martarelli, C.; Schüler, J.; Bieleke, M. High boredom proneness and low trait self-control impair adherence to social distancing guidelines during the COVID-19 pandemic. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 5420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sharp, E.H.; Coffman, D.L.; Caldwell, L.L.; Smith, E.A.; Wegner, L.; Vergnani, T.; Mathews, C. Predicting substance use behavior among South African adolescents: The role of leisure experiences across time. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2011, 35, 343–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dahlen, E.R.; Martin, R.C.; Ragan, K.; Kuhlman, M.M. Boredom proneness in anger and aggression: Effects of impulsiveness and sensation seeking. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2004, 37, 1615–1627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Biolcati, R.; Passini, S.; Mancini, G. “I cannot stand the boredom.” Binge drinking expectancies in adolescence. Addict. Behav. Rep. 2016, 3, 70–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Englert, C.; Taylor, I.M. (Eds.) Motivation and Self-Regulation in Sport and Exercise; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2021. [Google Scholar]
- Milyavskaya, M.; Inzlicht, M.; Johnson, T.; Larson, M.J. Reward sensitivity following boredom and cognitive effort: A high-powered neurophysiological investigation. Neuropsychologia 2019, 123, 159–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Blaszczynski, A.; McConaghy, N.; Frankova, A. Boredom proneness in pathological gambling. Psychol. Rep. 1990, 67, 35–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yakobi, O.; Danckert, J. Boredom proneness is associated with noisy decision-making, not risk-taking. Exp. Brain Res. 2021, 239, 1807–1825. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mugon, J.; Struk, A.; Danckert, J. A Failure to Launch: Regulatory Modes and Boredom Proneness. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bieleke, M.; Martarelli, C.; Wolff, W. If-then planning, self-control, and boredom as predictors of adherence to social distancing guidelines: Evidence from a two-wave longitudinal study with a behavioral intervention. Curr. Psychol. 2021; online ahead of print. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bieleke, M.; Keller, L.; Wolff, W. Getting Trapped in a Dead End? Trait Self-Control and Boredom Are Linked to Goal Adjustment. PsyArXiv, 2021; preprint. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cohen, R.; Baluch, B.; Duffy, L.J. Defining Extreme Sport: Conceptions and Misconceptions. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1974. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pedraza-Ramirez, I.; Musculus, L.; Raab, M.; Laborde, S. Setting the scientific stage for esports psychology: A systematic review. Int. Rev. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2020, 13, 319–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Keller, L.; Bieleke, M.; Wolff, W. Bursting balloons – comparison of risk taking between extreme sports, esports, and the general public. Curr. Psychol. 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lejuez, C.W.; Read, J.P.; Kahler, C.W.; Richards, J.B.; Ramsey, S.E.; Stuart, G.L.; Strong, D.R.; Brown, R.A. Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 2002, 8, 75–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dohmen, T.; Falk, A.; Huffman, D.; Sunde, U.; Schupp, J.; Wagner, G.G. Individual Risk Attitudes: Measurement, Determinants, and Behavioral Consequences. J. Eur. Econ. Assoc. 2011, 9, 522–550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Struk, A.A.; Carriere, J.S.A.; Cheyne, J.A.; Danckert, J. A Short Boredom Proneness Scale. Assessment 2017, 24, 346–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martarelli, C.S.; Bertrams, A.; Wolff, W. A Personality Trait-Based Network of Boredom, Spontaneous and Deliberate Mind-Wandering. Assessment 2020, 28, 1915–1931. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Arslan, R.C.; Brümmer, M.; Dohmen, T.; Drewelies, J.; Hertwig, R.; Wagner, G.G. How people know their risk preference. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 15365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kılıç, A.; Tilburg, W.A.; Igou, E.R. Risk-taking increases under boredom. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2020, 33, 257–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wolff, W.; Bieleke, M.; Englert, C.; Bertrams, A.; Schüler, J.; Martarelli, C. A single item measure of self-control—Validation and location in a nomological network of self-control, boredom, and if-then planning. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2021; in press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dahlen, E.R.; Martin, R.C.; Ragan, K.; Kuhlman, M.M. Driving anger, sensation seeking, impulsiveness, and boredom proneness in the prediction of unsafe driving. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2005, 37, 341–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Breivik, G.; Sand, T.S.; Sookermany, A.M. Sensation seeking and risk-taking in the Norwegian population. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2017, 119, 266–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zuckerman, M. Sensation Seeking and Risky Behavior; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Horvath, P.; Zuckerman, M. Sensation seeking, risk appraisal, and risky behavior. Personal. Individ. Differ. 1993, 14, 41–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frey, R.; Pedroni, A.; Mata, R.; Rieskamp, J.; Hertwig, R. Risk preference shares the psychometric structure of major psychological traits. Sci. Adv. 2017, 3, e1701381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bench, S.W.; Bera, J.; Cox, J. State boredom results in optimistic perception of risk and increased risk-taking. Cogn. Emot. 2021, 35, 649–663. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Farmer, R.; Sundberg, N.D. Boredom proneness—The development and correlates of a new scale. J. Personal. Assess. 1986, 50, 4–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bieleke, M.; Ripper, L.; Schüler, J.; Wolff, W. Boredom is the Root of All Evil—Or Is It? A Psychometric Network Approach to Individual Differences in Behavioral Responses to Boredom. PsyArXiv, 2021; preprint. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Variable | Climbers | Snowboarders | Esports Athletes | Total |
---|---|---|---|---|
N survey starts | 570 | 120 | 1288 | 1978 |
N (% completions) | 330 (58%) | 83 (69%) | 523 (41%) | 936 (47%) |
Age | 18–67 years old (M = 31.8 SD = 10.7) | 18–62 years old (M = 29.9 SD = 8.3) | 18–66 years old (M = 24.6 SD = 6.3) | M = 27.6 SD = 9.0 |
Gender | 82% male, 18% female | 80% male, 20% female | 95% male, 3% female, 2% other | 89% male, 10% female, 1% other |
Experience [years] | M = 10.1 SD = 9.2 | M = 15.3 SD = 7.7 | M = 5.3 SD = 5.1 a | M = 7.9 SD = 7.8 |
Training Sessions [per week] | M = 2.7 SD = 1.5 | M = 1.9 SD = 2.7 | M = 4.0 SD = 2.7 a | M = 3.4 SD = 2.5 |
Length Training Session [minutes] | M = 118.1 SD = 61.9 | M = 84.3 SD = 98.0 | M = 111.2 SD = 76.5 a | M = 111.4 SD = 74.2 |
Self-Reported Skill | M = 8.2 SD = 1.2 b | 5% Beginner 37% Advanced 35% Skilled 23% Expert | M = 5.2 SD = 1.6 a,c | |
Highest Contest Participation | 91% none or amateur 4% regional 4% national 1% international | 87% none or amateur 4% regional 4% national 6% international | 65% none or amateur 16% regional 7% national 13% international a | 76% none or amateur 11% regional 5% national 8% international |
Dependent Variable | β (ΔR²) | ||
---|---|---|---|
Control Variables | Risk Taking | Boredom Proneness | |
Immediate regret after taking a risk (1–6 scale) | (0.069) *** | −0.120 (0.013) *** | 0.060 (0.003) |
Delayed regret after taking a risk (0–100 scale) | (0.089) *** | −0.100 (0.009) ** | 0.067 (0.004) * |
Regret about having been too passive (0–100 scale) | (0.046) *** | 0.017 (0.000) | 0.147 (0.019) *** |
Variable | (1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | (6) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
(1) Gender | n = 926 | r = 0.057, p = 0.085 | r = −0.096, p = 0.003 | ρ = −0.077, p = 0.019 | r = 0.054, p = 0.098 | r = −0.178, p < 0.001 |
(2) Age | n = 930 | r = −0.006, p = 0.865 | ρ = −0.133, p < 0.001 | r = 0.585, p < 0.001 | r = −0.255, p < 0.001 | |
(3) Skill (z-scored) | n = 936 | ρ = 0.320, p < 0.001 | r = 0.324, p < 0.001 | r = −0.061, p = 0.062 | ||
(4) Competition level | n = 936 | ρ = 0.064, p = 0.050 | ρ = 0.041, p = 0.205 | |||
(5) Experience (in years) | n = 936 | r = −0.242, p < 0.001 | ||||
(6) Boredom Proneness | n = 936 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Wolff, W.; Bieleke, M.; Keller, L. Boredom Proneness Predicts Self-Assessed Decision Errors in Sports but Is Unrelated to Risk Taking in General. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 3479. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19063479
Wolff W, Bieleke M, Keller L. Boredom Proneness Predicts Self-Assessed Decision Errors in Sports but Is Unrelated to Risk Taking in General. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(6):3479. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19063479
Chicago/Turabian StyleWolff, Wanja, Maik Bieleke, and Lucas Keller. 2022. "Boredom Proneness Predicts Self-Assessed Decision Errors in Sports but Is Unrelated to Risk Taking in General" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 6: 3479. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19063479
APA StyleWolff, W., Bieleke, M., & Keller, L. (2022). Boredom Proneness Predicts Self-Assessed Decision Errors in Sports but Is Unrelated to Risk Taking in General. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19(6), 3479. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19063479