Next Article in Journal
A Study on Online Intervention for Early Childhood Eating Disorders during COVID-19
Previous Article in Journal
Neighborhood Makes or Breaks Active Ageing? Findings from Cross-Sectional Path Analysis
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Social Capital as a Mediator and Moderator in the Association between Loneliness and Health, Israel as a Case Study

1
School of Public Health, University of Haifa, Haifa 3498838, Israel
2
The Braun School of Public Health & Community Medicine, The Hebrew University-Hadassah, Jerusalem 9112102, Israel
3
Israel Healthy Cities Network, Jerusalem 91072, Israel
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(6), 3698; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19063698
Submission received: 25 January 2022 / Revised: 14 March 2022 / Accepted: 16 March 2022 / Published: 20 March 2022
(This article belongs to the Section Disease Prevention)

Abstract

:
Loneliness has been associated with poor health. Social capital (SC) could possibly prevent the ill effects of loneliness. The study aims to assess the association of loneliness with physical and mental health in four different communities in Israel and study the impact of structural and cognitive SC on that association. A cross-sectional face-to-face survey with 4620 adults in four towns was conducted. The questionnaire included self-rated health (SRH), mental health (MH), loneliness, cognitive and structural SC and socioeconomic characteristics. Logistic regression analysis and mediation and moderation effects were calculated. Loneliness was associated with worse SRH (OR = 0.4–0.5) and worse MH (OR = 2.0–10). Both SC variables were associated with health. However, towns differ in these associations. Structural SC serves as a significant mediator between loneliness and SRH in all towns and is a mediator between loneliness and MH in two towns. Cognitive social capital was a moderator between loneliness and MH in two towns. This study suggests that increasing SC could possibly compensate for loneliness and buffer its effect on health. The study reinforces the need for the performance of separate health profiles to assess possible interventions for each community, as not always can we generalize these results to all communities.

1. Introduction

During the last decade, loneliness has emerged as an important determinant of health [1,2,3,4]. Studies have suggested that loneliness is a major risk factor for morbidity and mortality [5,6]. It seems that loneliness is indirectly associated with these outcomes through psychological and behavioral factors [6]. During the COVID-19 pandemic, loneliness was a major concern for public health, increasing the need to understand the factors associated with it [7].
Most of the research regarding loneliness has been conducted in the developed world and individualistic societies, however, there are studies looking at other societies [8]. In a study comparing loneliness across cultures in 237 countries, findings showed that in societies that were more individualistic there were higher rates of loneliness that decreased with age [9].
In a recent editorial, a list of risk factors and negative effects associated with loneliness in old age was given. The authors suggested that building a compassionate social community could be a key approach to dealing with loneliness [10].
Social capital (SC) may help build such communities. Identifying key elements within SC that may inhibit or promote the ill effects of loneliness would be helpful for future intervention.

1.1. Social Capital and Health

Putnam’s SC is an umbrella term that draws together relationships within networks, norms of reciprocity, mutual support and trustworthiness [11,12]. Social capital is often defined as the norms, networks and associations that facilitate co-operative action [13]. Social capital can be divided into both structural dimensions (the number of social relationships) and cognitive dimensions (perceptions of the quality of social relationships) [14]. The current study measures cognitive SC represented by trust and reciprocity and structural SC represented by social involvement [15].
Social capital is associated with many health outcomes [16,17,18,19,20,21]. In a systematic review, Coll-Planas et al. found mixed effects of SC interventions targeting older people’s quality of life, wellbeing and self-perceived health [22]. Social capital is now considered a social determinant of the health discourse [23].
Social capital may serve as a protective factor for health and a resource for individuals and communities to improve quality of life [12,19]. However, how this works and if this is generalizable between communities is not clear [24]. The association between SC and health may be context-dependent, depending on the specific community studied [15,25].
In a previous study in Israel, Jews reported higher levels of SC compared to Arabs, except for higher rates of social contacts, and all measures of SC were associated with self-rated health among Jews. However, among Arabs, the association between SC measures and health was not significant, except for social support. Therefore, studying these two populations may have an added value for understanding the association between social attributes and health [25].

1.2. Social Capital and Loneliness

Loneliness and SC both seem to affect health, moreover, SC may serve as a resource by which individuals can decrease loneliness or prevent its ill effects. Studies suggest that there is an association between living in an area with high SC and lower levels of loneliness [26,27,28]. This association may not be causal, and there may be an interaction between socioeconomic status (SES) and age. A study in a suburb of Barcelona, Spain, suggested that there was an interaction between SES, neighborhood SC, age and loneliness. The authors suggest that increasing neighborhood SC could be an effective way of reducing the prevalence of loneliness [26].
Evidence is beginning to come out that promoting SC to tackle loneliness and its health effects could be an interesting approach to decrease inequalities [29]. However, not many studies have looked at all three variables simultaneously. Therefore, there is a need for more information regarding the associations between these social factors to be able to estimate the generalizability to all communities.
Over two decades of research with more than 850 studies looking at SC and health have resulted in elusive and contradictory results, especially regarding interventions [30]. Shiell et al. suggest interventions utilizing SC to improve health are especially context-specific.
Israel can serve as an interesting setting for studying different communities as it is a multicultural society with two major ethno-cultural groups: Jews and Arabs [31,32]. About one fifth of Israel’s population comprises Arabs; of them, about 85% are Muslims and 15% are mainly Christians and Druze. Jews and Arabs in Israel differ in socioeconomic status, religion, culture, and language. The Arab society is a more collectivist society in transition to a more western lifestyle. Health indicators in Israel suggest poorer health among Arabs compared to Jews [31,32]. For this study, we chose four towns that differed in ethnicity (one Jewish town, two Arab towns and one mixed town).

1.3. Aim of the Study

The study’s aim was to assess the association between loneliness, cognitive and structural SC and health (mental and general self-rated health (SRH)). Additionally, we want to see whether these associations are similar in the different towns. We hypothesize that the association between loneliness and health is mediated or moderated by both cognitive and structural SC (Figure 1).

2. Material and Methods

2.1. Design and Sample

This study is a secondary analysis of data collected in cross-sectional population surveys in four towns, as part of their health profile and needs assessment. This is performed as one of their commitments to the Healthy City Network [33]. As the questionnaire is a uniform questionnaire, it allows for analyzing and comparing data from the four towns.
Four surveys were conducted in four Israeli towns during the years 2011–2016. The study population was randomly selected, in each town, as a stratified sample of households by residential area, age, and sex, by the Israel Central Bureau of Statistics (ICBS), which also provided a Kish grid for a random selection of the interviewees out of a list of inhabitants aged 22 and above, in each household. The sample size calculation in each town relied upon the following: 50% prevalence of a variable, a 95% confidence interval, a 5% relative sampling error, and 70% compliance. Interviews were conducted in person, using a structured questionnaire, at participants’ residential locations, by trained interviewers. Participation rates were 54% in the mixed Arab-Jewish town (town C), 79.3% in town A, and 89% in both towns D and B.
The sample included 1898 Arabs and 2722 Jews, altogether being 4620. Participants were residents of two Arab towns (n = 981 and 673), one Jewish town (n = 1935), and one mixed town (n = 244 Arabs, 787 Jews). The towns in the study represent two lower socioeconomic status (SES) Arab towns, level 2 and 3 out of 10, (town A and B), a mixed town with Arabs and Jews (town C, level 4 out of 10) and a Jewish town (town D, level 7 out of 10) [34]. Interviews were conducted in Arabic or Hebrew according to the town and preference of the individual.

2.2. Measures

Sociodemographic variables were collected in person via self-report: gender, age and education. In town C, respondents were asked to define themselves as Jews or Arabs. Religiosity was assessed using two different versions of the question, one for Arabs and one for Jews, as the terms used in Hebrew and Arabic are different. The variable was dichotomized as not religious [2] and religious [1]. For Arabs, not religious included answers 3-not so religious and 4-not religious; religious included 1-very religious and 2-religious. For Jews, not religious included 4-not so religious, and 5-not religious or secular, and religious included 1-ultra-orthodox, 2-religious and 3-traditional.
Two health measures were analyzed: A. self-rated health (SRH) was measured using the question “How do you evaluate your general health status?” (1) Very good, (2) good, (3) not so good, or (4) not good. The answers were dichotomized into very good and good (good) and coded as a 1, while, not so good and not good (poor) were coded as a 0. B. Mental health was measured by asking the following question: “During the last year have you suffered from the following problems: (1) irritability/stress/anxiety (2) sleep problems (3) sadness/depression and (4) tiredness’’. Respondents could answer yes or no to each problem. The number of problems mentioned were summed to give a scale from 0 to 4 and dichotomized. Low mental health was defined as 0 and 1, and high-2–4. These four items were taken from the national yearly social survey of the Israeli Central Bureau of Statistics [35].
Loneliness was assessed using one item, “Are there situations in which you feel lonely?” [36,37], with four response options ranging from (1) never to (4) often. This was dichotomized to never or infrequently = not lonely and feeling lonely often or sometimes = lonely.
The cognitive measure of SC (trust and reciprocity) was measured asking A. “Generally do you think 1. people can be trusted or 2. you have to be careful of trusting people”.
B. “Do you think that people in your town or neighborhood: 1. usually help each other, 2. usually only look after themselves”. A composite variable was defined by adding the number of positive answers (0, 1, 2), which was further collapsed for the logistic regression as low (0) and high (1 = 1 + 2).
Structural SC was measured by asking about participation in eight social activities. Respondents were given a list of activities and asked if they had participated in each activity during the last year, not at all (1), infrequently (2) frequently (3). The answers were added up to give a scale from 1 to 24. The activities included: sports events, meeting friends, going to the mosque or synagogue, going to parties or performances, going on hikes or tours with others, attending political or professional activities, volunteering, meeting family that do not live with the respondent.

2.3. Data Analysis

Data were analyzed using SPSS-25. Chi2 and one-way ANOVA were conducted to examine differences among the four towns. Logistic regression models were run with cognitive SC, SRH, and mental health dichotomized as the dependent variables. For the two health measures, the first step, gender, age, education, religiosity and loneliness were added to the regression. For the second step, structural or cognitive SC was added (model 1) and in the third step, an interaction between loneliness and structural or cognitive SC were added in order to test the moderation effect of SC on the association between loneliness and health (model 2). See Figure 1. This moderation effect was further elaborated by running separate regression for people with low and high SC. Linear regressions were run for structural SC as the dependent variable, with the above mentioned independent variables. A regression model was run separately for each town after finding significant interactions between town and loneliness.
The mediating models were calculated with the R mediation package, version 4.0.2, the model was performed with the library mediation. All variables were standardized. The models were first calculated while controlling for gender (1-male, 0-female), age and education, then the other variables were added. The Average Causal Mediation Effect (ACME) was the measure of the mediation effect [38].

2.4. Ethics

Exemption from ethical approval was granted by the University Hospital’s Ethical Committee.
The interviewees were told they were free to stop the interview at any time with no implications to them. As the interviewees agreed to answer the questionnaire this suggests informed consent.

3. Results

Altogether, 4620 adults were interviewed in four towns, two Arab towns (A and B), one mixed town (C) with 804 Jews and 246 Arabs in the sample, and one Jewish town (D).
The samples from the towns differ significantly in all their characteristics except for the percent of men and women interviewed (Table 1).
The two health measures were correlated (r = 0.38 Pearson correlation coefficient). Better self-rated health (SRH) was reported by Arabs in the two Arab towns (A and B) and Jews in town C compared to Jews in town D. Arabs living in Arab towns (A and B) and Arabs in town C report better mental health than the Jews in towns C and D. In town C, the difference between Jews and Arabs for all measures was significant, except for mental health, where there was no significant difference between the two population groups. It seems that both aspects of health (SRH and mental health) in the two Arab towns are better than in the Jewish or mixed towns (Table 2).
In a logistic regression of the total population, adjusting for gender, age and education, there was no difference in SRH between Arabs and Jews, however, mental health was worse among Jews after the adjustment (data not presented).
Residents of town A by far reported higher levels of loneliness compared to the other respondents, 38.4% reported feeling lonely often or sometimes, whereas in the other towns, both Arab and Jews, reported between 14.1% and 25.3% feeling lonely often or sometimes. Respondents in town B reported the lowest levels of loneliness (Table 2).
The cognitive measure of SC (trust and reciprocity) was highest in the Arab towns A and B, and lowest in Jewish town D. However, within town C the Arabs had significantly lower levels of cognitive SC compared to Jews. Another measure of SC was the levels of participation in social activities (structural SC), these were higher in the two Arab towns A and B. In town C, Jews reported significantly higher levels of social activities compared to Arabs. Generally, respondents reported spending time with family and friends and going to parties most frequently (Table 2).
The activities reported varied significantly between the towns, going to a movie was more frequent in towns C and D. Spending time with family, attending sports events, parties and outings were more frequent in town A, and attending prayers in a prayer house was the most frequent activity in town B compared to the rest. This suggests the towns differ in their patterns of social lifestyle.
Logistic and linear regressions with cognitive and structural SC respectively as the dependent variable were run, adjusting for age, gender, education and religiosity (Table 3). Loneliness is associated with cognitive SC only in town C, where those reporting loneliness also reported lower cognitive SC (OR = 0.68, CI = 0.49, 0.94). Being religious is significantly associated with high cognitive SC in the total sample and cities A and D. Gender, age and education are not associated with cognitive SC in the total population and vary in their association by town.
Structural SC (social activities) was significantly inversely associated with loneliness in three of the four towns (towns A, C and D). This association was not significantly different between the towns (p > 0.05). Generally, structural SC is more dependent on gender, age and education compared to cognitive SC that is associated only with religiosity in the total population (Table 3).
Table 4 presents the logistic regressions for all the study population and separately for each town with SRH or mental health as the dependant variable and participation in social activities as the measure of structural SC. In these logistic regressions, we adjusted for gender, age, education and religiosity. As expected, when analysing all towns together the more respondents reported being lonely the worse was their self-rated health (SRH) (OR 0.48, CI 0.40, 0.58) and they reported worse mental health (OR 1.94, CI 1.66, 2.27). Structural SC was also significantly associated with SRH and mental health in the total population. We then added the interaction between loneliness and structural SC to test moderation effects (model 2). The interaction between loneliness and structural SC (participation in social activities) was significant for both types of health measures, while the strength of the association between structural SC and SRH or mental health decreased in the total population. To further explore the moderation effect, we analysed the association between loneliness and SRH and mental health of the total population, in two separate logistic regressions, one included respondents reporting low structural SC (range 1–12) and another regression with respondents reporting high structural SC (range 13–24). The association between loneliness and SRH was stronger among people with low structural SC (OR = 0.34, C.I-0.26, 0.46) than among people with high structural SC (OR = 0.58, C.I 0.46, 0.73). Similar results were revealed in relation to mental health. The association between loneliness and mental health was stronger among people with low structural SC (OR = 4.58, C.I-3.31, 6.32) than among people with high structural SC (OR = 1.42, C.I 1.18, 1.71). There was no significant moderation effect with cognitive SC, both for SRH and mental health (data not presented in the table). Adding town to the interaction did not yield a significant interaction (Table 4).
When running the regressions separately for each town with SRH or mental health as the dependant variable, loneliness was associated with SRH and mental health in towns B, C, and D, but not in town A. Structural SC was associated with SRH in all towns. In towns B, C and D, the addition of the interaction decreased the association between structural SC and SRH leaving it not significant. The interaction between loneliness and structural SC did not reach significance when the regressions were run separately for each town (Table 4).
To test the mediation effect of structural SC on the association between loneliness and SRH we calculated the mediation analysis for the total sample and each town separately.
The mediation effect of structural SC was significant for the total population, when including the interaction between town and loneliness. The ACME was significant (the estimate was 0.017 and p > 0.001). When running the regressions separately for each town the mediation remained significant for each town: in town A (ACME = 0.012, p < 0.0001), town B (ACME = 0.014 p = 0.04), town C (ACME = 0.016, p = 0.002), town D (ACME = 0.016, p < 0.001). Therefore, it seems that structural SC is a mediator in the association between loneliness and SRH.
Structural SC (participation in social activities) was also a significant mediator in the association between loneliness and mental health in the total population including the interaction between town and loneliness, (ACME = 0.0075, p < 0.0001). When running the regressions separately for each town the mediation was significant in town C (ACME = −0.008, p < 0.018) and town D (ACME = −0.01, p = 0.012) but not in the Arab towns A and B.
Table 5 presents the logistic regressions for all the study population and separately for each town with SRH or mental health as the dependant variable and cognitive SC and loneliness as the independent variables. Again, we adjusted for gender, age, education and religiosity.
Cognitive SC was associated with both SRH and mental health in the total population, where respondents with higher cognitive SC reported better health. Adding the interaction of loneliness and cognitive SC to the model decreases the association, leaving it non-significant. The interaction was not significant both for mental health and SRH, in the total population.
Cognitive SC was associated with SRH in towns C and D (OR = 1.54 CI = 0.99, 2.39 for town B, OR = 1.42 CI = 1.00, 2.01 for town C and OR = 1.61 CI = 1.17, 2.17 for town D), however it was significant (p < 0.05) only in town D. Adding the interaction eliminated the significance. The interaction was significant only in town C, therefore there may be a significant moderating effect in town C.
Cognitive SC was significantly associated with mental health in towns A, C and D, however, the direction varied. In Town A, the OR was 1.42 whereas in towns C and D the OR was 0.70 and 0.46 respectively. Adding the interaction eliminated the significance in all three towns. The interaction was significant for the two towns C and D. This suggests that cognitive SC may be a moderator in the association between loneliness and mental health in towns C and D (Table 5).
To test the mediation effect of cognitive SC on the association between loneliness and SRH we used the mediation analysis for the total sample and each town separately. There was no significant mediating effect for cognitive SC in the association between loneliness and SRH in the total sample (ACME = −0.0015, p = 0.12) or any of the towns separately. Cognitive SC was also not a significant mediator between loneliness and mental health in the total sample, (ACME = 0.0004 p = 0.3) and in the towns separately.
Structural SC may serve as a moderator and a mediator in the association between loneliness and health, both SRH and mental health. This seems not to be true for all towns. Cognitive SC may be a moderator only for mental health and only in two of the towns, however not a mediator.
It seems that the different components of SC vary in the way they mediate or moderate the ill effects of loneliness in the various communities and the two health measures.

4. Discussion

This study tries to understand if social capital (SC), both structural and cognitive, can help alleviate some of the ill effects of loneliness on health in certain communities. Studying these associations might support communities and social services in directing their investments to prevent the ill effects of loneliness on health. The model we tested, which presents the association between loneliness and health (SRH and mental health) and the effect of social capital on that association, indicated differences between cities. The direct association between loneliness and Self Reported Health and mental health, which was found in 3 of the 4 examined cities, is consistent with many previous studies [10,39,40].
As for the effect of social capital on this association, we separated the two components of social capital as was suggested by Rodgers and colleagues that each component of social capital represents a very different attribute of the community and may have very different effects on people in general. Putting them together under the umbrella of social capital does not suggest they have the same impact on society [41]. As part of the model, we analyzed the association of each social capital component with SRH and mental health. As in most previous studies, both cognitive and structural social capital were found to be positively associated with self-reported health in the total population and every town [16,17,18,19,20,21]. Less consistent findings were revealed for the association of social capital and mental health, as was reported by De Silva [15]. In 3 out of 4 cities, higher structural and cognitive social capital were associated with better mental health. In a review of cross-sectional and longitudinal studies measuring the association between social capital and common mental disorders [42] a negative association was found with cognitive social capital, but they did not find associations with structural social capital, except for studies among mothers in low-income settings, where participation in civic activities was positively associated with common mental disorders. Such a positive association we found only in town A. It seems that the type of social activities may influence the direction of the association. Croezen and his coauthors found that participation in religious organizations is negatively associated with depressive symptoms while participating in sports or social clubs or political organizations are positively associated with depressive symptoms [43]. In our analysis, we did not differentiate between types of social activities.
Findings suggest that structural social capital serves both as a significant mediator and a moderator in the association between loneliness and health, both SRH and mental health. We found a stronger association between loneliness and SRH and mental health among people with a low level of structural social capital compared to those with high structural social capital. Therefore, it is possible that being in contact with other people via participation in social activities may serve as a possible intervention to dampen or alleviate the ill effects of loneliness on health. Coll-Planas et al. suggest it is an understudied intervention strategy [29]. In a qualitative focus group study, they describe the ways in which a weekly group-based program can alleviate loneliness among older adults by promoting peer support and participation [44]. In our present study, we suggest structural social capital may improve self-reported health in all communities, however, it may improve mental health only in two of the communities, not in the Arab communities. It seems that generalizing from one community to the other is not possible and we suggest that before attempting to utilize a specific intervention it should be studied in that specific community. In other words, the contextual social environment is a major attribute to the way loneliness and social capital may affect health.
Cognitive social capital does not have a mediating effect. A moderation effect was found in relation to self-reported health in one town only and in relation to mental health in two of the towns. In the communities studied in these cross-sectional surveys, trust in social surroundings does not necessarily improve health.
Several interesting differences were found between the towns, for example, in town A, which is the poorest town, the highest rates of loneliness were reported. This is consistent with the findings of the highest risk of loneliness in the least wealthy groups, which were described in the Survey of Health, Ageing and Retirement in Europe (SHARE) [45,46]. Loneliness in town A was negatively associated with structural social capital and the latter was associated with self-reported health, but there was no direct association between loneliness and SRH (controlling for age, gender, education, religiosity and structural social capital), as was found in the other towns. This may indicate that town A may differ from the other towns in its contextual social environment. Poverty may explain the differences to some extent, but of course, there may be other explanatory social factors not measured [47].
The social and cultural differences between the two populations (Arabs and Jews) are large, with the Arabs being a poorer population, and more collective [31,32]. The differences between the communities correspond with national data [48]. However, there were also differences between the two Arab towns in our study.
The moderation effects identified in this study suggest that increasing the social contacts of lonely people, who may have a decreased ability to utilize resources available to help with their health, may affect their health by providing these resources to them. Cognitive social capital was significantly associated with both self-reported health and mental health in 3 out of the 4 towns. In town A, a weak association was detected with self-reported health and an association with mental health in the opposite direction. Similar to the results of town A in this study, in a previous study, trust was not associated with health among Arabs, however, in the Jewish community, it was [25]. The authors suggested that in more affluent communities, such as the Jewish communities, social capital may be associated with health to a higher extent and less so in more traditional communities. It may be that the concept of trust varies between the two communities, the more individualistic community and the more collective community. The lack of association between trust and self-reported health was reported in another study from South Africa [49], where the authors state that the results contradict many studies that do show these associations [50,51,52]. It seems that the impact of social capital on health varies with the setting, population and health measure [53].
Religiosity may also serve as a factor in the social context, as there were differences in religiosity between the towns, therefore it was important to adjust for this measure. Studies have shown that loneliness is more common in people without strong religious beliefs [54,55]. In this study, religiosity was associated with mental health in the total population and in the Arab town A but not in the other towns. Religiosity was associated with self-reported health only in the Jewish town D and not in any of the other towns, again suggesting variations in the social context in the different towns.
Further studies should look at levels of collectivism that could have an impact on these relationships. Dykstra (2009) suggested that contrary to the assumption that people in individualistic societies are more lonely, northern Europeans tend to be less lonely compared to the more familistic southern European countries [56]. This corresponds with our results.
Another aspect of the differences between the communities may be a cultural difference in how the different communities perceive loneliness. The questions used in this survey may vary in their validity and reliability between the communities, not only between Arabs and Jews, as suggested in several previous studies [57,58], but also between the Arab communities themselves, or the Jewish communities. In town A, people may tend to express and complain about feeling alone, more so than in other communities. As people in town A reported higher levels of loneliness, we would expect them to report less social activities, however, higher levels of participation in social activities were reported in town A compared to the other towns. As social activities were associated with lower levels of loneliness in all towns, the more activities within a community the less loneliness we would expect to find. The fact that even though people in town A reported high levels of loneliness and high levels of social activities suggests that in town A social activities do not alleviate loneliness in the same way as in the other towns [59,60], or as mentioned earlier, it may depend on the types of social activities [43].
Both social capital measures were higher in the Arab towns and lower in the mixed and Jewish towns. Other studies in the past suggested otherwise, where social capital measures were lower among Arabs [25]. The only measure of social capital that was higher among Arabs was social contacts, this measure may be a similar measure of social activities, even though they are measured in a different way. As the Arab community is a more collective community where close contacts are frequent, this is expected. However, in the previous study trust was higher in the Jewish community compared to the Arab community, but not so in this study. This may have changed with time and now present a true difference between the communities, or it may also be that town D, as the only Jewish town present in this study, do not represent other Jewish towns. In a study assessing acquiescence bias among Arabs and Jews, we found that Arabs tend to agree with questions more so than Jews [58]. Therefore, the higher rates among Arabs in this study could be due to this bias and not a true measure of their social capital.
In town C, the patterns of association between the variables in this study are similar to the Jews in town D, especially for the Arab community when dividing the sample into the two communities, Arabs and Jews. Living in close proximity in the same town may have an effect on both communities. For example, Arabs in town C gave estimates of their health more similar to the Jewish population than to the Arab population in towns A and B, this may only be the effect of the SES level but may also represent the effect of cohabitation. In addition, social capital was lower in town C compared to towns A and B and more similar to town D. Therefore, it seems that living in a mixed town (town C) reduces social differences between the two ethnic communities.
We may conclude that communities differ in the way social capital serves as a resource within that specific community and depend on the context of a certain community, for example, socioeconomic status and levels of collectivism. What specific attributes of a community are important in this context are not clear yet. In an important paper, Shiell et al. suggested rethinking social capital and social capital interventions aimed at improving health [30]. They suggest that studies measuring social capital and health inadequately take into account the context in which the study takes place. In their opinion, the way forward requires a renewed focus on specific components of social capital and how context affects the interaction between social capital and health. Our study supports these assertions.
A few study limitations should be mentioned. The study is a cross-sectional study that cannot infer causality, therefore follow up studies are needed to ascertain the directionality of the associations between social capital, loneliness and health. The cross-sectional methodology is not the optimal methodology in many cases. However, according to Spector [61], the cross-sectional methodology is appropriate when the association between variables is not clear, the timeframe for the effect is not known and we want to measure the association in four distinct communities. This methodology can help assess preliminary ideas on how we can move forward to alleviate the ill effects of loneliness on health.
The measure of loneliness is a single-item measure and as such is not ideal, however, there is no consensus on how to measure loneliness to date, standardisation is needed [62]. In other studies, certain parts of social capital are incorporated into the loneliness measure, such as frequency of contacts with friends, family and children and participation in social activities [47]. In this study, we assessed perceived loneliness as a single item and refer to levels of contact with other people separately as structural social capital. We do not regard this as a limitation of the study, on the contrary, separating the two aspects of loneliness provides a more in-depth analysis of factors effecting of loneliness and health.
An additional limitation of the study is the possible difference in the validity and reliability of the measures in Arabs and Jews. This has previously been discussed.
A broader look at the context of the communities is needed to further understand the complex dynamics between the studied variables and the possibility of how changing social capital can serve as an intervention to improve health [30].

5. Conclusions

This study suggests that increasing structural social capital could possibly compensate for loneliness and alleviate the harm it can do to health. However, we cannot generalize from one community to the other or for the various components of social capital. Therefore, this study reinforces the need for the performance of separate health profiles and the assessment of possible interventions in each community separately.
We may say that structural social capital is both a mediator and a moderator for the total sample with variations between the towns.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization was performed by all three authors; Data collection M.D.; Statistical Analysis, O.B.-E.; Writing—Original Draft Preparation, O.B.-E.; Writing—Review & Editing, O.B.-E., R.E.-B., M.D.; Project Administration, M.D. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The local authorities and the Healthy Cities Network funded the data collection.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Exemption from ethical approval was granted by the University Hospital’s Ethical Committee.

Informed Consent Statement

As the interviewees agreed to answer the questionnaire this suggests informed consent.

Data Availability Statement

Data available on request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Duke, L.H. The importance of social ties in mental health. Ment. Health Soc. Incl. 2017, 21, 264–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Richard, A.; Rohrmann, S.; Vandeleur, C.L.; Schmid, M.; Barth, J.; Eichholzer, M. Loneliness is adversely associated with physical and mental health and lifestyle factors: Results from a Swiss national survey. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0181442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Cacioppo, J.T.; Hawkley, L.C. Social isolation and health, with an emphasis on underlying mechanisms. Perspect. Biol. Med. 2003, 46, S39–S52. Available online: https://muse.jhu.edu/article/44865/summary (accessed on 11 November 2021). [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Courtin, E.; Knapp, M. Social isolation, loneliness and health in old age: A scoping review. Health Soc. Care Community 2017, 25, 799–812. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Cacioppo, J.T.; Hawkley, L.C.; Crawford, L.E.; Ernst, J.M.; Burleson, M.H.; Kowalewski, R.B.; Malarkey, W.B.; Van Cauter, E.; Berntson, G.G. Loneliness and health: Potential mechanisms. Psychosom. Med. 2002, 64, 407–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Christiansen, J.; Lund, R.; Qualter, P.; Andersen, C.M.; Pedersen, S.S.; Lasgaard, M. Loneliness, Social Isolation, and Chronic Disease Outcomes. Ann. Behav. Med. 2021, 55, 203–215. Available online: https://academic.oup.com/abm/article-abstract/55/3/203/5899648 (accessed on 11 November 2021). [CrossRef]
  7. Killgore, W.D.S.; Cloonan, S.A.; Taylor, E.C.; Dailey, N.S. Loneliness: A signature mental health concern in the era of COVID-19. Psychiatry Res. 2020, 290, 113117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Peltzer, K.; Pengpid, S. Loneliness correlates and associations with health variables in the general population in Indonesia. Int. J. Ment. Health Syst. 2019, 13, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Barreto, M.; Victor, C.; Hammond, C.; Eccles, A.; Richins, M.T.; Qualter, P. Loneliness around the world: Age, gender, and cultural differences in loneliness. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2021, 169, 110066. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Berg-Weger, M.; Morley, J.E. Loneliness in Old Age: An Unaddressed Health Problem. J. Nutr. Health Aging 2020, 24, 243–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Feldstein, L.; Putnam, R. Better Together; Simon Schuster: New York, NY, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  12. Putnam, R.D. The strange disappearance of civic America. Policy A J. Public Policy Ideas 1996, 24, 34–48. [Google Scholar]
  13. Putnam, R. The prosperous community: Social capital and public life. Am. Prospect 1993, 4, 35–42. [Google Scholar]
  14. Harpham, T. Measuring social capital within health surveys: Key issues. Health Policy Plan. 2002, 17, 106–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. De Silva, M.J.; Huttly, S.R.; Harpham, T.; Kenward, M.G. Social capital and mental health: A comparative analysis of four low income countries. Soc. Sci. Med. 2007, 64, 5–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Alvarez, E.C.; Kawachi, I.; Romani, J.R. Family social capital and health—A systematic review and redirection. Soc. Health Illn. 2017, 39, 5–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Moore, S.; Kawachi, I. Twenty years of social capital and health research: A glossary. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2017, 71, 513–517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Mackenbach, J.D.; Lakerveld, J.; Van Lenthe, F.J.; Kawachi, I.; McKee, M.; Rutter, H.; Glonti, K.; Compernolle, S.; De Bourdeaudhuij, I.; Feuillet, T.; et al. Neighbourhood social capital: Measurement issues and associations with health outcomes. Obes. Rev. 2016, 17, 96–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Kawachi, I.; Kennedy, B.P. Socioeconomic determinants of health: Health and social cohesion: Why care about income inequality? BMJ 1997, 314, 1037. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. De Silva, M.J. Social capital and mental illness: A systematic review. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2005, 59, 619–627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Hawe, P.; Shiell, A. Social capital and health promotion: A review. Soc. Sci. Med. 2000, 51, 871–885. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Coll-Planas, L.; Nyqvist, F.; Puig, T.; Urrútia, G.; Solà, I.; Monteserín, R. Social capital interventions targeting older people and their impact on health: A systematic review. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2017, 71, 663–672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Carrillo Álvarez, E.; Riera Romaní, J. Measuring social capital: Further insights. Gac. Sanit. 2017, 31, 57–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. McKenzie, K.; Whitley, R.; Weich, S. Social capital and mental health. Br. J. Psychiatry 2002, 181, 280–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Baron-Epel, O.; Weinstein, R.; Haviv-Mesika, A.; Garty-Sandalon, N.; Green, M.S. Individual-level analysis of social capital and health: A comparison of Arab and Jewish Israelis. Soc. Sci. Med. 2008, 66, 900–910. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Domènech-Abella, J.; Mundó, J.; Lara, E.; Moneta, M.V.; Haro, J.M.; Olaya, B. The role of socio-economic status and neighborhood social capital on loneliness among older adults: Evidence from the Sant Boi Aging Study. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2017, 52, 1237–1246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Nyqvist, F.; Victor, C.R.; Forsman, A.K.; Cattan, M. The association between social capital and loneliness in different age groups: A population-based study in Western Finland. BMC Public Health 2016, 16, 542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Kim, J.; Kim, S.E. A Study of Social Capital Patterns and Loneliness of the Elderly in Rural Areas. Proc. Spring Conf. 2018, 45, 207–210. [Google Scholar]
  29. Coll-Planas, L.; del Valle Gómez, G.; Bonilla, P.; Masat, T.; Puig, T.; Monteserin, R. Promoting social capital to alleviate loneliness and improve health among older people in Spain. Health Soc. Care Community 2017, 25, 145–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Shiell, A.; Hawe, P.; Kavanagh, S. Evidence suggests a need to rethink social capital and social capital interventions. Soc. Sci. Med. 2020, 275, 111930. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Marciano, H.; Kimhi, S.; Eshel, Y. Predictors of individual, community and national resiliencies of Israeli Jews and Arabs. Int. J. Psychol. 2020, 55, 553–561. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Saabneh, A.M. Arab-Jewish gap in life expectancy in Israel. Eur. J. Public Health 2016, 26, 433–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Janss Lafond, L.; Heritage, Z. National networks of Healthy Cities in Europe. Health Promot Int. 2009, 24, i100–i107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Israel Central Bearu of Statistics. Characterization and Classification of Geographical Units by the Socio-Economic Level of the Population 2017. Available online: https://www.cbs.gov.il/en/mediarelease/pages/2020/characterization-classification-geographicalunitsby-socio-economic-level-population2017.aspx (accessed on 1 March 2022).
  35. Israel Central Bearu of Statistics Social Survey 2021, Transportation and Road Safety, Language Use, Quality of Life Indicators. Available online: https://www.cbs.gov.il/he/publications/DocLib/2019/%D7%9C%D7%95%D7%97%D7%95%D7%AA%20%D7%AA%D7%9C%D7%95%D7%A9%D7%99%D7%9D/Social%20Survey%20Questionnaire%202021.pdf (accessed on 14 March 2022).
  36. Haron, N. Poverty and Social Exclusion; Betti, G., Lemmi, A., Eds.; Taylor and Francis Ltd.: Routledge, London, UK, 2013; Volume 9780203085, pp. 54–77. Available online: https://www.taylorfrancis.com/books/9781136196300 (accessed on 11 November 2021).
  37. Victor, C.R.; Dobbs, C.; Gilhooly, K.; Burholt, V. Loneliness in mid-life and older adults from ethnic minority communities in England and Wales: Measure validation and prevalence estimates. Eur. J. Ageing 2021, 18, 5–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  38. Tingley, D.; Yamamoto, T.; Hirose, K.; Keele, L.; Imai, K. Mediation: R package for causal mediation analysis. J. Stat. Softw. 2014, 59, 1–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Holt-Lunstad, J.; Smith, T.B.; Baker, M.; Harris, T.; Stephenson, D. Loneliness and Social Isolation as Risk Factors for Mortality: A Meta-Analytic Review. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2015, 10, 227–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Hawkley, L.C.; Cacioppo, J.T. Loneliness matters: A theoretical and empirical review of consequences and mechanisms. Ann. Behav. Med. 2010, 40, 218–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Rodgers, J.; Valuev, A.V.; Hswen, Y.; Subramanian, S.V. Social capital and physical health: An updated review of the literature for 2007–2018. Soc. Sci. Med. 2019, 236, 112360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Ehsan, A.M.; De Silva, M.J. Social capital and common mental disorder: A systematic review. J. Epidemiol. Community Health. 2015, 69, 1021–1028. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Croezen, S.; Avendano, M.; Burdorf, A.; Van Lenthe, F.J. Social Participation and Depression in Old Age: A Fixed-Effects Analysis in 10 European Countries. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2015, 182, 168–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Coll-Planas, L.; Rodríguez-Arjona, D.; Pons-Vigués, M.; Nyqvist, F.; Puig, T.; Monteserín, R. “Not Alone in Loneliness”: A Qualitative Evaluation of a Program Promoting Social Capital among Lonely Older People in Primary Health Care. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 5580. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Hoffman, S.; Warren, M.E. Democracy and Association. Contemp. Soc. 2002, 31, 360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Niedzwiedz, C.L.; Richardson, E.A.; Tunstall, H.; Shortt, N.K.; Mitchell, R.J.; Pearce, J.R. The relationship between wealth and loneliness among older people across Europe: Is social participation protective? Prev. Med. 2016, 91, 24–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  47. Shankar, A.; McMunn, A.; Demakakos, P.; Hamer, M.; Steptoe, A. Social isolation and loneliness: Prospective associations with functional status in older adults. Health Psychol. 2017, 36, 179–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  48. Myers-JDC-Brookdale. The Arab Population in Israel: Facts & Figures 2018. Jesuralem, 2018. Available online: https://brookdale.jdc.org.il/wp-content/uploads/2018/03/MJB_Facts_and_Figures_on_the_Arab_Population_in_Israel_2018-English.pdf (accessed on 14 November 2021).
  49. Oguttu, J.W.; Ncayiyana, J.R. Social capital and self-rated health of residents of Gauteng province: Does area-level deprivation influence the relationship? SSM—Popul. Health 2020, 11, 100607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Subramanian, S.V.; Kawachi, I.; Kennedy, B.P. Does the state you live in make a difference? Multilevel analysis of self-rated health in the US. Soc. Sci. Med. 2001, 53, 9–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Subramanian, S.V. Social Trust and Self-Rated Health in US Communities: A Multilevel Analysis. J. Urban Health 2002, 79, S21–S34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Sapag, J.C.; Aracena, M.; Villarroel, L.; Poblete, F.; Berrocal, C.; Hoyos, R.; Martinez, M.; Kawachi, I. Social capital and self-rated health in urban low income neighbourhoods in Chile. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2008, 62, 790–792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Van Hooijdonk, C.; Droomers, M.; Deerenberg, I.M.; Mackenbach, J.P.; Kunst, A.E. The diversity in associations between community social capital and health per health outcome, population group and location studied. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2008, 37, 1384–1392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Lauder, W.; Mummery, K.; Sharkey, S. Social capital, age and religiosity in people who are lonely. J. Clin. Nurs. 2006, 15, 334–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Johnson, D.P.; Mullins, L.C. Religiosity and Loneliness Among the Elderly. J. Appl. Gerontol. 1989, 8, 110–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Dykstra, P.A. Older adult loneliness: Myths and realities. Eur. J. Ageing 2009, 6, 91–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  57. Baron-Epel, O.; Friedman, N.; Lernau, O. Validity of self-reported mammography in a multicultural population in Israel. Prev. Med. 2008, 46, 489–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Baron-Epel, O.; Kaplan, G.; Weinstein, R.; Green, M.S. Extreme and acquiescence bias in a bi-ethnic population. Eur. J. Public Health 2010, 20, 543–548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  59. Van Staden, W.; Coetzee, K. Conceptual relations between loneliness and culture. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 2010, 23, 524–529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Rokach, A.; Orzeck, T.; Cripps, J.; Lackovic-Grgin, K.; Penezic, Z. The effects of culture on the meaning of loneliness. Soc. Indic. Res. 2001, 53, 17–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Spector, P.E. Do Not Cross Me: Optimizing the Use of Cross-Sectional Designs. J. Bus. Psychol. 2019, 34, 125–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Fried, L.; Prohaska, T.; Burholt, V.; Burns, A.; Golden, J.; Hawkley, L.; Lawlor, B.; Leavey, G.; Lubben, J.; O’Sullivan, R.; et al. A unified approach to loneliness. Lancet 2020, 395, 114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Hypothesised model depicting the association between loneliness and health.
Figure 1. Hypothesised model depicting the association between loneliness and health.
Ijerph 19 03698 g001
Table 1. Selected characteristics of the study in each town, mean, standard deviation (SD) or percent and number.
Table 1. Selected characteristics of the study in each town, mean, standard deviation (SD) or percent and number.
Town A
N = 981
Town B
N = 673
Town C
N = 1031
Town D
N = 1935
P between Towns
Jews N = 787Arabs N = 244
Mean (SD)Mean (SD)Mean (SD)Mean (SD)Mean (SD)
Age—mean years44.4 (15.3)47.4 (15.2)52.4 (17.11)47.0 (15.56)51.2 (15.3)<0.001
P § <0.001
%%%%%
Gender 0.13
Women51.055.355.653.355.2
Men49.044.744.446.744.8
P § 0.28
Education <0.001
Less than 12 years29.431.532.853.117.8
12–1449.346.750.037.142.3
15 and above20.318.017.29.839.7
P § <0.001
Religious72.391.933.041.811.4<0.001
Not religious27.38.167.058.288.6
P § 0.008
§ p value for difference between Jews and Arabs in town C.
Table 2. Distribution of Self Rated Health (SRH), mental health, loneliness and social capital variables in each town, mean, standard deviation (SD) or percent and number.
Table 2. Distribution of Self Rated Health (SRH), mental health, loneliness and social capital variables in each town, mean, standard deviation (SD) or percent and number.
Town A
N = 981
Town B
N = 673
Town C
N = 1050
Town D
N = 1935
P between Towns
JewsArabs
%%%%%
Self-rated health-(SRH) <0.001
Good (1)76.778.673.566.167.4
Poor (0)23.321.426.533.932.6
P § 0.017
Mental health <0.001
Good (0)50.152.842.544.135.6
Poor (1)49.947.257.555.964.4
P § 0.35
Loneliness <0.001
Never36.478.067.255.548.8
Infrequently24.88.011.219.230.1
Sometimes30.59.712.613.118.0
Often7.94.49.012.23.2
P § 0.002
Cognitive Social Capital <0.001
Low (0)42.038.741.251.076.3
High (1)58.061.358.849.023.7
P § 0.024
Structural Social Capital Mean (SD)15.8 (2.76)15.1 (3.06)11.8 (2.39)10.2 (2.75)13.6 (2.32)<0.001
P § <0.001
§ p value for difference between Jews and Arabs in town C.
Table 3. Variables associated with social capital, logistic regressions presenting odds ratio (OR) and confidence intervals (CI), and linear regression.
Table 3. Variables associated with social capital, logistic regressions presenting odds ratio (OR) and confidence intervals (CI), and linear regression.
TownIndependent VariablesCognitive Social Capital Logistic RegressionStructural Social Capital Linear Regression
ORCIBeta adjp
Town A
N = 954
Gender0.760.58, 0.99−0.34<0.001
Age0.100.99, 1.01−0.14<0.001
Education 1.070.86, 1.330.26<0.001
Religiosity0.650.47, 0.89−0.020.61
Loneliness0.770.59, 1.01−0.080.004
Town B
N = 613
Gender0.900.64, 1.27−0.24<0.001
Age0.990.98, 1.00−0.120.004
Education0.760.59, 0.990.23<0.001
Religiosity0.650.35, 1.19−0.050.25
Loneliness0.810.50, 1.32−0.050.15
Town C
N = 1022
Gender1.260.97, 1.65−0.18<0.001
Age1.011.00, 1.02−0.110.001
Education1.751.42, 2.150.15<0.001
Jews (1) vs. Arabs (2)0.860.63, 1.16−0.16<0.001
Religiosity1.100.84, 144−0.090.003
Loneliness0.680.49, 0.94−0.16<0.001
Town D
N = 1735
Gender1.261.01. 1.58−0.070.003
Age1.000.99, 1.01−0.19<0.001
Education0.940.79, 1.110.10<0.001
Religiosity0.380.28, 0.53−0.030.18
Loneliness0.980.73, 1.30−0.12<0.001
Total population
N = 4329
Gender1.010.89, 1.14−0.17<0.001
Age0.990.99, 1.00−0.21<0.001
Education0.910.83, 1.000.19<0.001
Religiosity0.400.36, 0.46−0.23<0.001
Loneliness0.880.76, 1.02−0.020.11
Note: The reference categories of the dichotomized variables are: Gender = male, religiosity = religious, loneliness = not lonely, p < 0.05 Bold.
Table 4. Variables associated with self-rated health (SRH) and mental health, including structural social capital (participation in social activities). Logistic regressions presenting odds ratios (OR) and confidence intervals (CI).
Table 4. Variables associated with self-rated health (SRH) and mental health, including structural social capital (participation in social activities). Logistic regressions presenting odds ratios (OR) and confidence intervals (CI).
TownIndependent VariablesSRHMental Health
Model 1Model 2Model 1Model 2
ORCIORCIORCIORCI
Total
N = 4279
Gender0.870.74, 1.030.880.74, 1.041.151.01, 1.301.141.00, 1.29
Age0.930.92, 0.930.930.92, 0.931.021.02, 1.021.021.01, 1.02
Education1.151.02, 1.301.151.02, 1.301.151.04, 1.261.151.04, 1.26
Religiosity1.130.94, 1.351.130.94, 1.351.171.03, 1.351.181.03, 1.35
Loneliness0.480.40, 0.580.260.06, 0.401.941.66, 2.2710.144.53, 22.7
Structural Social capital1.141.10, 1.181.020.93, 1.120.920.90, 0.941.060.99, 1.14
Loneliness * Structural Social capital--1.091.02, 1.17 0.890.84, 0.94
Town A
N = 941
Gender0.950.65, 1.400.940.64, 1.371.070.80, 1.411.070.81, 1.42
Age0.950.94, 0.970.950.94, 0.971.000.99, 1.031.000.99, 1.01
Education1.310.80, 2.071.310.96, 1.780.880.70, 1.100.880.70, 1.10
Religiosity1.290.81, 2.101.310.81, 2.100.600.44, 0.820.600.44, 0.82
Loneliness0.850.59, 1.214.190.54, 32.30.790.60, 1.040.430.09, 2.09
Structural Social capital1.181.10, 1.271.371.11, 1.680.980.93, 1.040.930.81, 1.08
Loneliness * Structural Social capital--0.900.79, 1.03 1.040,94, 1.15
Town B
N =611
Gender1.000.63, 1.601.000.63, 1.611.150.82, 1.621.150.82, 1.62
Age0.950.94, 0.970.950.94, 0.971.000.99, 1.021.000.99, 1.02
Education1.561.07, 2.281.571.08, 2.301.060.81, 1.381.060.81, 1.38
Religiosity0.800.33, 1.940.810.33, 1.971.560.83, 2.911.560.83, 2.91
Loneliness0.470.26, 0.830.230.01, 5.442.891.73, 4.821.700.12, 23.4
Structural Social capital1.191.09, 1.301.120.86, 1.470.970.92, 1.030.940.76, 1.15
Loneliness * Structural Social capital--1.050.84, 1.31 1.040.87, 1.24
Town C
N= 937
Gender0.830.57, 1.210.830.57, 1.121.300.98, 1.721.300.98, 1.72
Age0.940.93, 0.950.940.92, 0.951.011.00, 1.021.011.00, 1.02
Education1.491.14, 1.941.491.14, 1.941.000.81, 1.251.000.81, 1.25
Religiosity1.250.86, 1.271.250.86, 1.821.160.86, 1.551.160.86, 1.55
Loneliness0.260.17, 0.390.070.01, 0.669.505.64, 16.014.10.94, 213
Jews (1) Vs. Arabs (2)0.630.40, 0.990.580.38, 0.890.890.62, 1.280.890.62, 1.28
Structural Social capital1.101.01, 1.200.950.74, 1.231.060.99, 1.131.100.84, 1.44
Loneliness * Structural Social capital--1.120.92, 1.36 0.960.76, 1.23
Town D
N = 1785
Gender0.870.67, 1.140.860.66, 1.131.120.90, 1.381.110.90, 1.38
Age0.890.88, 0.900.890.88, 0.901.031.03, 1.041.031.03, 1.04
Education1.090.90, 1.331.090.90, 1.331.171.00, 1.381.170.99, 1.38
Religiosity1.791.15, 2.771.811.17, 2.801.020.72, 1.451.020.72, 1.45
Loneliness0.590.42, 0.820.110.02, 0.852.641.91, 3.641.240.20 7.84
Structural Social capital1.271.27, 1.361.090.90, 1.320.770.73, 0.820.720.62, 0.85
Loneliness * Structural Social capital--1.130.98, 1.32 1.060.93, 1.20
Note: The reference categories of the dichotomized variables are: Gender = male, religiosity = religious, loneliness = not lonely, * p < 0.05 bold.
Table 5. Variables associated with self-rated health (SRH) and mental health, including cognitive social capital. Logistic regressions presenting odds ratio (OR) and confidence intervals (CI).
Table 5. Variables associated with self-rated health (SRH) and mental health, including cognitive social capital. Logistic regressions presenting odds ratio (OR) and confidence intervals (CI).
TownIndependent VariablesSRHMental Health
Model 1Model 2Model 1Model 2
ORCIORCIORCIORCI
Total
N = 4279
Gender (Men-1, Women-2)0.760.65, 0.890.760.65, 0.901.121.01, 1.411.241.09, 1.41
Age0.920.92, 0.930.920.92, 0.931.021.02, 1.031.021.02, 1.03
Education1.301.16, 1.461.301.15, 1.461.070.97, 1.171.070.97, 1.18
Religiosity1.030.86, 1.221.030.87, 1.231.221.07, 1.391.221.06, 1.39
Loneliness0.440.37, 0.530.420.34, 0.511.981.69, 2.322.081.74, 2.49
Cognitive social capital1.571.33, 1.861.290.94, 1.780.720.63, 0.820.820.64, 1.04
Loneliness * Cognitive social capital-1.110.96, 1.29-0.930.83, 1.05
Town A
N = 941
Gender (Men-1, Women-2)0.700.49, 1.000.690.49, 0.971.140.87, 1.481.140.88, 1.49
Age0.950.94, 0.970.950.94, 0.971.000.99, 1.011.000.99, 1.01
Education1.531.14, 2.061.561.16, 2.100.860.69, 1.070.840.67, 1.05
Religiosity1.280.80, 2.061.290.81, 2.070.620.45, 0.850.620.45, 0.84
Loneliness0.790.56, 1.120.940.62, 1.430.810.61, 1.060.710.51, 0.98
Cognitive social capital1.120.79, 1.581.770.97, 3.621.431.10, 1.871.000.58, 1.71
Loneliness * Cognitive social capital-0.820.64, 1.07-1.170,95, 1.44
Town B
N = 611
Gender0.770.45, 1.200.770.49, 1.201.200.86, 1.671.200.86, 1.67
Age0.950.93, 0.970.950.93, 0.971.000.99, 1.071.000.99, 1.02
Education1.891.31, 2.731.891.31, 2.731.030.78, 1.331.030.81, 1.33
Religiosity0.760.32, 1.830.800.33, 1.941.540.82, 2.881.510.81, 2.84
Loneliness0.450.26, 0.800.380.20, 0.722.861.71, 4.782.891.79, 5.73
Cognitive social capital1.540.99, 2.391.050.47, 2.340.820.58, 1.141.020.55, 1.87
Loneliness * Cognitive social capital-1.280.83, 1.98-0.870.62, 1.22
Town C
N = 937
Gender0.690.48, 0.990.690.48, 0.991.300.98, 1.691.290.98, 1.69
Age0.930.92, 0.940.930.92, 0.941.011.00, 1.021.011.00, 1.02
Education1.581.22, 2.051.551.19, 2.011.050.84, 1.291.070.86, 1.32
Religiosity1.260.89, 1.801.290.91, 1.851.110.84, 1.471.090.82, 1.45
Loneliness0.230.15, 0.330.170.11, 0.279.005.60, 14.412.27.01, 21.3
Jews (1) Vs. Arabs (2)0.490.33, 0.740.480.32, 0.730.910,65, 1.270.920.66, 1.28
Cognitive social capital1.421.00, 2.010.740.39, 1.380.700.54, 0.931.190.72, 1.95
Loneliness * Cognitive social capital-1.491.08, 2.04-0.700.52, 0.93
Town D
N = 1785
Gender0.830.64, 1.090.830.63, 1.081.210.98, 1.501.210.98, 1.50
Age0.880.87, 0.890.880.87, 0.891.041.03, 1.051.041.03, 1.05
Education1.170.96, 1.421.170.96, 1.421.070.91, 1.261.070.91, 1.26
Religiosity1.681.08, 2.621.661.07, 2.580.910.64, 1.310.930.65, 1.34
Loneliness0.470.34, 0.650.420.29, 0.592.882.08, 3.973.372.35, 4.82
Cognitive social capital1.611.17, 2.170.910.47, 1.780.460.36, 0.590.780.47, 1.30
Loneliness * Cognitive social capital-1.400.99, 1.98-0.730.56, 0.96
Note: The reference categories of the dichotomized variables are: Gender = male, religiosity = religious, loneliness = not lonely, * p < 0.05 Bold.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Baron-Epel, O.; Elran-Barak, R.; Donchin, M. Social Capital as a Mediator and Moderator in the Association between Loneliness and Health, Israel as a Case Study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 3698. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19063698

AMA Style

Baron-Epel O, Elran-Barak R, Donchin M. Social Capital as a Mediator and Moderator in the Association between Loneliness and Health, Israel as a Case Study. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(6):3698. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19063698

Chicago/Turabian Style

Baron-Epel, Orna, Roni Elran-Barak, and Milka Donchin. 2022. "Social Capital as a Mediator and Moderator in the Association between Loneliness and Health, Israel as a Case Study" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 6: 3698. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19063698

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop