Next Article in Journal
Organization Mechanisms and Spatial Characteristics of Urban Collaborative Innovation Networks: A Case Study in Hangzhou, China
Next Article in Special Issue
Are Corporate Social Responsibility Courses Effective? A Longitudinal and Gender-Based Analysis in Undergraduate Students
Previous Article in Journal
Environmental Criteria for Assessing the Competitiveness of Public Tenders with the Replacement of Large-Scale LEDs in the Outdoor Lighting of Cities as a Key Element for Sustainable Development: Case Study Applied with PROMETHEE Methodology
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

How Does Reciprocity Affect Undergraduate Student Orientation towards Stakeholders?

by
José-Luis Godos-Díez
*,
Roberto Fernández-Gago
and
Laura Cabeza-García
Department of Business Administration, Universidad de León, Campus de Vegazana s/n, 24071 León, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sustainability 2019, 11(21), 5987; https://doi.org/10.3390/su11215987
Submission received: 6 September 2019 / Revised: 16 October 2019 / Accepted: 23 October 2019 / Published: 28 October 2019

Abstract

:
Nowadays, students are more aware of the impact of companies on their stakeholders and the need for properly handling their expectations to operationalize corporate social responsibility. Nevertheless, little is known about how certain individual traits may relate to their stance on the issue. This exploratory research contributes to stakeholder theory by analysing the effect of the individual’s decision-making process, including the consideration of their social preferences, on their orientation toward stakeholder management. Here, we draw upon a theoretical model for resource-allocation decision-making consisting of reciprocal and non-reciprocal components. Our data, from undergraduate students enrolled in different degrees, were collected through a questionnaire and two social within-subject experiments (ultimatum and dictator games). Thus, our results show that the presence of a reciprocal component when decisions are made is positively linked to an instrumental orientation toward stakeholders. In addition, a greater non-reciprocal component in the decision-making process corresponds to a more normative orientation.

1. Introduction

There is growing pressure all around the world on business leaders to manage sustainably and responsibly and some international business schools have responded by trying to provide their students with the required skills [1,2]. How to do this is not an easy task, but it should be based on good knowledge of the students’ attitudes towards the topic to be taught.
Students have shown a willingness to upgrade ethics and sustainability when they move from the description of present practice to expectations of what business should be doing [3]. It has also been detected a certain evolution in favour of a greater expectation of sustainability among the younger demographic [4]. In this sense, recent research on students belonging to the millennial generation have evidenced their sensitiveness to sustainability concerns and ethical issues [5,6,7], which may be reflected in their career decisions [8].
Nevertheless, it is important not only knowing the value students give to sustainable and responsible business practices but also finding out why they believe that they are valuable in the first place. Stakeholders management is a basic component of student understanding of responsible management, mainly due to its contribution to a holistic consciousness of the decision-making consequences (economic, environmental, and social) [9]. Ditlev-Simonsen and Midttun [3] found that students consider that stakeholders should be a key motivator for managers pursuing corporate social responsibility (CSR) but they did not go further to analyse if there was an instrumental or a normative reason for that. Additionally, little is known about how individual characteristics of the students may be related to their perception of responsible management. On the basis of previous work [10], student social and moral values and their ethical orientations in terms of “self” versus “others” have been linked to attitudes supportive of sustainable practices [11]. In this work we turned to behavioural economics in order to know some psychological traits of the students connected to their moral principles, which could explain to a certain degree why they believe stakeholder interests should be attended and address in this way the aforementioned research gap.
Proposing a management model that takes into account how making decisions affects others brings stakeholder theory closer to the social utility model [12]. The ultimatum and dictator games were applied on a sample of undergraduate students in a Spanish university to find evidence for a psychological explanation of the orientation an individual adopts with regard to stakeholders. Thus, the effect of potential reciprocity and social preference related to inequality aversion were tested to see if they have any influence on the orientation towards stakeholders from the instrumental and normative points of view. To our knowledge, there have been no previous empirical studies on this. The use of a sample of university students can contribute to the academic debate on the ethical development of students [13,14] and continues the research trend of experimental or quasi-experimental studies [15,16,17,18].
This research may contribute to the existing literature on stakeholder theory because it adopts an individual level of analysis, emphasising the importance of behavioural motives [19] and of talking about human beings [20], as they are the cornerstone of ethical decision-making [21]. In this sense, by focusing on micro-foundations, the complexity of human psychology is taken into account in order to build stakeholder theory on a more solid foundation [22,23]. We are thus in line with the proposal of Bridoux and Stoelthorst [24], which “replaces the traditional assumption of economic theory that all human behaviour is exclusively motivated by self-interest with less pessimistic assumptions derived from empirical research on human motivation”. Finally, discovering the psychological triggers behind management decisions with relation to company stakeholders may help firms to adopt control and management mechanisms guaranteeing that such relations are maintained within the guidelines set at corporate level.
The rest of the paper is structured as follows. Section 2 draws up the research hypotheses based on a review of stakeholder theory and social preferences. The data, method and results are described in Section 3. Finally, Section 4 concludes.

2. Theoretical Framework

2.1. Stakeholder Approach

Stakeholder theory adopts a view of business whereby all the groups and individuals that might affect or be affected by the achievement of business objectives are taken into account [25]. Such stakeholders include, among others, employees, suppliers, customers, governments, environmental groups, the media and even competitors, giving place to a multiple stakeholder orientation context [26]. This theoretical framework can be considered a key approach in order to study the long-term effects of business decisions on environmental and social issues [27]. Thus, considering a company as a network of stakeholders goes beyond the traditional business view in which almost the only concern was to protect shareholder interests [28].
Donaldson and Preston [29] consider that stakeholder theory has developed from three alternative but related approaches: the descriptive, the instrumental, and the normative approaches. Firstly, this theory can be applied to describe and explain the specific characteristics and behaviour of companies. It can also be conceived as instruments to achieve traditional business objectives. Finally, the core of this theory is normative, in that the interests of all stakeholders are accepted as having an intrinsic value and that a certain moral obligation exists towards them. We shall discuss below the instrumental and normative approaches in greater depth in order to establish the foundations for the hypotheses.
The instrumental view of stakeholder management proposes that meeting the demands of relevant stakeholders has a positive impact on the company’s performance [30,31]. Mutual trust and cooperation with stakeholders will tend to reduce transaction costs and to generate a competitive advantage [32]. Companies may benefit from a better reputation or greater social legitimacy because the stakeholders’ positive perceptions may help increase sales or reduce the cost of managing relations with them [33]. The empirical evidence, however, does not lead to the conclusion that stakeholder management is the optimum strategy for maximising a company’s financial performance from the traditional point of view [29]. For example, although stakeholder management practices may have positive effects on shareholder value over the long run, negative effects are commonly shown in the short run [34]. Moreover, using corporate resources for social issues that are not related to primary stakeholders may not generate value for shareholders [35]. Furthermore, the need to consider the many objectives of the various stakeholders goes against the notion that a company must have a single objective function that guides its managers in the process of investing the available resources [36,37].
The final positive effect of stakeholder management on business financial performance is irrelevant for the purpose of this research. What is really important here is whether managers consider it necessary to take stakeholders into account in business decisions just because they may be able to influence a company’s success. This circumstance is what really distinguishes the instrumental approach to stakeholder theory from the normative approach discussed below.
The normative nature of stakeholder theory rejects the existence of an exclusive fiduciary obligation towards shareholders as proposed by the agency theory [38], or at least considers that there will be similar obligations towards other stakeholders because the results of the company’s success or failure must be distributed among all the legitimate stakeholders [39]. Freeman [40] states that all groups should participate in decisions in order to determine the future of a company or an initiative in which they have some type of stake. This right to participate is the consequence of the obligation to not treat stakeholders only as the means for achieving an end and is consistent with the categorical imperative of Immanuel Kant [41]. The obligations to distribute the value created by the company among all its stakeholders, and include them in business decisions, are the fundamental normative implications of stakeholder theory [42], aiming to foster a relational view of society [43]. In consequence, this normative interpretation of the business function by stakeholder theory is based on the identification of moral or philosophical guidelines [29], since ethical and social values affect business decisions and behaviours [44]. In any case, the various constraints on firms and managers that may limit their freedom to follow certain moral prescriptions must never be disregarded [39].
These two approaches towards stakeholder management can be illustrated by a specific situation involving the interests of a particular stakeholder: work-life balance for employees. While work-life balance practices can be used as an instrument in order to improve organisational performance through less absenteeism and lateness, lower staff turnover rates and stronger employee commitment, among other factors [45], those same practices may be perceived as the ethical behaviour of managers respecting the legitimate interests of the employees [46].

2.2. Social Preferences

Traditional economic models assume that the behaviour of individuals is totally determined by self-interest. However, many social experiments have shown that people are prepared to incur personal costs to allow for a distribution of resources that they consider generally more desirable, rewarding those who act in a cooperative way and even punishing those who do not [47]. So human behaviour does not depend, at least not exclusively, on self-interest but may be determined by the social preferences of the decision-maker.
Amongst the motives for distribution decisions that do not tie in with the utility-maximising homo economicus model are: (1) inequality aversion among recipients; (2) efficiency, or the desire that the total amount to be distributed should be as large as possible; and (3) an interest in the largest possible quantity going to whoever is worst remunerated [48]. We shall focus on the first one, that is, inequality aversion, which will be later analysed attending to the role played by reciprocity. This factor assumes a social preference for fair results, with the individual being prepared to sacrifice part of the reward received in a given situation if this will make it more similar to what is received by the other participants [49,50]. Along these lines, the social experiments performed using the ultimatum game [51] and the dictator game [52] have traditionally served to detect and study this sort of social preferences in decision-making.
In the ultimatum game, one player receives an amount of money to be shared with another. If the latter accepts the offer, the money is allocated as planned but if he rejects it, neither of the players receives anything. Based on the self-interest assumption, the player that receives the offer should always accept it providing it is positive. Consequently, whoever acts first should propose the smallest positive amount allowed by the game. In the dictator game, the first player distributes the money and the other participant has no say in it. The expected offer by the first player now should take the smallest possible amount.
In spite of the above, the final behaviour of the participants in both experiments is surprisingly different to what is expected and the amounts offered by the first player are far from minimal [53]. This may be because the first player is averse to the unfairness of distributing the money in a way that is seen as unequal [54]. However, it is also true that this argument is insufficient for explaining why the amount offered is significantly larger in the ultimatum game than in the dictator game. As the offer becomes increasingly more unfair, the frequency of rejection by the second player in the ultimatum game increases [55,56]. This tendency to reject small offers means that in the ultimatum game the first player changes his proposal, that is, he takes into account the reciprocity that his decision might generate. So it is the recipient’s concern for fair distribution of an amount that finally determines the result of the ultimatum experiment [49]. The recipient’s inequality aversion will be taken into account by the proponent and it will be the trust or fear that the offer will be accepted that determines the amount.

2.3. Hypotheses

Drawing upon the previously mentioned empirical results of social experiments, and in order to better understand how individuals make decisions, a bounded rationality model for human-beings [57] seems to be more appropriate than the classical homo economicus model. We are talking now about individuals who try to maximize their own self-interest while respecting certain norms about fair procedures and outcome distribution [12,57].
Thus, such norms of fairness appear to play a key role in explaining individual conduct to the extent that it is widely recognized that the behaviour of economic actors are triggered primarily by their perceptions of fairness [19,58]. Among these social and/or moral norms, reciprocity emerges as a particularly relevant one [24,59], involving the mutual reinforcement by individuals of each other’s action [60]. Thus, considering a resource-allocation decision that has to be made by a specific individual, two components of such a decision can be identified: a non-reciprocal and a reciprocal one. Firstly, with regard to the non-reciprocal component, it can be inferred that part of the allocated resources will depend on the decision-maker’s own consideration of what a fair distribution of resources is. In other words, there will be a moral minimum amount of resources that an individual will always be willing to share or to give, no matter the context [12]. Secondly, actors are motivated by reciprocity in order to maintain the balance in their relationships [24] and how their actions may be perceived in the light of the others’ underlying norms, beliefs, and rules will be of key importance [19]. People reciprocate to the way they are treated by returning similar treatment [61] and anticipating that reaction can help to develop harmonious relationships [24].
Turning back to the description of the social experiments, the proposal made by the first player in an ultimatum game may reflect a self-centred motivation that is consistent with interest in maximising his own gains, because fairness in his offer may be partly due to fear that a lower offer might be rejected by the other player [62]. In the dictator case, however, since there is no possibility of rejection, fear as a motivation or trigger disappears and distributive justice prevails in the decision [63]. Therefore, if the two types of experiment are used simultaneously, it should be possible to estimate both the reciprocal and the non-reciprocal components.
Firstly, the greater the discrepancy between the offers made in the ultimatum and in the dictator games, the greater the reciprocal component will be in the first move [64]. This posture by an individual assumes there is a greater consideration of others to the extent that this might affect the reward he himself will receive. If this personal position is adopted with regard to a company’s stakeholders, it seems to correspond to an instrumental vision of them, whereby their interests will be taken into account because this will lead to a reward for the company [29]. This relation is reflected in the following hypothesis:
Hypothesis 1 (H1).
The reciprocal component in decision-making will contribute positively to a greater instrumental orientation towards stakeholders.
In addition, in the dictator case, the player’s decision is inspired by the non-reciprocal component because there is no dependence on what the other party does [63]. The presence of this component in an individual is consistent with a normative approach to stakeholders whereby there is a duty to recognise their legitimate rights to participate in the distribution of the company’s gains [39]. The following hypothesis is therefore posed:
Hypothesis 2 (H2).
The non-reciprocal component in decision-making will contribute positively to a greater normative orientation towards stakeholders.
Figure 1 shows the relations proposed in the hypotheses.

3. Analysis and Results

3.1. Sample

The data were collected by means of questionnaires and the experiments described in the following sub-section. A total of 212 second-year undergraduate students from three different faculties of a Spanish university participated. Only the individuals who took the role of first player in the ultimatum and dictator games were taken into account, that is, those who proposed a distribution of the received amount of money. Therefore, the final number of observations was 106. The students were distributed as follows: 56 were studying in the Faculty of Economic and Business Science, 30 in the Faculty of Education and 20 in the Faculty of Philosophy and Arts. Average age of the students was 20.8 years, 75.5% of them were female and 25.5% had previous working experience.

3.2. Design and Implementation of the Experiments

Our research proposal was coordinated with the Vice-Rectorate for Research, where issues related to research involving students were addressed, and previous authorisation by the Faculties involved was granted. In order to avoid the selection bias that might arise when the volunteers used come to a laboratory with economic motivations in response to a call for volunteers [65], the within-subject experiments were performed in classrooms without any advance warning being given to the students nor any prior indication that there would be payment. Nevertheless, informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study so that all students participated in the experiments voluntarily.
The social experiments were designed and performed taking maximum care to define the process in order to guarantee maximum quality in the information obtained and homogeneous replication. For experiments performed in the classroom as in this case, the ways in which the materials are distributed and collected, how confidentiality is guaranteed and information leaks avoided are crucial [65]. The experiments were therefore conducted following exactly the same patterns and at the same time to ensure that participants could not receive any information as to their content. In addition, since the fact that the experimenter was a teacher and therefore a figure of authority who might affect participants’ decisions if they feel judged [66], the tests were performed with groups of students with whom the experimenters had no relation.
Before the experiments began, the students in the classrooms were divided into two equal groups and the roles of first and second player were assigned to the students in each group at random. Even though this amounted to a loss of data, it was decided that each participant should have a single role in the experiments, to prevent the first players from being influenced in their distribution decision by the fact that it would then be them who would receive the offer. The students were then informed that they were going to be paired with another person who was in another of the various rooms being used and that they would never know who that person was either during the experiment or after it, and that that person would never know who had been his or her partner. We thus avoided the effect that a lack of anonymity can have on decisions [67]. At no time could students comment on their decisions with their peers.
The instructions handed out to the participants informed them that they were about to participate in two economic within-subject experiments (ultimatum and dictator) and that, as a result, they would receive the amount resulting from one of the two games chosen at random. The aim was to find out which decision was adopted by each individual that was to take the role of first player in both experiments, that is, to find out and compare their distribution offer in an ultimatum and dictator situation without being influenced by knowing which of the two would determine the final reward.
The starting amount used in both experiments was €10 and each participant also received a set payment of €3 for participating. A result that has been observed in previous studies is the small effect that the amount used for distribution has on the decisions of both players [68]. However, Andersen et al. [69] performed their experiment in poor towns in Northeast India with amounts between 20 and 20,000 rupees (equivalent to 1600 h of work), which amounted to a greater difference than those in previous experiments, and they noted that in the ultimatum game the amount offered increased as the amount available increased but the percentage of the offer decreased as did the probability of rejection of the offer received. Cherry et al. [70] did not find relevant differences in the offers in the case of the dictator game when they raised the amount from $10 to $40, but Carpenter et al. [71] did detect a reduction of 33–25% in the average offered when the amount rose from $10 to $100.

3.3. Measurement of Variables

Dependent variable: Firstly, a brief explanation of the stakeholder concept was provided for those who were not familiar with it. In order to identify the instrumental and normative orientations to stakeholders (INSTRUMENTAL and NORMATIVE), as covered respectively by the two hypotheses posed, the following two items measured on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = Totally disagree; 7 = Totally agree) were used:
“Firms should take into account their stakeholders’ interests only when such groups may affect the firms’ activities and goals now or in the future”.
“Firms should always take into account their stakeholders’ interests because that is the right thing to do, even when such groups are not able to affect the firms’ activities and goals now or in the future”.
Independent variable: The independent variable used to test the first hypothesis (RECIPROCAL) aimed to find how much of the inequality offered by the first player in the ultimatum experiment corresponds to his interest in achieving a positive response to his offer from the other player and not to his aversion to unfair distribution or the search for fairness, that is, the reciprocal component in his decision. It was calculated, therefore, by subtracting from the amount offered by an individual in an ultimatum situation the amount proposed in the dictator case [64].
The independent variable corresponding to the second hypothesis (NON-RECIPROCAL) aims to measure the intention of an individual in a dictator experiment to share with an unknown person, fairly and avoiding inequality, the money he has just received while knowing that the other person has no possibility of rejection however the money is distributed. The value of this variable corresponded to the amount that the first player actually handed over in the dictator experiment.
Control variables: The analyses included three control variables for individual factors.
Firstly, the training received by the respondent was identified by a dummy variable (ECON_EDUCATION) to know if the person had previously studied or was currently studying economics and business management, in which case the variable took value 1, otherwise it took 0. This variable was included because previous works emphasize the potential effect of economics or business education on the students’ moral reasoning and decision-making [72,73]. For instance, economics students have shown a greater non-cooperative behaviour, such as more freeriding or less charitable giving [74,75], and it has also been found that they are significantly more corrupt than others [76] and that graduate business students cheat more than their non-business peers in order to achieve their goals [77]. Specifically, these findings can be explained by the fact that most business programmes reinforce a “norm of self-interest” [78], instilling the belief that everyone intends to maximise personal material outputs [79].
Secondly, gender (GENDER) was also measured with a dummy variable, taking 1 for men and 0 for women. Many articles focused on student attitudes and perceptions on CSR and/or stakeholders have considered gender as a relevant explanatory variable (e.g., [80,81,82]). Although different effects have been found, most studies conclude that women tend to exhibit greater sensitivity to CSR [83], have stronger ethical orientation [80], and attach more importance to environmental issues [81]. These results are consistent with the ethics of care [84], which proposes that women speak and act in the language of caring and responsibilities. This means that, in the context of a relationship, women are likely to believe that all parties in the relationship have responsibilities [85].
Finally, the models also included a dummy variable (EXPERIENCE) indicating if the respondent had work experience, in which case it took value 1 and 0 otherwise. This variable has been included as it can affect a person’s ethical judgement [86,87], being expected in this case that the more experienced students were more ethically oriented [88]. A possible explanation for this could be that, due to their own work experience, they see themselves more as a stakeholder in the business context and may therefore value the consideration of stakeholders’ claims beyond instrumental motives [89].

3.4. Results

Table 1 and Table 2 give the main descriptive statistics and the correlation coefficients for the variables used in the study. Although some of them show a statistically significant correlation, analysis of the variance inflation factors (VIF) showed that there was no evidence of multicollinearity because in no case were the VIF above 10 [90]. It is worth mentioning that the amounts offered by the first participant in the experiments were not the lowest amounts possible but on average were €4.5 in the ultimatum and €3.2 in the dictator case, with the respective medians being €5 and €4. It can also be observed that the instrumental approach to stakeholders obtained on average (5.4) a higher value than the normative approach (4.5).
Table 3 shows the results of the regression analyses applied to test the hypotheses and relations, controlling for the respondents’ training in business management, gender and work experience. It is worth mentioning that we repeated the analysis taking into account the possibility of a heteroskedasticity problem and the results did not vary.
As the table shows, in the regression analysis of the extra amount offered in the ultimatum experiment over the money shared in the dictator game, which represented the reciprocal component of the decision, on the instrumental approach towards stakeholders the coefficient was positive and statistically significant (β = 0.166; p < 0.1), thus confirming the first hypothesis. In the second regression, it was found that the amount delivered in the dictator experiment, which reflected the decision-makers non-reciprocal component, was positively and significantly related to the normative approach to stakeholders (β = 0.160; p < 0.1), as proposed in the second hypothesis.
Regarding the control variables, gender was marginally significant to the instrumental approach (β = 0.195; p < 0.1), so that being a man seems to have a positive influence on this approach towards stakeholders. In addition, having received training in economics and business management had a negative impact on the normative approach (β = −0.285; p < 0.01).

4. Conclusions

Awareness about the social and environmental impact of business organisations beyond their economic role has increased in the last two decades. In particular, young people have shown a special sensitiveness to sustainability concerns and ethical issues in relation to business practices. Previous works on university students have evidenced the significance they give to stakeholder management within this context and this study contributes by including social preferences in the research on their stakeholder approach. With that purpose in mind, social experiments were carried out to be subsequently related to the participants’ orientation towards stakeholders.
The ultimatum and dictator experiments corroborated the presence of decisions that are not optimal for the monetary incentives given. The respective proposals about how to split the sum of money given to the first players were 45% and 32%. Although this context can be characterised by a low social distance and it could have positively conditioned the offers made by participants [91], our results are in line with the average values obtained by other studies applying within-subject social experiments in different contexts [68,92]. On average, players in this standard version of the ultimatum game offer about 40–50% of the total assigned, and these amounts are almost always accepted, while the rate of rejection increases for offers below 20% [68]. The average percentage offered in the dictator game is about 30%, and almost 65% of players shared the money with the other player [92]. The fact that there were no minimal offers in the dictator case assumes there is a concern for fair distribution, and the fact that more was offered in the ultimatum adds the reciprocal component that stems from considering the possibility of a reprisal or rejection by the other player if the amount proposed is perceived as inappropriate.
The reciprocal and non-reciprocal components in decision-making may be present when the need and legitimacy of meeting company stakeholders’ interests are considered. Our results indicate that the greater weight of the concern for the other party’s response to the payment granted may to some extent justify a greater instrumental approach towards stakeholders. The greater importance of the non-reciprocal component corresponds to a more normative approach with a greater moral implication regarding stakeholders.
Analysis of the control variables gave two main results. Firstly, it was detected that, although at a marginally significant level, men place greater value than women on the need to consider stakeholders according to their capacity for affecting the company results. That is, they valued more an instrumental stakeholder approach, although no gender effect on the normative approach was found. Prior empirical studies have shown that women tend to be more aware of their obligations towards the various stakeholders [93] and to be more sensitive to others than men [94]. Secondly, it was also found that training in economics and business management has a negative effect on the normative stakeholder approach [89]. To the extent that the normative approach is ethics-based, this result is in line with some of the criticisms made of training in business management (e.g., [95,96]) claiming that it is based on amoral theories that give maximum priority to profit, omitting to provide moral development to managers and leading to questionable ethics in their companies.
Some policy implications for management education can be drawn from the overall conclusions of this research about the relevance of individual characteristics when dealing with stakeholders and the differential effect of training in business and economics. We must keep in mind that, as a social science, what is taught in business studies is particularly relevant since theories can become self-fulfilling [97], in that they might influence the phenomena studied, and even make them true because a management theory can change the behaviour of managers, who start acting in accordance with the theory [72]. If educators wish their students contribute to building delightful organisations [98], they should teach them how they can do so [99].
Helping students to become more socially responsible and ethically sensitive is a significant part of the responsibility of business schools, who need to incorporate a more humanistic vision of the world [79,100]. Moreover, admitting a broader perspective of human nature in business management would help to come up with new ways of facing complex and turbulent situations [99].
In order to change business learning and education in this direction, academic programs should draw upon humanistic management postulates [101] and move from an organisation-centered towards a human-centered worldview [79]. Taking into account both notions, a people-oriented management education should emphasise the dignity of each person and the preeminence of people over material goods [102,103] and teach students to explore the multifaceted (not simply economic) repercussions of their decisions, forcing them to look at what is really happening as a result of such decisions [79]. As reflected by Trencher et al. [104], the ability to anticipate and envision differing types of harmful or desirable consequences in the future is essential for empowering societal change agents and required for sustainable development.
In this sense, management education should focus more on value-oriented skills [79] and aim at teaching students to think critically and act responsibly as managers, as employees, as customers, as citizens, and so on [101]. To develop such a macro-competence, students should be equipped with multiple theories, supplementing neoclassical economics with other economic perspectives (such as ecological and feminist economics) and diverse views from other disciplines (such as sociology or positive psychology) on economic behaviour [105]. Moreover, social experiments and resource-allocation contexts, as designed in the present research, may provide an opportunity to better align different courses or academic programs with societal needs, to the extent that they help simulate real-world contexts and stakeholder-thinking, beyond an instrumental approach.
Following the previous comments, to the extent that education is a relevant force in shaping students’ attitudes and perceptions, it could be interesting to include other forces, particularly at an institutional level, in further research. In this sense, studies with samples composed of students from different countries or regions might be relevant to analyse the potential influence of cultural factors. Previous empirical research has shown that these factors contribute to generate different attitudes and perceptions on stakeholders and CSR issues (e.g., [15,106,107,108]). Specifically, the cultural dimension of individualism-collectivism [109] may be relevant since more collectivist individuals have been found to be more concerned with society and the environment and to hold more socially responsible attitudes [110,111]. In particular, with business students as their unit of analysis, Ng and Burke [11] found that more collectivist individuals reported more positive attitudes toward sustainable business practices.
The line of research opened up here could be also broadened and enriched by taking into account the specific characteristics that may be present in the relation between a company and its stakeholders [112]. For instance, access to valuable but limited resources by any of the parties involved could be included as this determines the degree of power available according to social exchange theory [113] and the balance or imbalance of power in their relations. The ultimatum and dictator experiments are clearly different at this level of balance and power assignation, with greater compensation in the first case and absolute dependence for the second player in the dictator game. A specific study could therefore be carried out, comparing the approach towards specific stakeholders depending on the power attributed to them for influencing the company. Van Dijk and Vermunt [63] observed that the lack of power may increase the fairness motivation, in such a way that perceiving that others are more dependent for obtaining rewards generates a feeling of responsibility towards them and encourages an effort to promote their interests, even if there is no mutual reciprocity.
Finally, our study analyses only the negative utility arising when there are differences between the reward received by a person and what others receive, but other issues such as the maximisation of joint efficiency or consideration of the party that receives least in a distribution could also be included [48]. These issues may also be related to the decision on how the relation with the various stakeholders should be managed, which would be another direction for expanding this line of research.

Author Contributions

The three authors were involved in the methodological part of the paper. R.F.-G. and J.-L.G.-D. worked on the conceptualization and theoretical framework of the paper and reflected on its implications. L.C.-G. supervised the theoretical framework, built up the database and carried out the empirical analyses.

Funding

This research was funded by the Junta de Castilla y León (grant number LE004A10-1), Spain.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Waddock, S.; Lozano, J. Developing more holistic management education: Lessons learned from two programs. Acad. Manag. Learn. Educ. 2013, 12, 265–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Crossan, M.; Mazutis, D.; Seijts, G.; Gandz, J. Developing leadership character in business programs. Acad. Manag. Learn. Educ. 2013, 12, 285–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Ditlev-Simonsen, C.D.; Midttun, A. What motivates managers to pursue corporate responsibility? A survey among key stakeholders. Corp. Soc. Responsib. Environ. Manag. 2011, 18, 25–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Diamontopoulos, A.; Schelegelmilch, B.B.; Sinkovics, R.R.; Bohlen, G.M. Can socio-demographics still play a role in profiling green consumers? A review of the evidence and an empirical investigation. J. Bus. Res. 2003, 56, 465–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Bollani, L.; Bonadonna, A.; Peira, G. The Millennials′ concept of sustainability in the food sector. Sustainability 2019, 11, 2984. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Bell, N.S.; Connell, J.A.; McMinn, N.E. How will the next generation change the business world? A report on a survey. Insights A Chang. World J. 2011, 1, 45–55. [Google Scholar]
  7. Deloitte. Big Demands and High Expectations The Deloitte Millennial Survey Executive Summary. 2014. Available online: https://www2.deloitte.com/content/dam/Deloitte/global/Documents/About-Deloitte/gx-dttl-2014-millennial-survey-report.pdf (accessed on 11 October 2019).
  8. Klimkiewicz, K.; Oltra, V. Does CSR enhance employer attractiveness? The role of millennial job seekers’ attitudes. Corp. Soc. Responsib. Environ. Manag. 2017, 24, 449–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Nonet, G.; Kassel, K.; Meijs, L. Understanding responsible management: Emerging themes and variations from European business school programs. J. Bus. Ethics 2016, 139, 717–736. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Rokeach, M. The Nature of Human Values; Free Press: New York, NY, USA, 1973. [Google Scholar]
  11. Ng, E.; Burke, R. Predictor of business students’ attitudes toward sustainable business practices. J. Bus. Ethics 2010, 95, 603–615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Harrison, J.S.; Bosse, D.A.; Phillips, R.A. Managing for stakeholders, stakeholder utility functions, and competitive advantage. Strateg. Manag. J. 2010, 31, 58–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Borkowski, S.C.; Ugras, Y.J. Business students and Ethics: A meta-analysis. J. Bus. Ehtics 1998, 18, 1117–1127. [Google Scholar]
  14. Hummel, K.; Pfaff, D.; Rost, K. Does Economics and Business education wash away moral judgment competence? J. Bus. Ethics 2018, 150, 559–577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Ahmed, M.M.; Chung, K.Y.; Eichenseher, J.W. Business students’ perception on Ethics and moral judgment: A cross-cultural study. J. Bus. Ethics 2003, 43, 89–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Burton, S.; Johnston, M.W.; Wilson, E.J. An experimental assessment of alternative teaching approaches for introducing business ethics to undergraduate business students. J. Bus. Ethics 1991, 10, 507–517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Lawson, R.A. Is classroom cheating related to business students’ propensity to cheat in the “real world”? J. Bus. Ethics 2004, 49, 189–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Tang, L.; Peytcheva, M.; Li, P. Investor-paid ratings and conflicts of interest. J. Bus. Ethics 2018, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Hahn, T. Reciprocal stakeholder behavior: A motive-based approach to the implementation of normative stakeholder demands. Bus. Soc. 2015, 54, 9–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Freeman, R.E.; Harrison, J.S.; Wicks, A.C.; Parmar, B.L.; de Colle, S. Stakeholder Theory: The State of the Art; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  21. Ferrell, O.C.; Gresham, L.G. A contingency framework for understanding ethical decision making in marketing. J. Mark. 1985, 49, 87–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Bridoux, F.; Stoelhorst, J.W. Microfoundations for stakeholder theory: Managing stakeholders with heterogeneous motives. Strateg. Manag. J. 2014, 35, 107–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Felin, T.; Foss, N.J. Strategic organization: A field in search of micro-foundations. Strateg. Organ. 2005, 3, 441–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Bridoux, F.; Stoelhorst, J.W. Stakeholder relationships and social welfare: A behavioral theory of contributions to joint value creation. Acad. Manag. Rev. 2016, 41, 229–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Freeman, R.E. Strategic Management: A Stakeholder Approach; Pitman Publishing: Boston, MA, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  26. Greenley, G.E.; Hooley, G.J.; Rudd, J.M. Market orientation in a multiple stakeholder orientation context: Implications for marketing capabilities and assets. J. Bus. Res. 2005, 58, 1483–1494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Sharma, S.; Henriques, I. Stakeholder influences on sustainability practices in the Canadian forest products industry. Strateg. Manag. J. 2005, 26, 159–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Friedman, M. Capitalism and Freedom; Chicago University Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  29. Donaldson, T.; Preston, L. The stakeholder theory of the corporation: Concepts, evidence, and implications. Acad. Manag. Rev. 1995, 20, 65–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Mitchell, R.K.; Agle, B.R.; Wood, D.J. Toward a theory of stakeholder identification and salience: Defining the principle of who and what really counts. Acad. Manag. Rev. 1997, 22, 853–886. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Egels-Zandén, N.; Sandberg, J. Distinctions in descriptive and instrumental stakeholder theory: A challenge for empirical research. Bus. Ethics A Eur. Rev. 2010, 19, 35–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Jones, T.M. Instrumental stakeholder theory: A synthesis of ethics and economics. Acad. Manag. Rev. 1995, 20, 404–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Waddock, S.; Graves, S. The corporate social performance-financial performance link. Strateg. Manag. J. 1997, 18, 303–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. García-Castro, R.; Ariño, M.A.; Canela, M.A. Over the long-run? Short-run impact and long-run consequences of stakeholder management. Bus. Soc. 2011, 50, 428–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Hillman, A.J.; Keim, G.D. Shareholder value, stakeholder management, and social issues: What’s the bottom line? Strateg. Manag. J. 2001, 22, 125–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Cennamo, C.; Berrone, P.; Gomez-Mejia, L.R. Does stakeholder management have a dark side? J. Bus. Ethics 2009, 89, 491–507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Jensen, M. Value maximization, stakeholder theory, and the corporate objective function. Bus. Ethics Q. 2002, 12, 235–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Jensen, M.; Meckling, W. Theory of the firm: Managerial behavior, agency costs, and ownership structure. J. Financ. Econ. 1976, 3, 305–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Phillips, R.A.; Freeman, R.E.; Wicks, A.C. What stakeholder theory is not? Bus. Ethics Q. 2003, 13, 479–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Freeman, R.E. Stakeholder theory of the modern corporation. In Ethical Issues in Business: A Philosophical Approach; Donaldson, T., Werhane, P.H., van Zandt, J.D., Eds.; Prentice Hall: Englewood Cliffs, NJ, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
  41. Hill, T.E. Humanity as an end in itself. Ethics 2002, 91, 84–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Hasnas, J. Whither stakeholder theory? A guide for the perplexed revisited. J. Bus. Ethics 2013, 112, 47–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Garriga Cots, E. Stakeholder social capital: A new approach to stakeholder theory. Bus. Ethics A Eur. Rev. 2011, 20, 328–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Hood, J.N.; Logsdon, J.M. Business ethics in the NAFTA countries: A cross-cultural comparison. J. Bus. Res. 2002, 55, 883–890. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Lazar, I.; Osoian, C.; Ratiu, P. The role of work-life balance practices in order to improve organizational performance. Eur. Res. Stud. J. 2010, 13, 201–214. [Google Scholar]
  46. Cowart, T.; Gilley, A.; Avery, S.; Barber, A.; Gilley, J.W. Ethical leaders: Trust, work-life balance, and treating individuals as unique. J. Leadersh. Account. Ethics 2014, 11, 70–81. [Google Scholar]
  47. Henrich, J.; Boyd, R.; Bowles, S.; Camerer, C.; Fehr, E.; Gintis, H.; McElreath, R. In search of homo economicus: Behavioral experiments in 2001, 15 small-scale societies. Am. Econ. Rev. 2001, 91, 73–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Engelman, D.; Strobel, M. Inequality aversion, efficiency, and maximum preferences in simple distribution experiments. Am. Econ. Rev. 2004, 94, 857–869. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Bolton, G.E.; Ockenfels, A. ERC: A theory of equity, reciprocity and competition. Am. Econ. Rev. 2000, 90, 166–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Fehr, E.; Schmidt, K.M. A theory of fairness, competition and cooperation. Q. J. Econ. 1999, 114, 817–868. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Güth, W.; Schmittberger, R.; Schwarze, B. An experimental analysis of ultimatum bargaining. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 1982, 3, 367–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Kahneman, D.; Knetsch, J.L.; Thaler, R.H. Fairness and the assumptions of economics. J. Bus. 1986, 59, S285–S300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Forsythe, R.; Horowitz, J.; Savin, N.E.; Sefton, M. Fairness in simple bargaining experiments. Games Econ. Behav. 1994, 6, 347–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Roth, A.E. Bargaining experiment. In Handbook of Experimental Economics; Kagel, J., Roth, A.E., Eds.; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1995; pp. 253–348. [Google Scholar]
  55. Camerer, C.; Thaler, R.H. Anomalies: Ultimatums, dictators and manners. J. Econ. Perspect. 1995, 9, 209–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Handgraaf, M.J.J.; van Dijk, E.; De Cremer, D. Social utility in ultimatum bargaining. Soc. Justice Res. 2003, 16, 263–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Bosse, D.A.; Phillips, R.A. Agency theory and bounded self-interest. Acad. Manag. Rev. 2016, 41, 276–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Blau, P.M. Exchange and Power in Social Life; John Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  59. Bosse, D.A.; Phillips, R.A.; Harrison, J.S. Stakeholders, reciprocity, and firm performance. Strateg. Manag. J. 1964, 30, 447–456. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Simon, L.S. Industrial reciprocity as a business stratagem. Ind. Manag. Rev. 1966, 7, 27–39. [Google Scholar]
  61. Harrison, J.S.; Bosse, D.A. How much is too much? The limits to generous treatment of stakeholders. Bus. Horiz. 2013, 56, 313–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Weg, E.; Zwick, R. Toward the settlement of the fairness issues in ultimatum games. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 1994, 24, 19–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Van Dijk, E.; Vermunt, R. Strategy and fairness in social decision making: Sometimes it pays to be powerless. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2000, 36, 1–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Scheres, A.; Sanfey, A.G. Individual differences in decision making: Drive and reward responsiveness affect strategic bargaining in economic games. Behav. Brain Funct. 2006, 2, 35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Charness, G.; Gneezy, U.; Kuhn, M.A. Experimental methods: Extra-laboratory experiments-extending the reach of experimental economics. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2013, 91, 93–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Cox, J.C. Trust, fear, reciprocity, and altruism: Theory and experiment. Developments on Experimental Economics. Lect. Notes Econ. Math. Syst. 2007, 590, 75–90. [Google Scholar]
  67. Hoffman, E.; McCabe, K.; Smith, V.L. Social distance and other-regarding behaviour in dictator games. Am. Econ. Rev. 1996, 86, 653–660. [Google Scholar]
  68. Güth, W.; Kocher, M.G. More than thirty years of ultimatum bargaining experiments: Motives, variations, and a survey of the recent literature. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2014, 108, 396–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Andersen, S.; Ertac, S.; Gneezy, U.; Hoffman, M.; List, J.A. Stakes matter in ultimatum games. Am. Econ. Rev. 2011, 101, 3427–3439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Cherry, T.L.; Frykblom, P.; Shogren, J.F. Hardnose the dictator. Am. Econ. Rev. 2002, 92, 1218–1221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Carpenter, J.P.; Verhoogen, E.; Burks, S. The effect of stakes in distribution experiments. Econ. Lett. 2005, 86, 393–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  72. Ghoshal, S. Bad management theories are destroying good management practices. Acad. Manag. Learn. Educ. 2005, 4, 75–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Pfeffer, J. Why do bad management theories persist? A comment on Ghoshal. Acad. Manag. Learn. Educ. 2005, 4, 101–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Frank, R.H.; Gilovich, T.; Regan, D.T. Does studying economics inhibit cooperation? J. Econ. Perspect. 1993, 7, 159–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Marwell, G.; Ames, R.E. Economists free ride, does anyone else? Experiments on the provision of public goods, IV. J. Public Econ. 1981, 15, 295–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Frank, B.; Schulze, G.G. Does economics make citizens corrupt? J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2000, 43, 101–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. McCabe, D.L.; Butterfield, K.D.; Treviño, L.K. Academia dishonesty in graduate business programs: Prevalence, causes, and proposed action. Acad. Manag. Learn. Educ. 2006, 5, 294–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Miller, D. The norm of self-interest. Am. Psychol. 1999, 54, 1053–1060. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Giacalone, R.A.; Thompson, K.R. Business Ethics and social responsibility education: Shifting the worldview. Acad. Manag. Learn. Educ. 2006, 5, 266–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Galvão, A.; Mendes, L.; Marques, C.; Mascarenhas, C. Factors influencing students’ corporate social responsibility orientation in higher education. J. Clean. Prod. 2019, 215, 290–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Gligor-Cimpoieru, D.G.; Munteanu, V.P.; Nitju-Antonie, R.D.; Schneider, A.; Preda, G. Perceptions of future employees toward CSR environmental practices in tourism. Sustainability 2017, 9, 1631. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Murphy, M.J.; MacDonald, J.B.; Antoine, G.E.; Smolarski, J.M. Exploring Muslim Attitudes Towards Corporate Social Responsibility: Are Saudi Business Students Different? J. Bus. Ethics 2019, 154, 1103–1118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Fitzpatrick, J. Business Students’ Perceptions of Corporate Social Responsibility. Coll. Stud. J. 2013, 47, 86–95. [Google Scholar]
  84. Gilligan, C. In A Different Voice; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1993. [Google Scholar]
  85. Burton, B.K.; Dunn, C.P. Feminist ethics as moral grounding for stakeholder theory. Bus. Ethics Q. 1982, 6, 133–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Cron, W.L.; Dubinsky, A.J.; Michaels, R.E. The influence of career stages on components of salesperson motivation. J. Mark. 1988, 52, 78–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Weeks, W.A.; Moore, C.W.; McKinney, J.A.; Longenecker, J.G. The effects of gender and career stage on ethical judgment. J. Bus. Ethics 1999, 20, 301–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Eweje, G.; Brunton, M. Ethical perceptions of business students in a New Zealand university: Do gender, age and work experience matter? Bus. Ethics Eur. Rev. 2010, 19, 95–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Godos-Díez, J.L.; Fernández-Gago, R.; Cabeza-García, L. Business education and idealism as determinants of stakeholder orientation. J. Bus. Ethics 2015, 131, 439–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Kleinbaum, D.G.; Kupper, L.L.; Muller, K.E. Applied Regression Analysis and Other Multivariable Methods; PWS-KENT Publishing Company: Boston, MA, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
  91. Bechler, C.; Green, L.; Myerson, J. Proportion offered in the Dictator and Ultimatum games decreases with amount and social distance. Behav. Process. 1998, 115, 149–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  92. Engel, C. Dictator games: A meta study. Exp. Econ. 2011, 14, 583–610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Simgan-Mugan, C.; Daly, B.A.; Onkal, D.; Kavut, L. The influence on nationality and gender on ethical sensitivity: An application of the issue contingent model. J. Bus. Ethics 2005, 57, 139–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Eaton, T.V.; Giacomino, D.E. An examination of personal values: Differences between accounting students and managers and differences between genders. Teach. Bus. Ethics 2001, 5, 213–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Ghoshal, S.; Moran, P. Bad for practice: A critique of the transaction cost theory. Acad. Manag. Rev. 1996, 21, 13–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Mitroff, I.I. An open letter to the deans and faculties of American business schools. J. Bus. Ethics 2004, 54, 185–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Ferraro, F.; Pfeffer, J.; Sutton, R.I. Economics language and assumptions: How theories can become self-fulfilling. Acad. Manag. Rev. 2005, 30, 8–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Bennis, W. Managing the Dream: Reflections on Leadership and Change; Perseus: New York, NY, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  99. Fernández-Gago, R.; Martínez-Campillo, A. Teaching business management from a perspective beyond self-interest. Innovar J. Adm. Soc. Sci. 2012, 22, 165–174. [Google Scholar]
  100. Shareef, R. Want better business theories? Maybe Karl Popper has the answer. Acad. Manag. Learn. Educ. 2007, 6, 272–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Dierksmeier, C. From Jensen to Jensen: Mechanistic management education or humanistic management learning? J. Bus. Ethics 2019, 1–15, in press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Dierksmeier, C. Reorienting Management Education: From the Homo Economicus to Human Dignity. Humanistic Management Network, Research Paper Series No. 13–05. 2011. Available online: http://ssrn.com/abstract=1766183 (accessed on 11 October 2019). [CrossRef]
  103. Melé, D. Understanding Humanistic Management. Humanist. Manag. J. 2016, 1, 33–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  104. Trencher, G.; Vincent, S.; Bahr, K.; Kudo, S.; Markham, K.; Yamanaka, Y. Evaluating core competencies development in sustainability and environmental master’s programs: An empirical analysis. J. Clean. Prod. 2018, 181, 829–841. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Moosmayer, D.C.; Waddock, S.; Wang, L.; Hühn, M.P.; Dierksmeier, C.; Gohl, C. Leaving the Road to Abilene: A Pragmatic Approach to Addressing the Normative Paradox of Responsible Management Education. J. Bus. Ethics 2019, 157, 913–932. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Ang, S.H.; Leong, S.M. Out of the mouths of babes: Business ethics and youths in Asia. J. Bus. Ethics 2000, 28, 129–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Axinn, C.N.; Blair, M.E.; Heorhiadi, A.; Thach, S.V. Comparing ethical ideologies across cultures. J. Bus. Ethics 2004, 54, 103–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Marta, J.K.M.; Attia, A.; Singhapakdi, A.; Atteya, N. A Comparison of Ethical Perceptions and Moral Philosophies of American and Egyptian Business Students. Teach. Bus. Ethics 2003, 7, 1–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Hofstede, G. National cultures in four dimensions: A research-based theory of cultural differences among nations. Int. Stud. Manag. Organ. 1983, 13, 46–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Laroche, M.; Bergeron, J.; Tomiuk, M.-A.; Barbero-Forleo, G. Cultural differences in environmental knowledge, attitudes, and behaviours of Canadian consumers. Can. J. Adm. Sci. 2002, 19, 267–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Mourali, M.; Laroche, M.; Pons, F. Individualistic orientation and customer susceptibility to interpersonal influence. J. Serv. Mark. 2005, 19, 164–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Bal, M.; Bryde, D.; Fearon, D.; Ochieng, E. Stakeholder engagement: Achieving sustainability in the construction sector. Sustainability 2013, 6, 695–710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Molm, L.D.; Quist, T.M.; Wisely, P.A. Imbalanced structures, unfair strategies: Power and justice in social exchange. Am. Sociol. Rev. 1994, 59, 98–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Component in decision-making and stakeholder management orientation.
Figure 1. Component in decision-making and stakeholder management orientation.
Sustainability 11 05987 g001
Table 1. Descriptive statistics a.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics a.
VariableMeanStd. Dev.MedianMinimumMaximum% b
INSTRUMENTAL5.4381.129637
NORMATIVE4.4951.288517
RECIPROCAL1.3161.7650.505
NON-RECIPROCAL3.1891.908405
ECON_EDUCATION 52.8
GENDER 24.5
EXPERIENCE 25.5
a n = 106. b Percentage of observations with value = 1.
Table 2. Correlation matrix a.
Table 2. Correlation matrix a.
Variable1234567
1. INSTRUMENTAL1
2. NORMATIVE−0.195 *1
3. RECIPROCAL0.164 −0.1371
4. NON-RECIPROCAL−0.190 0.212 *−0.870 **1
5. ECON_EDUCATION0.193 *−0.335 **0.116−0.165 1
6. GENDER0.207 *−0.101−0.0960.0470.275 **1
7. EXPERIENCE−0.0650.137−0.0990.079−0.185 0.0691
a n = 106. p < 0.10; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.
Table 3. Linear regression analysis a.
Table 3. Linear regression analysis a.
Independent VariablesDependent Variable: INSTRUMENTALDependent Variable: NORMATIVE
RECIPROCAL0.166
(1.716)
NON-RECIPROCAL 0.160
(1.707)
ECON_EDUCATION0.112
(1.102)
−0.285 **
(−2.867)
GENDER0.195
(1.945)
−0.035
(−0.358)
EXPERIENCE−0.042
(−0.427)
0.074
(0.785)
R20.0920.143
F2.555 *4.221 **
a Standardised coefficients with t value in brackets; n = 106. p < 0.10; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Godos-Díez, J.-L.; Fernández-Gago, R.; Cabeza-García, L. How Does Reciprocity Affect Undergraduate Student Orientation towards Stakeholders? Sustainability 2019, 11, 5987. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11215987

AMA Style

Godos-Díez J-L, Fernández-Gago R, Cabeza-García L. How Does Reciprocity Affect Undergraduate Student Orientation towards Stakeholders? Sustainability. 2019; 11(21):5987. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11215987

Chicago/Turabian Style

Godos-Díez, José-Luis, Roberto Fernández-Gago, and Laura Cabeza-García. 2019. "How Does Reciprocity Affect Undergraduate Student Orientation towards Stakeholders?" Sustainability 11, no. 21: 5987. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11215987

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop