Next Article in Journal
Fast Air-to-Liquid Sampler Detects Surges in SARS-CoV-2 Aerosol Levels in Hospital Rooms
Next Article in Special Issue
A Survey Exploring How Watch Officers Manage Effects of Sleep Restrictions during Maritime Navigation
Previous Article in Journal
Degradation of Dye Wastewater by a Novel mBT-MPR Visible Light Photocatalytic System
Previous Article in Special Issue
Impact of Sleep Fragmentation on Cognition and Fatigue
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Different Effects of Social Jetlag and Weekend Catch-Up Sleep on Well-Being of Adolescents According to the Actual Sleep Duration

Department of Psychology “Renzo Canestrari”, University of Bologna, 40127 Bologna, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20(1), 574; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20010574
Submission received: 24 November 2022 / Revised: 21 December 2022 / Accepted: 28 December 2022 / Published: 29 December 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Sleep Deprivation and Related Health Issues)

Abstract

:
The aim of this study was to explore the potentially different associations between two common aspects of adolescents’ life, namely social jetlag and weekend catch-up sleep, with well-being and physical health, according to the actual sleep duration, i.e., <7 h and ≥7 h. To this end, 504 participants (42.1% males), with a mean age of 16.17 (standard deviation = 1.39), were examined in the this cross-sectional study. Participants were asked to wear the Micro Motionlogger Watch actigraph (Ambulatory Monitoring, Inc., Ardlsey, NY, USA) around their non-dominant wrist for seven consecutive days in order to objectively assess social jetlag and weekend catch-up sleep. Participants were also asked to fill in the Mental Health Continuum—Short Form for the assessment of subjective, social, and psychological well-being, as well as the SF-36 Health Survey for the perception of physical health. In adolescents sleeping less than 7 h, those experiencing weekend catch-up sleep longer than 120 min reported significantly lower subjective well-being compared to those with a weekend catch-up sleep duration between 0 and 59 min. These data pointed out the detrimental effect of long weekend catch-up sleep on self-reported well-being only in adolescents getting less than the recommended amount of sleep.

1. Introduction

According to a systematic review [1] which examined the time period between the year 1905 and the year 2008, a decrease of more than one hour of sleep per night was observed in children and adolescents. Moreover, it has been observed that over the time period between the year 1991 and the year 2012, the number of adolescents sleeping for at least seven hours per night has reduced, with the largest decrease taking place in 15-year-old adolescents [2]. Chronic sleep deprivation in teenagers has been associated with several negative outcomes, such as anxiety and school absenteeism [3].
In order to try to stem chronic sleep deprivation in adolescents, two different types of interventions have been conceived: on one hand, delaying the school start time [4,5] and, on the other, trying to soften the physiological phase delay of adolescents’ circadian system through morning light [6] or dawn simulation [7]. Both solutions have been tested and seem to have an effect on sleep duration; however, their feasibility in different social contexts is somewhat reduced. For example, delaying the school start time may lead to critical issues, such as management of the public transport timetable and socio-family rhythms. A third way to help adolescents to increase the overall amount of sleep they actually get is represented by psychoeducational interventions, mainly based on sleep hygiene rules [8]. However, the sleep/wake cycle of adolescents is still poorly understood.
Adolescents, during the weekend, can carry out compensative behaviors for sleep deprivation. These behaviors are, in turn, affected by the adolescents’ desire to have fun. Two of the most important behaviors have been outlined: (1) delaying bedtime and get-up time during weekend days compared to weekdays, which is known as social jetlag [9]; (2) extending their sleep duration during weekend days compared to weekdays, which is referred to as weekend catch-up sleep [10].
Social jetlag is defined as a discrepancy between biological and social timing [9]. The term jetlag was used because this phenomenon looks like a marked shifting of several hours in bedtime and get-up time between weekdays and weekend days. As previously observed [11], due to a mismatch between the biological phase delay and the early school start time, the prevalence of social jetlag reaches its maximum value during adolescence. Social jetlag in adolescents has been associated with an increased body mass index [12], low academic performance [13], as well as anxious [14] and depressive [15] symptomatology, which is considered a matter of public health [16].
Weekend catch-up sleep can be described as an extended duration of sleep during the weekend days compared to the weekdays [10]. While Kang et al. [17] observed an association between weekend catch-up sleep duration in adolescents and suicide attempts, as well as self-injuries, subsequent studies highlighted that the effects of weekend catch-up sleep seem to be different according to its duration. In this regard, Zhang et al. [18] reported that an amount of weekend catch-up sleep longer than 2 h was significantly associated with higher risks of mood and behavior disorders, as well as suicidality, tobacco smoking, and lower perception of mental and physical health, compared to a condition of weekend catch-up sleep ranging in length between 1 min and 2 h. Moreover, this last duration of the weekend catch-up sleep was related to a lower risk of anxiety and behavior disorders, as well as tobacco smoking and perception of low physical health compared to the absence of weekend catch-up sleep.
Comparing adolescents who get enough sleep (i.e., sleep duration ≥ 7 h [2]) with those who do not, the aim of the current study was to explore the potentially different associations between the different behaviors adopted during the weekend and their relationships with indices of psychophysical well-being. In more detail, the behavior adopted during the weekend was monitored via actigraphy in order to objectively identify those who engage in social jetlag, in weekend catch-up sleep, in both behaviors, and in neither of the two. Subjective indices of psychophysical well-being (in terms of subjective, social, and psychological well-being, as well as physical health perception) were also collected. Therefore, the ultimate aim of our study was to highlight if there is a behavior that qualifies as the most protective sleep-recovery strategy for the psychophysical well-being of adolescents. The promotion of such an evidence-based strategy in psychoeducational interventions could have relevant implications for the everyday life of adolescents.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

An overall number of 504 participants (42.1% of males), with a mean age of 16.17 (standard deviation = 1.39, range = 14.35–18.87), were examined in the current study. Participants were attending either the first year (50.7%) or the third year (49.3%) of secondary high school.

2.2. Actigraphy

The actigraph model Micro Motionlogger Watch (Ambulatory Monitoring, Inc., Ardsley, NY, USA) was used in the current study. The device was equipped with a triaxial accelerometer with a sensitivity of 0.01 g, while the filters and sampling frequency were set at 2–3 Hz and 32 Hz, respectively. Actigraphs were initialized through the Motionlogger Watchware software (Ambulatory Monitoring, Inc., Ardsley, NY, USA) in zero crossing mode to collect data in 1 min epochs, while actigraphic records were analyzed through the software Action W2 (Ambulatory Monitoring, Inc., Ardsley, NY, USA) using the algorithms validated by Cole and Kripke [19] and Cole et al. [20].

2.3. Actigraphic Social Jetlag and Weekend Catch-Up Sleep

Social jetlag was computed as the difference in the midpoint of sleep (i.e., the time of day that splits in half the time spent in bed) recorded during weekend days and school days. The length of the social jetlag is expressed in hours and minutes, with positive values pointing to a delayed midpoint of sleep during weekend days compared to school days. Participants were classified according to the following length of social jetlag: <60 min, 60–119 min, and ≥120 min. We selected those categories based on the criterion of the length of social jetlag, in line with the classification by Islam et al. [21]. We observed that 27.8% of participants were in the lowest duration category, 38.3% were in the middle duration category, and 33.9% were in the highest duration category.
The weekend catch-up sleep was calculated as the difference in the total sleep time (i.e., the sum in minutes of all the sleep epochs between sleep onset and get-up time) between weekend days and school days. Therefore, a positive value means a higher duration of total sleep time during weekend days compared to school days. Participants were grouped according to the following length of weekend catch-up sleep: <0 min; 0–59 min; 60–119 min; ≥120 min. We selected those categories according to the criterion of the frequency distribution, keeping the quartiles as a reference, with 22.1% in the first, 27% in the second, 25.4% in the third, and 25.5% in the fourth. This classification was also overall in line with that adopted by Kim et al. [22].

2.4. Subjective Measures of Well-Being and Physical Health Perception

Adolescents self-reported their well-being through the Italian version [23] of the Mental Health Continuum–Short Form (MHC–SF) [24]. The MHC-SF is composed of 14 items on a 6-level Likert scale with answers ranging from “never” to “every day”. Out of the 14 items, 3 are used to compute subjective well-being (e.g., “During the last four months, how often did you feel satisfied with life?”), five for social well-being (e.g., “During the last four months, how often did you feel that you belonged to a community (like a social group, or your neighborhood)?”, and six for psychological well-being (e.g., “During the last four months, how often did you feel that you liked most parts of your personality?”). Subjective, social, and psychological well-being are computed by summing up the scores of the corresponding items, with higher scores pointing to higher well-being.
Physical health perception was self-reported through the Italian version [25] of the subscale “General Health (GH)” from the SF-36 Health Survey [26]. The subscale is composed of four items on a 5-level Likert scale, ranging from “poor” to “excellent” for the first item (i.e., “In general, would you say your health is…”) and from “completely untrue” to “completely true” for the remaining three items (e.g., “My health is excellent”). Physical health perception is computed by summing up the scores of the four items, with higher scores pointing to higher physical health perception.

2.5. Procedure

Participants were drawn from the ongoing ERC-Consolidator project IDENTITIES “Managing identities in diverse societies: A developmental intergroup perspective with adolescents” project. This longitudinal research involved 1st and 3rd-year students (at the first assessment) from several high schools located in the northern part of Italy (i.e., the Emilia-Romagna region). Schools were selected through a stratified (by school track and level of urbanization) randomized method, and principals were approached to present the project. Active consent from parents was obtained prior to the children’s participation. Active consent was also obtained from adolescents of age, while their underage peers provided their assent to participate in the project. Participation in the study was voluntary, and students were informed that they could withdraw their consent at any time.
The longitudinal ongoing project started in 2022 and includes different assessments. For the present study, data from the wave collected April–May 2022 have been used.
Participants were invited to wear the actigraph Micro Motionlogger Watch (Ambulatory Monitoring, Inc., Ardsley, NY, USA) around their non-dominant wrist for seven consecutive days, for 24 h per day. Moreover, they were instructed to push the event-marker button of the device to signal their bedtime and the get-up time. At the end of the actigraphic recording, participants filled in the questionnaires during class hours.
Participants were included in the final database for statistical analyses only if, for the actigraphic recording, they had at least three valid nights during school days and one valid night during the weekend. According to the criteria put forward by Keyes et al. [2], participants were then split into two groups based on sleep duration: (1) those who slept enough, i.e., ≥7 h; (2) those who did not get enough sleep, i.e., <7 h.

2.6. Statistical Analyses

Pearson’s correlation analyses were performed separately for each sleep duration group, examining the duration (in minutes) of social jetlag and weekend catch-up sleep from one side and the scores of subjective, social, and psychological well-being, as well as physical health perception on the other.
One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed with the sleep duration group (two levels) as the between-subjects factor and subjective, social, and psychological well-being, and physical health perception scores as the dependent variables.
Moreover, we verified the distribution of participants, classified in categories according to the duration of social jetlag and weekend catch-up sleep, among the sleep duration group through a set of chi-squared tests.
Finally, separately for each sleep duration group, we independently assessed the effects of the social jetlag and weekend catch-up sleep categories as independent variables on the scores of subjective, social, and psychological well-being besides physical health perception as the dependent variable, through a set of one-way ANOVAs. In case of significant effects, the HSD Tukey post hoc test was used, with the significance level set to p < 0.05.

3. Results

As reported in Table 1, significant correlations were observed only in adolescents sleeping less than 7 h. In particular, a higher duration of weekend catch-up sleep was significantly associated with lower subjective-, social-, and psychological well-being, as well as a reduced perception of physical health.
As shown in Table 2, adolescents did not significantly differ in subjective, social, and psychological well-being or perception of physical health according to the duration of sleep (i.e., <7 h or ≥7 h).
As highlighted in Table 3, the distribution of adolescents sleeping < 7 h or ≥ 7 h was not significantly different among the categories based on the duration of the social jetlag (χ22 = 2.53; p = 0.28).
On the contrary, a significantly different distribution of adolescents, based on their sleep duration, was observed among the categories of weekend catch-up sleep (χ2 = 32.04; p < 0.001). In particular, among adolescents sleeping less than 7 h, it was observed that a higher percentage of participants presented a weekend catch-up sleep equal to or longer than 120 min compared to the adolescents sleeping ≥ 7 h (Table 4).
As shown in Table 5, within both groups of adolescents sleeping less than 7 h and equal to or higher than 7 h per night, no significant effects of the social jetlag categories on the outcomes of well-being and physical health perception were observed.
With reference to the group of adolescents sleeping for less than 7 h, we observed a significant effect of the weekend catch-up sleep categories on subjective well-being (Table 6). Performing the HSD Tukey post hoc test, adolescents with an amount of weekend catch-up sleep equal to or higher than 120 min reported a significantly lower level of subjective well-being than those showing a duration of the weekend catch-up sleep between 0 and 59 min (p < 0.05), while the other post hoc comparisons did not reach significance. As regards the group of adolescents sleeping 7 or more hours per night, no significant effects of the weekend catch-up sleep categories were observed in the outcomes of this study (Table 6).

4. Discussion

The main goal of the current study was to explore the potentially different effects of two frequent behaviors adopted by adolescents, namely social jetlag and weekend catch-up sleep, on psychophysical well-being. In more detail, we were interested in determining whether these relationships were different between adolescents getting enough sleep and those who were chronically sleep deprived.
The main result of the current study is that no behavior adopted during the weekend seems to be related to the well-being of adolescents” with “no behavior per se, adopted during the weekend, seems to be related to the well-being of adolescents. On the contrary, it seems that only the interaction between the weekend behavior and the actual sleep duration was able to modulate adolescents’ adjustment. In more detail, it was observed that longer weekend catch-up sleep was significantly associated with lower well-being only in the group of sleep-deprived adolescents (Table 1), therefore pointing out that the well-being of adolescents depends on the starting condition (namely, the sleep deprivation) that interacts with the compensatory weekend behavior (that is, the weekend catch-up sleep).
Moreover, it is also interesting to underline that, within the group of sleep-deprived adolescents, only a duration of weekend-catch-up sleep higher than 120 min was significantly related to lower well-being, compared to a duration between 0 and 59 min (Table 6). These data highlight that the negative effects of weekend catch-up sleep are not independent of its duration while, on the contrary, they seem to appear only when its length is over the cut-off value of 2 h. It is noteworthy that in the current study, among the group of adolescents sleeping less than 7 h, more than one-third (i.e., 34.1%) of the participants presented an amount of weekend catch-up sleep higher than the critical cut-off value of 2 h (Table 4), which could be interpreted as a compensative behavior to the starting condition, i.e., the sleep deprivation.
Based on our data, therefore, it seems that increasing adolescents’ sleep duration may have a protective effect on their well-being. To increase the duration of sleep, a specific intervention could be assessed and, in case of positive outcomes, promoted. More in detail, a short daily restorative nap [27,28] during schooldays that on one hand, may soften the sleep deprivation with a small recovery period and, on the other, may avoid the weekend sleep binge, could be introduced. In other words, based on the data of our study, i.e., negative effects of weekend catch-up sleep longer than 2 h on well-being in sleep-deprived adolescents, a short systematic daily nap during schooldays may be implemented, to maintain a period of weekend catch-up sleep no longer than 1 h. In case of positive outcomes on adolescents’ well-being, this strategy may be promoted as a sleep hygiene rule [8] within psychoeducational interventions.
The present study has some strengths; the assessment of sleep behaviors was objective (i.e., through actigraphy) and ecological (i.e., in the everyday life environment), besides being carried out in a large sample of adolescents. However, this study is not free from limitations; indeed, an accurate anamnesis, in order to exclude common pediatric diseases affecting the sleep/wave cycle as adenoid hypertrophy, tonsillar hypertrophy, allergic rhinitis or chronic rhinosinusitis, was not carried out. Moreover, since this is a study carried out in the real world, it was not possible to assess and control for potential confounding factors such as hormonal changes and pathological and psychophysical diseases. Furthermore, only the well-being of adolescents was examined as an outcome measure of social jetlag and weekend catch-up sleep, without taking into account the cognitive impact of both strategies—in other words, the academic performance of adolescents. Previous studies have reported an effect of social jetlag [13] and weekend catch-up sleep [29], although most studies measured these two behaviors subjectively. As highlighted in the systematic review by Sun and colleagues [29], “Future research should consider adopting longitudinal and experimental designs, and the use of objective sleep measures to further examine the impacts of weekday-to-weekend sleep differences on mental and physical health in the young population and to gain more insight into the mechanisms underlying their associations” (page 51). We believe that the current ongoing longitudinal study may have the potential, in the future, to address this specific point, taking into account the answers of participants to a question about the current grade-point average (GPA). Moreover, we should keep in mind that we have taken just one picture of the behavior of adolescents for one week and their perception of well-being during that week, i.e., we have observed a sort of immediate-phasic effect of the behavior adopted by adolescents on their well-being. Therefore, we are not currently able to answer this question: are there any effects of adolescents’ behavior on well-being in the short-, medium-, and long-term? However, the current study might have the potential to answer this question properly. Indeed, this ongoing longitudinal study will examine adolescents over time, potentially detecting the effects in the short (e.g., 4 months), medium (e.g., 1 year), and long term (e.g., 2 years or more). If feasible, the length of actigraphic monitoring should be extended to at least two weeks [30]. Finally, it will also be interesting to examine the potential role played by the socio-economic status of the families [31] and other significant contextual factors, such as the school track in which adolescents are enrolled.

5. Conclusions

The aim of the present study was to explore the potentially different associations, depending on the length of sleep, between two frequent behaviors adopted by adolescents, i.e., social jetlag and weekend catch-up sleep, and well-being. The main result is that, among sleep-deprived adolescents, weekend catch-up sleep longer than 120 min was significantly associated with lower well-being compared to those with a weekend catch up-sleep duration between 0 and 59 min. The take-home message of the current study is that no specific behavior per se was associated with well-being, but it seems that a sort of interaction between the starting condition—namely sleep deprivation—and one behavior—that is, weekend catch-up sleep—may play the primary role in the modulation of adolescents’ well-being.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, V.N. and E.C.; methodology, V.N. and E.C.; formal analysis, V.B.; investigation, L.T., A.A., V.B., M.G., V.N. and E.C.; data curation, A.A., V.B., M.G.; writing—original draft preparation, L.T.; writing—review and editing, L.T., A.A., V.B., M.G., V.N. and E.C.; supervision, V.N. and E.C.; funding acquisition, E.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by a grant from the European Research Council (ERC) under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme (ERC-CoG IDENTITIES Grant agreement N. 101002163; Principal investigator: Elisabetta Crocetti).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The present study was approved by the Ethics Committe of the Alma Mater Studiorum University of Bologna (Italy), Prot. n. 263836 del 14/10/2021, as part of the ERC-Consolidator project IDENTITIES “Managing identities in diverse societies: A developmental intergroup perspective with adolescents”.

Informed Consent Statement

Active consent from parents was obtained prior to the children’s participation. Active consent was also obtained from adolescents of age, while their underage peers provided their assent to participate in the project.

Data Availability Statement

The data are not publicly available and cannot be shared due to ethical issues.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funder had no role in the design of the study, in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data, in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results. The views and opinions expressed in this presentation are the sole responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the European Commission.

References

  1. Matricciani, L.; Olds, T.; Petkov, J. In search of lost sleep: Secular trends in the sleep time of school-aged children and ado-lescents. Sleep Med. Rev. 2012, 16, 203–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Keyes, K.M.; Maslowsky, J.; Hamilton, A.; Schulenberg, J. The Great Sleep Recession: Changes in Sleep Duration Among US Adolescents, 1991–2012. Pediatrics 2015, 135, 460–468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Seton, C.; Fitzgerald, D.A. Chronic sleep deprivation in teenagers: Practical ways to help. Paediatr. Respir. Rev. 2021, 40, 73–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Alfonsi, V.; Palmizio, R.; Rubino, A.; Scarpelli, S.; Gorgoni, M.; D’Atri, A.; Pazzaglia, M.; Ferrara, M.; Giuliano, S.; De Gennaro, L. The Association Between School Start Time and Sleep Duration, Sustained Attention, and Academic Performance. Nat. Sci. Sleep 2020, 12, 1161–1172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Minges, K.E.; Redeker, N.S. Delayed school start times and adolescent sleep: A systematic review of the experimental evidence. Sleep Med. Rev. 2015, 28, 86–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. Crowley, S.J.; Velez, S.L.; Killen, L.G.; Cvengros, J.A.; Fogg, L.F.; Eastman, C.I. Extending weeknight sleep of delayed ad-olescents using weekend morning bright light and evening time management. Sleep 2022, zsac202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Tonetti, L.; Fabbri, M.; Erbacci, A.; Filardi, M.; Martoni, M.; Natale, V. Effects of dawn simulation on attentional performance in adolescents. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 2014, 115, 579–587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Kira, G.; Maddison, R.; Hull, M.; Blunden, S.; Olds, T. Sleep education improves the sleep duration of adolescents: A ran-domized controlled pilot study. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 2014, 10, 787–792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Wittmann, M.; Dinich, J.; Merrow, M.; Roenneberg, T. Social Jetlag: Misalignment of Biological and Social Time. Chronobiol. Int. 2006, 23, 497–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Kim, S.J.; Lee, Y.J.; Cho, S.-J.; Cho, I.-H.; Lim, W.; Lim, W. Relationship Between Weekend Catch-up Sleep and Poor Performance on Attention Tasks in Korean Adolescents. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 2011, 165, 806–812. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Roenneberg, T.; Allebrandt, K.V.; Merrow, M.; Vetter, C. Social Jetlag and Obesity. Curr. Biol. 2012, 22, 939–943. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  12. Jankovic, N.; Schmitting, S.; Krüger, B.; Nöthlings, U.; Buyken, A.; Alexy, U. Changes in chronotype and social jetlag during adolescence and their association with concurrent changes in BMI-SDS and body composition, in the DONALD Study. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2021, 76, 765–771. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Tamura, N.; Komada, Y.; Inoue, Y.; Tanaka, H. Social jetlag among Japanese adolescents: Association with irritable mood, daytime sleepiness, fatigue, and poor academic performance. Chrono Int. 2021, 39, 311–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. de Souza, C.M.; Hidalgo, M.P. Midpoint of sleep on school days is associated with depression among adolescents. Chronobiol. Int. 2014, 31, 199–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Mathew, G.; Li, X.; Hale, L.; Chang, A.-M. Sleep duration and social jetlag are independently associated with anxious symptoms in adolescents. Chrono Int. 2019, 36, 461–469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Touitou, Y. Adolescent sleep misalignment: A chronic jet lag and a matter of public health. J. Physiol. 2013, 107, 323–326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Kang, S.-G.; Lee, Y.J.; Kim, S.J.; Lim, W.; Lee, H.-J.; Park, Y.-M.; Cho, I.H.; Cho, S.-J.; Hong, J.P. Weekend catch-up sleep is independently associated with suicide attempts and self-injury in Korean adolescents. Compr. Psychiatry 2014, 55, 319–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Zhang, J.; Paksarian, D.; Lamers, F.; Hickie, I.B.; He, J.; Merikangas, K.R. Sleep Patterns and Mental Health Correlates in US Adolescents. J. Pediatr. 2017, 182, 137–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Cole, R.J.; Kripke, D.F. Progress in automatic sleep/wake scoring by wrist actigraph. Sleep Res. 1988, 17, 331. [Google Scholar]
  20. Cole, R.J.; Kripke, D.F.; Gruen, W.; Mullaney, D.J.; Gillin, J.C. Automatic Sleep/Wake Identification from Wrist Activity. Sleep 1992, 15, 461–469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Islam, Z.; Hu, H.; Akter, S.; Kuwahara, K.; Kochi, T.; Eguchi, M.; Kurotani, K.; Nanri, A.; Kabe, I.; Mizoue, T. Social jetlag is associated with an increased likelihood of having depressive symptoms among the Japanese working population: The Fu-rukawa Nutrition and Health Study. Sleep 2020, 43, zsz204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Kim, K.M.; Han, S.M.; Min, I.K.; Heo, K.; Kim, W.-J.; Chu, M.K. Weekend catch-up sleep and depression: Results from a nationally representative sample in Korea. Sleep Med. 2021, 87, 62–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Petrillo, G.; Capone, V.; Caso, D.; Keyes, C.L.M. The Mental Health Continuum–Short Form (MHC–SF) as a Measure of Well-Being in the Italian Context. Soc. Indic. Res. 2014, 121, 291–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Keyes, C.L.M. Mental Illness and/or Mental Health? Investigating Axioms of the Complete State Model of Health. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 2005, 73, 539–548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Apolone, G.; Mosconi, P. The Italian SF-36 Health Survey: Translation, Validation and Norming. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 1998, 51, 1025–1036. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Ware, J.E.; Gandek, B.; The IQOLA Project Group. The SF-36 health survey: Development and use in mental health research and the IQOLA project. Int. J. Mental Health 1994, 23, 49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Naitoh, P. Circadian cycles and restorative power of naps. In Biological Rhythms, Sleep and Shift Work; Johnson, L.C., Tepas, D.I., Colquhoun, W.P., Colligan, M.J., Eds.; SP Medical and Scientific Books: New York, NY, USA, 1981; pp. 553–580. [Google Scholar]
  28. Faraut, B.; Andrillon, T.; Vecchierini, M.-F.; Leger, D. Napping: A public health issue. From epidemiological to laboratory studies. Sleep Med. Rev. 2017, 35, 85–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Sun, W.; Ling, J.; Zhu, X.; Lee, T.M.; Li, S.X. Associations of weekday-to-weekend sleep differences with academic per-formance and health-related outcomes in school-age children and youths. Sleep Med. Rev. 2019, 46, 27–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Meltzer, L.J.; Montgomery-Downs, H.E.; Insana, S.P.; Walsh, C.M. Use of actigraphy for assessment in pediatric sleep research. Sleep Med. Rev. 2012, 16, 463–475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Sosso, F.A.E.; Holmes, S.D.; Weinstein, A.A. Influence of socioeconomic status on objective sleep measurement: A systematic review and meta-analysis of actigraphy studies. Sleep Health 2021, 7, 417–428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Pearson’s correlation values (with the p-value within brackets) between social jetlag and weekend catch-up sleep on one side and subjective, social, and psychological well-being, and physical health perception on the other, separately for the sleep duration group. Significant correlations are highlighted in bold.
Table 1. Pearson’s correlation values (with the p-value within brackets) between social jetlag and weekend catch-up sleep on one side and subjective, social, and psychological well-being, and physical health perception on the other, separately for the sleep duration group. Significant correlations are highlighted in bold.
Adolescents Sleeping < 7 h
Subjective
Well-Being
Social Well-BeingPsychological
Well-Being
Physical Health
Perception
Social jetlag−0.011
(p = 0.852)
0.092
(p = 0.117)
0.038
(p = 0.518)
0.037
(p = 0.530)
Weekend catch-up sleep−0.194
(p < 0.001)
−0.147
(p < 0.05)
−0.166
(p < 0.01)
−0.124
(p < 0.05)
Adolescents sleeping ≥ 7 h
Subjective
Well-Being
Social Well-BeingPsychological
Well-Being
Physical Health
Perception
Social jetlag−0.029
(p = 0.679)
0.010
(p = 0.881)
−0.046
(p = 0.518)
−0.049
(p = 0.481)
Weekend catch-up sleep−0.030
(p = 0.672)
−0.015
(p = 0.830)
−0.016
(p = 0.816)
−0.012
(p = 0.865)
Table 2. Means and standard deviations of the subjective, social, and psychological well-being as well as physical health perception by sleep duration group. Statistics are also reported.
Table 2. Means and standard deviations of the subjective, social, and psychological well-being as well as physical health perception by sleep duration group. Statistics are also reported.
Adolescents
Sleeping < 7 h
Adolescents Sleeping ≥ 7 hStatistics
Subjective well-being4.06 ± 1.094.08 ± 1.16F1,494 = 0.03; p = 0.86
Social well-being3.11 ± 1.263.12 ± 1.18F1,495 = 0.01; p = 0.94
Psychological well-being3.87 ± 1.133.95 ± 1.14F1,490 = 0.50; p = 0.48
Physical health perception3.81 ± 0.673.81 ± 0.64F1,502 = 0.01; p = 0.93
Table 3. Distribution of participants sleeping < 7 h or ≥ 7 h among the categories based on the duration of social jetlag. Frequency and percentage are shown.
Table 3. Distribution of participants sleeping < 7 h or ≥ 7 h among the categories based on the duration of social jetlag. Frequency and percentage are shown.
Adolescents Sleeping < 7 hAdolescents Sleeping ≥ 7 h
FrequencyPercentageFrequencyPercentage
<60 min8125.37031.5
60–119 min12739.78036
≥120 min112357232.4
Table 4. Distribution of participants sleeping < 7 h or ≥ 7 h among the categories based on the duration of weekend catch-up sleep. Frequency and percentage are shown.
Table 4. Distribution of participants sleeping < 7 h or ≥ 7 h among the categories based on the duration of weekend catch-up sleep. Frequency and percentage are shown.
Adolescents Sleeping < 7 hAdolescents Sleeping ≥ 7 h
FrequencyPercentageFrequencyPercentage
<0 min5717.86328.4
0–59 min7924.76629.7
60–119 min7523.46428.8
≥120 min10934.12913.1
Table 5. Means and standard deviations of subjective, social, and psychological well-being as well as physical health perception according to the extent of the social jetlag, separately for sleep duration group. Statistics are also reported.
Table 5. Means and standard deviations of subjective, social, and psychological well-being as well as physical health perception according to the extent of the social jetlag, separately for sleep duration group. Statistics are also reported.
Social JetlagAdolescents Sleeping < 7 h
<60 min60–119 min>120 minStatistics
Subjective well-being 3.99 ± 0.994.16 ± 1.103.99 ± 1.15F2,288 = 0.85; p = 0.43
Social well-being 2.94 ± 1.193.15 ± 1.273.19 ± 1.29F2,288 = 0.97; p = 0.38
Psychological
well-being
3.70 ± 1.054.01 ± 1.123.84 ± 1.19F2,285 = 1.8; p = 0.17
Physical health
perception
3.88 ± 0.673.79 ± 0.693.79 ± 0.64F2,294 = 0.49; p = 0.62
Social jetlag Adolescents sleeping ≥ 7 h
<60 min60–119 min120 minStatistics
Subjective well-being 4.14 ± 1.224.03 ± 1.224.07 ± 1.03F2,202 = 0.17; p = 0.84
Social well-being 3.18 ± 1.282.98 ± 1.163.21 ± 1.10F2,203 = 0.79; p = 0.46
Psychological
well-being
4.02 ± 1.243.93 ± 1.133.90 ± 1.07F2,201 = 0.20; p = 0.82
Physical health
perception
3.85 ± 0.733.77 ± 0.603.81 ± 0.61F2,204 = 0.23; p = 0.79
Table 6. Means and standard deviations of subjective, social, and psychological well-being as well as physical health perception according to the extent of the weekend catch-up sleep, separately for sleep duration group. Statistics are also reported, with significant effects in bold.
Table 6. Means and standard deviations of subjective, social, and psychological well-being as well as physical health perception according to the extent of the weekend catch-up sleep, separately for sleep duration group. Statistics are also reported, with significant effects in bold.
Weekend
Catch-Up Sleep
Adolescents Sleeping < 7 h
<0 min0–59 min60–119 min≥120 minStatistics
Subjective
well-being
4.31 ± 0.884.31 ± 1.113.83 ± 1.153.91 ± 1.08F3,287 = 3.94; p < 0.01
Social well-being 3.40 ± 1.103.26 ± 1.333 ± 1.322.94 ± 1.22F3,287 = 1.98; p = 0.12
Psychological
well-being
4.07 ± 0.884.09 ± 1.093.78 ± 1.083.70 ± 1.28F2,284 = 2.41; p = 0.07
Physical health
perception
3.86 ± 0.763.84 ± 0.663.92 ± 0.653.70 ± 0.63F2,293 = 1.74; p = 0.16
Weekend catch-up sleep Adolescents sleeping ≥ 7 h
<0 min0–59 min60–119 min≥120 minStatistics
Subjective
well-being
4.14 ± 1.133.96 ± 1.294.19 ± 1.073.97 ± 1.08F3,201 = 0.53; p = 0.70
Social well-being3.11 ± 1.103.03 ± 1.273.24 ± 1.143.10 ± 1.27F3,202 = 0.30; p = 0.82
Psychological
well-being
3.98 ± 1.103.81 ± 1.264.11 ± 1.103.85 ± 1.05F3,200 = 0.77; p = 0.51
Physical health perception3.76 ± 0.703.88 ± 0.683.81 ± 0.583.73 ± 0.56F3,203 = 0.48; p = 0.70
Significant effect is highlighted in bold.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Tonetti, L.; Andreose, A.; Bacaro, V.; Grimaldi, M.; Natale, V.; Crocetti, E. Different Effects of Social Jetlag and Weekend Catch-Up Sleep on Well-Being of Adolescents According to the Actual Sleep Duration. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, 574. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20010574

AMA Style

Tonetti L, Andreose A, Bacaro V, Grimaldi M, Natale V, Crocetti E. Different Effects of Social Jetlag and Weekend Catch-Up Sleep on Well-Being of Adolescents According to the Actual Sleep Duration. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2023; 20(1):574. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20010574

Chicago/Turabian Style

Tonetti, Lorenzo, Alice Andreose, Valeria Bacaro, Martina Grimaldi, Vincenzo Natale, and Elisabetta Crocetti. 2023. "Different Effects of Social Jetlag and Weekend Catch-Up Sleep on Well-Being of Adolescents According to the Actual Sleep Duration" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 20, no. 1: 574. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20010574

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop