Next Article in Journal
Testing a New Protocol of Nature-Based Intervention to Enhance Well-Being: A Randomized Control Trial
Previous Article in Journal
Analysis of Risk Factors for In-Hospital Death Due to COVID-19 in Patients Hospitalised at the Temporary Hospital Located at the National Stadium in Warsaw: A Retrospective Analysis
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Associations between Time Processing Ability, Daily Time Management, and Dementia Severity

by
Ann-Christine Persson
1,2,*,
Gunnel Janeslätt
3,4,
Lena Dahlberg
5,6,
Monika Löfgren
1,2 and
Marika Möller
1,2
1
Department of Clinical Sciences, Danderyd Hospital, Karolinska Institutet, 18288 Stockholm, Sweden
2
Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Danderyd University Hospital, 18288 Stockholm, Sweden
3
Department of Public Health and Caring Sciences, Uppsala University, 75105 Uppsala, Sweden
4
Center for Clinical Research in Dalarna, Box 712, 79129 Falun, Sweden
5
School of Health and Welfare, Dalarna University, 79188 Falun, Sweden
6
Aging Research Center, Karolinska Institutet & Stockholm University, 17165 Solna, Sweden
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(7), 3928; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19073928
Submission received: 9 March 2022 / Revised: 22 March 2022 / Accepted: 23 March 2022 / Published: 25 March 2022
(This article belongs to the Section Aging)

Abstract

:
This study investigated associations between time processing ability (TPA), daily time management (DTM), and dementia severity. Persons with dementia (PwDs) (n = 53) and their significant others (n = 49) participated in this cross-sectional study. Bivariate analyses were used to investigate associations between TPA and DTM and the dementia severity. Linear regression models were used to further predict the contribution of the subtests in the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) for TPA results. The results showed significant correlations between TPA and dementia severity, where visuospatial functions were the most highly correlated. TPA also showed a significant correlation to proxy-rated DTM. In addition, proxy-rated DTM was significantly correlated with dementia severity and PwDs’ own self-ratings of their DTM. Knowledge of the association between TPA, dementia severity, and visuospatial functions can enable early detection of TPA impairments. For a comprehensive assessment of TPA and DTM, objective measures should be used in combination with self-ratings and proxy-ratings. The findings can be used in clinical research and healthcare settings to develop methods to compensate for impaired TPA and support DTM in PwDs.

1. Introduction

Dementia is characterised by a progressive deterioration in cognition, affecting, among other things, memory and visuospatial functions [1], time perception and time orientation [2,3], and the ability to manage time [4]. Persons with dementia (PwDs) often need frequent support in daily time management (DTM) by significant others or formal caregivers, for example, to keep track of time, to arrive on time for scheduled activities, and to plan activities in a time-specific order with enough time allocated [5,6,7].
In the International Classification of Functioning, Disability, and Health (ICF) [8], time-related concepts are described at both the body function and activity and participation domains. Orientation to time, experience of time, and time management are included in the body function domain. Orientation to time includes both time concepts, such as dates, and objective time, such as, for example, understanding and using the clock or other devices for orientation in time. Experience of time includes knowledge of time duration. Time management is part of executive functions and encompasses knowing when to undertake an activity, for how long, and allocating time for different activities. These functions together form the hierarchical construct of time processing ability (TPA) [9,10]. DTM, on the other hand, is part of several components in the category “carrying out a daily routine” in the activity and participation domain, which illustrates the complexity of DTM [8,11]. DTM requires, for example, the ability to plan for and carry out daily activities in a time-specific order within requested time limits, and, if necessary, to adjust for unforeseen events. Thus, DTM has a central role in the everyday functioning.
Though dementia can affect both time-related cognitive functions and time-related occupations in daily life, in-depth assessments of TPA and DTM for PwDs are limited [7]. Until now, there have been a lack of validated instruments assessing all three levels of TPA and DTM for PwDs [12], although common assessments such as the Clock Drawing Test and Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) [13] include aspects of the ability to understand and orient oneself in time and space. Previous research on children with and without neurodevelopmental diagnoses has found that TPA is related to parent-rated DTM [14] and, in youth, there is a low but non-significant correlation with self-rated DTM [15], but there is a lack of knowledge if this is also valid for PwDs. Such knowledge would increase understanding on how assessments of TPA and DTM in PwDs should be interpreted in relation to each other.
Furthermore, to be able to provide appropriate support at the right time for PwDs, it is also important to enhance the understanding of how TPA and DTM are related to the progress of cognitive deficits in dementia. Previous research has found that orientation to time is related to the dementia severity [16,17,18], a relationship that might be linked to the hippocampus, which processes both spatial and temporal information [19]. Changes in the hippocampus and frontal lobes affecting spatial functions have also been connected to time perception impairments at an early stage in persons with Alzheimer’s disease [2,20]. However, orientation to time is just one component of TPA. Further research about the relation between dementia severity and all three levels of TPA can promote the development of assessment methods in healthcare services. Early detection of time-related impairments can increase the possibility that interventions—such as time assistive technology or compensatory strategies—can be implemented in a phase when cognitive abilities are relatively preserved and PwDs have more resources available to adopt new courses of action [7]. Examples of time assistive technology are devices that support time orientation by displaying time, day and date, or electronic calendars with information about and reminders for important activities. Compensatory strategies in DTM could be to keep a calendar in a visible place in the home to be able and see and refer to it frequently, to get ready in advance by gathering items that need to be brought to an appointment, or to use a direct debit to pay bills on time. Therefore, increased understanding of the relationships between TPA and DTM and dementia severity is essential, because it is a cornerstone in clinical assessments and future planning of intervention studies. The aim of this study was to investigate the associations between TPA, DTM, and dementia severity.

2. Materials and Methods

This study has a cross-sectional, explorative design and is part of the intervention study “Managing Time with Dementia” project. A study protocol is registered at www.clinicaltrials.gov, accessed on 8 March 2022, reg. no. NCT03677284. The study was approved by the Regional Ethical Review Board in Uppsala (reg. no. 2018/059, approved 16 May 2018).

2.1. Participants

PwDs (n = 53) and significant others (SOs; n = 49), such as spouses and children, were included (Table 1). The inclusion criteria for PwDs were a dementia diagnosis established by nationally established methods at the memory clinics, ≥11 on the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) [13,21], problems with DTM identified by occupational therapists through memory assessments, age ≥ 60 years, ability to communicate in Swedish, and no mental illness unrelated to dementia. The inclusion criteria for SOs were knowledge about the PwD’s daily life and DTM, and the ability to communicate in Swedish.

2.2. Materials

2.2.1. Demographic Items

Demographic data were collected via study-specific questionnaires including questions about gender, age, dementia diagnosis, MMSE, family situation (single, living alone; partner, not living together; partner/family, living together), and education (elementary school, high school, or college/university) for PwDs, and questions about gender, age, relation to participant (spouse, child), and support given days/week (≤1; 2–3; daily) for SOs.

2.2.2. Assessment to Measure TPA

KaTid® is a standardised and validated test to measure TPA in persons with cognitive impairment [8,9,14,22,23,24]. KaTid was originally developed for children and youths [9]. A new version, KaTid-Senior, has been developed for persons with mild to moderate dementia. The test consists of 26 items across four subcategories: (a) time perception/time sense (3 items), (b) time orientation/time concepts (8 items), (c) time orientation/objective time, (10 items) and (d) time management (5 items). Most of the items are scored Can (1) or Cannot (0), and three items are scored Can (2), On the way (1), or Cannot (0). The new version was validated with Rasch analyses showing acceptable psychometric properties [12], which enabled transformation of ordinal raw total scores of the KaTid-Senior into interval measures [25].

2.2.3. Assessments to Measure DTM

Time-S© is an instrument for self-rating DTM that has been validated for adults with mental and neurodevelopmental disabilities [26]. Time-S has been adapted and validated for older adults with mild to moderate dementia (Time-S Senior) by removing non-relevant items, e.g., items specific to adults in working age [12]. Time-S Senior contains 17 items with a Likert-like frequency scale with four response alternatives ranging from Never (1) to Always (4).
Time-Proxy© is an instrument used for proxy-rating of a person’s DTM. Previously, Time-Proxy has been used for youth with physical and intellectual disabilities [24,27]. The questions in Time-Proxy largely correspond to the questions in Time-S. In this study, significant others’ ratings of the PwDs’ DTM were made in an adjusted and validated version of Time-Proxy [12]. This version encompasses 10 items with five rating options: four ranging from Never (1) to Always (4), along with “I don’t know.” “I don’t know” was treated as a missing value.

2.2.4. Approximation Level of Dementia

The MMSE is a widely used screening test of cognitive functions [13]. In this study, the Swedish version of the MMSE was used to estimate cognitive level in persons with dementia [28]. The MMSE, with a total score of 30, includes the following sub-items: orientation (10), registration (3), attention and calculation (5), recall (3), language (8), and figure copying (1).

2.3. Procedure

During 2018–2021, participants were recruited from ten memory clinics in ten different regions in Sweden. Twelve registered occupational therapists working at the memory clinics conducted the recruitment and data collection using the demographic questionnaire, KaTid-Senior form, and Time-S Senior form for PwDs. The assessments were carried out in clinical settings or in the PwDs’ homes. At the same time, a significant other of the PwD answered the demographic questionnaire and the Time-Proxy form.
Before the data collection started, all occupational therapists on the memory clinics were trained in using the instruments. Data on dementia diagnoses and MMSE results were retrieved from medical records.

2.4. Statistical Analyses

A power analysis with the intent to use a multiple regression analysis including three independent variables and an assumption of an adjusted R square of 20% showed that a group of at least 36 persons is needed in the study to have 80% power and 5% significance level.
Depending on the results of normal distribution testing, Pearson’s r or Spearman’s rho were used to examine bivariate associations between measurements and t-tests for controlling for gender differences for potential associations between outcomes and demographic data. The calculations were made with IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 27.0. Armonk, NY, USA.
To further explore the association between TPA and dementia severity, MMSE sub-items correlating with the KaTid-Senior with r ≥0.30 were selected for multiple linear regression analysis. Prior to the regression analyses, analyses to ensure that there were no violations of the assumptions of linearity, independence, normality, and equal variance were conducted. Any possible impact of outliers was evaluated for each model by removing outliers before running the regression analysis again, and then comparing models. Adjusted R2 was used to evaluate model fit. The level of significance was set at p < 0.05. Statistical analyses were calculated with IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 27.0. Armonk, NY, USA. No adjustment to experimental alpha was made for multiple testing, and therefore, caution should be added for the risk of inflated Type I error rate.

3. Results

3.1. Characteristics of Participants

The gender distribution was relatively even for PwDs (47.2% women, 52.8% men), while a small majority of SOs were women (57.1%). The mean age was 74.9 years for PwDs and 67.1 years for SOs. Alzheimer’s disease was the most common dementia diagnosis (71.7%) and the mean result for PwDs on the MMSE was 22.4 out of 30. The majority of PwDs were living together with someone (81.1%), and most of the SOs were spouses (83.7%). Many SOs (71.4%) provided daily support to the person with dementia (Table 1).

3.2. Bivariate Associations between TPA and DTM and Severity of Dementia

The bivariate analyses presented in Table 2 showed a significant positive correlation between TPA (KaTid-Senior) and the dementia severity (MMSE); that is, the more severe dementia, the lower the TPA. In addition, analyses of total TPA in relation to MMSE sub-items showed significant weak correlations between TPA and orientation, attention and calculation, and language, and a moderate correlation between TPA and figure copying. Further bivariate analyses between the KaTid-Senior subgroups and the MMSE sub-items showed significant correlations in the following: time orientation/time concepts correlated with orientation, recall, and figure copying; time orientation/objective time correlated with attention and calculation, language, and figure copying; and time management correlated with attention and calculation, and figure copying.
No significant correlation was found between TPA (KaTid-Senior) and self-rated DTM (Time-S Senior), but significant correlations were found between TPA and proxy-rated DTM (Time-Proxy), and between proxy-rated DTM and self-rated DTM. Proxy-rated DTM also correlated with dementia severity (MMSE). No associations were found between either the TPA, the self-rated DTM, or the proxy-rated DTM and demographic data such as age, gender, education, or support given (not presented in Table 2).

3.3. Association between TPA and Cognitive Functions

As shown in Table 3, in the multiple regression model the MMSE sub-items, orientation, attention and calculation, and, particularly, figure copying were significant predictors of TPA (dependent variable). One outlier was removed to improve the model.

3.4. Associations between TPA Subcategories and Cognitive Functions

Three multiple linear regression analyses between the dependent TPA variables time orientation/time concepts, time orientation/objective time, and time management (KaTid-Senior subgroups), and the independent cognitive variables orientation, attention and calculation, and figure copying (MMSE sub-items), revealed several significant correlations as reported in Table 4. Figure copying was the most significant predictor of all TPA variables: time orientation/time concepts, time orientation/objective time, and time management. The sub-item attention and calculation was significant in predicting time orientation/objective time and time management. Omitting occasional outliers only slightly improved models and, therefore, the results reported in Table 4 include outliers.

4. Discussion

The aim of this study was to investigate the associations between TPA and DTM and dementia severity. There were significant correlations between TPA and dementia severity, especially with visuospatial functions. Significant correlations were also found between proxy-rated DTM and TPA, dementia severity, and PwDs’ self-ratings of DTM. However, no significant correlations were found between the PwDs’ self-rated DTM and TPA or dementia severity.
By revealing associations between all three levels of TPA and dementia severity, this study adds knowledge as to how underlying time functions are related to dementia severity. Previous research has shown that time orientation functions and spatial functions are connected to the hippocampus [29] and are processed in the right posterior parietal cortex, which is often affected by dementia [29,30]. Correspondingly, a significant moderate correlation was found between TPA and the figure copying—a function that is dependent on good visuospatial ability. Figure copying was also found to be associated with time orientation/time concepts, time orientation/objective time, and time management. This indicates that spatial functions are necessary for persons with dementia to be able to process time at all levels. The correlation between objective time/time concepts and the MMSE sub-item orientation could also be expected as both involve time orientation. Correlations between time orientation/objective time, time management, and MMSE sub-items attention and calculation could be anticipated as well, as previous research has suggested that short-term memory and attention are required for time estimation in persons with Alzheimer’s disease [31]. Importantly, the strongest correlations for the sub-item figure copying, as well as attention and calculation, were related to the higher levels of TPA.
However, the MMSE and other cognitive tests are frequently used for supporting diagnostics [2,3] and not for further rehabilitation planning or efforts to support daily living. Our findings suggest the need for in-depth TPA and DTM assessments at an early stage of dementia, especially when visuospatial or attentional impairments are observed. A previous study revealing associations between MMSE sub-items and activities of daily living (ADL) for patients with Alzheimer’s disease suggested that this could be useful information for clinicians, enabling early detection of patients in need of ADL support [32]. Similar assumptions could be made from the association between the MMSE and TPA. Early detection of TPA impairments is also advantageous since the timing of interventions, such as early implementation of time assistive technologies or compensatory strategies to enable time-dependent occupations, is of utmost importance as declining cognitive skills can make it too difficult to learn how to use new strategies or devices [7]. Research has shown that when the degree of cognitive impairment increases, older adults become less able to implement compensatory strategies [33]. As dementia is a progressive disease, it is important for health care professionals to be able to provide early interventions when the PwDs are most likely to benefit from them. However, MMSE results cannot, on their own, be used to predict TPA and DTM impairments, but rather indicate a need for an extended assessment of TPA and DTM—especially if failure on the visuospatial sub-item (figure copying) is observed. Further research is needed to confirm these findings.
Although this study found no significant associations between self-rated DTM (Time-Senior), TPA (KaTid-Senior), and dementia severity (MMSE), it is important to consider a PwDs’ own perception of their DTM. A significant association between TPA (KaTid-Senior) and self-rated DTM (Time-S Senior) might have been expected because these two concepts are suggested to be related [22]. One conclusion is that KaTid-Senior and Time-S Senior measure different aspects of time. Another explanation might be the discrepancy between an objective measure (KaTid-Senior) and self-ratings (Time-S), which is in accordance with research, suggesting that objective measurements do not always correspond with the individual’s experiences or opinions [15,34,35]. There is also a risk of response bias in cognitive self-ratings due to, for example, impaired memory functions, emotional responses on sensitive questions, or overestimation of one’s ability [36,37]. Another important factor is that impaired awareness of dementia and its consequences is common and has been also observed in mild stages of Alzheimer’s disease [38], even if persons with mild and moderate dementia have been found to be partially aware of their disabilities and able to rate their everyday functioning with reasonable accuracy [38,39,40]. Moreover, this study found a significant association of medium effect size between self-reported DTM in Time-S Senior and proxy-rated DTM in Time-Proxy. Thus, it is important to consider PwDs’ perceptions and experiences of their DTM, and to evaluate awareness of problems individually. Using self-ratings along with objective measures and proxy-ratings can help health care professionals in understanding a PwDs’ own conceptions of their DTM and form a basis for dialogue when planning interventions to enhance occupational performance.
Proxy ratings are widely used in research and clinical settings and this study emphasises the importance of information provided by significant others. According to the results, there was not only a significant association between proxy-rated DTM (Time-Proxy) and self-rated DTM (Time-S Senior), but also significant associations between proxy-rated DTM and TPA (KaTid-Senior), time orientation/time concepts and time orientation/objective time (KaTid subgroups), dementia severity (MMSE), and the MMSE sub-item orientation. The associations between proxy-rated DTM and time orientation functions may be because impairments in orientation to time often lead to repeated questions about time and dates and, therefore, become extra apparent, while impairments in experience of time may be more difficult to notice. Time management impairments might not be discovered if, for example, a partner has the main responsibility for time planning. Thus, there are reasons to combine proxy-ratings with self-ratings and observational assessments to get a comprehensive overview of a PwD’s abilities. Moreover, earlier research has shown that proxy-ratings tend to underestimate the functional ability of PwDs, indicating that over-reliance on proxy-ratings in research and clinical settings may be problematic [41]. Noteworthy, there are also previous studies that did not find significant correlations between proxy ratings and self-ratings [38,41], while another study found a significant correlation between TPA and proxy-rated DTM, but no significant correlation between TPA and self-rated DTM [10]. Hence, relying on proxy ratings or self-ratings alone may be inadvisable, thus suggesting that they should be used in combination to get a comprehensive picture of a PwD’s DTM. Taken together, the toolbox for assessment of time functions and skills should include both observational instruments on a PwDs’ TPA on a functional level, such as KaTid-Senior, as well as self-ratings and proxy ratings of DTM that add more in-depth knowledge and give a broader picture on an activity level. For a client-centred approach, self-ratings and proxy ratings of DTM are important complements that should be considered in the light of the PwD’s awareness of the consequences of dementia and within the experience of significant others, especially in the context of caregiving relationships.

5. Study Limitations and Implications for Future Research

Even though the power estimation showed that a sample of at least 36 participants would be sufficient for the planned analyses, a larger sample size might have provided more reliable estimates and the results should therefore be interpreted with caution. However, the COVID-19 pandemic significantly reduced the possibility for the memory clinics to recruit new participants.
Further investigation is warranted, for example, by including comparisons of TPA across different types of dementia. Interpretation of occasional outliers in the current analyses indicated that there might be differences between different dementia diagnoses, but there were not enough data to be able to draw such conclusions. In future research, it would also be of interest to explore changes in PwDs’ TPA and DTM over time and in relation to interventions. Moreover, the sample in this study included PwDs ≥60 years. Research has shown that there are differences in cognitive functioning between persons with early-onset and late-onset dementia [42]; therefore, results might not be generalised across age groups.

6. Conclusions

The correlations between TPA and dementia severity, and TPA and visuospatial abilities, are in line with previous research on the association between spatial functions and time-related functions. This study adds that, when visuospatial impairments are observed, it could also be a sign of TPA impairments, which requires further investigation of TPA and DTM—preferably at an early stage of dementia. Likewise, attentional deficits could be an indication of TPA impairments. For a comprehensive assessment of TPA and DTM, objective measures should be used in combination with self-ratings and proxy ratings as a basis for individually-adapted intervention planning. The results from this study can be used in further development of methods to identify and compensate for impaired TPA and support DTM in PwDs.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, all authors; Analysis, A.-C.P. and M.M.; Data Curation, A.-C.P.; Writing—Original Draft Preparation, A.-C.P.; Writing—Interpretation, Review & Editing, all authors; Supervision, G.J., L.D., M.L. and M.M.; Project Administration, G.J.; Funding Acquisition, all authors. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This study was funded by the Swedish Research Council for Health Working Life and Welfare (FORTE; grant no. 2017-00029) and the Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Danderyd University Hospital.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Regional Ethical Review Board in Uppsala (reg. no. 2018/059, approved 16 May 2018).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study. All participants received the following oral and written information about the study: the data is covered by the Public Access to Information and Secrecy Act (2009:400), the data will be processed so that no unauthorized person can take part of individual responses and the results will be presented on group level in scientific reports, such as journal articles and conference presentations, participation in the study is completely voluntary and can be cancelled at any time without explanation and without impact on future care or support. Participating PwDs were considered able to understand the study information and independently decide on their participation.

Data Availability Statement

Anonymized data can be available from the authors on reasonable request after all the planned manuscripts for the project “Managing Time with Dementia” have been published.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank all participants in the study. We also thank the occupational therapists that recruited participants and collected data: Marie Edqvist, Anita Gustafsson, Kristin Gylén, Eva Hallin Busk, Elisabeth Henley, Terése Kans, Kerstin Kåwe, Anna Lindqvist, Riitta Montgomery, Lena Nilsson, Anna Oldebring, and Afat Sharify.

Conflicts of Interest

G.J. originally developed the instruments KaTid, Time-S, and Time-P and sells KaTid material and training courses to professionals. No other author reports conflict of interest.

References

  1. Salimi, S.; Irish, M.; Foxe, D.; Hodges, J.R.; Piguet, O.; Burrell, J.R. Can visuospatial measures improve the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease? Alzheimer’s Dement. Diagn. Assess. Dis. Monit. 2018, 10, 66–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Dumurgier, J.; Dartigues, J.-F.; Gabelle, A.; Paquet, C.; Prevot, M.; Hugon, J.; Tzourio, C. Time Orientation and 10 Years Risk of Dementia in Elderly Adults: The Three-City Study. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2016, 53, 1411–1418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. El Haj, M.; Gandolphe, M.-C.; Wawrziczny, E.; Antoine, P. Flashbulb memories of Paris attacks: Recall of these events and subjective reliving of these memories in a case with Alzheimer disease. Medicine 2016, 95, e5448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Evans, D. An exploration of the impact of younger-onset dementia on employment. Dementia 2019, 18, 262–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Boger, J.; Quraishi, M.; Turcotte, N.; Dunal, L. The identification of assistive technologies being used to support the daily occupations of community-dwelling older adults with dementia: A cross-sectional pilot study. Disabil. Rehabil. Assist. Technol. 2014, 9, 17–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Evans, N.; Collier, L. An exploration of the experience of using calendar reminders for people with dementia and family carers. Dementia 2019, 18, 1912–1933. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Persson, A.-C.; Boman, I.-L.; Dahlberg, L.; Janeslätt, G.; Möller, M.C.; Löfgren, M. Lack of time and dependence on significant others: Occupational therapists´ experiences of prescribing time assistive technology for persons with dementia. Scand. J. Occup. Ther. 2020, 27, 614–624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health (ICF). Available online: https://apps.who.int/classifications/icfbrowser/ (accessed on 8 March 2022).
  9. Janeslätt, G. Validity in assessing time processing ability, test equating of KaTid-Child and KaTid-Youth. Child Care Health Dev. 2012, 38, 371–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Janeslätt, G.; Granlund, M.; Alderman, I.; Kottorp, A. Development of a new assessment of time processing ability in children, using Rasch analysis. Child Care Health Dev. 2008, 34, 771–780. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. ICF Update Platform. Available online: https://extranet.who.int/icfrevision/nr/loginICF.aspx (accessed on 8 March 2022).
  12. Persson, A.C.; Möller, M.C.; Dahlberg, L.; Löfgren, M.; Janeslätt, G. Assessing Time Processing Ability and Daily Time Management in Persons with Dementia: Psychometric Properties of Three Instruments. AOTJ 2022. Manuscript submitted for publication. [Google Scholar]
  13. Folstein, M.F.; Folstein, S.E.; McHugh, P.R. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 1975, 12, 189–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Janeslätt, G.; Granlund, M.; Kottorp, A. Measurement of time processing ability and daily time management in children with disabilities. Disabil. Health J. 2009, 2, 15–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Sköld, A.; Janeslätt, G.K. Self-rating of daily time management in children: Psychometric properties of the Time-S. Scand. J. Occup. Ther. 2017, 24, 178–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Guerrero-Berroa, E.; Luo, X.; Schmeidler, J.; Rapp, M.A.; Dahlman, K.; Grossman, H.T.; Haroutunian, V.; Beeri, M.S. The MMSE orientation for time domain is a strong predictor of subsequent cognitive decline in the elderly. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2009, 24, 1429–1437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Nishiura, Y.; Nihei, M.; Nakamura-Thomas, H.; Inoue, T. Effectiveness of using assistive technology for time orientation and memory, in older adults with or without dementia. Disabil. Rehabil. Assist. Technol. 2021, 16, 472–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. O’Keeffe, E.; Mukhtar, O.; O’Keeffe, S.T. Orientation to time as a guide to the presence and severity of cognitive impairment in older hospital patients. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 2011, 82, 500–504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Eichenbaum, H.; Dudchenko, P.; Wood, E.; Shapiro, M.; Tanila, H. The Hippocampus, Memory, and Place Cells: Is It Spatial Memory or a Memory Space? Neuron 1999, 23, 209–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. El Haj, M.; Moroni, C.; Samson, S.; Fasotti, L.; Allain, P. Prospective and retrospective time perception are related to mental time travel: Evidence from Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Cogn. 2013, 83, 45–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Kudlicka, A.; Martyr, A.; Bahar-Fuchs, A.; Woods, B.; Clare, L. Cognitive rehabilitation for people with mild to moderate dementia. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2019, 8, cd013388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Janeslätt, G.; Granlund, M.; Kottorp, A.; Almqvist, L. Patterns of Time Processing Ability in Children with and without Developmental Disabilities. J. Appl. Res. Intellect. Disabil. 2010, 23, 250–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Janeslätt, G.; Kottorp, A.; Granlund, M. Evaluating intervention using time aids in children with disabilities. Scand. J. Occup. Ther. 2014, 21, 181–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Persson, M.; Janeslätt, G.; Peny-Dahlstrand, M. Daily time management in children with spina bifida. J. Pediatr. Rehabil. Med. 2017, 10, 295–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Linacre, J.M. Optimizing rating scale category effectiveness. J. Appl. Meas. 2002, 3, 85–106. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  26. Janeslätt, G.; Lindstedt, H.; Adolfsson, P. Daily time management and influence of environmental factors on use of electronic planning devices in adults with mental disability. Disabil. Rehabil. Assist. Technol. 2015, 10, 371–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Wennberg, B.; Janeslätt, G.; Kjellberg, A.; Gustafsson, P.A. Effectiveness of time-related interventions in children with ADHD aged 9–15 years: A randomized controlled study. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2018, 27, 329–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Palmqvist, S. Validation of Brief Cognitive Tests in Mild Cognitive Impairment, Alzheimer’s Disease and Dementia with Lewy Bodies; Lund University: Lund, Sweden, 2011; Volume 2011. [Google Scholar]
  29. Oliveri, M.; Koch, G.; Caltagirone, C. Spatial–temporal interactions in the human brain. Exp. Brain Res. 2009, 195, 489–497. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Bueti, D.; Bahrami, B.; Walsh, V. Sensory and Association Cortex in Time Perception. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2008, 20, 1054–1062. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Papagno, C.; Allegra, A.; Cardaci, M. Time estimation in Alzheimer’s disease and the role of the central executive. Brain Cogn. 2004, 54, 18–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Han, G.; Maruta, M.; Ikeda, Y.; Ishikawa, T.; Tanaka, H.; Koyama, A.; Fukuhara, R.; Boku, S.; Takebayashi, M.; Tabira, T. Relationship between Performance on the Mini-Mental State Examination Sub-Items and Activities of Daily Living in Patients with Alzheimer’s Disease. J. Clin. Med. 2020, 9, 1537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Tomaszewski Farias, S.; Schmitter-Edgecombe, M.; Weakley, A.; Harvey, D.; Denny, K.G.; Barba, C.; Gravano, J.T.; Giovannetti, T.; Willis, S. Compensation Strategies in Older Adults: Association with Cognition and Everyday Function. Am. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. Other Dement.® 2018, 33, 184–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Kempert, H.; Benore, E.; Heines, R. Physical and occupational therapy outcomes: Adolescents’ change in functional abilities using objective measures and self-report. Scand. J. Pain 2017, 14, 60–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Wright, F.V.; Rosenbaum, P.L.; Goldsmith, C.H.; Law, M.; Fehlings, D.L. How do changes in body functions and structures, activity, and participation relate in children with cerebral palsy? Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2008, 50, 283–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Clare, L.; Whitaker, C.J.; Nelis, S.M. Appraisal of Memory Functioning and Memory Performance in Healthy Ageing and Early-Stage Alzheimer’s Disease. Aging, Neuropsychol. Cogn. 2010, 17, 462–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Hill, N.L.; Mogle, J.; Whitaker, E.B.; Gilmore-Bykovskyi, A.; Bhargava, S.; Bhang, I.Y.; Sweeder, L.; Tiwari, P.A.; Van Haitsma, K. Sources of Response Bias in Cognitive Self-Report Items: “Which Memory Are You Talking About?”. Gerontologist 2019, 59, 912–924. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Martyr, A.; Nelis, S.M.; Clare, L. Predictors of perceived functional ability in early-stage dementia: Self-ratings, informant ratings and discrepancy scores. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2014, 29, 852–862. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Trindade, P.G.E.; Santos, R.L.; Johannessen, A.; Neto, J.P.S.; Dourado, M.C.N. Awareness of Functional Status: People with Alzheimer’s Disease Abilities to Self-Report Impairment in Activities of Daily Living. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. Rep. 2020, 4, 405–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Öhman, A.; Josephsson, S.; Nygaard, L. Awareness through interaction in everyday occupations: Experiences of people with Alzheimer’s disease. Scand. J. Occup. Ther. 2008, 15, 43–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Martyr, A.; Clare, L. Awareness of functional ability in people with early-stage dementia. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2018, 33, 31–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Velayudhan, L.; Baillon, S.; Daby, L.; Suntharamoorthy, P.; Kablan, A.; Tromans, S.; Lindesay, J. Predictors of Disease Progression in Early-Onset Alzheimer’s Dementia: A Retrospective Cohort Study. J. Am. Med. Dir. Assoc. 2020, 21, 1735–1739. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Basic characteristics of participants with dementia and their significant others.
Table 1. Basic characteristics of participants with dementia and their significant others.
Persons with Dementia (n = 53)
Gender n (%)Women25 (47.2)
Men28 (52.8)
Age, years (mean, SD) 74.9 (6.9)
Diagnosis n (%)Alzheimer’s disease38 (71.7)
Vascular dementia6 (11.3)
Dementia with Lewy bodies4 (7.5)
Dementia (other)3 (5.7)
Alcohol dementia1 (1.9)
MMSE (mean, SD) 22.4 (4.0)
Family situation n (%)Single, living alone7 (13.2)
Partner, not living together3 (5.7)
Partner/family, living together43 (81.1)
Education n (%)Elementary school22 (41.5)
High school14 (26.4)
College/University16 (302.2)
Missing information1 (1.9)
Significant others (n = 49)
Gender n (%)Women28 (57.1)
Men21 (42.9)
Age, years (mean, SD) 67.1 (9.7)
Relation to participant n (%)Spouse41 (83.7)
Child7 (14.3)
Missing information1 (2.0)
Support given (days/week) n (%)≤1 6 (12.2)
2–36 (12.2)
Daily35 (71.4)
Missing information2 (4.1)
Table 2. Bivariate associations between time processing ability (KaTid-Senior), daily time management (Time-S Senior), proxy daily time management (Time-Proxy), and dementia severity (MMSE).
Table 2. Bivariate associations between time processing ability (KaTid-Senior), daily time management (Time-S Senior), proxy daily time management (Time-Proxy), and dementia severity (MMSE).
VariablesKaTid-Senior Time-S SeniorTime-Proxy
Total Score
(Logits)
A Time Perception/Time SenseB Time Orientation/Time ConceptsC Time Orientation/Objective TimeD Time
Management
Time-S Senior (logits)0.132−0.0490.1640.1120.127
Time-Proxy (logits)0.381 ** 10.2250.398 **0.323 *0.2170.363 *
MMSE total score0.359 **10.2120.420 **0.500 **0.491 **0.2090.325 * 1
MMSE orientation0.315 *0.1960.330 *0.2040.2540.2220.301 *
MMSE registration0.037−0.173−0.0320.1160.0230.0530.118
MMSE attention,
calculation
0.367 **−0.0100.2090.373 **0.462 **0.0720.133
MMSE recall0.2430.2380.315 *0.1230.1330.0450.137
MMSE language0.296 *0.2160.1730.295 *0.2110.1700.256
MMSE figure copying0.507 **0.1630.366 **0.462 **0.577 **0.0770.244
1 Correlations performed with Pearson’s r. All other correlations were performed with Spearman’s rho. ** p < 0.01; * p < 0.05.
Table 3. Multiple linear regression model of time processing ability (KaTid-Senior) to orientation, attention and calculation, and figure copying (MMSE sub-items).
Table 3. Multiple linear regression model of time processing ability (KaTid-Senior) to orientation, attention and calculation, and figure copying (MMSE sub-items).
Dependent TPA VariableIndependent Cognitive VariablesB95% CIp-Valuep-Value (Sig.), R2 and Adjusted R2 for the Model
Lower BoundUpper Bound
KaTid-SeniorMMSE Orientation0.2360.0000.4710.050p < 0.001
MMSE Attention, calculation0.1970.0030.3920.047R2 = 0.407
MMSE Figure copying1.1800.4701.8900.002Adjusted R2 = 0.368
Abbreviations: TPA = time processing ability, B = regression coefficient, CI = confidence interval.
Table 4. Multiple linear regression models of time orientation/time concepts (KaTid-Senior part B), time orientation/objective time (KaTid-Senior part C), and time management (KaTid-Senior part D), respectively, to orientation, attention and calculation, and figure copying (MMSE sub-items).
Table 4. Multiple linear regression models of time orientation/time concepts (KaTid-Senior part B), time orientation/objective time (KaTid-Senior part C), and time management (KaTid-Senior part D), respectively, to orientation, attention and calculation, and figure copying (MMSE sub-items).
Dependent TPA Variables
(KaTid-SENIOR Subcategories)
Independent
Cognitive Variables
B95% CIp-Valuep-Value (Sig.), R2 and Adjusted R2 for the Model
Lower BoundUpper Bound
Time orientation/
time concepts
MMSE Orientation0.3720.0870.6570.012p = 0.001
R2 = 0.291
Adjusted R2 = 0.245
MMSE Attention, calculation0.082−0.1500.3130.482
MMSE Figure copying1.0740.2251.9230.014
Time orientation/
objective time
MMSE Orientation0.092−0.3270.5100.662p < 0.001
R2 = 0.345
Adjusted R2 = 0.302
MMSE Attention, calculation0.4640.1230.8040.009
MMSE Figure copying1.7320.4852.9790.008
Time managementMMSE Orientation0.169−0.0630.4020.149p < 0.001
R2 = 0.460
Adjusted R2 = 0.424
MMSE Attention, calculation0.2260.0370.4150.020
MMSE Figure copying1.4350.7442.126<0.001
Abbreviations: TPA = time processing ability, B = regression coefficient, CI = confidence interval.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Persson, A.-C.; Janeslätt, G.; Dahlberg, L.; Löfgren, M.; Möller, M. Associations between Time Processing Ability, Daily Time Management, and Dementia Severity. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 3928. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19073928

AMA Style

Persson A-C, Janeslätt G, Dahlberg L, Löfgren M, Möller M. Associations between Time Processing Ability, Daily Time Management, and Dementia Severity. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(7):3928. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19073928

Chicago/Turabian Style

Persson, Ann-Christine, Gunnel Janeslätt, Lena Dahlberg, Monika Löfgren, and Marika Möller. 2022. "Associations between Time Processing Ability, Daily Time Management, and Dementia Severity" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 7: 3928. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19073928

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop