Next Article in Journal
Physical Inactivity Levels of European Adolescents in 2002, 2005, 2013, and 2017
Previous Article in Journal
Assessment of Patient Treatment and Rehabilitation Processes Using Electromyography Signals and Selected Industry 4.0 Solutions
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Pedestrians’ Perceptions of Motorized Traffic Variables in Relation to Appraisals of Urban Route Environments

by
Dan Andersson
1,
Lina Wahlgren
1,
Karin Sofia Elisabeth Olsson
1 and
Peter Schantz
1,2,*
1
The Research Unit for Movement, Health and Environment, Department of Physical Activity and Health, The Swedish School of Sport and Health Sciences (GIH), 114 86 Stockholm, Sweden
2
Department of Public Health and Clinical Medicine, Section of Sustainable Health, Umeå University, 901 87 Umeå, Sweden
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20(4), 3743; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20043743
Submission received: 18 October 2022 / Revised: 1 February 2023 / Accepted: 9 February 2023 / Published: 20 February 2023
(This article belongs to the Section Environmental Health)

Abstract

:
It is important to examine how motorized traffic variables affect pedestrians along a gradient from rural to inner urban settings. Relations between pedestrians’ perceptions of four traffic variables and appraisals of route environments as hindering–stimulating for walking as well as unsafe–safe for reasons of traffic, were therefore studied in the inner urban area of Stockholm, Sweden (n = 294). The pedestrians rated their perceptions and appraisals with the Active Commuting Route Environment Scale (ACRES). Correlation, multiple regression, and mediation analyses were used to study the relationships between the traffic variables and the outcome variables. Noise related negatively to both hindering–stimulating for walking, and to unsafety–safety for traffic reasons. Vehicle speed related negatively to unsafety–safety for traffic reasons. Furthermore, vehicle speed protruded as an important origin of the deterring effects of traffic among those who commute by foot. The study shows the value of both partial and simultaneous analyses of the effect of all four traffic variables in relation to outcome variables relevant for walking.

Graphical Abstract

1. Introduction

Walking has been a major source of physical activity in the history of mankind. Today, its benefits on health are well recognized [1,2,3]. At the same time, a considerable portion of the adult population is physically inactive [4], which calls for a search for pathways to promote physical activity. From that perspective, walking is of particular interest since it can be easily undertaken and demands no specific equipment. It is, however, important that the external environments support this form of locomotion, both as a behaviour and through creating environmental wellbeing.
Due to the introduction of motorized vehicles on a large scale, from the second world war and onwards, there has been a dramatic change in the route environments amongst those who walk. It is very important to examine how these altered traffic landscapes affect pedestrians along a gradient from rural to inner urban settings.
Many environmental assessment tools related to walking have been developed. Examples of these include the Neighborhood Environment Walkability Scale (NEWS) [5], the Twin Cities Walking Survey (TCWS) [6], and the Saint Louis Environment and Physical Activity Instrument [7]. They all have questions about aspects of the built and traffic environments, primarily with four-point response scales of the Likert type.
However, to understand the effect of individual traffic variables on walking behaviour and/or wellbeing while walking is challenging. Multiple variables, that capture different aspects of the environment, e.g., vehicle speed, vehicle flow, noise, and exhaust fumes, should preferably be included in the analyses. This requirement has, in many cases, not been fulfilled, e.g., [8,9,10]. Hence, diverse, and sometimes contradictory findings have been reported regarding transportation walking. For example, one study reported that participants preferred streets with little traffic [11], while in another study, traffic volume did not show an association with walking [12]. It is also evident from that research field that one might walk regardless of an unsupportive external environment, e.g., for economic reasons, or that areas are not serviced by public transport. Thus, walking behaviours may not be related to traffic environments.
Furthermore, the lack of a common nomenclature makes comparisons between studies complicated. For example, Humpel et al., [8] used the following items: “Crossing busy roads is a big problem” and “Traffic makes it dangerous or unpleasant”. How should these items be interpreted in relation to the traffic variables?
Given that, a more fruitful way could be to analyse how different traffic variables are perceived, and route environments appraised, by those who regularly walk in those settings. For that purpose, we suggest a conceptual framework involving three interconnected levels (Figure 1). In order to explore such relationships between active transportation and the route environment, the Active Commuting Route Environment Scale (ACRES) has been developed and evaluated [13,14]. The ACRES is a self-report tool which assesses cycling and pedestrian commuters´ perceptions and appraisals of their individual commuting route environments.
This conceptual framework is based on several inputs. One is long-term educational experience by one of the authors (PS). Further support comes from the scientific literature. Safety (for different reasons) has been reported as an important issue among those who walk [11,16,17]. In addition, a need for the supplementary outcome of hinders–stimulates walking has been identified [18]. Furthermore, Panter et al. evaluated associations between shifts in perceptions of the route environment, and changes in active commuting [19]. When it became less pleasant to walk, there was a drop in the amount of walking. This refers to the walking behavior in the final level of the model. Physical activity can affect wellbeing [20], which refers to the psychological effects in the model. It is important to separate that effect on wellbeing from how walking routes can affect environmental wellbeing. An example of the latter is that traffic volume and speed has been described as both as a barrier for walking and affecting wellbeing negatively [9].
As stated by Anciaes et al., [9] many researchers have only used one variable as a proxy for traffic. That can be a reasonable strategy, but only if it is based on analyses supporting the choice of such proxies. In our mind, this prompt attempts to sort out the relationships between four traffic variables: speeds of motor vehicles, flow of motor vehicles, noise, and exhaust fumes, and how they relate to the outcomes hindersstimulates walking and unsafesafe traffic (Figure 2).
We consider speeds of motor vehicles and flow of motor vehicles as basic traffic variables and noise and exhaust fumes as intermediate outcomes. This is also reflected by the fact that models for calculating noise depends on both flow and speeds of motor vehicles [21,22].
In this explorative and relational study, our focus is therefore on the relationships between: (a) the four traffic variables, (b) the basic variables and the intermediate outcomes, respectively, as well as (c) how the four traffic variables, and combinations of them, relate to the outcome variables. To our knowledge there exist no previous studies of these issues. We have studied male and female walking commuters (n = 294) rating their commuting routes using ACRES in the inner urban area of Stockholm, Sweden.
The rationales for these foci are to understand these relations as such, and to create a basis for how study protocols, audit tools and questionnaires can be outlined, possibly through choosing relevant proxy variables. Furthermore, the present study can assist in understanding results of studies which have used less than four traffic variables in their ambition to highlight how traffic affects pedestrians. Research such as this can also provide urban and traffic planners, and others, with important information on how to create attractive route environments, hence, contributing to active transportation and improved public health.

2. Method

2.1. Procedure and Participants

This study is part of the Physically Active Commuting in Greater Stockholm (PACS) research project. Individuals who walk or cycle to their place of work or study, were recruited via two major newspapers in Stockholm, in May and June 2004. The participants had to be at least 20 years old, reside in the County of Stockholm (excluding Norrtälje Municipality)(see Figure 3), and actively commute the entire distance at least once a year.
The advertisement resulted in 2148 individuals volunteering to participate. A first survey, the Physically Active Commuting in Greater Stockholm Questionnaire (PACS Q1) was distributed in September 2004 (response frequency = 94%). A second survey (PACS Q2) was distributed in May 2005 (response rate = 92%). The respondents commuted in the inner urban or suburban–rural areas of Greater Stockholm, or in both of these settings (see Figure 3 and Figure 4). For additional information about the recruitment process, see Wahlgren and Schantz [13,23].
Advertisement recruitment, as a sampling method, has been compared to street recruitment on cycle commuters regarding ratings of route environments [13]. It was hypothesized that the street-recruitment strategy could represent those who actively commuted with greater certainty than the advertisement-recruited sample. Overall, the ratings indicated a good correspondence between the two groups. Furthermore, the characteristics of the two samples, with respect to background factors such as age, employment, and education, showed reasonable compliance [24].
In this study, we have solely utilised data from pedestrian commuting in the inner urban area. Some of them (26%) commuted in the suburban area as well, but only data from the inner urban area has been used. A previous study by Wahlgren and Schantz [13], has indicated that cycle commuting in both areas, does not affect ratings in any of the areas.
A total of 294 participants (77% women) were included in the analyses: 56.5% were pedestrians and 43.5% dual-mode commuters, i.e., individuals who alternate between walking and cycling. For descriptive characteristics of the participants, see Table 1, and for characteristics of their walking behavior, see Table A1, Table A2 and Table A3 in Appendix A. The Ethics Committee North of the Karolinska Institute at the Karolinska Hospital approved the study (Dnr 03-637). The participants gave their informed consent.

2.2. Descriptive Characteristics of the Participants

Descriptive characteristics of the participants were obtained from PACS Q1 and Q2, see Table 1.

2.3. The Physically Active Commuting in Greater Stockholm Questionnaires (PACS Q1 and Q2)

PACS Q1 and PACS Q2 are self-report surveys in Swedish. The questionnaires include background questions and questions related to active commuting. Both surveys, including English versions, can be found as supporting information in Schantz et al. [25]. In total, they consist of 103 items.

The Active Commuting Route Environment Scale (ACRES)

To investigate the relationships between perceptions and appraisals of active commuters and the route environment, the Active Commuting Route Environment Scale (ACRES) was created [13,14]. The ACRES is included in PACS Q2. The pedestrian version consist of 13 items and each item considers both the inner urban area, and the suburban—rural areas. All items have two parallel response lines, one for each area. The scales have adjectival opposites (see Table 2) and a neutral mid-position.
The participants were encouraged to rate their experience of their route environments based on their commuting during the preceding two weeks. A more in-depth portrayal of the development of the scale, as well as its validity and reliability for cyclists, has been reported by Wahlgren et al. [14] and by Wahlgren and Schantz [13].

2.4. Study Area

The commuting environments are located in the inner urban area of Stockholm, Sweden. The city itself stretches across several islands where Lake Mälaren flows into the Baltic Sea. When data was collected, the population was approximately 1.9 million. The inner urban area, in our geographical division of the city, includes the following areas: Gamla stan, Södermalm, Kungsholmen, Vasastan, Norrmalm and Östermalm (Figure 4). Blocks are organized in grid-like streetscapes, constituting a different environment compared to the suburban and rural parts of Stockholm County. Throughout the city there are green and blue spaces, squares, and historic buildings.
Within these inner urban areas, several different roads exist, e.g., local streets, collector roads, minor and major arterial roads. Typically, the arterials have bidirectional traffic, whereas local streets normally have unidirectional traffic. Sidewalks are always present on both sides of the streets. The motor vehicles were cars, lorries and buses with internal combustion engines. Data was collected prior to the introduction of electric motor vehicles, electrically-assisted bikes, and e-scooters. Furthermore, pedestrians did not use headphones/earphones nearly as much as they do today.
In a sample of local streets, a collector road, and minor arterial roads, the vehicle flow on weekdays varied between 222–1726 vehicles per hour in both directions during the morning rush hour (7–9 a.m.), and between 291–1827 vehicles per hour in both directions during the evening rush hour (4–6 p.m.). The corresponding vehicle speeds ranged between 24.4–38.4 and 24.8–40.9 km per hour in the morning and evening rush hours, respectively. For a more in-depth portrayal of vehicle flows and vehicle speeds on different roads, at different times, in these inner urban areas, as well as where the measurements took place, see Figure A1, Figure A2 and Figure A3 in Appendix A. When considering that the pedestrians also walked along local streets with less traffic, it can be concluded that a variety of traffic settings formed the basis for the ratings. The great majority of the respondents walked in good natural light conditions (Table A2 and Table A3) cf. [26].

2.5. Statistical Analyses

Data were entered into the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) and analysed in version 27.0 (IBM SPSS Inc., Somers, NY, USA). Data from PACS Q2, which include the ACRES, were checked for accuracy. Some respondents were excluded, predominantly because of incomplete or incorrect ACRES data. Data from those with no missing ACRES values were used for the route environmental analyses.
Differences between ratings of ACRES with respect to gender were examined with independent t-tests. Correlation analyses between the predictor and outcome variables were assessed with Pearson’s correlation coefficient (r). The correlations were, in absolute values, r ≤ 0.774. Correlations between the background variable age and the predictor variables of motor traffic were, in absolute values, r ≤ −0.137. According to Tabachnik and Fidell, [27] multicollinearity can be a problem with correlations ≥ 0.90 whereas Field, [28] suggests that attention should be paid to predictors with a correlation above 0.80.
Multiple regression analysis was used to examine the relationships between the predictors and the outcomes.
The values from the multiple regression analyses are displayed as y-intercepts, unstandardized coefficients B, and their 95% confidence intervals, as well as adjusted R square for the overall models. A statistical level corresponding to at least p < 0.05 was chosen to indicate significance. Prior to running the analyses, the linearity between the variables was assessed visually with the help of boxplots, error bars and scatterplots. The variables showed acceptable linearity.
Mediation analyses using PROCESS, a macro to SPSS, were run in models where a direct or indirect effect was hypothesized to occur. PROCESS, written by Professor Andrew F. Hayes, can be downloaded from www.processmacro.org [29] (accessed on 1 February 2022). To test the indirect effect, we used 5000 bootstrap samples to generate a confidence interval.
Four background variables were included in the multiple regression analyses and in the mediation analyses: sex (binary variable), age (continuous variable), education (binary variable: educated at university level or not educated at university level) and income (categorical variable coded into three categories: ≤25,000 SEK/month, 25,001–30,000 SEK/month and ≥30,001 SEK/month; Swedish crowns (SEK) 2005: €1 ≈ 9 SEK; US$1 ≈ 8 SEK). Only significant background variables are reported in the multiple regression ana-lyses.
The variance inflation factor (VIF) was used to assess multicollinearity. All models with predictor and outcome variables, including the four background variables, were checked (all values ≤ 3.30, mean: 1.22) indicating no dilemma [28]. Cook’s distance was used to identify extreme data cases. No such cases were found in either of the models, (all values ≤ 0.086, mean: 0.004) indicating no problem [28]. The top limit for inclusion of standardized residuals was set to ± 3 SD. A total of 30 individual cases (in 22 models) had a standardized residual above the top limit. Since they were few, relatively close to the limit for inclusion, and had no problems with extreme data cases, they were included in the multiple regression analyses. A table with the VIF, the Cook’s distance and the standardized residuals can be found in Appendix A (Table A4).

3. Results

3.1. Perceptions of the Environmental Variables in Males and Females

Levels of the outcome variables hindersstimulates walking and unsafesafe traffic as well as of the predictor variables vehicle speed, vehicle flow, noise and exhaust fumes are given in Table 3. A significant difference between males and females was noted in vehicle speed.

3.2. Correlations between the Environmental Variables

There were positive correlations between all predictor variables (range: 0.462–0.774), whereas they correlated negatively with the outcome variables hindersstimulates walking (range: −0.210–−0.328) and unsafesafe traffic (range: −0.183–−0.238) (Table 4, Figure 5 and Figure A4 in Appendix A).

3.3. Relations between the Predictor Variables

The relations between the predictor variables were analysed with simultaneous multiple regression analyses. All regression coefficients were positive (range: 0.508–0.770; p < 0.001) (Table 5 and Figure 5).

3.4. Relations between the Basic Variables Vehicle Speed and Vehicle Flow and the Intermediate Outcomes Noise and Exhaust Fumes

Both vehicle speed and vehicle flow were significant in relation to the intermediate outcome noise. Only vehicle flow was significant in relation to exhaust fumes. For all values, see Table 6 and Figure 6.

3.5. Relations between the Predictor Variables and the Outcome Hinders–Stimulates Walking

All regression coefficients were significant and negative; range: −0.198–−0.296, with the lowest value for vehicle speed and the highest for noise. For all values see Table A5 and Figure A4 in Appendix A.

3.6. Relations between Combinations of Predictor Variables and the Outcome Hinders–Stimulates Walking

In the first combination, vehicle flow was negatively related with hinders–stimulates walking, and in the second combination noise had the corresponding role. When all traffic variables were included as predictors, only noise was negatively related to hinders–stimulates walking (Table 7, Figure 7 and Figure 8).

3.7. Relations between the Predictor Variables and the Outcome Unsafe–Safe Traffic

All relations between each single traffic variable and the outcome unsafe–safe traffic were negative; range: −0.170–−0.247, with the lowest value for vehicle flow and the highest for noise (Table A6 and Figure A4 in Appendix A).

3.8. Relations between Combinations of Predictor Variables and the Outcome Unsafe–Safe Traffic

In the first combination, vehicle speed was negatively related with unsafe–safe traffic, and in the second combination, noise had the equivalent role. When all traffic variables were included as predictors, both vehicle speed and noise were significantly related to the outcome (Table 8, Figure 8 and Figure 9).

3.9. Mediation

Mediation analyses were run in models where a possible mediating effect was of interest to investigate (Table 9). In addition to previously analysed variables, a composite variable was created by multiplying vehicle flow with vehicle speed since this combination possibly could affect the outcome variables. Only mediated effects (p < 0.05) corresponding to 40% or more are commented upon. An indirect effect of vehicle speed on noise is mediated by vehicle flow (78%), and an indirect effect of vehicle speed on exhaust fumes is mediated by vehicle flow (95%). Noise mediates the effect of vehicle flow and vehicle speed to hinders–stimulates walking (the indirect effects were 81% and 82% respectively). Noise mediates the effect of vehicle flow and vehicle speed to unsafe–safe traffic (the indirect effects were 104% and 42% respectively).

3.10. A Graphic Illustration of Important Pathways Based on the Commuting Pedestrians’ Perceptions and Appraisals of Their Route Environments

When adding mediated effects (p < 0.05) corresponding to 40% or more to Figure 8, the following aggregated illustration emerges, see Figure 10.

4. Discussion

One of the main results of this study, on four motorized traffic variables, was that noise was negatively related to both of the outcomes hinders–stimulates walking and unsafe–safe traffic, whereas vehicle speed was negatively related to unsafe–safe traffic (Figure 8 and Figure 10). The regression equation for the relation between the predictors and the outcome hinders–stimulates walking was y = 11.8 − 0.24 noise (all p-values ≤ 0.006, Adj. R2 = 0.11). The regression equation for the other outcome unsafe–safe traffic was y = 13.2 − 0.21 noise −0.16 vehicle speed (all p-values ≤ 0.05, Adj. R2 = 0.05).
The external validity of these results needs to be commented upon in relation to different subpopulations. In a study where it was examined how different route settings were appraised in terms of plausible or not plausible choices among four groups of cyclists (regular, frequent, occasional, and potential), the order of the results was consistent among the groups. Thus, the principal preferences for different route environment components were the same. However, the sensitivity in the ratings between the groups increased between regular, frequent, occasional, and potential cyclists, when the settings became less attractive. To put it briefly, regular cyclists were less demanding regarding their route environments [30]. If this is translated to the present study, it suggests that regular pedestrians might be less sensitive to high levels of motorized vehicle flow, speed, noise and exhaust fumes. Thus, in the general population, the unstandardised beta coefficients might be higher and the y-intercepts lower.
Another result was that the perception of each traffic variable was negatively related to both the outcome hinders–stimulates walking, and to unsafe–safe traffic. This means that if imprecise or few traffic variables, e.g., [8,9,10,31] have been included as indicators for traffic, then diverse results can be a consequence. This can be misleading in a search for more critical variables.
How these and other relations can be further analysed will be discussed below. In doing so, we will integrate the results from correlation (CA), multiple regression (MRA), and mediation analyses (MA).

4.1. The Relationships between Perceptions of the Predictor Variables of Motor Traffic

There were some high positive correlations between the four traffic variables. The highest was between vehicle flow and noise (r = 0.77) followed by between noise and exhaust fumes (r = 0.74).
These high correlations indicate that the analyses of possible separate effects of single traffic variables in relation to hinders–stimulates walking and unsafe–safe traffic can be hampered when they are jointly involved in MRA analyses. That dilemma can possibly be circumvented with more participants involved, or research strategies that aim to try isolate the effect of separate individual traffic variables. Our approach has been to analyse the relationships between the traffic and outcome variables step by step, with different tools, and thereby hopefully disclosing important information.

4.2. Vehicle Speed and Vehicle Flow in Relation to Noise and Exhaust Fumes

The level of vehicle flow is equal to the number of vehicles passing a certain reference point during a given time frame. Vehicle speed impacts vehicle flow, as was reflected in a correlation of 0.61 and a regression coefficient of 0.72.
Vehicle speed and vehicle flow were both individually and jointly related to noise, whereas only vehicle flow was related to exhaust fumes in MRA. However, MA showed that exhaust fumes was also affected by vehicle speed through a mediation via vehicle flow (Table 9 and Figure 10). The magnitudes of the mediation from vehicle speed via vehicle flow to both noise (78%) and exhaust fumes (95%) were surprisingly high, and have, to the best of our knowledge, not been shown before. Even though perceptions of exhaust fumes was unrelated to both of our outcomes, note that in urban areas, pedestrians are frequently exposed to invisible and odourless threats, e.g., particulate matter (PM).

4.3. The Traffic Variables in Relation to the Outcome Variable Hinders–Stimulates Walking

When combining the basic variables vehicle speed and vehicle flow, and analysing them in MRA, with hinders–stimulates walking as an outcome, only vehicle flow was negatively related to the outcome. Noise had the corresponding role when combining the intermediate outcomes noise and exhaust fumes in the MRA. Finally, when all traffic variables were included as predictors, only noise was negatively related to hinders–stimulates walking (Figure 7). The MA showed that both vehicle flow and vehicle speed, per se, and to about 80%, are mediated via noise to hinders–stimulates walking. Again, this shows that it is critical which variables are included in the analyses and how the understanding can be developed by using both MRA and MA. Note that we created a composite variable of vehicle flow × vehicle speed (Table 9). The rationale was to examine if the basic variables combined would have a different effect on the outcome variables compared to when the variables acted on their own. However, the results were in general accordance with those of the single predictors.
Anciaes et al. [9] studied residents close to highly trafficked roads and noted that when both the traffic volume and the traffic speed were rated as high, it appeared that walking was hindered, and the wellbeing was rated lower. This is in line with McGinn et al. (2007) stating that “It is only when high-traffic volume is combined with high-traffic speeds that being physically active outdoors may pose a problem” [32]. However, given the present results those findings could, just as well, be due to higher levels of noise, a variable that Anciaes et al. [9] and McGinn et al. [32] did not study.
As stated, noise was related to hindering walking in the present study and has been reported to be negatively associated with walking in other studies, e.g., [11,33,34]. In line with that, transportation noise annoyance can lead to lower levels of physical activity [35]. Proximity to major roads has also been associated with reduced levels of physical activity, independently of noise annoyance [35], and it can possibly be due to a barrier effect of such roads, as has been shown when located between residential and green areas [36].
Both the MRA and the MA analyses point in the direction that vehicle speed is an important origin of the deterring effects of noise (Figure 10). Speed management should, therefore, be a key issue. In line with that, the World Health Organization suggests a speed limit of 30 km/h in urban areas [37]. Indeed, as is evident from Figure A1 and Figure A2, such an implementation would be important also in the present study area.
The “noise-problem” can only be partially solved with electric cars. There is a thres-hold at approximately 30 km/h [38,39]. Below this speed, an electric car makes less noise than a conventional car (with an internal combustion engine). For speeds above 30 km/h, which are frequent in urban areas, the friction of the tires is the major source of the noise.

4.4. Comments on Relations between Noise and Vehicle flow

It is noteworthy that the MA indicated that there was a certain portion (19%) of the direct effect from vehicle flow on hinders–stimulates walking, although not significant. This might be due to its high correlation to noise (r = 0.77), and the not very high level of participants.
Although vehicle flow is highly related to noise, we see reasons to point out that our perception of a given vehicle flow can vary to a great extent. The vehicle flow can be more or less visually hidden behind motor vehicles, vegetation, land masses, buildings, etc. (Figure 11a,b). Furthermore, the vehicle flow might be close by or distant, visible in different angles, and moving towards or away from us. Finally, it might be behind us and therefore not visible. Thus, a given vehicle flow, can affect us visually very differently. This is, indeed, in contrast to noise, which reaches us even if we close our eyes. From these perspectives, it is reasonable that noise protrudes as a major hindering factor in this study.
The external validity of our results needs to be discussed in relation to both the overall environmental setting and the mode of active transportation. In cross-sectional studies of cyclists in suburban-rural settings, it has been noted that both vehicle flow and noise were negatively related to inhibiting–stimulating cycling [40]. This discrepancy regarding vehicle flow in relation to our results can possibly be explained by differences in settings, which may affect some of our perceptions and appraisals fundamentally different. A plausible explanation could be that massive buildings in the inner urban area masks or diminishes the way vehicle flow protrudes compared to in the suburban–rural setting. In support for such an interpretation is an experimental pilot study in a green area, where it was shown that intra-individual comparisons between seeing a high vehicle flow with a high speed on a highway, led to significantly higher (34%) levels of environmental unwellbeing compared to when the vehicle flow was masked, despite identical noise levels [41]. We therefore suggest that these relationships should be further studied. One promising way forward is to try to isolate the variables when assessing them. This can be done by stage-managing the assessment site as was done in the pilot study above, where visibility was restricted or non-restricted in situ.
A final comment regarding isolating the effect of vehicle flow on outcomes relates to whether perceptions and appraisals are undertaken with or without daylight. The effect of vehicle lights, in dark surroundings, can be disturbing. Note that, as stated in Methods, the natural light conditions in the present study were good.

4.5. The Traffic Variables in Relation to the Outcome Variable Unsafe–Safe Traffic

When combining the basic variables vehicle speed and vehicle flow in MRA, vehicle speed related negatively with unsafe–safe traffic, whereas when the intermediate outcomes noise and exhaust fumes were combined, noise related negatively with unsafe–safe traffic. Finally, when all traffic variables were included as predictors, both vehicle speed and noise related negatively to the outcome (Figure 9). Somewhat surprisingly, we noted that the unstandardized beta coefficient for noise was 28% higher than for vehicle speed. Although this difference was not significant, it deserves attention in future studies.
Interestingly, both vehicle speed and vehicle flow, respectively, were mediated via noise on the outcome unsafe–safe traffic. The effect of vehicle flow on unsafe–safe traffic was fully mediated via noise whereas the effect of vehicle speed was mediated to a lesser degree.
Safety has been reported as an important issue for pedestrians [11,16,17,42]. Walking has, however, also been reported to be unrelated to route safety and traffic [43]. Still, in the present study, perceptions of both noise and vehicle speed impacted appraisals of unsafe–safe traffic negatively. The observed relation between noise and unsafe–safe traffic is an unexpected and important finding that demands more attention.
The speed of vehicles directly influences the risk of a crash, as well as the severity of injuries including the likelihood of a fatality [44]. Therefore, the negative relation between vehicle speed and unsafe–safe traffic is not surprising and is in line with indications that streets with slow traffic are appreciated by those who walk [11].

5. Conclusions

Noise, which depends on vehicle flow and vehicle speed, is the dominant negative predictor variable in relation to both the outcome hinders–stimulates walking and to unsafe–safe traffic. Vehicle flow contributed, however, more powerfully to perceptions of noise than vehicle speed did. Vehicle speed had a negative role in relation to unsafe–safe traffic.
The mediation analyses revealed that vehicle speed acts on noise via vehicle flow. Vehicle speed protrudes, therefore, as an important origin to the deterring effects of traffic on walking. From the point of promoting walking, speed management and noise reduction should, therefore, be a local, regional, national, and supranational concern.
Finally, this study can further the understanding of how to interpret results from other studies, which may have used fewer traffic variables in an ambition to highlight the influence of the traffic environment. This understanding can assist the research, transport, and planning societies in choosing relevant proxy variables. The results from this study can also be of importance for decision makers and others when aiming to create a sustainable future with higher levels of physical activity within the population, and a decreased local, regional, and global dependency on fossil fuels and electricity.
To support such aims we have used the results from this study and combined it with other predictors, such as greenery and aesthetics, to further our understanding of which the relevant factors are that deter or facilitate walking [45].

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, P.S.; data curation, L.W.; formal analysis, D.A., K.S.E.O. and P.S.; funding acquisition, P.S.; supervision, L.W. and P.S.; visualization, D.A. and K.S.E.O.; writing—original draft, D.A., L.W. and P.S.; writing—review and editing, D.A., L.W., K.S.E.O. and P.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This study was supported by funding received from the Research Funds of the Swedish Transport Administration (TRV 2017/63917-6522). The funding source was not involved in any aspect of the study.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The Ethics Committee North of the Karolinska Institute at the Karolinska Hospital approved the study (Dnr 03-637).

Informed Consent Statement

The participants gave their informed consent.

Data Availability Statement

Data is available upon request.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to the volunteers for participating in the study, and for the technical assistance of Erik Stigell, Cecilia Schantz-Eyre, Lina Wahlgren, Golam Sajid, Eva Minten and Per Brink, as well as to Peter Schantz and Erik Stigell for the development of the ACRES. We extend our gratitude to Rui Wang for statistical advice, Charlotta Eriksson for valua-ble comments on noise and health and Gina Lovasi for valuable comments on the manuscript. Fi-nally, we thank Tobias Johansson at the City of Stockholm Transport Department for sharing traf-fic data, and the reviewers for constructive comments.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Tables and figures illustrating walking behavior, methodological issues, results, and relations are provided here.
Table A1. Descriptive characteristics of the walking commuting trips (mean ± SD, (95% CI) and n participants).
Table A1. Descriptive characteristics of the walking commuting trips (mean ± SD, (95% CI) and n participants).
DistanceDurationSpeedFrequency of
Walking Trips in May
Total Number of
Walking Trips over the Year *
kmminkm·h−1trips·week−1
Men2.98 ± 1.9131.4 ± 18.45.64 ± 1.196.24 ± 5.89274 ± 172
(2.52–3.45)(26.9–35.9)(5.35–5.93)(4.75–7.72)(229–319)
6766666359
Women2.91 ± 1.6332.7 ± 17.65.27 ± 1.395.83 ± 4.99279 ± 158
(2.70–3.13)(30.3–35.1)(5.08–5.45)(5.12–6.54)(255–303)
220213213192165
Notes: The number (n) of participants varies between the different variables and sexes due to missing or incorrectly reported data. * One male and three females had remarkably high numbers of walking trips per year (range: 893–1183). These outliers were therefore replaced with mean values for the remaining males and females, respectively. After replacing the outliers, the final values were calculated for both sexes.
Table A2. The distribution of time when commuting from home to the place of work or study (n participants and percentage within each sex).
Table A2. The distribution of time when commuting from home to the place of work or study (n participants and percentage within each sex).
a.m.5–6>6–7>7–8>8–9>9–10>10 a.m.–<5 a.m.
Menn17242962
(n = 69)%1.410.134.842.08.72.9
Womenn3168689912
(n = 215)%1.47.440.041.44.25.6
Notes: Ten females have been excluded due to missing or incorrectly reported data. One female reported that she walked between 7 and 9 a.m. She was placed in the >7–8 category.
Table A3. The distribution of time when commuting from the place of work or study to home (n participants and percentage within each sex).
Table A3. The distribution of time when commuting from the place of work or study to home (n participants and percentage within each sex).
p.m.2–3>3–4>4–5>5–6>6–7>7–8>8 p.m.–<2 p.m.
Menn1523231232
(n = 69)%1.47.233.333.317.44.32.9
Womenn31271912459
(n = 215)%1.45.633.042.311.22.34.2
Notes: Ten females have been excluded due to missing or incorrectly reported data. Two females reported that they walked between 4 and 6 p.m. They were placed in the >4–5 category.
Figure A1. Vehicle flow (a) and average vehicle speed (b) per hour on local streets and collector roads in inner urban areas of Stockholm. The measurement points show data for both driving directions during a typical weekday (Monday or Wednesday) in April 2005. The ratio between light motor vehicles (≤3500 kg) and heavy (>3500 kg) was: 94.6%. Data has been collected by the City of Stockholm Transport Department (in Swedish: Trafikkontoret, Stockholms stad). Further information about these traffic measurements can be found in the City of Stockholm report [46].
Figure A1. Vehicle flow (a) and average vehicle speed (b) per hour on local streets and collector roads in inner urban areas of Stockholm. The measurement points show data for both driving directions during a typical weekday (Monday or Wednesday) in April 2005. The ratio between light motor vehicles (≤3500 kg) and heavy (>3500 kg) was: 94.6%. Data has been collected by the City of Stockholm Transport Department (in Swedish: Trafikkontoret, Stockholms stad). Further information about these traffic measurements can be found in the City of Stockholm report [46].
Ijerph 20 03743 g0a1
Figure A2. Vehicle flow (a) and average vehicle speed (b) per hour on minor arterials in inner urban areas of Stockholm. The measurement points show data for both driving directions during a typical weekday (Tuesday to Thursday) in April 2005. The ratio between light motor vehicles (≤3500 kg) and heavy (>3500 kg) was: 96.4%. Data has been collected by the City of Stockholm Transport Department (in Swedish: Trafikkontoret, Stockholms stad). Further information about these traffic measurements can be found in the City of Stockholm report [46].
Figure A2. Vehicle flow (a) and average vehicle speed (b) per hour on minor arterials in inner urban areas of Stockholm. The measurement points show data for both driving directions during a typical weekday (Tuesday to Thursday) in April 2005. The ratio between light motor vehicles (≤3500 kg) and heavy (>3500 kg) was: 96.4%. Data has been collected by the City of Stockholm Transport Department (in Swedish: Trafikkontoret, Stockholms stad). Further information about these traffic measurements can be found in the City of Stockholm report [46].
Ijerph 20 03743 g0a2
Figure A3. Aerial view of Stockholm from 2005 with the ten different selected roads (local streets, collector roads and minor arterials): (A) Norr Mälarstrand, (B) Kungsholmsgatan, (C) Odengatan, (D) Dalagatan, (E) Sveavägen, (F) Upplandsgatan, (G) Birger Jarlsgatan, (H) Sibyllegatan, (I) Götgatan, and (J) Rosenlundsgatan. These roads have lanes in both directions. The arrows mark the approximate location where the traffic measurement took place. The area inside the yellow line demarcates the inner urban study area. North is at the top of the image. (Copyright: Lantmäteriverket, Gävle, Sweden, 2011; Permission 81055230).
Figure A3. Aerial view of Stockholm from 2005 with the ten different selected roads (local streets, collector roads and minor arterials): (A) Norr Mälarstrand, (B) Kungsholmsgatan, (C) Odengatan, (D) Dalagatan, (E) Sveavägen, (F) Upplandsgatan, (G) Birger Jarlsgatan, (H) Sibyllegatan, (I) Götgatan, and (J) Rosenlundsgatan. These roads have lanes in both directions. The arrows mark the approximate location where the traffic measurement took place. The area inside the yellow line demarcates the inner urban study area. North is at the top of the image. (Copyright: Lantmäteriverket, Gävle, Sweden, 2011; Permission 81055230).
Ijerph 20 03743 g0a3
Table A4. The VIF, the Cook´s distance and the standardized residuals.
Table A4. The VIF, the Cook´s distance and the standardized residuals.
TablePredictorsOutcomesVIFCookStd. Residual
> (±3 SD)
MeanMaximumMeanMaximumNMaximum
5Vehicle speedNoise1.0761.1500.0040.0502−3.498
5Vehicle flowNoise1.0761.1500.0040.070
5Vehicle speedExhaust fumes1.0761.1500.0040.0562−3.134
5Vehicle flowExhaust fumes1.0761.1500.0030.0812−4.274
5NoiseExhaust fumes1.0701.1510.0040.0613−3.977
5Vehicle speedVehicle flow1.0761.1500.0040.052
6Vehicle speed
Vehicle flow
Noise1.2691.6230.0040.06713.088
6Exhaust fumes1.2691.6230.0040.0862−4.300
7Vehicle speed
Vehicle flow
Hinders–stimulates walking1.2691.6230.0040.0622−3.149
7Noise
Exhaust fumes
Hinders–stimulates walking1.4702.2440.0040.0632−3.215
7Vehicle speed
Vehicle flow
Noise
Exhaust fumes
Hinders–stimulates walking1.8753.3000.0040.0502−3.211
8Vehicle speed
Vehicle flow
Unsafe–safe traffic1.2691.6230.0030.0591−3.073
8Noise
Exhaust fumes
Unsafe–safe traffic1.4702.2440.0030.0421−3.020
8Vehicle speed
Vehicle flow
Noise
Exhaust fumes
Unsafe–safe traffic1.8753.3000.0040.064
A5Vehicle speedHinders–stimulates walking1.0761.1500.0040.0631−3.395
A5Vehicle flowHinders–stimulates walking1.0761.1500.0040.0732−3.131
A5NoiseHinders–stimulates walking1.0701.1510.0040.0722−3.244
A5Exhaust fumesHinders–stimulates walking1.0771.1500.0040.0702−3.123
A6Vehicle speedUnsafe–safe traffic1.0761.1500.0030.0621−3.051
A6Vehicle flowUnsafe–safe traffic1.0761.1500.0030.0611−3.012
A6NoiseUnsafe–safe traffic1.0701.1510.0030.0371−3.023
A6Exhaust fumesUnsafe–safe traffic1.0771.1500.0030.046
Notes: N = the number of individuals exceeding ± 3 SD. The background variables sex, age, education, and income were included in all analyses.
Table A5. Relations between the predictor variables and hinders–stimulates walking as an outcome.
Table A5. Relations between the predictor variables and hinders–stimulates walking as an outcome.
Outcomey-Intercept (95% CI)p-ValuePredictorRegression Coefficient, Unstandardized B (95% CI)p-ValueAdj. R2
Hinders–stimulates walking10.5<0.000Vehicle speed−0.198<0.0000.048
(8.33–12.7)(−0.308–−0.089)
Hinders–stimulates walking11.1<0.000Vehicle flow−0.224<0.0000.082
(9.03–13.2)(−0.315–−0.134)
Hinders–stimulates walking11.5<0.000Noise−0.296<0.0000.114
(9.47–13.5)(−0.395–−0.198)
Hinders–stimulates walking11.1<0.000Exhaust fumes−0.226<0.0000.074
(8.95–13.2)(−0.322–−0.129)
Notes: The background variables sex, age, education, and income were included in the analyses. There were no significant background variables.
Table A6. Relations between the predictor variables and unsafe–safe traffic as an outcome.
Table A6. Relations between the predictor variables and unsafe–safe traffic as an outcome.
Outcomey-Intercept
(95% CI)
p-ValuePredictorRegression Coefficient, Unstandardized B (95% CI)p-ValueAdj. R2
Unsafe–safe traffic12.5<0.000Vehicle speed−0.243<0.0000.039
(9.97–14.9)(−0.369–−0.117)
Unsafe–safe traffic11.9<0.000Vehicle flow−0.1700.0020.024
(9.44–14.4)(−0.277–−0.063)
Unsafe–safe traffic12.4<0.000Noise−0.247<0.0000.049
(10.0–14.8)(−0.364–−0.130)
Unsafe–safe traffic12.0<0.000Exhaust fumes−0.1850.0010.026
(9.55–14.5)(−0.298–−0.072)
Notes: The background variables sex, age, education, and income were included in the analyses. There were no significant background variables.
Figure A4. Relations between the individual predictor variables and the outcome variables. Notes: r = Pearson’s correlation coefficient and β = unstandardized beta. The figure is based on Table 4, Table A5 and Table A6.
Figure A4. Relations between the individual predictor variables and the outcome variables. Notes: r = Pearson’s correlation coefficient and β = unstandardized beta. The figure is based on Table 4, Table A5 and Table A6.
Ijerph 20 03743 g0a4

References

  1. Hall, K.S.; Hyde, E.T.; Bassett, D.R.; Carlson, S.A.; Carnethon, M.R.; Ekelund, U.; Evenson, K.R.; Galuska, D.A.; Kraus, W.E.; Lee, I.M.; et al. Systematic review of the prospective association of daily step counts with risk of mortality, cardiovascular disease, and dysglycemia. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2020, 17, 78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Kelly, P.; Kahlmeier, S.; Gotschi, T.; Orsini, N.; Richards, J.; Roberts, N.; Scarborough, P.; Foster, C. Systematic review and meta-analysis of reduction in all-cause mortality from walking and cycling and shape of dose response relationship. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2014, 11, 132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Morris, J.N.; Hardman, A.E. Walking to health. Sports Med. 1997, 23, 306–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Hallal, P.C.; Andersen, L.B.; Bull, F.C.; Guthold, R.; Haskell, W.; Ekelund, U. Global physical activity levels: Surveillance progress, pitfalls, and prospects. Lancet 2012, 380, 247–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Saelens, B.E.; Sallis, J.F.; Black, J.B.; Chen, D. Neighborhood-Based Differences in Physical Activity: An Environment Scale Evaluation. Am. J. Public Health 2003, 93, 1552–1558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Oakes, J.M.; Forsyth, A.; Schmitz, K.H. The effects of neighborhood density and street connectivity on walking behavior: The Twin Cities walking study. Epidemiol. Perspect. Innov. 2007, 4, 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Brownson, R.C.; Chang, J.J.; Eyler, A.A.; Ainsworth, B.E.; Kirtland, K.A.; Saelens, B.E.; Sallis, J.F. Measuring the Environment for Friendliness Toward Physical Activity: A Comparison of the Reliability of 3 Questionnaires. Am. J. Public Health 2004, 94, 473–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Humpel, N.; Owen, N.; Iverson, D.; Leslie, E.; Bauman, A. Perceived Environment Attributes, Residential Location, and Walking for Particular Purposes. Am. J. Prev. Med. 2004, 26, 119–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Anciaes, P.R.; Stockton, J.; Ortegon, A.; Scholes, S. Perceptions of road traffic conditions along with their reported impacts on walking are associated with wellbeing. Travel Behav. Soc. 2019, 15, 88–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Foster, C.; Hillsdon, M.; Thorogood, M. Environmental perceptions and walking in English adults. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 2004, 58, 924–928. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Van Cauwenberg, J.; Van Holle, V.; Simons, D.; Deridder, R.; Clarys, P.; Goubert, L.; Nasar, J.; Salmon, J.; De Bourdeaudhuij, I.; Deforche, B. Environmental factors influencing older adults’ walking for transportation: A study using walk-along interviews. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2012, 9, 85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Lee, C.; Moudon, A.V. Correlates of Walking for Transportation or Recreation Purposes. J. Phys. Act. Health 2006, 3, S77–S98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Wahlgren, L.; Schantz, P. Bikeability and methodological issues using the active commuting route environment scale (ACRES) in a metropolitan setting. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 2011, 11, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Wahlgren, L.; Stigell, E.; Schantz, P. The active commuting route environment scale (ACRES): Development and evaluation. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2010, 7, 58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Schantz, P. Physical Activity Behaviours and Environmental Well-Being in a Spatial Context; Swedish National Defence College: Stockholm, Sweden; Norwegian University of Science and Technology (NTNU): Trondheim, Norway; Universität Bonn: Bonn, Germany, 2014; p. 150. ISBN 978-91-86137-30-4. [Google Scholar]
  16. Alfonzo, M.A. To Walk Or Not To Walk? The Hierarchy of Walking Needs. Environ. Behav. 2016, 37, 808–836. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Van Cauwenberg, J.; Clarys, P.; De Bourdeaudhuij, I.; Van Holle, V.; Verte, D.; De Witte, N.; De Donder, L.; Buffel, T.; Dury, S.; Deforche, B. Physical environmental factors related to walking and cycling in older adults: The Belgian aging studies. BMC Public Health 2012, 12, 142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Schantz, P. De Upplevda Landskapen för Cykling. Påverkan på Hälsan (The Perceived Landscapes for Cycling. Impact on Health); Trafikverket: Borlänge, Sweden, 2018; p. 25. ISBN 978-91-7725-375-4.
  19. Panter, J.; Griffin, S.; Ogilvie, D. Active commuting and perceptions of the route environment: A longitudinal analysis. Prev. Med. 2014, 67, 134–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Buecker, S.; Simacek, T.; Ingwersen, B.; Terwiel, S.; Simonsmeier, B.A. Physical activity and subjective well-being in healthy individuals: A meta-analytic review. Health Psychol. Rev. 2021, 15, 574–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Kumar, P.; Nigam, S.P.; Kumar, N. Vehicular traffic noise modeling using artificial neural network approach. Transp. Res. Part C Emerg. Technol. 2014, 40, 111–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Naturvårdsverket. Vägtrafikbuller: Nordisk Beräkningsmodell, Reviderad 1996 (Road Traffic Noise: Nordic Calculation Model, Revised 1996); Naturvårdsverket: Stockholm, Sweden, 1996; p. 5. ISBN 91-620-4653-5. [Google Scholar]
  23. Wahlgren, L.; Schantz, P. Exploring bikeability in a metropolitan setting: Stimulating and hindering factors in commuting route environments. BMC Public Health 2012, 12, 168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Stigell, E. Assessment of Active Commuting Behavior—Walking and Bicycling in Greater Stockholm (Dissertation); Örebro University: Örebro, Sweden, 2011; pp. 65–69. ISBN 978-91-7668-805-2. [Google Scholar]
  25. Schantz, P.; Salier Eriksson, J.; Rosdahl, H. The heart rate method for estimating oxygen uptake: Analyses of reproducibility using a range of heart rates from cycle commuting. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0219741. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Stigell, E.; Schantz, P. Active Commuting Behaviors in a Nordic Metropolitan Setting in Relation to Modality, Gender, and Health Recommendations. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 15626–15648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Tabachnik, B.; Fidell, L. Using Multivariate Statistics, 6th ed.; Pearson Education: Boston, MA, USA, 2013; pp. 88–90. ISBN 978-0-205-84957-4. [Google Scholar]
  28. Field, A. Discovering Statistics Using IBM SPSS Statistics, 5th ed.; SAGE Publications: London, UK, 2018; pp. p. 402, p. 383. ISBN 978-1-5264-1952-1. [Google Scholar]
  29. Hayes, A.F. The PROCESS macro for SPSS, SAS, and R. Available online: http://www.processmacro.org/index.html (accessed on 2 December 2022).
  30. Winters, M.; Teschke, K. Route preferences among adults in the near market for bicycling: Findings of the cycling in cities study. Am. J. Health Promot. 2010, 25, 40–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Marquart, H.; Stark, K.; Jarass, J. How are air pollution and noise perceived en route? Investigating cyclists’ and pedestrians’ personal exposure, wellbeing and practices during commute. J. Transp. Health 2022, 24, 101325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. McGinn, A.P.; Evenson, K.R.; Herring, A.H.; Huston, S.L.; Rodriguez, D.A. Exploring Associations between Physical Activity and Perceived and Objective Measures of the Built Environment. J. Urban Health 2007, 84, 162–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Annear, M.J.; Cushman, G.; Gidlow, B. Leisure time physical activity differences among older adults from diverse socioeconomic neighborhoods. Health Place 2009, 15, 482–490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Herrmann-Lunecke, M.G.; Mora, R.; Vejares, P. Perception of the built environment and walking in pericentral neighbourhoods in Santiago, Chile. Travel Behav. Soc. 2021, 23, 192–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Foraster, M.; Eze, I.C.; Vienneau, D.; Brink, M.; Cajochen, C.; Caviezel, S.; Heritier, H.; Schaffner, E.; Schindler, C.; Wanner, M.; et al. Long-term transportation noise annoyance is associated with subsequent lower levels of physical activity. Environ. Int. 2016, 91, 341–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Kardell, L.; Pehrson, K. Stockholmarnas Friluftsliv: Vanor och Önskemål: En Enkät- och Intervjustudie (Stockholmers Outdoors: Use of Nature Areas: A Mail Questionnaire and a Home Interview Study); Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences: Uppsala, Sweden, 1978; pp. 1–122. ISBN 91-576-0003-1. [Google Scholar]
  37. WHO. Walking and Cycling: Latest Evidence to Support Policy-Making and Practice; World Health Organization: Copenhagen, Denmark, 2022; p. 60. ISBN 978-92-890-5788-2. [Google Scholar]
  38. Iversen, L.; Skov, R.S. Noise from Electric Vehicles—Measurements; Danish Road Directorate: Copenhagen, Denmark, 2015; p. 35.
  39. Garay-Vega, L.; Hastings, A.; Pollard, J.K.; Zuschlag, M.; Stearns, M.D. Quieter Cars and the Safety of Blind Pedestrians: Phase I; National Highway Traffic Safety Administration: Washington, DC, USA, 2010; p. 3.
  40. Wahlgren, L.; Schantz, P. Exploring bikeability in a suburban metropolitan area using the Active Commuting Route Environment Scale (ACRES). Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2014, 11, 8276–8300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Schantz, P.; Ceci, R. (The Unit for Road Safety, Department of Planning, Swedish Transport Administration, Borlänge, Sweden). Personal communication. 2021. [Google Scholar]
  42. Dadpour, S.; Pakzad, J.; Khankeh, H. Understanding the Influence of Environment on Adults’ Walking Experiences: A Meta-Synthesis Study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2016, 13, 731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Sugiyama, T.; Neuhaus, M.; Cole, R.; Giles-Corti, B.; Owen, N. Destination and Route Attributes Associated with Adults’ Walking: A Review. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2012, 44, 1275–1286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. WHO. Global Status Report on Road Safety 2018: Summary; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2018; p. 8. [Google Scholar]
  45. Andersson, D.; Wahlgren, L.; Schantz, P. Pedestrians´ perceptions of route environments in relation to deterring or facilitating walking. Front. Public Health 2023, 10, 1012222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. City of Stockholm Transport Department. Analys av biltrafiken inför Stockholmsförsöket—April 2005 (Analysis of Car Traffic Before The Stockholm Trial—April 2005); City of Stockholm Transport Department: Stockholm, Sweden, 2005; pp. 1–53. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. A conceptual model illustrating how perceptions of variables in different route environmental domains may affect pedestrians (modified from [15]). Route environments consist of different environmental domains: the physical environment (stationary objects such as buildings and trees), the traffic environment (mobile objects such as pedestrians, and cars causing e.g., noise, exhaust fumes and particulate matter (PM)), the social environment (interaction between individuals), weather (wind, rain, sun etc.), and light conditions (natural and artificial light). These environmental domains represent a number of predictor variables, whereof some (e.g., noise and exhaust fumes), have the roles of also representing basic variables (e.g., flow and speeds of motor vehicles). They can, thereby, be interpreted as intermediate outcomes. Perceptions of predictor variables in the initial level of the model, can influence how we appraise unsafety–safety due to traffic, and unsafety–safety due to other reasons (such as crime), as well as how we appraise the environment with respect to if it is hindering–stimulating walking. These appraisals can, consecutively, affect our walking behaviour (such as route choice and the amount of walking), have physiological, psychological, and medical effects, and affect our environmentally induced unwellbeing–wellbeing. The bidirectional hatched lines between the boxes indicate potential mutual relationships.
Figure 1. A conceptual model illustrating how perceptions of variables in different route environmental domains may affect pedestrians (modified from [15]). Route environments consist of different environmental domains: the physical environment (stationary objects such as buildings and trees), the traffic environment (mobile objects such as pedestrians, and cars causing e.g., noise, exhaust fumes and particulate matter (PM)), the social environment (interaction between individuals), weather (wind, rain, sun etc.), and light conditions (natural and artificial light). These environmental domains represent a number of predictor variables, whereof some (e.g., noise and exhaust fumes), have the roles of also representing basic variables (e.g., flow and speeds of motor vehicles). They can, thereby, be interpreted as intermediate outcomes. Perceptions of predictor variables in the initial level of the model, can influence how we appraise unsafety–safety due to traffic, and unsafety–safety due to other reasons (such as crime), as well as how we appraise the environment with respect to if it is hindering–stimulating walking. These appraisals can, consecutively, affect our walking behaviour (such as route choice and the amount of walking), have physiological, psychological, and medical effects, and affect our environmentally induced unwellbeing–wellbeing. The bidirectional hatched lines between the boxes indicate potential mutual relationships.
Ijerph 20 03743 g001
Figure 2. Among the motorized traffic variables, the basic variables generate the intermediate outcomes. All of the traffic variables can, in principle, independently of each other, and in different combinations, relate to the two outcome variables. Flow of motor vehicles is equal to the number of vehicles passing a reference point during an established time period. The bidirectional arrow between flow of motor vehicles and speeds of motor vehicles illustrates that there is a relation between these variables. Under uninterrupted flow conditions, flow, density of vehicles (vehicles × km−1) and speed are all related by the following equation: q = k × v where q is flow, k is density, and v is speed.
Figure 2. Among the motorized traffic variables, the basic variables generate the intermediate outcomes. All of the traffic variables can, in principle, independently of each other, and in different combinations, relate to the two outcome variables. Flow of motor vehicles is equal to the number of vehicles passing a reference point during an established time period. The bidirectional arrow between flow of motor vehicles and speeds of motor vehicles illustrates that there is a relation between these variables. Under uninterrupted flow conditions, flow, density of vehicles (vehicles × km−1) and speed are all related by the following equation: q = k × v where q is flow, k is density, and v is speed.
Ijerph 20 03743 g002
Figure 3. Map over Sweden and Stockholm County, with the inner urban study area of the Municipality of Stockholm in green. The marking for the densely built-up areas illuminates the conditions in 2010. North is at the top of the image.
Figure 3. Map over Sweden and Stockholm County, with the inner urban study area of the Municipality of Stockholm in green. The marking for the densely built-up areas illuminates the conditions in 2010. North is at the top of the image.
Ijerph 20 03743 g003
Figure 4. Aerial view of Stockholm in 2005. The area inside the yellow line demarcates the inner urban study area according to our geographical division of the city. North is at the top of the image. (Copyright: Lantmäteriverket, Gävle, Sweden, 2011; Permission 81055230).
Figure 4. Aerial view of Stockholm in 2005. The area inside the yellow line demarcates the inner urban study area according to our geographical division of the city. North is at the top of the image. (Copyright: Lantmäteriverket, Gävle, Sweden, 2011; Permission 81055230).
Ijerph 20 03743 g004
Figure 5. Correlation and regression coefficients between the four predictor variables. Notes: r = Pearson’s correlation coefficient and β = unstandardized beta. The figure is based on Table 4 and Table 5.
Figure 5. Correlation and regression coefficients between the four predictor variables. Notes: r = Pearson’s correlation coefficient and β = unstandardized beta. The figure is based on Table 4 and Table 5.
Ijerph 20 03743 g005
Figure 6. Regression coefficients between vehicle speed and vehicle flow in relation to noise and exhaust fumes. Notes: β = unstandardized beta. The figure is based on Table 6.
Figure 6. Regression coefficients between vehicle speed and vehicle flow in relation to noise and exhaust fumes. Notes: β = unstandardized beta. The figure is based on Table 6.
Ijerph 20 03743 g006
Figure 7. Regression coefficients between combinations of the predictor variables in relation to hinders–stimulates walking. Note: β = unstandardized beta. The figure is based on Table 7.
Figure 7. Regression coefficients between combinations of the predictor variables in relation to hinders–stimulates walking. Note: β = unstandardized beta. The figure is based on Table 7.
Ijerph 20 03743 g007
Figure 8. Relations based on multiple regression analysis between the predictor variables (MRA 1), and between the predictor variables and the outcome variables (MRA 2). The solid arrows are based on Table 6. They illustrate that when both vehicle speed and vehicle flow are analysed jointly as predictors, they relate positively with noise whereas only vehicle flow relates to exhaust fumes. The dashed arrows are based on Table 7 and Table 8. They illustrate that when all traffic variables are analysed jointly as predictors, only noise relates negatively to both outcome variables. Vehicle speed has the same role in relation to unsafe–safe traffic. β = unstandardized beta.
Figure 8. Relations based on multiple regression analysis between the predictor variables (MRA 1), and between the predictor variables and the outcome variables (MRA 2). The solid arrows are based on Table 6. They illustrate that when both vehicle speed and vehicle flow are analysed jointly as predictors, they relate positively with noise whereas only vehicle flow relates to exhaust fumes. The dashed arrows are based on Table 7 and Table 8. They illustrate that when all traffic variables are analysed jointly as predictors, only noise relates negatively to both outcome variables. Vehicle speed has the same role in relation to unsafe–safe traffic. β = unstandardized beta.
Ijerph 20 03743 g008
Figure 9. Regression coefficients between combinations of the predictor variables in relation to unsafe–safe traffic. Notes: β = unstandardized beta. The figure is based on Table 8.
Figure 9. Regression coefficients between combinations of the predictor variables in relation to unsafe–safe traffic. Notes: β = unstandardized beta. The figure is based on Table 8.
Ijerph 20 03743 g009
Figure 10. The solid arrows between the predictor variables are based on the multiple regression analyses in Table 6 (MRA 1). They illustrate that when both vehicle speed and vehicle flow were analysed jointly as predictors, they related positively with noise whereas only vehicle flow related to exhaust fumes. The dotted arrows are based on the mediation analyses in Table 9. They illustrate that vehicle speed had an indirect effect on both noise and exhaust fumes, and that both vehicle speed and vehicle flow have indirect effects on both outcome variables via noise. The dashed arrows are based on Table 7 and Table 8 (MRA 2). They illustrate that when all traffic variables are analysed jointly as predictors, only noise relates negatively to both outcome variables. Vehicle speed had the same role in relation to unsafe–safe traffic. Given these different roles for vehicle speed, it protrudes as an important origin to the deterring effects of traffic on walking.
Figure 10. The solid arrows between the predictor variables are based on the multiple regression analyses in Table 6 (MRA 1). They illustrate that when both vehicle speed and vehicle flow were analysed jointly as predictors, they related positively with noise whereas only vehicle flow related to exhaust fumes. The dotted arrows are based on the mediation analyses in Table 9. They illustrate that vehicle speed had an indirect effect on both noise and exhaust fumes, and that both vehicle speed and vehicle flow have indirect effects on both outcome variables via noise. The dashed arrows are based on Table 7 and Table 8 (MRA 2). They illustrate that when all traffic variables are analysed jointly as predictors, only noise relates negatively to both outcome variables. Vehicle speed had the same role in relation to unsafe–safe traffic. Given these different roles for vehicle speed, it protrudes as an important origin to the deterring effects of traffic on walking.
Ijerph 20 03743 g010
Figure 11. (a,b) Two photos from the inner urban study area of Stockholm in March 2022. The predominant street network is laid out in a grid pattern with a mixture of different streets. Cars are often parked along streets protecting the pedestrians (physically) by providing a barrier that sometimes will hide the vehicle flow and distance the pedestrians from the vehicles. However, noise will reach those on the pavement regardless of the barrier provided by the vehicles. When data were collected in May 2005, the trees had leaves. Photo: Dan Andersson.
Figure 11. (a,b) Two photos from the inner urban study area of Stockholm in March 2022. The predominant street network is laid out in a grid pattern with a mixture of different streets. Cars are often parked along streets protecting the pedestrians (physically) by providing a barrier that sometimes will hide the vehicle flow and distance the pedestrians from the vehicles. However, noise will reach those on the pavement regardless of the barrier provided by the vehicles. When data were collected in May 2005, the trees had leaves. Photo: Dan Andersson.
Ijerph 20 03743 g011
Table 1. Descriptive characteristics of participants (n = 282–294) *.
Table 1. Descriptive characteristics of participants (n = 282–294) *.
Background Factors
Females **, %77
Age in years **, mean ± SD49.5 ± 10.4
Weight in kg, mean ± SD68.4 ± 10.6
Height in cm, mean ± SD171.1 ± 8.2
Body mass index, mean ± SD23.3 ± 2.7
Gainful employment, %97
Educated at university level **, %79
Income **:
≤25,000 SEK *** a month, %37
25,001–30,000 SEK *** a month, %27
≥30,001 SEK *** a month, %35
Participant and both parents born in Sweden, %80
Having a driver’s licence, %89
Usually access to a car, %56
Leaving home 7–9 a.m. to walk to place of work or study, %80
Leaving place of work or study 4–6 p.m. to walk home, %73
Number of walking commuting trips per year ****, mean ± SD278 ± 162
Overall physical health either good or very good, %77
Overall mental health either good or very good, %85
Notes: * 294 individuals have complete data regarding ACRES and the four background variables that are used in the analyses; ** Included in the multiple regression analyses and in the mediation analyses; *** SEK = Swedish crowns (SEK) 2005: €1 ≈ 9 SEK; US$1 ≈ 8 SEK; **** The number of walking commuting trips is based on 224 participants, whereof four had remarkably high values, which therefore were substituted with mean values for the remainder of the participants. The low response rate is due to missing values in at least one of the 12 months leading to exclusion in the sum score.
Table 2. The applied items from the ACRES assessing pedestrian’s perceptions of the environment.
Table 2. The applied items from the ACRES assessing pedestrian’s perceptions of the environment.
Question15-Point Response ScaleVariable Name
115
Environmental VariablesOutcome VariablesDo you think that, on the whole, the environment you walk in hinders/stimulates your commuting?Hinders a lotStimulates a lotHinders–stimulates walking
How unsafe/safe do you feel in traffic as a pedestrian along your route?Very unsafeVery safeUnsafe–safe traffic
Predictor
Variables
How do you find the speeds of motor vehicles (taxis, lorries, ordinary cars, buses) along your route?Very lowVery highSpeeds of motor vehicles
How do you find the flow of motor vehicles (number of cars) along your route?Very lowVery highFlow of motor vehicles
How do you find the noise levels along your route?Very lowVery highNoise
How do you find the exhaust fume levels along your route?Very lowVery highExhaust fumes
Notes: This is a translation of the original ACRES in Swedish. Regarding the two variables speeds of motor vehicles and flow of motor vehicles; from now and onwards we will use the truncated terms: vehicle speed and vehicle flow.
Table 3. Perceptions of the environmental variables in males and females (mean, SD, and (95% CI)). For explanation of ratings of the scale, see Table 2.
Table 3. Perceptions of the environmental variables in males and females (mean, SD, and (95% CI)). For explanation of ratings of the scale, see Table 2.
Outcome VariablesPredictor Variables
Hinders–Stimulates WalkingUnsafe–Safe
Traffic
Vehicle
Speed
Vehicle
Flow
NoiseExhaust
Fumes
Men
(n = 69)
10.411.08.909.809.519.30
3.043.182.743.303.343.45
(9.70–11.2)(10.2–11.7)(8.24–9.56)(9.01–10.6)(8.71–10.3)(8.47–10.1)
Women
(n = 225)
10.410.89.77 *10.39.989.88
2.953.473.153.763.263.45
(10.1–10.8)(10.4–11.3)(9.36–10.2)(9.83–10.8)(9.55–10.4)(9.43–10.3)
Note: * Significant difference (p < 0.05).
Table 4. Correlation matrix for the environmental variables (r).
Table 4. Correlation matrix for the environmental variables (r).
Hinders–Stimulates WalkingUnsafe–Safe
Traffic
Vehicle
Speed
Vehicle
Flow
NoiseExhaust
Fumes
Hinders–
stimulates walking
Unsafe–safe traffic0.313 *
Vehicle speed−0.210 *−0.222 *
Vehicle flow−0.284 *−0.183 *0.612 *
Noise−0.328 *−0.238 *0.549 *0.774 *
Exhaust fumes−0.274 *−0.189 *0.462 *0.728 *0.737 *
Note: * = Pearson´s correlation coefficient (r) is significant at the 0.01 level.
Table 5. Relations between the predictor variables.
Table 5. Relations between the predictor variables.
Outcomey-Intercept
(95% CI)
p-ValuePredictorRegression Coefficient, Unstandardized B (95% CI)p-ValueAdj. R2
Noise4.32<0.000Vehicle
speed
0.584<0.0000.292
(2.26–6.38)(0.480–0.688)
Noise2.000.011Vehicle
flow
0.698<0.0000.595
(0.46–3.53)(0.631–0.764)
Exhaust fumes6.69<0.000Vehicle
speed
0.508<0.0000.213
(4.40–8.97)(0.392–0.623)
Exhaust fumes3.72<0.000Vehicle
flow
0.681<0.0000.526
(1.97–5.47)(0.606–0.757)
Exhaust fumes4.00<0.000Noise0.770<0.0000.547
(2.32–5.69)(0.688–0.852)
Vehicle
flow
4.61<0.000Vehicle
speed
0.721<0.0000.373
(2.46–6.77)(0.612–0.830)
Notes: The background variables sex, age, education, and income were included in the analysis. Only significant (p < 0.05) background variables and their level of significance are reported. The background variable age was significant in two models; exhaust fumes (as an outcome) and vehicle speed (as a predictor): unstandardized B = −0.035 (p = 0.047) and exhaust fumes (as an outcome) and noise (as a predictor): unstandardized B = −0.031 (p = 0.019).
Table 6. Relations between the basic variables vehicle speed and vehicle flow and the intermediate outcomes noise and exhaust fumes.
Table 6. Relations between the basic variables vehicle speed and vehicle flow and the intermediate outcomes noise and exhaust fumes.
Intermediate Outcomey-Intercept (95% CI)p-ValuePredictorRegression Coefficient, Unstandardized B (95% CI)p-ValueAdj. R2
Noise1.40
(−0.19–2.99)
0.083Vehicle speed0.1280.0110.602
(0.030–0.226)
Vehicle flow0.632<0.000
(0.549–0.715)
Exhaust fumes3.60
(1.77–5.43)
<0.000Vehicle speed0.0260.6570.525
(−0.088–0.139)
Vehicle flow0.668<0.000
(0.573–0.764)
Notes: The background variables sex, age, education, and income were included in the analysis. There were no significant background variables.
Table 7. Relations between combinations of predictor variables and the outcome hinders–stimulates walking.
Table 7. Relations between combinations of predictor variables and the outcome hinders–stimulates walking.
Outcomey-Intercept
(95% CI)
p-ValuePredictorRegression Coefficient, Unstandardized B (95% CI)p-ValueAdj. R2
Hinders–stimulates walking11.4
(9.20–13.6)
<0.000Vehicle speed−0.0580.3980.081
(−0.194–0.077)
Vehicle flow−0.1940.001
(−0.308–−0.080)
Hinders–stimulates walking11.6
(9.54–13.7)
<0.000Noise−0.268<0.0000.111
(−0.413–−0.122)
Exhaust fumes−0.0370.600
(−0.177–0.102)
Hinders–stimulates walking11.8
(9.61–14.0)
<0.000Vehicle speed−0.0260.7000.106
(−0.162–0.109)
Vehicle flow−0.0240.771
(−0.184–0.137)
Noise−0.2420.006
(−0.415–−0.070)
Exhaust fumes−0.0260.732
(−0.176–0.124)
Notes: The background variables sex, age, education, and income were included in the analysis. There were no significant background variables.
Table 8. Relations between combinations of predictor variables and the outcome unsafe–safe traffic.
Table 8. Relations between combinations of predictor variables and the outcome unsafe–safe traffic.
Outcomey-Intercept (95% CI)p-ValuePredictorRegression Coefficient, Unstandardized B (95% CI)p-ValueAdj. R2
Unsafe–safe traffic12.8
(10.2–15.4)
<0.000Vehicle speed−0.1900.0190.040
(−0.349–−0.032)
Vehicle flow−0.0730.285
(−0.206–0.061)
Unsafe–safe traffic12.5
(10.0–15.0)
<0.000Noise−0.2290.0100.045
(−0.402–−0.056)
Exhaust fumes−0.0240.779
(−0.189–0.142)
Unsafe–safe traffic13.2
(10.6–15.8)
<0.000Vehicle speed−0.1630.0450.052
(−0.322–−0.004)
Vehicle flow0.0830.388
(−0.106–0.272)
Noise−0.2080.046
(−0.411–−0.004)
Exhaust fumes−0.0370.684
(−0.213–0.140)
Notes: The background variables sex, age, education, and income were included in the analysis. There were no significant background variables.
Table 9. Mediation analyses between the four predictor variables of motor traffic (MA1–MA4), between the same predictor variables and the two outcome variables (MA5–MA8), as well as between the composite variable (vehicle flow × vehicle speed) and the outcome variables (MA9–MA10).
Table 9. Mediation analyses between the four predictor variables of motor traffic (MA1–MA4), between the same predictor variables and the two outcome variables (MA5–MA8), as well as between the composite variable (vehicle flow × vehicle speed) and the outcome variables (MA9–MA10).
ModelPredictor (X)Mediator (M)Outcome (Y)Standardized Total Effect of X on YStandardized Direct Effect of X on YStandardized Indirect Effect of X on Y
bp-Valuebp-Valueb95% CI% of Total Effect
MA1Vehicle flowVehicle speedNoise0.779<0.0000.706<0.0000.0730.010–0.1409
MA2Vehicle speedVehicle flowNoise0.549<0.0000.1200.0110.4280.343–0.51378
MA3Vehicle flowVehicle speedExhaust fumes0.721<0.0000.707<0.0000.014−0.049–0.0772
MA4Vehicle speedVehicle flowExhaust fumes0.452<0.0000.0230.6570.4290.343–0.51595
MA5Vehicle flowNoiseHinders–stimulates walking−0.276<0.000−0.0540.543−0.223−0.366–−0.09081
MA6Vehicle speedNoiseHinders–stimulates walking−0.206<0.000−0.0370.574−0.168−0.248–−0.10082
MA7Vehicle flowNoiseUnsafe–safe traffic−0.1830.0020.0070.939−0.190−0.324–−0.042104
MA8Vehicle speedNoiseUnsafe–safe traffic−0.220<0.000−0.1270.066−0.093−0.172–−0.01942
MA9Composite variableNoiseHinders–stimulates walking−0.266<0.000−0.0650.411−0.201−0.319–−0.09976
MA10Composite variableNoiseUnsafe–safe traffic−0.229<0.000−0.1180.148−0.111−0.229–0.00848
Notes: Covariables included in the analyses were sex, age, education, and income. Age was significant in model MA4 with respect to the total effect of X on Y.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Andersson, D.; Wahlgren, L.; Olsson, K.S.E.; Schantz, P. Pedestrians’ Perceptions of Motorized Traffic Variables in Relation to Appraisals of Urban Route Environments. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, 3743. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20043743

AMA Style

Andersson D, Wahlgren L, Olsson KSE, Schantz P. Pedestrians’ Perceptions of Motorized Traffic Variables in Relation to Appraisals of Urban Route Environments. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2023; 20(4):3743. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20043743

Chicago/Turabian Style

Andersson, Dan, Lina Wahlgren, Karin Sofia Elisabeth Olsson, and Peter Schantz. 2023. "Pedestrians’ Perceptions of Motorized Traffic Variables in Relation to Appraisals of Urban Route Environments" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 20, no. 4: 3743. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20043743

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop