Effects of Exposure to Formal Aquatic Activities on Babies Younger Than 36 Months: A Systematic Review
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
Quality Assessment
3. Results
3.1. Effects of Baby Swimming Programs on Infants’ Health
3.2. Effects of Baby Swimming Programs on Infants’ Development
3.3. Effects of Aquatic Therapy on Infants’ Health and Physiological Parameters
3.4. Effects of Aquatic Therapy on Infants’ Development
3.5. Quality Assessment
4. Discussion
4.1. Health Domain
4.2. Physiological Domain
4.3. Development Domain
4.4. Strengths and Limitations of the Review
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Fouace, J. Pourquoi Nager Avant de Marcher…et Après; France Maman: Paris, France, 1970. [Google Scholar]
- Swimming Teachers’ Association. Growing Demand for Baby Swimming Lessons, STA Survey Reveals. Available online: https://www.sta.co.uk/news/2015/02/01/growing-demand-for-baby-swimming-lessons-sta-survey-reveals/ (accessed on 2 February 2023).
- Allen, G.; Velija, P.; Dodds, J. ‘We just thought everyone else is going so we might as well’: Middle-class parenting and franchised baby/toddler swimming. Leis. Stud. 2021, 40, 169–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Camus, J. Las Práticas Aquáticas del Bebé; Paidotribo: Barcelona, Spain, 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Yuniati, E. Enterprise Innovation in Service Independent Practice of Midwives in Indonesia: Literature Review. Turk. J. Comput. Math. Educ. 2021, 12, 1834–1840. [Google Scholar]
- McGraw, M.B. Growth: A Study of Johnny and Jimmy; Appleton Century: New York, NY, USA, 1935. [Google Scholar]
- McGraw, M.B. Swimming behavior of the human infant. J. Pediatr. 1939, 15, 485–490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- World Health Organization. Global Report on Drowning: Preventing a Leading Killer; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Franklin, R.C.; Peden, A.E.; Hamilton, E.B.; Bisignano, C.; Castle, C.D.; Dingels, Z.V.; Hay, S.I.; Liu, Z.; Mokdad, A.H.; Roberts, N.L.S.; et al. The burden of unintentional drowning: Global, regional and national estimates of mortality from the Global Burden of Disease 2017 Study. Inj. Prev. 2020, 26, i83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- American Academy of Pediatrics. Committee on Pediatric Aspects of Physical Fitness, Recreation, and Sports. Swimming Instructions for Infants. Pediatrics 1980, 65, 847. [Google Scholar]
- Committee on Sports Medicine and Fitness and Committee on Injury and Poison Prevention. American Academy of Pediatrics. Swimming Programs for Infants and Toddlers. Pediatrics 2000, 105, 868–870. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brenner, R.A.; Taneja, G.S.; Haynie, D.L.; Trumble, A.C.; Qian, C.; Klinger, R.M.; Klebanoff, M.A. Association between swimming lessons and drowning in childhood: A case-control study. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 2009, 163, 203–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gardner, H.G.; Baum, C.R.; Dowd, M.D.; Durbin, D.R.; Ebel, B.E.; Lichenstein, R.; Limbos, M.A.; O’Neil, J.; Quinlan, K.P.; Scholer, S.J.; et al. Prevention of drowning. Pediatrics 2010, 126, 178–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weiss, J.; American Academy of Pediatrics Committee on Injury and Poison Prevention. Prevention of drowning. Pediatrics 2010, 126, e253–e262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Uyan, Z.S.; Carraro, S.; Piacentini, G.; Baraldi, E. Swimming pool, respiratory health, and childhood asthma: Should we change our beliefs? Pediatr. Pulmonol. 2009, 44, 31–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chowdhury, S.; Alhooshani, K.; Karanfil, T. Disinfection byproducts in swimming pool: Occurrences, implications and future needs. Water Res. 2014, 53, 68–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- García, J.L.; Sánchez-López, A.M.; Baena García, L.; Noack Segovia, J.P.; Aguilar-Cordero, M.J. Influencia de la actividad física acuática sobre el neurodesarrollo de los bebés: Revisión sistemática. Nutr. Hosp. 2016, 33, 10–17. [Google Scholar]
- Wallis, B.A.; Watt, K.; Franklin, R.C.; Taylor, M.; Nixon, J.W.; Kimble, R.M. Interventions associated with drowning prevention in children and adolescents: Systematic literature review. Inj. Prev. 2015, 21, 195–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Taylor, D.H.; Franklin, R.C.; Peden, A.E. Aquatic Competencies and Drowning Prevention in Children 2–4 Years: A Systematic Review. Safety 2020, 6, 31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Getz, M.; Hutzler, Y.; Vermeer, A. Effects of aquatic interventions in children with neuromotor impairments: A systematic review of the literature. Clin. Rehabil. 2006, 20, 927–936. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jacques, K.d.C.; Drumond, N.R.; Andrade, S.A.F.; Chaves Júnior, I.P.; Toffol, W.C.d. Effectiveness of the hydrotherapy in children with chronic encephalopathy no progressive of the childhood: A systematic review. Fisioter. Mov. 2010, 23, 53–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Page, M.J.; McKenzie, J.E.; Bossuyt, P.M.; Boutron, I.; Hoffmann, T.C.; Mulrow, C.D.; Shamseer, L.; Tetzlaff, J.M.; Akl, E.A.; Brennan, S.E.; et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 2021, 372, n71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sterne, J.A.; Hernán, M.A.; Reeves, B.C.; Savović, J.; Berkman, N.D.; Viswanathan, M.; Henry, D.; Altman, D.G.; Ansari, M.T.; Boutron, I.; et al. ROBINS-I: A tool for assessing risk of bias in non-randomised studies of interventions. BMJ 2016, 355, i4919. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Moola, S.; Munn, Z.; Tufanaru, C.; Aromataris, E.; Sears, K.; Sfetcu, R.; Currie, M.; Lisy, K.; Qureshi, R.; Mattis, P.; et al. Chapter 7: Systematic reviews of etiology and risk. In JBI Manual for Evidence Synthesis; JBI: Adelaide, Australia, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- Jeong, J.; Franchett, E.E.; Ramos de Oliveira, C.V.; Rehmani, K.; Yousafzai, A.K. Parenting interventions to promote early child development in the first three years of life: A global systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS Med. 2021, 18, e1003602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chaimay, B.; Thinkhamrop, B.; Thinkhamrop, J. Risk factors associated with language development problems in childhood—A literature review. J.-Med. Assoc. Thail. 2006, 89, 1080. [Google Scholar]
- Oudgenoeg-Paz, O.; Mulder, H.; Jongmans, M.J.; van der Ham, I.J.M.; Van der Stigchel, S. The link between motor and cognitive development in children born preterm and/or with low birth weight: A review of current evidence. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017, 80, 382–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Boonzaaijer, M.; Suir, I.; Mollema, J.; Nuysink, J.; Volman, M.; Jongmans, M. Factors associated with gross motor development from birth to independent walking: A systematic review of longitudinal research. Child Care Health Dev. 2021, 47, 525–561. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Been, J.V.; Lugtenberg, M.J.; Smets, E.; van Schayck, C.P.; Kramer, B.W.; Mommers, M.; Sheikh, A. Preterm birth and childhood wheezing disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS Med. 2014, 11, e1001596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Curran, E.A.; O’Neill, S.M.; Cryan, J.F.; Kenny, L.C.; Dinan, T.G.; Khashan, A.S.; Kearney, P.M. Research Review: Birth by caesarean section and development of autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2015, 56, 500–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rutayisire, E.; Huang, K.; Liu, Y.; Tao, F. The mode of delivery affects the diversity and colonization pattern of the gut microbiota during the first year of infants’ life: A systematic review. BMC Gastroenterol. 2016, 16, 86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harter, L.; Frost, F.; Grunenfelder, G.; Perkins-Jones, K.; Libby, J. Giardiasis in an infant and toddler swim class. Am. J. Public Health 1984, 74, 155–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nystad, W.; Nja, F.; Magnus, P.; Nafstad, P. Baby swimming increases the risk of recurrent respiratory tract infections and otitis media. Acta Paediatr. 2003, 92, 905–909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schoefer, Y.; Zutavern, A.; Brockow, I.; Schafer, T.; Kramer, U.; Schaaf, B.; Herbarth, O.; von Berg, A.; Wichmann, H.E.; Heinrich, J.; et al. Health risks of early swimming pool attendance. Int. J. Hyg. Environ. Health 2008, 211, 367–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nystad, W.; Haberg, S.E.; London, S.J.; Nafstad, P.; Magnus, P. Baby swimming and respiratory health. Acta Paediatr. 2008, 97, 657–662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Font-Ribera, L.; Villanueva, C.M.; Ballester, F.; Santa Marina, L.; Tardon, A.; Espejo-Herrera, N.; Esplugues, A.; Rodriguez Dehli, C.; Basterrechea, M.; Sunyer, J. Swimming pool attendance, respiratory symptoms and infections in the first year of life. Eur. J. Pediatr. 2013, 172, 977–985. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schuez-Havupalo, L.; Karppinen, S.; Toivonen, L.; Kaljonen, A.; Jartti, T.; Waris, M.; Peltola, V. Association between infant swimming and rhinovirus-induced wheezing. Acta Paediatr. 2014, 103, 1153–1158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sigmundsson, H.; Hopkins, B. Baby swimming: Exploring the effects of early intervention on subsequent motor abilities. Child Care Health Dev. 2010, 36, 428–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dias, J.A.; Manoel Ede, J.; Dias, R.B.; Okazaki, V.H. Pilot study on infant swimming classes and early motor development. Percept. Mot. Skills 2013, 117, 950–955. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pereira, K.R.G.; Valentini, N.C.; Saccani, R.; de Azevedo, H.A. Influência de atividades aquáticas no desenvolvimento motor de bebês. J. Phys. Educ. 2011, 22, 159–168. [Google Scholar]
- Borioni, F.; Biino, V.; Tinagli, V.; Pesce, C. Effects of Baby Swimming on Motor and Cognitive Development: A Pilot Trial. Percept. Mot. Skills 2022, 129, 977–1000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blystad, J.B.; van der Meer, A.L.H. Longitudinal study of infants receiving extra motor stimulation, full-term control infants, and infants born preterm: High-density EEG analyses of cortical activity in response to visual motion. Dev. Psychobiol. 2022, 64, e22276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leo, I.; Leone, S.; Dicataldo, R.; Vivenzio, C.; Cavallin, N.; Taglioni, C.; Roch, M. A Non-Randomized Pilot Study on the Benefits of Baby Swimming on Motor Development. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 9262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhao, S.; Xie, L.; Hu, H.; Xia, J.; Zhang, W.; Ye, N.; Chen, B. A study of neonatal swimming (water therapy) applied in clinical obstetrics. J. Matern. Fetal Neonatal Med. 2005, 17, 59–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vignochi, C.; Teixeira, P.P.; Nader, S.S. Efeitos da fisioterapia aquática na dor e no estado de sono e vigília de recém-nascidos pré-termo estáveis internados em unidade de terapia intensiva neonatal. Braz. J. Phys. Ther. 2010, 14, 214–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Novakoski, K.R.M.; Valderramas, S.R.; Israel, V.L.; Yamaguchi, B.; Andreazza, M.G. Back to the liquid environment: Effects of aquatic physiotherapy intervention performed on preterm infants. Rev. Bras. Cineantropometria Desempenho Hum. 2018, 20, 566–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Silva, H.A.; Silva, K.C.; Reco, M.d.O.N.; Santos Costa, A.; Almeida Soares-Marangoni, D.; Merey, L.F. Efeitos fisiológicos da hidroterapia em balde em recém-nascidos prematuros. Revista de Terapia Ocupacional da Universidade de São Paulo 2017, 28, 309–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McManus, B.M.; Kotelchuck, M. The effect of aquatic therapy on functional mobility of infants and toddlers in early intervention. Pediatr. Phys. Ther. 2007, 19, 275–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Araujo, L.B.d.; Mélo, T.R.; Israel, V.L. Improvements in babies’ neuropsychomotor development after family-centered Kids Intervention Therapy—Aquatic Environment (KITE): Biopsychosocial approach. Early Child Dev. Care 2023, 193, 33–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Varni, J. The PedsQL Measurement Model for the Pediatric Quality of Life Inventory. Available online: https://www.pedsql.org/about_pedsql.html (accessed on 31 July 2022).
- Zelazo, P.R.; Weiss, M.J. Infant Swimming Behaviors: Cognitive Control and the Influence of Experience. J. Cogn. Dev. 2006, 7, 1–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mitra, S.; Rennie, J. Neonatal jaundice: Aetiology, diagnosis and treatment. Br. J. Hosp. Med. 2017, 78, 699–704. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Perry, M.; Tan, Z.; Chen, J.; Weidig, T.; Xu, W.; Cong, X.S. Neonatal Pain: Perceptions and Current Practice. Crit. Care Nurs. Clin. N. Am. 2018, 30, 549–561. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Field, T. Preterm newborn pain research review. Infant Behav. Dev. 2017, 49, 141–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Martins, M.; Silva, A.; Marinho, D.; Pereira, A.; Moreira, A.; Sarmento, P.; Barbosa, T. Assessment of heart rate during infants’ swim session: Original research article. Int. SportMed J. 2010, 11, 336–344. [Google Scholar]
- Goksor, E.; Rosengren, L.; Wennergren, G. Bradycardic response during submersion in infant swimming. Acta Paediatr. 2002, 91, 307–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Veloso, E.; Barreiros, J.; Santos, C. Breathing and orientation underwater in swimming pool: Effects of age, practice, and duration of an aquatic stimulation program in babies and infants. Braz. J. Mot. Behav. 2007, 2, 11–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Diem, L. Early Motor Stimulation and Personal Development. J. Phys. Educ. Recreat. Danc. 1982, 53, 23–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Domain | Country | 1st Author, Year | Age | Intervention Group N | Control Group N | Study Design | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Baby Swimming | Health | USA | Harter, 1984 [32] | <3 years | 70 | 18 | Analytical, Observational, Cross-sectional, Prospective |
Norway | Nystad, 2003 [33] | 0–11 months | 155 | 2707 | Analytical, Observational, Cross-sectional, Retrospective | ||
Germany | Schoefer, 2008 [34] | <1 year | 660 | 655/877 | Analytical, Observational, Longitudinal, Retrospective | ||
Norway | Nystad, 2008 [35] | 6–18 months | 7717 | 23,152 | Analytical, Observational, Cross-sectional, Retrospective | ||
Spain | Font-Ribera, 2013 [36] | <1 year | 1106 | 1099 | Analytical, Observational, Cross-sectional, Retrospective | ||
Finland | Schuez-Havupalo, 2014 [37] | 0–17 months | 469 | 569 | Analytical, Observational, Longitudinal, Prospective | ||
Development | Norway | Sigmundsson, 2010 [38] | 2–7 months | 19 | 19 | Analytical, Observational, Cross-sectional, Retrospective | |
Brazil | Dias, 2013 [39] | 7–9 months | 6 | 6 | Analytical, Quasi-experimental, Longitudinal, Prospective | ||
Brazil | Pereira, 2011 [40] | 1–18 months | 40 | 40 | Analytical, Observational, Cross-sectional, Prospective | ||
Italy | Borioni, 2022 [41] | 0–3 years | 12 | 15 | Analytical, Quasi-experimental, Longitudinal, Prospective | ||
Norway | Blystad, 2022 [42] | <12 months | 10 | 10 + 10 preterm | Analytical, Quasi-experimental, Longitudinal, Prospective | ||
Italy | Leo, 2022 [43] | 6–10 months | 14 | 14 | Analytical, Observational, Cross-sectional, Prospective | ||
Baby Therapy | Health | China | Zhao, 2005 [44] | Newborns | 377 | 154 | Analytical, Observational, Longitudinal, Prospective |
Brazil | Vignochi, 2010 [45] | Newborns | 12 | Descriptive, Observational, Cross-sectional, Prospective | |||
Brazil | Novakoski, 2018 [46] | Newborns | 22 | Descriptive, Observational, Cross-sectional, Prospective | |||
Physiology | Brazil | Silva, 2017 [47] | Newborns | 30 | Descriptive, Observational, Longitudinal, Prospective | ||
Development | USA | McManus, 2007 [48] | 6–30 months | 15 | 22 | Analytical, Quasi-experimental, Longitudinal, Prospective | |
Brazil | Araujo, 2023 [49] | 4–18 months | 24 | 37 | Analytical, Quasi-experimental, Longitudinal, Prospective |
1st Author, Year | Research Aim and Design | Results | Risk of BIAS |
---|---|---|---|
Harter, 1984 [32] | Effect of swimming pool attendance on infants’ (0–3 years of age) giardia diagnosis. The presence of giardia cysts (in stool sample) was compared between 70 participants in swimming programs (93% of children, 87% of mothers and 36% of fathers) and 18 non-swimming siblings and playmates. | Giardia positivity (G+) for 61% children, 39% mothers and 28% fathers of the swim group; G+ children were found in each of the nine programs swim classes Children G+ for control group = 0% No association between G+ and age (p = 0.35) G+ with higher prevalence (71%) for attendance > 7 sessionsG+ prevalence for < 7 sessions = 35%. Swimming pool attendance increased the odds of infants to become Giardia positive. | ** Confounding Statistical analysis |
Nystad, 2003 [33] | Effect of baby swimming programs attendance on respiratory tract infections and otitis media in the first year of life. The standard questionnaire of the International Study of Asthma and Allergies in Childhood (ISAAC) was applied to 2862 children 6–16-years-old to access the presence of recurrent respiratory tract infections (RRTI), bronchitis, bronchiolitis, pneumonia, otitis media during the period 0–11 months age. A second questionnaire of parental history of atopy (asthma, eczema or hay fever), demographic information, early exposures and childhood health was applied one year later. | No association between respiratory tract infections and baby swimming. Risk of RRTI [adjusted odds ratio (aOR) 2.08, 95% confidence interval (95% CI) 1.08–4.03] and otitis media (aOR 1.77, 95% CI 0.96–3.25) increases only in children of parents with atopy. Baby swimming programs increased the RRTI and otitis media in infancy among children of parents with a history of atopy. | ** Confounding Measurement of exposure Outcomes measurement |
Schoefer, 2008 [34] | Effect of swimming pool attendance on early infections and development of airway diseases after 1st year of life. On a 6-year longitudinal study with questionnaires administered to parents on a regular basis (aged 6, 12, 18 months and 2, 4, 6 years), information on socioeconomic factors, medical history (hey fever, asthma, eczema, airway infections, otitis media, diarrhea), and lifestyle factors of 2192 children was obtained. Parental atopy, age of first pool attendance [(a) 1st year baby swimming (N = 660), (b) 1st year occasionally (N = 655) and (c) later or never (N = 877)] and frequency of pool attendance was also accessed. | Non-swimming babies had lower rates of infection of(i) diarrhea: OR = 0.68 (0.54–0.85), CI 95%; (ii) otitis media: OR = 0.81 (0.62–1.05), CI 95%; (iii) airway infections: OR 0.85 CI 95% 0.67–1.09 in the 1st year of life. No clear association between late or non-swimmers and hay fever or atopic dermatitis were found. Higher rates of asthma were found (OR 2.15 95% CI 1.16–3.99), however, potentially due to reverse causation. Swimming pool attendance increased gastrointestinal infections (i.e., diarrhea) during the first year of life, but no association of swimming pool attendance and atopic diseases and airway infections was found. | * Confounding |
Nystad, 2008 [35] | Effect of baby swimming in the first 6 months of life on respiratory diseases from 6 to 18 months. Maternal retrospective report (at 18 months age) about their infants’ lower respiratory tract infections (LRTI), wheeze and otitis media between 6 and 18 months of age (N = 30,870) in the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study (MoBa). History of maternal atopy was also accessed. | LRTI and otitis media were not associated with baby swimming attendance. An increased risk of wheeze [adjusted odds ratios (aOR) 1.24 (95% CI 1.11, 1.39)], on children who attended baby swimming was only observed on children with atopic mothers. Baby swimming programs increased the likelihood of wheeze in infants with a maternal history of atopy. | ** Measurement of exposure Measurement of condition Outcomes measurement |
Font-Ribera, 2013 [36] | Effect of baby swimming programs attendance on respiratory symptoms and infections during the first year of life. Parent report (at 14 months age) about LRTI, persistent cough, wheezing, otitis and atopic eczema during the first year of life (N = 2205 infants). Swimming pool attendance during the first year of life and parental atopy was also accessed. | Adjusted OR of wheezing [1.06 (95%CI, 0.88–1.28)] and LRTI [1.09 (0.90–1.31)] for babies not attending vs. babies attending swimming pools. Type of swimming pool (indoor or outdoor), and parental atopy did not modify the results. Swimming pool attendance during the first year of life was not associated with LRTI, otitis, wheezing, atopic eczema or persistent cough. | ** Measurement of exposure Outcomes measurement |
Schuez-Havupalo, 2014 [37] | Effect of baby swimming programs attendance on infants’ (0–17 months of age) respiratory tract infections. Wheezing, bronchiolitis, number of days per year with rhinorrhea, cough or fever recorded. 1827 children were followed up from birth until 17 months of age, on baby swimming attendance, wheezing, bronchiolitis, number of days per year with rhinorrhea, fever or cough. Viral diagnostics were performed in a subset of children with all respiratory tract infections. | An increased likelihood of wheezing illness was observed in swimming children when compared to non-swimming children (p = 0.11). Rhinoviruses were more correlated with wheezing in swimming children [11/296 (3.7%)] than non-swimming children [4/339 (1.2%)] (p = 0.04). Baby swimming attendance had an odds ratio of 1.71 (p = 0.05) for bronchiolitis and 3.57 (p = 0.06) for rhinovirus- associated wheezing. Baby swimming attendance was associated with rhinovirus-associated wheezing for children with atopic eczema (p = 0.006). Infant swimming programs increased respiratory tract infections in atopic infants. | * None |
1st Author, Year | Research Aim and Design | Results | Risk of BIAS |
---|---|---|---|
Sigmundsson, 2010 [38] | Effects of baby swimming programs attendance on infants’ (2–7 months) subsequent motor abilities. Motor abilities of 19 four-years-old children who attended baby swimming programs during first year of life (mostly between 2 and 7 months of age) were tested using Standardized Movement Assessment Battery for Children and compared with an age-matched control group of 19 who did not attend baby swimming programs. | Performance in prehension, ball skill sub-test (p < 0.05), and static balance, one-leg balance sub-test (p < 0.017) were better in the swimming group. Baby swimming programs promote better motor skill development specifically in provision of vestibular stimulation and eye–hand coordination. | ** Confounding Criteria for inclusion in the sample Measurement of exposure |
Dias, 2013 [39] | Effect of baby swimming programs attendance on infants’ (7–9 months of age) gross motor development. Gross motor skills were accessed using Alberta Infant Motor Scale (AIMS) before and after four months of weekly playful baby swimming classes with babies (N = 6) and compared with a control (N = 6). | Results revealed a difference between pre- and post-tests (p < 0.02) for both groups, with Cohen’s r = 0.90 in experimental group indicating a larger effect than observed in the control group (Cohen’s r = 0.69); and a larger effect size in the experimental group (r = 0.47) of the change in comparison to the control group (r = 0.06). No differences between groups were observed. Baby swimming programs attendance facilitated the development of infants’ gross motor skills; however, the sample size was too small to generate significant differences. | * Confounding |
Pereira, 2011 [40] | Effect of participating in baby swimming programs and program participation period in infants’ (1–18 months of age) motor development. Motor development, accessed using Alberta Infant Motor Scale (AIMS), was tested on a group of infants who participated in a program of aquatic activities (N = 40), and compared with a matched control group of non-swimmers (N = 40). | Motor development of non-swimming child was lower (Chi2 = 16.59; p < 0.001). A significant correlation was found (rho = 0.42; p = 0.012) between the time attending baby swimming program and percentile values: longer exposure was related with higher percentile values. Baby swimming program attendance and participation period had positive influence in infants’ motor development. | ** Confounding |
Borioni, 2022 [41] | Effect of baby swimming programs attendance on infant’ (0–3 years of age) motor and cognitive development. Peabody Developmental Motor Scale was applied to assess 0–3 years old gross motor skills (GM), fine motor skills (FM) and total motor skills (TM), and core tests of executive functions was applied to access Cognitive development (delayed response for working memory, object retrieval for inhibition and A-not-B for response shifting) before and after 10 weekly 45 min sessions of baby swimming intervention (N = 12), as well as control group (N = 15). | Motor development: For the intervention group, post-test of GM, FM, and TM scores were higher than pre-test scores (GM: Z = −2.98, p = 0.003; FM: Z = −2.97, p = 0.003; TM: Z = −3.08, p = 0.002). For the control group no significant different between pre and post-test were observed. GM, FM, and TM scores were higher for intervention group (GM: U = 35.50, n1 = 12, n2 = 15, p = 0.006; FM: U = 25.50, n1 = 12, n2 = 15, p = 0.001; TM: U = 25.00, n1 = 12; n2 = 15, p = 0.001). Cognitive performance: No differences between pre and post-test were found in either group. A marginal significant change in inhibition speed (Z = −2.12, p = 0.034), response shifting accuracy (Z = −1.87, p = 0.062) and in perseveration errors (Z = −2.00, p = 0.046) were observed on intervention group (given the adjusted p < 0.016 for three comparisons). Baby swimming programs attendance may benefit motor development and early executive function skills. | * Confounding |
Blystad, 2022 [42] | Effect of extra motor stimulation in the form of baby swimming on development of visual motion perception during first year of life. Brain responses to visual motion, accessed using EEG recordings and onset of self-produced locomotion (documented with parental video records) were obtained on a longitudinal study design at the ages of 4–5 months and 9–12 months on infants that received extra stimulation in the form of baby swimming (N = 10), infants that received a traditional Western upbringing (N = 10), and preterm infants (N = 10). Infants were presented with visual motion on a large screen simulating forward optic flow, reversed optic flow, and random visual motion. | Infants receiving extra motor stimulation and infants in the control group showed developmental improvements in visual motion perception, with a greater improvement for intervention group. Extra-stimulated infants also showed significantly shorter N2 latencies for visual motion and started to locomote at a younger age than the control and preterm groups. Baby swimming programs attendance during first year of life promotes accelerated developmental improvements of visual motion perception. | * Confounding |
Leo, 2022 [43] | Effect of baby swimming programs on infants’ (6–10 months of age) motor development Motor development was assessed using Peabody Developmental Motor Scale-2 in a group of infants attending baby swimming programs (N = 14) and a control group (N = 14). | Better scores on measures of reflexes (t = −2.2, p < 0.05), grasping (t = −3.8, p < 0.001), fine-motor quotient (t = −3.4, p < 0.01), and total-motor quotient (t = −2.4, p < 0.05) were observed in the intervention group. Baby swimming programs positively influence early motor development in infants and toddlers. | ** Measurement of exposure |
1st Author, Year | Research Aim and Design | Results | Risk of BIAS |
---|---|---|---|
Zhao, 2005 [44] | Effect of neonatal swimming necklace (water therapy) during hospitalization on newborns’ clinical parameters. Clinical parameters (weight before discharge, time of first defecation, meconium turning yellow) were recorded via daily monitoring in newborns exposed to aquatic exercises helped by nurse, twice/day for 10–15 min using neonatal swimming necklace (N = 223) and control group who received normal bathing (N = 154). | Weight at discharge: Spontaneous vaginal delivered infants (IG = 3.29 ± 0.35 kg; CG = 3.09 ± 0.38; p < 0.01); Caesareans delivered infants (IG = 3.51 ± 0.40 kg; CG = 3.17 ± 0.48; p < 0.01). Time of first defecation: Spontaneous vaginal delivered infants (IG = 7.03 + 4.80 h; CG = 8.53 + 5.06; p < 0.05); Caesareans delivered infants (IG = 6.54 + 3.59 h; CG = 8.13 + 4.16; p < 0.05) Time of meconium turning yellow: Spontaneous vaginal delivered infants (IG = 39.15 + 15.88 h; CG = 48.01 + 19.42 h; p < 0.01); Caesareans delivered infants (IG = 39.02 + 13.60 h; CG = 55.67 + 25.05; p < 0.05). Neonatal swimming necklace therapy promoted babies’ growth, earlier onset of first defecation and onset of meconium turning yellow in the early stage. | * None |
Vignochi, 2010 [45] | Effects of aquatic therapy on pain, sleep cycle and wakefulness on preterm infants.
| Sleep-wakefulness cycle: before intervention = 6; during intervention = 4; end of intervention = 3; 30 min after = 1.5; 60 min after = 1 (p < 0.001). Pain: Compared with baseline, the mean of pain measure decreased during the intervention (p = 0.012), at the end, after 30 and 60 min (p < 0.001). No significant differences for mean blood pressure and body temperature before to after intervention. HR and RR were significantly lower (p = 0.001 and p < 0.001) and SaO2 significantly higher (p < 0.001) comparing baseline with 30 and 60 min after intervention. Aquatic therapy reduced pain and improved sleep quality in preterm infants. | * Confounding Outcomes measurement |
Novakoski, 2018 [46] | Effects of aquatic physiotherapy on physiological variables, sleep disturbances, wakefulness, and pain on preterm infants.
| Pain reduction was observed between evaluation moments: before intervention = 3.68 ± 0.25; assessment 2 = 1.04 ± 0.12; assessment 3 = 0.40 ± 0.12 (p = < 0.001). Sleep and wakefulness improvement between evaluation moments: before intervention = 4.45 ± 0.30; assessment 2 = 3.54 ± 0.19; assessment 3 = 2.81 ± 0.21 (p = < 0.05). Body temperature decreased from first evaluation (36.52 °C ± 0.62 °C) to assessment 2 (36.24 ± 0.07 °C, p < 0.01); but was maintained from assessment 2 to assessment 3 (36.22 ± 0.06 °C, p = 1.0). HR rates decrease between first evaluation (154.27 ± 2.6 bpm) and third evaluation (143.72 ± 3.38 bpm, p = 0.003). SaO2 increased between evaluation 1 (94.50% ± 0.60%) and evaluation 2 (97.31% ± 0.36%, p = 0.001); gains were maintained in evaluation 3 (97.86% ± 0.33%). Aquatic therapy was effective in improving sleep, wakefulness and physiological parameters and reducing pain in preterm newborns. | * Confounding Outcomes measurement |
Silva, 2017 [47] | Effects of bucket aquatic therapy on physiological parameters in preterm newborns. Thirty preterm newborns were submerged in a bucket with warm water, up to the height of clavicles, during 10 min in two sessions in alternated days.
| A significant reduction of HR between pre-test (152.23 ± 3.13) and follow-up test (146.53 ± 2.92) was observed (p < 0.05). No significant differences for RR and SaO2 between assessment moments was observed. Bucket aquatic therapy with warm water decreased HR in hospitalized premature newborns. | ** Confounding |
1st Author, Year | Research Aim and Design | Results | Risk of BIAS |
---|---|---|---|
McManus, 2007 [48] | Effect of aquatic therapy on functional mobility in 6–30 months infants with delayed functional mobility. Functional mobility, measured using Gross Motor Subscale of the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL), was accessed in 15 infants diagnosed with delayed functional mobility before and after receiving 36 weekly 30 min aquatic therapy sessions in a pediatric pool in addition to 60 min home-based early intervention with a physical therapist or occupational therapist. A randomly selected comparison group (N = 22) received home-based early intervention only. | Age-adjusted normalized scores increased in the intervention group (baseline: 27.5 ± 9.8; follow-up: 30.5 ± 11.2; mean change score: 2.6 ± 9.3 points) and decreased in the control group (baseline: 32.5 ± 9.6; follow-up: 28.4 ± 10.1; mean change score: −2.6 ± 8.7 points). The intervention group had significantly greater (p < 0.05) gains in functional mobility than the comparison group. Aquatic therapy improved infants’ functional mobility (gross motor development). | * Confounding Criteria for inclusion in the sample |
Araujo, 2023 [49] | Effect of Kids Intervention Therapy–Aquatic Environment (KITE) program on 4–18-months-old infants’ neuropsychomotor development. Alberta Infant Motor Scale (AIMS) and Denver II Developmental Screening Test were initially applied to assess and stratify sample as well as at the end of the intervention and after four weeks (to assess retention). Regarding initial characteristics, neonatal and gestation characteristics, and family characteristics; children were classified as Typical or At-risk and delayed. Paediatric Quality of Life Inventory™ Infant Scales (PedsQL™) and Affordance in the Home Environment for Motor Development–Infant Scale (AHEMD-IS) were applied to both groups at the same 3 moments: before intervention, after 4 weeks of intervention and 4 weeks after. Intervention group (N = 24) received four weeks of aquatic environment therapy: 45–60 min, twice a week, of fun aquatic activities (Kids Intervention Therapy–Aquatic Environment (KITE) program); Control group (N = 37) had no intervention besides the daycare center participation. | In the intervention group, number of typical children increased at post-intervention (p = 0.001) and retention (p = 0.002), with a large intervention effect (η2 = 0.178 and 0.156) and delayed/at-risk cases decreased in post-intervention test, with a medium intervention effect in IG (η2 = 0.055). The intervention group had a significant medium effect of QOL on intragroup physical capacity at post-intervention (p = 0.023). No significant differences between the groups, at baseline, in QOL and in home stimulation were observed. No significant change in CG throughout the research. Fun aquatic activities had positive effects on typical and delayed/at-risk infants’ neuropsychomotor development, motor learning through retention and on QOL physical capacity domain. | * None |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Santos, C.; Burnay, C.; Button, C.; Cordovil, R. Effects of Exposure to Formal Aquatic Activities on Babies Younger Than 36 Months: A Systematic Review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, 5610. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20085610
Santos C, Burnay C, Button C, Cordovil R. Effects of Exposure to Formal Aquatic Activities on Babies Younger Than 36 Months: A Systematic Review. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2023; 20(8):5610. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20085610
Chicago/Turabian StyleSantos, Carlos, Carolina Burnay, Chris Button, and Rita Cordovil. 2023. "Effects of Exposure to Formal Aquatic Activities on Babies Younger Than 36 Months: A Systematic Review" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 20, no. 8: 5610. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20085610
APA StyleSantos, C., Burnay, C., Button, C., & Cordovil, R. (2023). Effects of Exposure to Formal Aquatic Activities on Babies Younger Than 36 Months: A Systematic Review. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 20(8), 5610. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20085610