Understanding Pro-Environmental Behavior in the US: Insights from Grid-Group Cultural Theory and Cognitive Sociology
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Theoretical Perspectives and Relevant Research
2.1. Grid-Group Cultural Theory
2.2. Cognitive Sociology
2.3. Other Influences on Pro-Environmental Behavior
3. Research Methodology
3.1. Sample
3.2. Measures
3.3. Methods of Analysis
4. Results
4.1. Quantitative Results
4.1.1. Descriptive Statistics
4.1.2. Regression Results
4.1.3. ANOVA Results
4.2. Qualitative Results
4.2.1. Concepts
- I used to not care about the environment because I lacked knowledge of
- what was actually going on. But then I saw a documentary on pollution and
- it shocked me into thinking about my part in the problem. On New Year’s Eve
- I made a resolution not to litter and I kept that resolution and then I challenged
- all of my friends and family members to do the same.
- I am concerned about our water. As a kid we could swim all summer in the Rock
- River or any of the lakes and streams. Now I can’t even allow my dog to swim in
- them. I seriously had to take her to the vet because she kept getting a skin disorder,
- we finally figured out it was from swimming in the river.
4.2.2. Culturally Specific Environmental Socio-cognitive Schemas
5. Discussion and Conclusion
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Pro-Environmental Behavior [40] | ||
---|---|---|
1. | Would you be willing to pay higher consumer prices so that industry could better preserve and protect the environment? | yes, no |
In the past several years, have you: | ||
2. | Made any changes in your day-to-day behavior because of concerns about the environment? | yes, no |
3. | Contributed money to an environmental, conservation, or wildlife organization? | yes, no |
4. | Boycotted a company’s products because of its record on the environment? | yes, no |
5. | Volunteered for an environmental, conservation, or wildlife protection group? | yes, no |
To what extent do you agree or disagree with each of the following (SD =1, D=2, N=3, A=4, SA=5): | ||
6. | I would be willing to give up convenience products and services I now enjoy if it meant | 1–5 |
helping preserve our natural environment. | ||
7. | I would be willing to spend a few hours a week of my own time helping to reduce the | 1–5 |
pollution problem. | ||
Cultural Group [41] | ||
To what extent do you agree or disagree with each of the following (SD=1, D=2, N=3, A=4, SA=5): | ||
1. | A person is better off if he or she doesn’t trust anyone. | 1–5 |
2. | In a family adults and children should have the same influence in decisions. | 1–5 |
3. | When I have problems I try to solve them on my own. | 1–5 |
4. | There is no use in doing things for other people—you only get taken advantage of. | 1–5 |
5. | It is important to preserve our customs and cultural heritage. | 1–5 |
6. | Firms and institutions should be organized in a way that everybody can influence | 1–5 |
important decisions. | ||
7. | I would not participate in civic action groups. Those in power do what they like anyway. | 1–5 |
8. | I prefer clear instruction from my superiors about what to do | 1–5 |
9. | The freedom of the individual should not be limited for reasons for preventing crime. | 1–5 |
10. | It is important to me that in the case of important decisions at work everyone is asked. | 1–5 |
11. | I prefer tasks where I work something out on my own. | 1–5 |
12. | Order is probably an unpopular but important virtue. | 1–5 |
13. | Important questions for our society should not be decided upon by experts but by the people. | 1–5 |
14. | An intact family is the basis of a functioning society. | 1–5 |
Environment Identity [37] | ||
Think about how you view yourself in relationship to the natural environment and indicate where you would place yourself between each statement (1–5): | ||
1. | in competition with the environment….. in cooperation with the environment | 1–5 |
2. | detached from the environment….. connected to the environment | 1–5 |
3. | very concerned about the environment….. indifferent about the environment | 1–5 |
4. | very protective of the environment….. not at all protective of the environment | 1–5 |
5. | superior to the environment….. inferior to the environment | 1–5 |
6. | very passionate towards the environment….. not at all passionate towards the environment | 1–5 |
7. | not respectful of the environment….. very respectful of the environment | 1–5 |
8. | independent from the environment….. dependent on the environment | 1–5 |
9. | an advocate of the environment….. disinterested in the environment | 1–5 |
10. | wanting to preserve the environment….. wanting to utilize the environment | 1–5 |
11. | nostalgic thinking about the environment….. emotionless thinking about the environment | 1–5 |
Pro-Environmental Orientation [42] | ||
To what extent do you agree or disagree with each of the following (SD=1, D=2, N=3, A=4, SA=5): | ||
1. | We are approaching the limit of the number of people the earth can support. | 1–5 |
2. | Humans have the right to modify the natural environment to suit their needs. | 1–5 |
3. | When humans interfere with nature it often produces disastrous results. | 1–5 |
4. | Human ingenuity will insure that we do NOT make the earth unlivable. | 1–5 |
5. | Humans are severely abusing the environment. | 1–5 |
6. | The earth has plenty of natural resources if we just learn how to develop them. | 1–5 |
7. | Plants and animals exist primarily to be used by humans. | 1–5 |
8. | The balance of nature is strong enough to cope with the impacts of modern industrial nations. | 1–5 |
9. | Despite our special abilities humans are still subject to the laws of nature. | 1–5 |
10. | The so–called “ecological crisis” facing humankind has been greatly exaggerated. | 1–5 |
11. | The earth is like a spaceship with very limited room and resources. | 1–5 |
12. | Humans were meant to rule over the rest of nature. | 1–5 |
13. | The balance of nature is very delicate and easily upset. | 1–5 |
14. | Humans will eventually learn enough about how nature works to be able to control it. | 1–5 |
15. | If things continue on their present course, we will soon experience a major ecological catastrophe. | 1–5 |
References
- Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (IPCC). Special Report: Global Warming of 1.5 °C. 2018. Available online: https://www.ipcc.ch/sr15/ (accessed on 27 December 2018).
- Macnaghten, P. Embodying the environment in everyday life practices. Sociol. Rev. 2003, 51, 63–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Newholm, T. Case studying ethical consumers. In The Ethical Consumer; Harrison, R., Newholm, T., Shaw, D., Eds.; Sage: London, UK, 2005; pp. 107–124. [Google Scholar]
- Norgaard, K.M. Living in Denial: Climate Change, Emotions, and Everyday Life; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Jacques, P. The rearguard of modernity: Environmental skepticism as a struggle of citizenship. Glob. Environ. Politics 2006, 6, 76–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Armitage, K.C. State of denial: The United States and the politics of global warming. Globalizations 2005, 2, 417–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jacques, P.; Dunlap, R.; Freeman, M. The organization of denial: Conservative think tanks and environmental skepticism. Environ. Politics 2008, 17, 349–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCright, A.M.; Dunlap, R.K. Defeating Kyoto: The conservative movement’s impact on U.S. climate change policy. Soc. Probl. 2003, 50, 348–373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCright, A.M.; Dunlap, R.K. The politicization of climate change and polarization in the American public’s views of global warming, 2001–2010. Sociol. Quart. 2011, 52, 155–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hardin, G. The tragedy of the commons. Science 1968, 162, 1243–1248. [Google Scholar]
- Stern, P.C. Toward a coherent theory of environmentally significant behavior. J. Soc. Issues 2000, 56, 407–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schwartz, S.H. Normative influences on altruism. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology; Berkowitz, L., Ed.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1997; Volume 10, pp. 271–279. [Google Scholar]
- Fishbein, M.; Ajzen, I. Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Behavior; Addison-Wesley: Reading, MA, USA, 1975. [Google Scholar]
- Stern, P.C.; Dietz, T.; Abel, T.; Guagnano, G.A.; Kalof, L. A value-belief-norm theory of support for social movements: The case of environmental concern. Hum. Ecol. Rev. 1999, 6, 81–97. [Google Scholar]
- Haller, M.; Hadler, M. Dispositions to act in favor of the environment: Fatalism and readiness to make. sacrifices in a cross-national perspective. Sociol Forum 2008, 23, 281–311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goldman, M.; Shurman, R.A. ‘Closing the great divide’: New social theory on society and nature. Annu. Rev Sociol 2000, 26, 563–584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Swidler, A. Culture in action: Symbols and strategies. Am. Sociol. Rev. 1986, 51, 273–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Douglas, M.; Wildavsky, A. Risk and Culture: An Essay on the Selection of Technological and Environmental Dangers; University of California Press: Berkley, CA, USA, 1982. [Google Scholar]
- Dake, K. Myths of nature: Culture and the social construction of risk. J. Soc. Issues 1992, 48, 21–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dake, K. Orienting dispositions in the perception of risk: An analysis of contemporary worldviews and cultural biases. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 1991, 22, 61–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dake, K.; Thompson, M. The meanings of sustainable development: Household strategies for managing needs and resources. In Human Ecology: Crossing Boundaries; Wright, S.D., Dietz, T., Borden, R., Young, G., Guagnano, G., Eds.; The Society for Human Ecology: Fort Collins, CO, USA, 1993; pp. 421–436. [Google Scholar]
- Douglas, M. A History of Grid-Group Cultural Theory; University of Toronto Press: Toronto, ON, Canada, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Dake, K.; Thompson, M. Making ends meet, in the household and on the planet. GEOJ 1999, 47, 417–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ellis, R.J.; Thompson, M. Culture and the environment in the Pacific Northwest. Am. Political Sci. Rev. 1997, 91, 885–897. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martensson, M.; Pettersson, R. Everyday life contexts and the environment. In Individual and Structural Determinants of Environmental Practice; Biel, A., Hansson, B., Martensson, M., Eds.; Ashgate: Burlington, VT, USA, 2003; pp. 26–65. [Google Scholar]
- Steg, L.; Sievers, I. Cultural theory and individual perceptions of environmental risk. Environ. Behav. 2000, 32, 250–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Poortinga, W.; Steg, L.; Vlek, C. Environmental risk concern and preferences for energy-saving measures. Environ. Behav. 2002, 34, 455–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- DiMaggio, P. Culture and cognition. Ann. Rev. Sociol. 1997, 23, 263–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zerubavel, E. Social Mindscapes: An Invitation to Cognitive Sociology; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Berger, P.; Luckmann, T. The Social Construction of Reality: A Treatise on the Sociology of Knowledge; Doubleday: New York, NY, USA, 1966. [Google Scholar]
- Zerubavel, E.; Smith, E.R. Transcending cognitive individualism. Soc. Psychol. Quart. 2010, 73, 321–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zerubavel, E. The Elephant in the Room: Silence and Denial in Everyday Life; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Markle, G. Accounting for the performance of environmentally significant behavior: The symbolic significance of recycling. Symb. Interact. 2014, 37, 246–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Norgaard, K.M. ‘People want to protect themselves a little bit’: Emotions, denial, and social movement nonparticipation. Sociol. Inq. 2006, 76, 372–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Collier, P.; Callero, P. Role theory and social cognition: Learning to think like a recycler. Self Identity 2005, 4, 45–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Finger, M. From knowledge to action? Exploring the relationships between environmental experiences, learning, and behavior. J. Soc. Issues 1994, 50, 141–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stets, J.E.; Biga, C.F. Bringing identity theory into environmental sociology. Sociol. Theor. 2003, 21, 398–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wade-Benzoni, K.A.; Li, M.; Thompson, L.L.; Bazerman, M.H. The malleability of environmentalism. Anal. Soc. Issues Public Policy 2007, 7, 163–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- U.S. Census Bureau. Educational Attainment in the United States: 2015. Available online: https://www.census.gov/content/dam/Census/library/publications/2016/demo/p20-578.pdf (accessed on 21 January 2017).
- Dunlap, R.E.; Scarce, R. The polls—Poll trends: Environmental problems and protection. Public Opin. Quart. 1991, 55, 651–672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rippl, S. Cultural theory and risk perception: A proposal for a better measurement. J. Risk Res. 2002, 5, 147–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dunlap, R.E.; Van Liere, K.D.; Mertig, A.G.; Jones, R. Measuring endorsement of the New Ecological Paradigm: A revised NEP scale. J. Soc. Issues 2000, 56, 425–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- LaRossa, R. Grounded theory methods and qualitative family research. J. Marriage Fam. 2005, 67, 837–857. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hunter, L.M.; Hatch, A.; Johnson, A. Cross-national gender variation in environmental behaviors. Soc. Sci. Quart. 2004, 85, 677–694. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Douglas, M. The depoliticization of risk. In Culture Matters: Essays in Honor of Aaron Wildavsky; Ellis, R., Thompson, M., Eds.; Westview Press: Boulder, CO, USA, 1997; pp. 121–132. [Google Scholar]
- Thompson, M.; Ellis, R.; Wildavsky, A. Cultural Theory; Westview Press: Boulder, CO, USA, 1990. [Google Scholar]
- Kahan, D.M. More statistics, less persuasion: A cultural theory of gun-risk perceptions. Univ. Pa. Law Rev. 2003, 151, 1291–1327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hesse-Biber, S. Mixed Methods Research: Merging Theory with Practice; Guilford: New York, NY, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Creswell, J.W.; Plano Clark, V.L. Designing and Conducting Mixed Methods Research, 2nd ed.; Sage: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Gardner, G.T.; Stern, P.C. Environmental Problems and Human Behavior; Allyn and Bacon: Boston, MA, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Pew Research Center. Political Polarization in the American Public: How Increasing Ideological Uniformity and Partisan Antipathy Affect Politics, Compromise, and Everyday Life. 2016. Available online: http://www.people-press.org/2014/06/12/political-polarization-in-the-american-public/ (accessed on 5 November 2016).
Means and Standard Deviations | M | SD | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Pro-Environmental Behavior | 8.39 | 2.33 | ||
Pro-Environmental Orientation | 58.44 | 10.43 | ||
Environment Identity | 43.23 | 7.15 | ||
Frequencies | % | N | ||
Pro-Environmental Behavior | ||||
2–7 | 34.1 | 135 | ||
8–10 | 48.6 | 192 | ||
11–13 | 17.3 | 68 | ||
Pro-Environmental Orientation | ||||
15–44 | 16.5 | 65 | ||
45–59 | 49.9 | 197 | ||
60–75 | 33.6 | 133 | ||
Environment Identity | ||||
11–32 | 12.4 | 49 | ||
33–43 | 36.7 | 145 | ||
44–55 | 50.9 | 201 |
Pro-environmental Behavior | ||
---|---|---|
Independent Variable: | B | β |
Cultural Orientation | ||
Hierarchist | −0.563 * | −0.120 |
(0.315) | ||
Individualist | −0.796 ** | −0.164 |
(0.325) | ||
Pro-Environmental Orientation | 0.050 *** | 0.225 |
(0.014) | ||
Environment Identity | 0.124 *** | 0.375 |
(.020) | ||
Environmental Influence | 0.296 ** | 0.129 |
(0.106) | ||
Constant | −0.615 | |
N | 395 | |
R2 | 0.310 |
Culture | Culture | Mean Difference | SD | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Variable | (A) | (B) | (A–B) | Error |
Pro-Environmental | Egalitarian | Hierarchist | 2.10 *** | 0.268 |
Behavior | Egalitarian | Individualist | 1.43 *** | 0.280 |
Hierarchist | Individualist | −0.68 * | 0.233 | |
Pro-Environmental | Egalitarian | Hierarchist | 14.74 *** | 1.156 |
Orientation | Egalitarian | Individualist | 11.14 *** | 1.210 |
Hierarchist | Individualist | −3.60 ** | 1.006 | |
Environment | Egalitarian | Hierarchist | 8.81 *** | 1.065 |
Identity | Egalitarian | Individualist | 6.31 *** | 1.114 |
Hierarchist | Individualist | −0.50 * | 0.926 |
© 2019 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Markle, G. Understanding Pro-Environmental Behavior in the US: Insights from Grid-Group Cultural Theory and Cognitive Sociology. Sustainability 2019, 11, 532. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11020532
Markle G. Understanding Pro-Environmental Behavior in the US: Insights from Grid-Group Cultural Theory and Cognitive Sociology. Sustainability. 2019; 11(2):532. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11020532
Chicago/Turabian StyleMarkle, Gail. 2019. "Understanding Pro-Environmental Behavior in the US: Insights from Grid-Group Cultural Theory and Cognitive Sociology" Sustainability 11, no. 2: 532. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11020532
APA StyleMarkle, G. (2019). Understanding Pro-Environmental Behavior in the US: Insights from Grid-Group Cultural Theory and Cognitive Sociology. Sustainability, 11(2), 532. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11020532