Introduction
Electronic books (e-books) have become an important part on the book market. Advantages like a storage function, the possibility of adjusting font size or searching for keywords make e-books an interesting advancement of the classic paper book. An e-book can be defined as the digitized copy of a printed book, or an electronic book that is available in a digital format or a text in a digital format. E-publishing arised in the early 1970s (
Ardito, 2000). Several companies launched e-reading devices like the Newton or the Palm Pilot. The new generation of e-readers includes, among others, the Sony reader, the Amazon Kindle or the Barnes & Noble Nook Reader. This generation of e-readers is equipped with e-ink display technology. E-ink technology has several advantages: The battery life is astounding. For example the new Kindle reader gets up to a month of battery life. Another advantage is that they can be used outside without glare. E-ink displays look more like printed-paper than any other display does. Their technology is based on tiny microcapsules, with a diameter in the size of a human hair. Each microcapsule contains positively charged white particles and negatively charged black particles suspended in a clear fluid. When an electric field is applied, the white particles move to the top of the microcapsule where they become visible to the user. This makes the surface appear white at that spot. At the same time, the black particles are at the bottom of the microcapsules where they are not visible. By reversing the electric field, the black particles appear at the top of the capsule, which now makes the surface appear dark at that spot ("E Ink: Technology", 2011). Some of the new eink-readers are equipped with touch screen technology. This often goes along with a more glare-proned screen ("cnet reviews", 2010).
With the launch of the Apple iPad in April 2010 the tablet became popular as an e-reading device. These ereading devices are based on TFT‒LCDs (Thin Film Transitor-Liquid Crystal Displays), which in contrast to e-ink displays are active displays. A LCD is a thin, flat electronic visual display that uses the light modulating properties of liquid crystals (LCs). Each pixel of an LCD typically consists of a layer of molecules aligned between two transparent electrodes, and two polarizing filters, the axes of transmission of which are (in most of the cases) perpendicular to each other ("bit-tech.net", 2011). While e-ink readers are limited for reading, tablets are small computers and they are conceptualized for everything else. This is also the big advantage of tablets, they are not just a replacement for a book, they are multifunctional devices which can be used for communication, organization or leisure activities.
Along with the different screen technologies both devices have specific characteristics, i.e., battery life, storage capacity, user interface or screen size. Both e-ink-reader and tablets have become everyday technology and it can be expected that they will become even more common in the next years. If one of these technologies will become prevalent for reading in the future is unknown and is likely depending on their legibility and usability (Siegenthaler, Wurtz, & Groner, 2010; Siegenthaler et al., 2011). Beside new possibilities like for example searching for text, increasing or decreasing font size, bookmark, annotation functions or the function for linking to other documents, the main function still is reading and the question remains whether reading behavior is affected by the screen technology. The discussion whether e-reader with e-ink technology or tablets with LCD-technology are better for reading is emotional and scientific evidence is sparse. Many users have the conventional wisdom that e-ink displays, which look similar to paper, are better for reading ("Reflective LCD vs. e-ink? - MobileRead Forums", 2011). In user forums about mobile reading, there are many statements about the advantages of e-ink compared to LCDs, like the follow- ing example show: “E-INK is better for your eyes as it looks exactly like a paper book. Human eye sees printed material better than it sees material that is on an LCD. Reading on E-ink with a light on the screen (at night) is better than TFT…“ ("Reflective LCD vs. e-ink? - Mobil- eRead Forums", 2011). Research departments of big companies report inconsistent results: E-ink producer postulate that e-ink technology is better for reading, while tablet producer do not see any disadvantages when reading on tablets (
Carnoy, 2010). For example William Lynch, Barnes & Noble's CEO said about the reading on Nook Color screen “the company had done extensive research on displays and discovered that eyestrain with LCDs was not the huge issue many people were making it out to be”(
Carnoy, 2010). But there is no scientific evidence for this statement.
Results from past studies about reading on computer displays (Visual Display Units) show that compared to reading from paper, reading from Visual Display Units is less accurate, slower, more fatiguing and less liked by readers (
Creed et al., 1987;
Dillon, 1992;
Gould & Grischkowsky, 1884;
Mayes et al., 2001). As mentioned before new electronic reading devices feature new display technologies. Displays based on e-ink technology or new LCDs are different and results from previous studies cannot be transferred one to one on new reading devices. Previous studies about reading on e-ink-readers show that the reading behaviour on e-ink-readers is indeed very similar to the reading behaviour on print (
Siegenthaler et al., 2011).
Siegenthaler et al. (
2011) showed that participants shared similar proportion of regressive saccades while reading on e-ink-readers and print. Moreover results suggested that e-readers, in some situations, may even provide better legibility (
Siegenthaler et al., 2011).
Nielsen (
2010) compared reading on an e-ink-reader (Kindle), on a Tablet (Apple iPad) and on a classic paper book by experimenting on himself. He found that reading on the e-ink reader was 10.7% slower than reading on a classic paper book. Reading on a tablet was 6.2% slower than reading on a classic paper book (
Nielsen, 2010). Wurtz, Siegenthaler & Bergamin (2010) compared e-ink-readers and tablets with TFT-LCDs for reading over extended period of time (several hours). They measured visual fatigue (with subjective and objective measures) every hour and found no difference in visual fatigue when reading over extended time (
Wurtz et al., 2010). They concluded that compared to visual display units of the past, both e-ink-reader and tablets are good for reading over extended time.
In the present study we want to evaluate and compare reading behavior on e-ink-reader and on tablets (LCDs) as measured by eye tracking. If reading on e-ink displays is not similar to reading on LCDs, then differences in eye movement patterns (e.g., progressive- and regressive saccades, fixations) and in reading performance (e.g., reading speed) should be found.
Discussion
Overall the results suggest that the legibility of the current e-reader generation is fairly good. The analysis of eye movement data shows that reading behaviour on LCDs (tablets) is very similar to the reading behaviour on e-ink displays. There was no significant difference in fixation durations which gives evidence that participants didn’t have more difficulties with reading on LCDs compared to e-ink displays. Fixation duration can be used as a measure of legibility. During fixations, when the eye stands still for a short period of time, visual information is extracted and cognitive processing is applied (
Rayner & Pollatsek, 1989). Indeed there is an advantage for the LCD; significant difference in reading speed was found, participants read faster on the tablet than on the two eink-readers. This is partially in consent with Nielsens' report (
Nielsen, 2010). He found that reading on the e-ink reader was 10.7% slower than reading on a classic paper book. Reading on a tablet was 6.2% slower than reading on a classic paper book (
Nielsen, 2010). In contradiction
Siegenthaler et al. (
2011) found no difference in reading speed between reading on e-ink devices and reading on a classic paper book (
Siegenthaler et al., 2011). Furthermore, participants did significantly less regressive saccades when reading on tablets. If a reader does more regressive saccades, it can be taken as empirical evidence that the reader has problems in extracting visual and/or linguistic information. Since the same text was read, counterbalanced over readers and devices, we interpret more regressive saccades as being caused by lower legibility. Our results show that participants did significantly more regressive saccades when reading from an e-ink display compared with the LCD (tablet). The question arises in what respect LCDs show better legibility than eink displays. The result could be caused by the different screen size. The Apple iPad which has a 148 x 196 mm display is bigger than the two Sony e-ink displays (PRS505/PRS-600: 90 x 122.4 mm). Earlier studies show heterogeneous results of the influence of line length on legibility (
Dyson, 2004;
Shahikh, 2007). Font size was constant (3 mm) that means that number of characters per line is caused by line length and not confounded with font size. The devices were selected for the experiment because they were the most popular devices on the Swiss market at this time. In a future experiment we will test a TFT-LCD and an e-ink display with the same screen size for make out the effect of display size. Another explanation for the result is that the percentage of regressive saccades is related to contrast. Under artificial (low light) conditions LCDs have a better contrast (CW -38.87) than e-ink displays (CW -2.97/-3.275). If the result is caused by the contrast we could conclude that under specific artificial light conditions LCDs have a better legibility than e-ink displays. The mean percentage of regressive saccades over all devices (17% iPad; 22% PRS-505; 23% PRS-600) is rather high. According to Findlay (
Findlay & Gilchrist, 2003) 18% regressive saccades is normal when reading a scientific text. The text in the experiment was not scientific. The rather high rate of regressive saccades could be explained on one hand by the laboratory setting and the relatively large reading distance to the eye tracker or by the effort of participants to perform as good as possible in the experiment.
An interesting fact seems to be that we did not find any difference in the reading behavior between the Sony PRS-505 and the Sony PRS-600. The new Sony PRS-600 is equipped with a touch-screen. This often goes along with a more glare-proned screen. The results show a tendency that the touch-screen (Sony PRS-600) is some-what less readable but because of no statistical difference, we can say that the touch-screen on e-ink devices does not reduce legibility of the device.
Subjective interview data revealed that participants changed their opinion in the second session after having made some experiences with the devices. While participants judged the legibility of the iPad and the Sony PRS505 as equal (4.95) in the first session, the legibility of the iPad was judged better (5.25) and the legibility of the Sony PRS-505 was judged worse (4.41) in the second session. This fact shows how important it is to use different methods (subjective and objective methods) in Human-Computer-Research. The result demonstrates also that legibility judgments are influenced by the user experience with the devices.
As we know from earlier studies the reading process when reading on an e-ink-reader is very similar to the reading process when reading a classic paper book (
Siegenthaler et al., 2011). Since the results in the present study show that reading on a tablet is not worse than reading on an e-ink-reader we can conclude that reading on a tablet is under artificial light conditions not worse than reading on a classic paper book.