Next Article in Journal
Parental Stress in Raising a Child with Developmental Disabilities in a Rural Community in South Africa
Previous Article in Journal
Attention and Sentiment of the Chinese Public toward a 3D Greening System Based on Sina Weibo
Previous Article in Special Issue
Bullying and Emotional Problems in Pupils from 11 to 13 Years Old: Joint Detection through Self-Report
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Development of Emotion Understanding among Five- and Six-Year-Old Left-Behind Children in Rural China

School of Education, Guangzhou University, Guangzhou 510006, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20(5), 3974; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20053974
Submission received: 5 January 2023 / Revised: 11 February 2023 / Accepted: 15 February 2023 / Published: 23 February 2023

Abstract

:
The left-behind children (LBC), separated from their mother/father or parents for a long period of time, have long been discussed as a subject of concern in China. Existing research has concluded that rural children who did not migrate with parents are subject to emotional risks. In the present study, the purpose is to study the impact of parental migration on early emotional understanding. Purposeful sampling was used to recruit 180 children aged five to six years in rural areas of Guangdong province, including LBC and non-left-behind children (NLBC). Their level of emotional understanding (EU) was assessed by the emotional comprehension test (TEC) adapted to the Chinese context. The results showed that, on the three levels (External, Internal, Reflective) of emotional understanding, LBC aged five- to six- years old scored significantly lower than NLBC as counterparts. On the whole, the emotional comprehension ability of preschool LBC was significantly lower than that of NLBC. However, there were no significant differences within LBC nurtured by single parents, grandparents, and other relatives. This study confirmed that parental migration in early childhood considerably impacted rural LBC’s emotional understanding and affectional adjustment, which provided a significant basis for increasing parental care and early childhood companionship in rural areas.

1. Introduction

In China, social development is mainly reflected in urbanization, which has brought about labor migration (rural–urban and urban–urban) to meet the needs of urban industrial development and alleviate the pressure of living in a rural family setting [1]. However, children who are left behind at home are perceived to have special needs resulting from China’s big labor movement, and their special situation has attracted the attention of the whole society. One of the main concerns deals with the psychological adaptation of left-behind children (LBC), defined as children who have one or both migrant parents working in cities leaving them with other caregivers at home [2]. Someone argued that parental migration has adverse effects on young children’s mental well-being [3]. Other research indicated that the situation of LBC was so difficult to handle that the impact on children’s well-being and socialization was prone to being ignored or mixed [4]. Emotional interpretation is critical for the social development of young children. The integrity and stability of the children’s family structures may play a large role in this development [5]. In the context of a rural revitalization strategy [6], a considerable amount of research about the status of LBC as the product of social migration has drawn wide attention over the past couple of years. Exploring the level of emotional understanding of Chinese LBC in early childhood would further promote the understanding of this special population and their families.

1.1. Emotion Understanding in Early Childhood

Understanding of emotional states refers to the ability to identify, express, and communicate regarding comprehension of one’s own and others’ psychological activities, behaviors, and situations [7,8]. According to existing research, emotional understanding (EU) generally includes a set of skills that enable the understanding of some emotional elements or events (regulation of an experienced emotion, distinction between different emotions experienced, and external expression of what they feel) [9]. This competence mainly involves the understanding of facial expressions, situation-based emotions, desire-based emotions, belief-based emotions, reminder-based emotions, emotional display rules or hiding emotions, mixed emotions, moral emotions, emotional attribution or external cause, and emotional regulation [10,11]. Furthermore, the previous reviews and studies presented three hierarchical levels of emotional understanding, including “External” (e.g., recognition, situation, and reminder); “Internal” (e.g., belief, desire, and display); and “Reflective” (e.g., mixed, regulation, cause, and morality) [12,13,14,15]. In accordance with differential emotions theory and the social processing theory of emotion, early emotional development would change through neural maturation and socialization experiences [16,17]. Moreover, much evidence has examined that children’s understanding of emotions was highly correlated with social competence, as well as being especially linked to children’s prosocial behaviors [18]. This means early emotional understanding may also be discrete or divergent.
Children’s early emotional understanding also could be divided into different stages [19,20]. Almost all the three-year-old children were able to recognize a minimum of four out of five emotions based on facial expression alone [11]. Around the age of five, children appreciated the fact that people have different desires and beliefs, and they even correctly identify external causes and understand the impact of reminders about those emotions [11,21]. Some research has indicated that children aged five began to experience and understand mixed emotions [22]. Therefore, the levels of early competence to understand emotions would best be examined at around the age of five to six years old.

1.2. Parental Presence and Child’s Emotional Development

The social-ecological model proposes that children’s development is inextricably linked to the multilevel environment, especially parental care [23]. Parental presence is of great significance to the emotional and mental health of children. According to parent-child attachment theory, parental company was significantly related to children’s emotional functioning, especially their emotional understanding [24,25].
According to the parental acceptance–rejection theory, paternal neglect correlates closely with children’s psychological maladjustment [26]. The absence of parents hindered children’s feelings of parent-child cohesion and psychological needs satisfaction [27]. The impact of parental separation on children’s emotional indifference was not only short-term, but lasted well into adulthood [28]. The longer the early childhood separation, the more negative the effect of parental absence on psychological well-being, like emotional desertification [29,30]. Moreover, parental socialization of emotion was related to children’s emotional comprehension [31]. Therefore, the absence of parents may have an important impact on EU in early childhood.

1.3. Left-Behind Children in China

The living standard has increased rapidly in China since the 1980s, followed by large-scale population movements. In the countryside, children and adolescents were left behind at home by their rural-to-urban migrant parents in search of better jobs as a result of the household registration system [32]. In 2018, the LBC population reached 6.97 million [33]. Regarding the duration of the separation, the length of parental absence for LBC was generally more than six months [34]. From the perspective of family system theory [35], the changes of parental migration and family life, due to a trade-off between an increase in family income and a decrease in parental care causes, has seemingly contradictory impacts on LBC’s social and emotional adjustment [36].
Close parent–child emotional bonding can occur only through physical closeness between parents and their children, such as physical contact, caresses, kisses, and clear demonstrations of affection [37]. LBC obviously lacked such opportunities for intimate contact and were easily exposed to all kinds of psycho-social well-being risks [38,39], especially those involving core emotional level [40]. Children’s emotion comprehension was closely correlated to parents and their interactions [41,42]. Particularly, LBC would have more difficulty frequently communicating with their parents. Therefore, they would suffer more emotional crises than NLBC [43,44]. A meta-analytic review showed that most studies focused on the emotional functioning of LBC over the age of seven [45]. In fact, the first five years was the critical period for emotional and social development [1]. Therefore, the effect of early parental separation on a child’s emotional adaptation should receive more attention [46,47,48]. Furthermore, the impact of LBC’s actual caregivers on their EU was not yet clear.

1.4. The Current Study

Therefore, the present study sought to analyze the effect of the experience of being left behind and caregiver types on EU in early childhood. We explored whether five- and six-year-old LBC and non-left-behind children (NLBC) differed with regard to the three levels (External, Internal, Reflective) of understanding emotions. This study explored whether there were EU differences for young LBC living with different family members or actual caregivers. In terms of caregivers or guardians, referring to previous research [49,50], three types of LBC’s caregivers were considered: single parent, grandparents, or other relatives. Since living alone or only with siblings is rarely present at the preschool ages, it was not considered. Therefore, it could be predicted that LBC aged five to six years would score lower on the three levels of EU than NLBC.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

The participants were 200 young children invited randomly from kindergartens in rural Guangdong province. Excluding 20 participants with invalid and wrong data, the final sample size was 180, among which 90 children whose parents had left them behind at least six months prior and 90 young children who still had both parents living at home. Among the LBC, there were equal numbers of boys and girls, 45 in each group. Moreover, there were 30 LBC in each of the three caregiver types. The inclusion criteria for participants in the study were preschool children aged five and six years, having receptive language and picture processing skills, as well as mental-state and conversational stability [51,52,53]. The study was reviewed approved by the research ethics committee of Guangzhou University (Protocol Number: GZHU 2019019).

2.2. Design

To compare the differences in EU between LBC and NLBC, the study used two mixed design of 2 (Gender: Boy or girl) × 2 (Children’s status: Left-behind or non-left-behind). Gender and children’s status were independent variable, and the scores of emotional understanding were dependent variables. The age differences between the two groups were compared and the T-test results showed that the difference was not significant (t(178) = 0.28, p = 0.56, Cohen’s d = 0.76).

2.3. Materials and Procedures

In order to exclude the influence of the participants by the age or gender of the protagonists of the picture, this study specially found two children of similar age to the research samples. A seven-point scale questionnaire was produced, and twenty-five kindergarten teachers rated the facial expressions pictures and their naming (1 is very inconsistent, 7 is very consistent), and then picked the facial expression photos with the high score. Therefore, ten pictures of facial expressions (e.g., happy, sad, angry, fearful and neutral) of children aged 5 to 6 were successfully selected as basic materials (to create an equal gender endowment). The expressions of real children and similar ages made them easier for participants to understand and identify. Based on the results of the initial examination of the general child, these photos can convey the corresponding emotions.
The emotion comprehension test (TEC) has been widely translated into 25 languages to assess children’s levels of understanding emotions before this study. It has shown good test–retest reliability, as well as concurrent, criterion, and construct validity [10,20]. The standardized Chinese version of the TEC was successfully adapted and administered in Chinese preschool children [12,13]. The test materials consisted of the sample scenarios. Beneath each scenario, there were some facial expressions represented as emotional story outcomes. There were ten main components of TEC as follows.
In the External understanding level, there were three elements: recognition, situation, and reminders. Recognition mainly tested children’s understanding of facial expressions, such as happy, including naming and recalling (e.g., What do you call these facial expressions?); Situation primarily tested how children understand emotions in social context (e.g., How did he/she feel when he/she got the gift from Mom?); Reminders mainly examined how children interpret the emotions of protagonists based on the clues provided (e.g., He/she was happy before, how did he/she feel when seeing dog’s death?). The total scores of External level ranged 0 to 24.
In the Internal understanding level, three elements were: desires, belief, and display. Desires were primarily about understanding emotions when the protagonist’s wishes were fulfilled or unfulfilled (e.g., How did he and she feel after being given an apple?); Belief examined participants put themselves in other’s position to understand emotions (e.g., How did he/she feel before opening the toy box?); Display tested children’s understanding of emotional exposure within the constraints of social interaction rules (e.g., How did he/she feel as the winner while his/her friend was the loser?). The total scores of Internal level ranged 0 to 16.
In the Reflective understanding level, four components were: mixed, regulation, morality, and causes. Mixed was about understanding the complex and diverse emotions of the same protagonist (e.g., How did he/she feel when falling off the bike and being laughed at by others?); Regulation mainly tested how participants understand the ability of protagonists to control and regulate their emotional state (e.g., Can he/she would feel good though his/her cat was dead?); Morality was supposed to analyze how the sample understands emotions in a moral context (e.g., How did he/she feel after stealing candy of someone else?), Causes would examine how children interpret the reasons behind emotions (e.g., Why was he/she so angry?) [54]. The total scores of Reflective level ranged 0 to 37. The total scores of TEC was 77. The higher the score, the stronger the comprehension.
In order to avoid the sequential effect, a Latin-square design was used to establish the sequence of all facial expressions and combined emotional scenario stories. Each session lasted approximately 5–10 min.

2.4. Data Analysis

The data were collected from ten tasks designed to measure children’s understanding of emotions. All the data were analyzed using SPSS 25.0. According to the systematic procedures of the existing research [12,13,14,15,55], these ten elements were divided into three levels (External, Internal, Reflective) for analysis. The MANOVA was conducted to examine whether there would be differences in the three levels of understanding emotions, including differences between gender and children’s status. Besides, the comparison of LBC cared for by different caregivers was carried out to further analyze the impact of changes in actual caregivers and split family.

3. Results

The MANOVA on three levels of EU was performed. The result of External emotions understanding level showed that gender had no main effect (F(3, 174) = 1.97, p = 0.12, η2 = 0.03) and no significant interaction with children’s status (F(3, 174) = 2.30, p = 0.09, η2 = 0.04). The main effect of children’s status was significant (F(3, 174) = 11.34, p = 0.00 < 0.001, η2 = 0.16). Further analysis revealed that recognition (F(1, 179) = 13.50, p = 0.00 < 0.001, η2 = 0.07), situation (F(1, 179) = 20.79, p = 0.00 < 0.001, η2 = 0.11), and reminders (F(1, 179) = 7.80, p = 0.006 < 0.01, η2 = 0.04) also showed a significant difference. NLBC scored significantly higher than the LBC.
The results of Internal emotion understanding level indicated that there was also no main effect on gender (F(3, 174) = 3.75, p = 0.12, η2 = 0.06). Moreover, the interaction between gender and children’s status was not significant as well (F(3, 174) = 1.01, p = 0.38, η2 = 0.02). On the contrary, children’s status showed the main effect (F(3, 174) = 7.21, p = 0.00 < 0.001, η2 = 0.11). The main effect of children’s status was reflected in the desires (F(1, 179) = 18.90, p = 0.00 < 0.001, η2 = 0.10) and display (F(1, 179) = 9.35, p = 0.003 < 0.01, η2 = 0.05), instead of belief (F(1, 179) = 1.63, p = 0.20, η2 = 0.01). NLBC scored significantly higher than the LBC on the components as desires and display.
Furthermore, the results of Reflective showed that the main effect of gender (F(4, 173) = 3.02, p = 0.02 < 0.05, η2 = 0.07) and children’s status (F(4, 173) = 4.19, p = 0.003 < 0.01, η2 = 0.09) were both significant. However, the interaction effect between gender and children’s status was not significant (F(4, 173) = 0.18, p = 0.95, η2 = 0.004). In terms of four components of Reflective, gender has a main effect on morality (F(1, 179) = 4.47, p = 0.04 < 0.05, η2 = 0.03). Girls scored significantly higher for understanding moral emotions than boys. Other elements including mixed (F(1, 179) = 0.47, p = 0.50, η2 = 0.003), regulation (F(1, 179) = 2.52, p = 0.11, η2 = 0.01) and causes (F(1, 179) = 3.17, p = 0.08, η2 = 0.02) did not present main effect. The main effect of children’s status was shown in the components as mixed (F(1, 179) = 4.60, p = 0.03 < 0.05, η2 = 0.03) and causes (F(1, 179) = 16.32, p = 0.00 < 0.001, η2 = 0.09). LBC scored significantly higher than NLBC on the understanding of mixed and causes (see Table 1). There was no main effect of the children’s status in the comprehension of regulation (F(1, 179) = 0.63, p = 0.43, η2 = 0.004) and morality (F(1, 179) = 0.15, p = 0.70, η2 = 0.001) (see Table 1).
Since the main differences were examined between LBC and NLBC from the three levels of EU, the t-test of the total scores of understanding emotions was conducted to further explore whether there are differences in the ability of LBC and NLBC to understand emotions as a whole. The result indicated that the total score of NLBC was significantly higher than that of LBC (t(178) = 5.34, p = 0.00 < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.80). This implied that NLBC actually had much higher levels of understanding emotions than LBC at five to six years of age.
One-way ANOVA of total scores of LBC’s emotion understanding between different caregiver types, e.g., one parent, grandparents, or other relatives, was performed. The results indicated that the differences between the three groups did not reach statistical significance (F(2, 89) = 0.82, p = 0.44, η2 = 0.01).

4. Discussion

In rural China, long-term parental migration in search of better jobs in the cities was a fact of life. LBC should deserve more research attention, even children in their preschool period. This study investigated the three developmental levels of emotional understanding competence between five- and six-year-old LBC and NLBC. The results showed that the experience of parental absence was truly correlated with the LBC’s weaker EU, regardless of level. Further analysis revealed that there was no significant change in EU among LBC being taken care of by one parent, grandparents, or other relatives.

4.1. Poor Emotional Understanding of LBC

The present study demonstrated that the EU competence of rural LBC aged five to six years was significantly lower than that of NLBC at any of the three levels (External, Internal, Reflective). This was closely consistent with the previous studies and conclusions [42,56]. Emotional development fluctuated intricately in the preschool years. Young children begin to experience sophisticated emotional events in real life. Between the ages of two and four, children may pair these kinds of familiar situations with appropriate emotional reactions, and they may come to understand which desire affected which kinds of emotions at five to six years old [57,58]. In this way, it was easy for them to assess what makes individuals’ emotional reactions so different. Children’s three levels of EU competence could benefit from training and social setting factors as potential moderators [59]. However, longer duration of being left behind was significantly associated with more total difficulties in psychological adjustment and emotional cost [1,60]. Specifically, LBC fell behind in EU regarding facial expressions, situations, reminders, desires, display rules, morality, external causes, and mixed features. On the whole, the early emotional comprehension ability of LBC was indeed adversely affected by the left-behind experience.

4.2. Family Emotional Function Would Make Sense in LBC’s EU

This study also confirmed that once the family structure changed or became unstable because of parental migration. No matter who acted as actual caregiver of the LBC, the impact on emotional understanding ability showed basically no difference. Family system theory argues that parental presence is irreplaceable for the development of infants and toddlers [35]. With regard to emotional faces, for example, a previous study indicated that the facial expressions of parents, such as mothers, could bring about significant fluctuations in children’s affective reactions, regulatory coping behaviours, and meaning-making [61]. However, LBC had poor emotional interactions about emotions with their parents and primary caregivers. For example, migrant parents usually hide their emotions to prevent their children from missing them. Besides, not being a permanent adoption, actual guardians might not show their own true emotions to these LBC in some cases. Adverse family life experiences in early childhood would create a disadvantage in emotional comprehension, such as facial emotion understanding [62,63]. Even if only one parent left home, another parent could not make up for the emotional lack young children experience [64]. In other words, once the operation of the family emotional function was affected due to parental migration, the LBC would have a similar disadvantage in understanding emotions regardless of who the actual caregivers are.

4.3. Left-Behind Experience Effected Early EU Left-Behind

Chinese LBC with attachment problems fare less well, both physically and emotionally, compared to Chinese children growing up in stable homes [65]. Particularly in early childhood, the quality of attachment with parents was correlated with the social-emotional development of children [66]. For example, a previous study indicated that children’s emotion regulation had close associations with attachment security [67]. During preschool, the ability to process emotions would actually depend on the level of attachment security [68]. It is difficult for young LBC at home to have access to such psychological resources. From the inter-group comparison of LBC, it was found that a split family might bring negative psychological effects and sensitivity to safety issues.
Children’s differences in early emotional understanding were related to specific aspects of family experiences [69]. This early adverse deprivation of emotional experience may have a negative impact on the recognition of facial emotion [70]. The results showed that LBC at five and six-years old fall behind in processing and identifying some facial expressions like sad and fearful. A previous study of LBC with urban Internet-addiction showed that they focused more on negative emotions [71]. The younger the age of separation, the greater the negative effects on children’s emotional cognition and experience [72,73]. It is clear that children who are left behind in early childhood are more affected by parental absence and perform less well in emotional understanding.

4.4. Limitations

The present study had several limitations that should be recognized. The emotional comprehension of LBC may be affected by a variety of factors, especially for preschool children at a critical stage of social development. For instance, the length of separation needs to be further verified in the future. Besides, there was no statistical difference of EU in the types of caregivers of LBC, which may be due to the relatively small number of participants who were recruited by a non-randomized sampling method in Guangdong province in southeast China. Caution must be taken when generalizing the reported findings to LBC from other communities.

5. Conclusions

The present study investigated the three levels (External, Internal, Reflective) of emotional understanding of LBC and NLBC. Indeed, the emotional understanding competence of young LBC was significantly worse than that of NLBC at any level. Overall, LBC’s early emotional understanding competence were at a disadvantage. Among the three types of caregivers, the early internal differences of LBC were not significant, even when one of parents did not migrate. Emotion understanding is crucial for early social interaction and adjustment. Therefore, this was an important reference for exploring the development of emotional understanding in younger and larger age groups. To reduce the effect of negative psychological suffering of LBC in early childhood, efforts should be made to prevent the occurrence of being left behind, so that parents can shoulder the responsibility of parenting, and all can obtain support.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, R.T., H.F. and S.C.; methodology, R.T. and H.F.; validation, S.C. and R.T.; formal analysis, R.T.; data curation, R.T. and S.C.; writing—original draft preparation, R.T., H.F. and S.C.; writing—review and editing, R.T. and S.C.; visualization, R.T.; supervision, S.C.; project administration, S.C.; funding acquisition, S.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This study was funded by the Key Project of Guangdong Office of Philosophy and Social Science (Grant No. GD22CXL02).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the research ethics committee of Guangzhou University (Protocol Number: GZHU 2019019).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained before the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank all the participants for their time and support for the present study. Phoenix Lake Kindergarten provided technical support for this study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Fan, F.; Su, L.; Gill, M.K.; Birmaher, B. Emotional and behavioral problems of Chinese left-behind children: A preliminary study. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2010, 45, 655–664. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Zhang, H.; Chi, P.; Long, H.; Ren, X. Bullying victimization and depression among left-behind children in rural China: Roles of self-compassion and hope. Child Abuse Negl. 2019, 96, 104072. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Fellmeth, G.; Rose-Clarke, K.; Zhao, C.; Busert, L.K.; Zheng, Y.; Massazza, A.; Sonmez, H.; Eder, B.; Blewitt, A.; Lertgrai, W.; et al. Health impacts of parental migration on left-behind children and adolescents: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet 2018, 392, 2567–2582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Jordan, L.P.; Graham, E. Resilience and well-being among children of migrant parents in South-East Asia. Child Dev. 2012, 83, 1672–1688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Lu, Y.; Zhang, R.; Du, H. Family Structure, Family Instability, and Child Psychological Well-Being in the Context of Migration: Evidence from Sequence Analysis in China. Child Dev. 2021, 92, e416–e438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Xue, E.; Li, J.; Li, X. Sustainable development of education in rural areas for rural revitalization in china: A comprehensive policy circle analysis. Sustainability 2021, 13, 13101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Camras, L.A. Children’s understanding of facial expressions used during conflict encounters. Child Dev. 1980, 51, 879–885. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Ontai, L.L.; Thompson, R.A. Patterns of attachment and maternal discourse effects on children’s emotion understanding from 3 to 5 years of age. Soc. Dev. 2002, 11, 433–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Albanese, O.; De Stasio, S.; Di Chiacchio, C.; Fiorilli, C.; Pons, F. Emotion comprehension: The impact of nonverbal intelligence. J. Genet. Psychol. 2010, 171, 101–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Downs, A.; Smith, T. Emotional understanding, cooperation, and social behavior in high-functioning children with autism. J. Autism. Dev. Disord. 2004, 34, 625–635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Pons, F.; Harris, P.L.; de Rosnay, M. Emotion comprehension between 3 and 11 years: Developmental periods and hierarchical organization. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 2004, 1, 127–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Li, N.; Zhang, F.J.; Wu, J.L. An study on emotional understanding of hearing-impaired chilren. Chin. J. Spec. Educ. 2009, 144, 22–27. (In Chinese) [Google Scholar]
  13. Tang, Y.; Harris, P.L.; Pons, F.; Zou, H.; Zhang, W.; Xu, Q. The understanding of emotion among young Chinese children. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2018, 42, 512–517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Pons, F.; Harris, P. Test of Emotion Comprehension: TEC; University of Oxford: Oxford, UK, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  15. Gross, A.L.; Ballif, B. Children’s understanding of emotion from facial expressions and situations: A review. Dev. Rev. 1991, 11, 368–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Dickson, K.L.; Fogel, A.; Messinger, D. The Development of Emotion from a Social Process View. In What Develops in Emotional Development? Emotions, Personality, and Psychotherapy; Mascolo, M.F., Griffin, S., Eds.; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Thompson, R.A. Doing it with feeling: The emotion in early socioemotional development. Emot. Rev. 2015, 7, 121–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Carlo, G.; Knight, G.P.; Eisenberg, N.; Rotenberg, K.J. Cognitive processes and prosocial behaviors among children: The role of affective attributions and reconciliations. Dev. Psychol. 1991, 27, 456–461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Pons, F.; Lawson, J.; Harris, P.L.; de Rosnay, M. Individual differences in children’s emotion understanding: Effects of age and language. Scand. J. Psychol. 2003, 44, 347–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Pons, F.; Giménez-Dasí, M.; Daniel, M.; Auriac-Slusarczyk, E.; Businaro, N.; Viana, K.M. Impact of a low-cost classroom dialogue-based intervention on preschool children’s emotion understanding. Eur. Early Child Educ. 2019, 27, 630–646. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Rieffe, C.; Terwogt, M.M.; Koops, W.; Stegge, H.; Oomen, A. Preschoolers’ appreciation of uncommon desires and subsequent emotions. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2001, 19, 259–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Larsen, J.T.; To, Y.M.; Fireman, G. Children’s understanding and experience of mixed emotions. Psychol. Sci. 2007, 18, 186–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Bronfenbrenner, U. Ecology of the family as a context for human development: Research perspectives. Dev. Psychol. 1986, 22, 723–742. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Borelli, J.L.; Crowley, M.J.; David, D.H.; Sbarra, D.A.; Anderson, G.M.; Mayes, L.C. Attachment and emotion in school-aged children. Emotion 2010, 10, 475–485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Laible, D.J.; Thompson, R.A. Attachment and emotional understanding in preschool children. Dev. Psychol. 1998, 34, 1038–1045. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Khaleque, A. Perceived parental neglect, and children’s psychological maladjustment, and negative personality dispositions: A meta-analysis of multi-cultural Studies. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2015, 24, 1419–1428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Shao, J.; Zhang, L.; Ren, Y.; Xiao, L.; Zhang, Q. Parent-Child Cohesion, Basic Psychological Needs Satisfaction, and Emotional Adaptation in Left-Behind Children in China: An Indirect Effects Model. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Amato, P.R. Parental absence during childhood and depression in later life. Soc. Q. 1991, 32, 543–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Amato, P.R.; Anthony, C.J. Estimating the effects of parental divorce and death with fixed effects models. J. Marriage Fam. 2014, 76, 370–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Chen, M.; Chan, K.L. Parental absence, child victimization, and psychological well-being in rural China. Child Abuse Negl. 2016, 59, 45–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Denham, S.A.; Mitchell-Copeland, J.; Strandberg, K.; Auerbach, S.; Blair, K. Parental Contributions to Preschoolers’ Emotional Competence: Direct and Indirect Effects. Motiv. Emot. 1997, 21, 65–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Xiang, B. How far are the left-behind left behind? A preliminary study in rural China. Popul. Space Place 2007, 13, 179–191. [Google Scholar]
  33. Ministry of Civil Affairs. Chart: Data of Rural Left-Behind Children in 2018; Ministry of Civil Affairs: Beijing, China, 2018. Available online: https://www.Mca.Gov.Cn/article/gk/tjtb/201809/20180900010882.Shtml (accessed on 5 May 2022).
  34. Beh, L.S. China’s Left-behind Children (Liu-shou-er-tong): Development and Challenges for the Future. Cph. J. Asian Stud. 2014, 32, 58–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Cox, M.J.; Paley, B. Families as systems. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 1997, 48, 243–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Wang, F.; Lin, L.; Xu, M.; Li, L.; Lu, J.; Zhou, X. Mental Health among Left-Behind Children in Rural China in Relation to Parent-Child Communication. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 1855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Valtolina, G.G.; Colombo, C. Psychological well-being, family relations, and developmental issues of children left behind. Psychol. Rep. 2012, 111, 905–928. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Chen, M.; Sun, X.; Chen, Q.; Chan, K.L. Parental Migration, Children’s Safety and Psychological Adjustment in Rural China: A Meta-Analysis. Trauma Violence Abus. 2020, 21, 113–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Zhao, C.; Wang, F.; Li, L.; Zhou, X.; Hesketh, T. Persistent effects of parental migration on psychosocial wellbeing of left-behind children in two Chinese provinces: A cross-sectional survey. Lancet 2016, 388, S6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Beazley, H.; Butt, L.; Ball, J. ‘Like it, don’t like it, you have to like it’: Children’s emotional responses to the absence of transnational migrant parents in Lombok, Indonesia. Child Geogr. 2018, 16, 591–603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Cooke, J.E.; Stuart-Parrigon, K.L.; Movahed-Abtahi, M.; Koehn, A.J.; Kerns, K.A. Children’s emotion understanding and mother-child attachment: A meta-analysis. Emotion 2016, 16, 1102–1106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Zhang, H.Y. Relations of parent-child interaction to Chinese young children’s emotion understanding. J. Pac. Rim Psychol. 2018, 12, 9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Wen, M.; Lin, D. Child development in rural China: Children left behind by their migrant parents and children of nonmigrant families. Child Dev. 2012, 83, 120–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Zhou, C.; Lv, Q.; Yang, N.; Wang, F. Left-Behind Children, Parent-Child Communication and Psychological Resilience: A Structural Equation Modeling Analysis. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 5123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Wang, L.; Mesman, J. Child Development in the Face of Rural-to-Urban Migration in China: A Meta-Analytic Review. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2015, 10, 813–831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Antia, K.; Boucsein, J.; Deckert, A.; Dambach, P.; Račaitė, J.; Šurkienė, G.; Jaenisch, T.; Horstick, O.; Winkler, V. Effects of International Labour Migration on the Mental Health and Well-Being of Left-Behind Children: A Systematic Literature Review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 4335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Lindström, M.; Rosvall, M. Parental separation in childhood and self-reported psychological health: A population-based study. Psychiatry Res. 2016, 246, 783–788. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Shi, H.; Wang, Y.; Li, M.; Tan, C.; Zhao, C.; Huang, X.; Dou, Y.; Duan, X.; Du, Y.; Wu, T.; et al. Impact of parent-child separation on children’s social-emotional development: A cross-sectional study of left-behind children in poor rural areas of China. BMC Public Health 2021, 21, 823. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Yao, J.; Mao, Y. Rural left-behind children’s academic psychology in Western China and the school management countermeasures. Front. Educ. China 2008, 3, 535–546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Zhao, F.; Yu, G. Parental Migration and Rural Left-Behind Children’s Mental Health in China: A Meta-Analysis Based on Mental Health Test. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2016, 25, 3462–3472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Gao, X.; Maurer, D. Influence of intensity on children’s sensitivity to happy, sad, and fearful facial expressions. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2009, 102, 503–521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Hughes, C.; Dunn, J.; White, A. Trick or treat?: Uneven understanding of mind and emotion and executive dysfunction in “hard-to-manage” preschoolers. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 1998, 39, 981–994. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Ridgeway, D.; Waters, E.; Kuczaj, S.A. Acquisition of emotion-descriptive language: Receptive and productive vocabulary norms for ages 18 months to 6 years. Dev. Psychol. 1985, 21, 901–908. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Denham, S.A.; Couchoud, E.A. Young preschoolers’ understanding of emotions. Child Study J. 1990, 20, 171–192. [Google Scholar]
  55. Cavioni, V.; Grazzani, I.; Ornaghi, V.; Pepe, A.; Pons, F. Assessing the factor structure and measurement invariance of the test of emotion comprehension (TEC): A large cross-sectional study with children aged 3–10 years. J. Cogn. Dev. 2020, 21, 406–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Chen, X.; Chen, H.; Ling, Z.; Zou, Q.; Zheng, C.; Ren, Y. Research progress on psychosocial competence of rural left-behind children. Appl. Educ. Psychol. 2022, 3, 73–81. [Google Scholar]
  57. Thompson, R.A.; Lagattuta, K.H. Feeling and Understanding: Early Emotional Development. In Blackwell Handbook of Early Childhood Development; McCartney, K., Phillips, D., Eds.; Blackwell Publishing: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2006; pp. 317–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Shiverick, S.M.; Moore, C.F. Fulfilment of intention and desire in children’s judgements of emotion for sociomoral events. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2013, 31, 395–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Sprung, M.; Münch, H.M.; Harris, P.L.; Ebesutani, C.; Hofmann, S.G. Children’s emotion understanding: A meta-analysis of training studies. Dev. Rev. 2015, 37, 41–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Jingzhong, Y.; Lu, P. Differentiated childhoods: Impacts of rural labor migration on left-behind children in China. J. Peasant Stud. 2011, 38, 355–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Weinberg, M.K.; Beeghly, M.; Olson, K.L.; Tronick, E. A Still-face Paradigm for Young Children: 2½ Year-olds’ Reactions to Maternal Unavailability during the Still-face. J. Dev. Process. 2008, 3, 4–22. [Google Scholar]
  62. Pears, K.C.; Fisher, P.A. Emotion understanding and theory of mind among maltreated children in foster care: Evidence of deficits. Dev. Psychopathol. 2005, 17, 47–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Paine, A.L.; van Goozen SH, M.; Burley DT Anthony, R.; Shelton, K.H. Facial emotion recognition in adopted children. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2021, 32, 87–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Zhao, J.; Liu, X.; Wang, M. Parent-child cohesion, friend companionship and left-behind children’s emotional adaptation in rural China. Child Abuse Negl. 2015, 48, 190–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Chen, X.; Wang, L.; Wang, Z. Shyness-sensitivity and social, school, and psychological adjustment in rural migrant and urban children in China. Child Dev. 2009, 80, 1499–1513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Erickson, M.F.; Sroufe, L.A.; Egeland, B. The relationship between quality of attachment and behavior problems in preschool in a high-risk sample. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 1985, 50, 147–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Kiel, E.J.; Kalomiris, A.E. Current Themes in Understanding Children’s Emotion Regulation as Developing from within the Parent-Child Relationship. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2015, 3, 11–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Ştefan, C.A.; Avram, J. The multifaceted role of attachment during preschool: Moderator of its indirect effect on empathy through emotion regulation. Early Child Dev. Care 2018, 188, 62–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Nixon, C.L.; Watson, A.C. Family experiences and early emotion understanding. Merrill Palmer Q. 2001, 47, 300–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Moulson, M.C.; Fox, N.A.; Zeanah, C.H.; Nelson, C.A. Early adverse experiences and the neurobiology of facial emotion processing. Dev. Psychol. 2009, 45, 17–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  71. Ge, Y.; Zhong, X.; Luo, W. Recognition of Facial Expressions by Urban Internet-Addicted Left-Behind Children in China. Psychol. Rep. 2017, 120, 391–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Cooke, J.E.; Kochendorfer, L.B.; Stuart-Parrigon, K.L.; Koehn, A.J.; Kerns, K.A. Parent-child attachment and children’s experience and regulation of emotion: A meta-analytic review. Emotion 2019, 19, 1103–1126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Peng, Y.S.; Hu, K.; Wang, Y.L. Effects of separation age and separation duration among left-behind children in China. Chin. J. Clin. Psychol. 2017, 25, 731–738. [Google Scholar]
Table 1. Comparison of EU between two groups of children.
Table 1. Comparison of EU between two groups of children.
Children’s Status
LBC (n = 90)NLBC (n = 90)F95% CI
MSDMSD
Externalrecognition7.983.449.773.1013.50 ***[−2.75, −0.83]
situation2.710.143.520.1020.79 ***[−1.16, −0.46]
reminders1.831.492.401.427.80 **[−0.97, −1.66]
Internaldesires3.440.214.680.1918.90 ***[−1.79, −0.67]
belief0.880.131.130.161.63[−0.66, 0.14]
display1.340.111.740.089.35 **[−0.65, −0.14]
Reflectivemixed2.740.173.270.174.60 *[−1.00, −0.04]
regulation1.170.071.100.050.63[−0.10, 0.23]
causes13.080.7316.530.4616.32 ***[−5.14, −1.77]
morality0.440.100.390.100.15[−0.23, 0.34]
Note: * p < 0.05. ** p < 0.01. *** p < 0.001. Abbreviations: LBC, left-behind children; NLBC, non-left-behind children.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Tan, R.; Fang, H.; Chen, S. The Development of Emotion Understanding among Five- and Six-Year-Old Left-Behind Children in Rural China. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, 3974. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20053974

AMA Style

Tan R, Fang H, Chen S. The Development of Emotion Understanding among Five- and Six-Year-Old Left-Behind Children in Rural China. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2023; 20(5):3974. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20053974

Chicago/Turabian Style

Tan, Ruifeng, Huimin Fang, and Suiqing Chen. 2023. "The Development of Emotion Understanding among Five- and Six-Year-Old Left-Behind Children in Rural China" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 20, no. 5: 3974. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20053974

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop