Next Article in Journal
Emerging Urban Forests: Opportunities for Promoting the Wild Side of the Urban Green Infrastructure
Next Article in Special Issue
Teaching Down to Earth—Service-Learning Methodology for Science Education and Sustainability at the University Level: A Practical Approach
Previous Article in Journal
Daytime or Overnight Deliveries? Perceptions of Drivers and Retailers in São Paulo City
Previous Article in Special Issue
Embedding Sustainability Competences into Engineering Education. The Case of Informatics Engineering and Industrial Engineering Degree Programs at Spanish Universities
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Improvement on Social Representation of Climate Change through a Knowledge-Based MOOC in Spanish

1
Department of Mathematics and Science Education, Universidad de Salamanca, 37008 Salamanca, Spain
2
Grupo de Investigación en Pedagoxía Social e Educación Ambiental SEPA-interea, Departamento de Pedagoxía e Didáctica, Universidade de Santiago de Compostela, 15705 Santiago de Compostela, Spain
3
Instituto Universitario de Ciencias de la Educación, Universidad de Salamanca, 37008 Salamanca, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sustainability 2019, 11(22), 6317; https://doi.org/10.3390/su11226317
Submission received: 30 September 2019 / Revised: 21 October 2019 / Accepted: 24 October 2019 / Published: 11 November 2019
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Technology & Engineering Education )

Abstract

:
Climate Change is the most important threat to our society and all species on Earth. Large alterations in the climate are affecting every aspect of our society and in order to limit this impact we must decarbonize the economy before 2050. Although science presents solid evidence on the magnitude of the problem and outlines precisely the consequences, people do not act accordingly and do not consider this issue a priority for their survival. The reason behind this paradox might be a non-appropriate Social Representation of Climate Change in society as the Social Representation conditions and forms the response of the society. In this paper, we extend previous investigations of how this Social Representation is formed in order to find ways to improve it through a Massive Online Open Course on the Science of Climate Change. Using a validated questionnaire, we investigated the knowledge dimension of the Social Representation of Climate Change in a group of students of a MOOC on Climate Change. A pre- and posttest revealed general improvements in all the categories that were considered in this study. A detailed analysis showed different degrees of improvement for different groups, providing new insights in the efficiency of knowledge-based online courses. Well designed Massive Online Open Courses, based on scientific evidence, targeted to the general public might improve the Social Representation of Climate Change, which may in turn trigger awareness and an effective mobilization to address this important and urgent topic.

1. Introduction

Climate Change is the most important threat to humans and all species on Earth [1]. A vast collection of scientific evidence shows fast and fundamental changes of the climatic conditions compared to preindustrial times where conditions remained relatively stable during the last 10,000 years, allowing the development of our current civilization. The global combined land and sea temperatures show a warming of 0.85 °C over the period 1880–2012 [2], sea levels are rising, the polar ice caps are melting and glaciers are retreating almost everywhere in the world. A higher frequency of extreme weather events, such as heat waves, droughts, floods, cyclones or wildfires [3], is observed and has been related to Climate Change. In the oceans, an increase of carbon dioxide (CO 2 ) concentrations in the upper water column is changing the chemistry of the oceans [2]. These, and other evidence [4], have led to a unanimous consensus among the scientific community on the importance of Climate Change and the need to have a better understanding of what is happening so that a rapid global response can be created.
The causes of these changes are anthropogenic: human activities are changing the Earth’s energy budget, mainly through the emission of carbon dioxide ( C O 2 ), methane ( C H 4 ) and other gases which enhance the greenhouse effect and increase the temperature of the Earth. This is the most important, but not the only, aspect of anthropogenic Climate Change which is causing major alterations in the global climate [2].
Climate Change is transforming ecosystem all over the globe, from polar terrestrial to tropical marine environments [5]. These changes are altering the biological diversity through extinction, over-exploitation and translocation of species [6,7,8,9,10]. The survival of the human species depends on natural systems and the changes produced in the climate are putting those at risk. Further changes will create enormous challenges and costs for societies worldwide, particularly those in developing countries [11].
The impacts of Climate Change affect almost every aspect of our life: food production, rights to resources, health, economic growth and social issues. The vulnerability and exposure to the negative impacts of Climate Change depend on geographic location, socioeconomic status or ethnic and age structures. Therefore, the impacts will not be shared equally, and it has been suggested that the impacts will be larger for the more vulnerable regions [12].
There is also a link between Climate Change and social instability [13] since it can potentially affect food security [14], water availability (e.g., the Syrian conflict is at least partially caused by a long drought [15]) and housing [13]. More frequent extreme weather events such as hurricanes, superstorms or typhoons will damage infrastructures in human settlements [16]. Climate Change is a factor which aggravates many social problems and will produce large migration flows in the following years [17,18,19,20].

1.1. Mitigation and Adaptation

Mitigation and adaptation are the main strategies to deal with Climate Change. Their purpose is to limit the consequences of Climate Change by dealing with the causes of the problem (mitigation) and reducing the impact of the consequences which already exist or are expected to occur (adaptation). These strategies can be implemented in all levels of society: from personal habits to family consumption patterns and all the way to local and national governments and international agencies.
The guidelines for a global response to Climate Change were determined at the Paris agreement of 2015 [21], where 175 Parties (174 states and the European Union) decided to act according to the science of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (IPCC). The main objective of the Paris agreement is to decarbonize the economy by 2050, a strategy that has been designed to limit the increase of the global temperature below 1.5 °C and limit the impact of Climate Change.
This enormous task requires large transformations in society at all levels (personal, familiar and local, national and international governments) and therefore affects citizens worldwide. Strategies have already been developed at all levels and are being implemented in many countries, but they may not be effective without the necessary social engagement needed to achieve those transformations [22].
Mitigation and adaptation to Climate Change remain challenging for all societies around the world, and results depend on changes in social behavior [23]. Citizens need to participate in a sustainable way of life and take responsibility in formulating solutions that link social, economic, environmental and cultural issues [23,24].
Progress in Climate Change mitigation and adaptation is slow and is not prioritized by people or governments, which leads to the following paradox: while the science presents solid evidence on the magnitude of the problem, precisely outlines the devastating impacts that Climate Change will have and proposes comprehensive routes to minimize the impacts, societies around the world do not seem to perceive the urgency of the problem and do not consider this issue as a priority for their survival.

1.2. Social Representation and Education

One possible reason behind this paradox may be the misconceptions in the Social Representation (SR) of Climate Change in society. To understand the discrepancy between the scientific “alarm” and the disinterest of the general public, we need to look into the SRs: how they are formed and how they condition the response of the society to this issue.
The SR of Climate Change is our subjective perception of reality and, to great extent, conditions our response [25]. The SR helps individuals and groups to anchor their position in situations and events relevant to their immediate concerns [26]. A misalignment between the SR and the reality (based on scientific evidence) will result in a response incoherent with the urgency of the problem.
It is important to understand how the SR is formed. The SR of Climate Change, similar to common culture, is formed through complex and poorly understood processes that integrate, absorb and incorporate new concepts—in this case related to Climate Change—into everyday concepts [27]. Once the scientific information has propagated into the field of the common culture, the new concept is not a perfect copy of the scientific representation as it becomes a new profane and autonomous theory [28].
Although this “new” representation contains scientific elements such as concepts, images, data, abstractions, etc., it has now become a SR which no longer responds to scientific logical thinking but is subject to a series of cultural processes that are involved in formalizing common sense. The newly acquired SRs are an interpretative and pragmatic framework which is always present, even to those who only have privileged access to science.
As described above, the scientific representation of Climate Change is in stark contrast to the weakness and inconsistencies of the SR of Climate Change that society might have. The inconsistency of this representation with the scientific reality may explain why skeptic and negationist beliefs still exist. It may also explain why most of the population does not perceive the urgency of the problem and why Climate Change occupies a peripheral place on public and personal agendas.
The results of a recent national survey [29] show that in the Spanish society almost everybody (8 out of 10 persons) accepts that Climate Change is happening and recognizes its anthropogenic origin. Despite these high values, some doubts on the unanimity of the scientific community with respect to the anthropogenic origin of Climate Change still persists within the SR and, as discussed above, Climate Change is not perceived as a relevant or important problem.
The SRs are a set of structures which have three main components: The information, the structure and the attitude (See Scheme 1 bellow). The information that feeds the SRs has different origins, some of the elements come from scientific sources, while others come from common culture. The structure of the SR refers to the order and hierarchy of the information where beliefs, values and ideology conditions how this information articulates and gives meaning to the subject of representation. The final element is the attitude, which provides the positive or negative predisposition to the corresponding action [30].

1.3. The Formation of Social Representation

The formation of the SR of Climate Change has been explored extensively during the last decade by the SEPA-interea research group from the Universidade de Santiago de Compostela (USC) through the Resclima project [31,32,33,34,35].
This project has studied and analyzed thoroughly how the SR of Climate Change is formed and how it evolves over time [36,37,38]. Studies about how the SR conditions the societal response to Climate Change are helpful to suggest guidelines for educational [39,40] and communication [35,41,42] strategies for mitigation and adaptation.
The Spanish society represents Climate Change in a diffuse way; the representation is often linked to other environmental problems that are not necessarily scientifically related. Within this representation, Climate Change is considered a threat and is associated with negative emotions, but is not perceived with the same urgency and relevance as suggested by scientific evidence [43]. Again, within the representation, people have the perception that the likelihood of negative impacts in their lives and surroundings is very low, which inhibits a sense of responsibility and personal compromise with the corresponding policies [44].
In the second part of the Resclima project, a study was launched to test the SR of Climate Change among university students from different degrees. For this, a 32-item questionnaire was designed to evaluate the knowledge of the Information and Structure of the SR. The items were statements on different aspects, actions and knowledge about Climate Change with a Likert scale; the content from the statements were designed according to the principles of the essentials in climate literacy described in a publication by the United States Global Warming Research Program (USGCRP) [45].
In the questionnaire, some statements were included to detect the prevalence of some of the most extended profane theories on Climate Change: the false causal link between the hole in the ozone layer and Climate Change; the relationship between Climate Change and other phenomena such as acid rain, natural tectonic processes (earthquakes or tsunamis); or the confusion between Climate Change and the greenhouse effect. A good score indicated a correct identification of scientifically correct statements and false associations.
Several findings of the previously mentioned study are relevant for this paper [37,46,47]. The results from the questionnaires applied to students of the Universidad de Granada (Spain) and the Universidade de Santiago de Compostela (Spain) reveal that, even though students from Natural Sciences degrees scored higher in the items related to the knowledge of physical and chemical processes and causes and consequences of Climate Change than students of Social Sciences, they still failed to identify false statements, which means that their SR contains misconceptions. This study, hereafter referred to as the “Resclima study” [48], describes the technical details of the 32-item questionnaire that we used in this study.
When comparing the results of students in their first and their last years of the same degree (1st and 4th), no significant differences were found, suggesting that the information incorporated into the SR of Climate Change comes from external sources such as informal interactions, media or information from basic education. This same study also shows that certain common misconceptions, such as identifying the ozone layer as a cause of Climate Change, were found among these students even though they were supposed to be scientific literates.
In this paper we try to link two important issues: education and SRs of Climate Change. As SRs condition and determine the response of individuals, and ultimately society, we suggest that they can be improved through evidence based Education.
Education is widely recognized as the main tool to improve the SR and adjust it to the scientific reality [21]. In their last report, the IPCC identifies and recognizes the role of education in facing this challenge: the task of an educational program, in order to mitigate Climate Change and promote the adaptation to its inevitable consequences, is to represent a global collective problem at individual and social level [1].
To investigate this, the SR of a group of students was tested before and after a course a MOOC on Climate Change to see if some aspects of the SR improved. If successful, this could prove that education, for this group, is an effective tool for Climate Change mitigation and adaptation, as suggested in previous studies [49].
It is expected that a more accurate and scientific representation of Climate Change may trigger a better response, coherent with the urgency of the problem. At the same time, this exploration could teach us something about this particular MOOC format as a tool to learn the basic Science of Climate Change. The results of the analysis can also be used later as a guide to further develop other courses and contents.

1.4. Massive Online Open Courses

We are interested in how Education can be used to improve the SRs among different groups. It could be expected that a better alignment between the scientific reality and the SR mediated by Education could mobilize society into climate actions and educational tools that have a wide impact with small resources, such as the Massive Online Open Courses (MOOCs), are of special interest.
MOOCs are a relatively recent online phenomena that have attracted millions of learners around the world. These courses generate considerable media attention and interest from governments, higher education institutions and commercial organizations [50] and reflect the vastly changing societal needs for knowledge and lifelong learning.
There are several important features that make the MOOC format attractive to educate people in- and outside the traditional educational system. In particular, it may be very good to learn about Climate Change as this topic is not contemplated in the traditional curriculum of compulsory education and is only included in the form of specialized topics or postgraduate programs in universities.
The MOOC format allows a wide reach and customized contents relevant to Climate Change, and therefore has the possibility to have a positive impact on the SR of Climate Change. This is crucial given the urgency of the climate crisis. In the following paragraph, we discuss how this format can have advantages over other formats.
  • Global reach. The MOOC format has a global reach and can fill the need and scarcity of educational resources on this topic in Spanish, compared to the number of resources in English.
  • Updated information. The materials can be easily updated. The pace of scientific consensus around the diagnostics and descriptions of Climate Change is much faster than the pace of changes in national curricula. Therefore, it is important to have a course that can provide the most up to date version of the scientific consensus to Spanish speaking teachers worldwide.
  • An effective transmission of the message. The MOOC uses visual imagery to capture the audience and to communicate the concepts. Long descriptions are avoided and videos are used in all modules of the MOOC. In addition, text materials are available for consultation during and after the course and are valuable resources to enrich teacher’s daily classes. The evaluation of the course is carried out through a multiple response test and a peer-to-peer activity that consisted in searching and analyzing a media story related to Climate Change, paying special attention to identifying the causes and consequences of Climate Change.
There are, of course, some critics to the MOOC format. A major concern that is often raised is that, although a massive number of students enrol in MOOCs, especially since enrolment is free and is as easy as clicking a mouse button, a very small proportion (∼10–20%) completes their courses. Given their large impact, MOOCs have been the subject of a large debate (e.g., [51]), but are widely considered powerful pedagogical instruments to democratize access to high-quality education in developing and underdeveloped countries [52] as learning content is available to anyone with an Internet connection.

2. Methods

In this paper, we explore the role of Education in improving the SR of Climate Change. In particular, we look into the first aspect of SRs, knowledge, as a tool to refute common misconceptions and bring the SR closer to the scientific reality. Knowledge of the concepts, causes, consequences and impacts is essential to adjust the SR, which eventually may trigger individual action and mobilize society.

2.1. Design of the Study

To test if there are changes in the SRs after taking the MOOC, we applied a pre-experimental design, without control group and with pre- and posttest measurements [53] to the participants of the second edition of the MOOC “Awareness and training on climate change for Primary and Secondary teachers” [54] (hereafter referred to as “the MOOC”), hosted in the MiriadaX portal (https://miriadax.net/). The pre-experimental design of minimum control (without control group) [53,55,56] was selected as we investigate into the evolution of this group after taking the MOOC. A comparison of our MOOC sample against a sample of student made in a previous study is presented in the first part of the study.
Our main research questions are the following:
  • Does a knowledge-based MOOC affect the Information of the SR as measured with our questionnaire?
  • Is the effect of the MOOC equal for all four main areas (definitions, causes, consequences and responses) used in the design of the MOOC?
  • Are there significant differences in the group of students taking the MOOC?

2.2. Characteristics of the Sample

Five hundred thirty (530) people who took the MOOC participated voluntarily in our study, which took place during the second edition of our MOOC starting in March 2019. The pretest was carried out before starting Module 1 of the second edition of the MOOC, and the posttest took place six weeks later at the end of the modules of the MOOC. These students came from 24 countries, mainly from Latin America and Spain, and to a less extent from Italy, Portugal and Africa (Table 1). Students were 52% female and 48% male. Occupations of participants varied, with 46.5% being teachers, 26.4% students and the remaining 27.1% were non-teaching professionals. All participants from Africa were teachers. The age of the participants ranged from 14 to 77 (Mean = 36.1, Standard Deviation = 13.6). It is worth noting that the percentage of participants with university education is relatively high, being 72%. Two hundred fifty-five (255) participants completed the posttest after finishing the course (April 2019). The final questionnaire was exactly the same as the initial one. The difference in the number of students who completed the pretest and posttest is indicative of the MOOC completion rate.
We compared results from our sample with those obtained by the research group from the USC in the “Resclima study” [48]. They applied a non-probabilistic and intentional sampling to four hundred eighty-eight college students, with a mean age of 20.5; 42.8% of the participants were men and 57.2% were women.

2.3. Evaluation Instrument

The questionnaire employed in the present study was developed during the Resclima project at USC [46,47,48] and was used to test the first dimension of the SR among students of a MOOC on the Science of Climate Change. The technical details of the questionnaire can be found in the “Resclima study” [48]. The 32 items of the questionnaire can be found in Appendix A.
Our questionnaire has two different sections. The first section is a demographic analysis of the participants including questions related to age, sex, level of studies, country of origin, etc. The second section, which has been introduced previously in the “Resclima study” [48], consists of 32 items designed to explore the participant’s interactions between scientific and common culture. The questionnaire measures student’s competence through the identification of the scientific veracity of a series of statements, which are classified into four areas of knowledge related to the design of MOOC materials.
  • Area 1. Physical processes related to CC (8 statements)
  • Area 2. Consequences of CC (10 statements)
  • Area 3. Causes of CC (10 statements)
  • Area 4. Responses to CC (4 statements)
To assess the degree of knowledge of participants, the close-ended statements were based on a Likert scale of four categories: “Totally true” (TT), “Probably true” (PT), “Probably false” (PF) and “Totally false” (TF). This scale allows measurement of the scientific accuracy of the answers on a scale from 1 to 4, with 4 being the maximum value of agreement “accuracy” between scientific culture and common culture. The correct answers are based on the reports of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change [1]. We purposely excluded the option “Don’t know/don’t answer” to avoid non-reflective or evasive responses. The final score is an indication of the participant’s ability to read, interpret and assess scientific texts properly. High scores indicate a more accurate scientific view, while low scores indicate a prevalence of false information. The index of reliability of the instrument was determined by calculating the alpha coefficient of Cronbach, which had a value of 0.88. Scores of 0.7 or higher are considered reliable [57]. The results of the questionnaire were exported to CSV format to be analyzed in SPSS, version 25.

2.4. Training Program: A MOOC on the Science of Climate Change

The MOOC titled “The Science of Climate Change for primary and secondary teachers” has a duration of six weeks and its first edition was launched in 2018 under the banner of the Universidad de Salamanca and through the MiriadaX platform. This MOOC was designed and developed by some of the authors of this paper at the Universidad de Salamanca [58].
Prior to the elaboration of our MOOC, we observed an overall scarcity of learning materials related to Climate Change available in Spanish, a language with 480 million speakers with native competence and 577 million people speaking Spanish as a first or second language [59]. Therefore, this course was designed to target a broad audience: Spanish speaking teachers worldwide. The MOOC uses a traditional top-down structured design approach (xMOOC type), which is based on the interaction with content and essentially adopts a behaviorist learning approach [60].
The main objective of the MOOC “The Science of Climate Change for primary and secondary teachers” is to provide fundamental scientific knowledge and create awareness on Climate Change. The four main questions that the course tries to answer are:
  • What is Climate Change?
  • What are its causes?
  • What are its consequences?
  • What can we do?
The first three questions are answered through scientific knowledge. The last one is related to the concepts of adaptation and mitigation, and how education can (and must) help to create citizen awareness, promote a better understanding of the issue and help build capacities and strategies to successfully face this global problem.
Our analysis of the knowledge of the SR of Climate Change is made using the four categories that were used to design the MOOC. The way we refer to these categories are: definitions, causes, consequences and responses.
The contents of the course were structured in seven sequential modules that also had a discussion forum where participants were able to interact with each other (Table 2). During the course, participants must follow a predetermined schedule and have to be positively evaluated in a test at the end of each module and in a peer-to-peer activity.
To stimulate engagement and ensure instructional quality, several strategies were followed: (a) each module has short and high quality videos presented by teachers, but avoiding excessive use of the “talking head” format; (b) the contents of the course are contextualized, describing and analyzing real and concrete problems that students can relate to; (c) the course has been constructed based on updated empirical evidence following the scientific consensus of the IPCC reports; (d) the language used is simple but rigorous, and a positive narrative is used to transmit scientific rigor and urgency, but also hope and positivism; and (e) the course avoids complex representations and does not include complex or irrelevant graphics, always using meaningful and high quality imagery that can be used by teachers in their classrooms [52,61,62,63,64].
To this day, the MOOC has been offered in two editions (June–August 2018 and March–April 2019) and this study was carried out during the 2019 edition.

2.5. Data Analysis

We first compared our results with previous results of other groups of university students [46,47,48]. Afterwards, the evolution between the pre- and posttest was analyzed for our sample data only. This provides insight into how some misconceptions can be corrected and updated through a MOOC course. The data also show different improvements for different subgroups of the sampled group, indicating a variable level of adaptation to the format of the course.
Since answers to the questionnaires are not paired with students id, because they are anonymous, the Kolmogorov–Smirnoff test was used to determine the normality of the variables and all the test results had p-values < 0.05 , suggesting that our data do not present a normal distribution. For the hypotheses contrast, we applied the nonparametric Mann–Whitney U t-test, this t-test compares the distributions of two unmatched groups when data are not normally distributed. The Mann–Whitney U t-test resulted in a p-value < 0.05 , which indicates that the distribution of the criterion variable is significantly different when comparing pretest and posttest. The level of significance used in all contrasts is analyzed under 5%.
In all cases, the calculation of the significance of the differences was accompanied with the calculation of the effect size based on the Cohen d-statistic [65,66]. Therefore, differences between the pre- and posttest were not only based on the statistical criteria provided by the hypothesis contrast, but also on the amount of effect existing in the particular contrast. These differences were considered as small ( d = 0.20 ) , moderate ( d = 0.50 ) or big ( d = 0.80 ) . All hypothesis contrasts were accompanied by statistical power, which is the ability of a test to detect differences, conventionally 80% ( 1 β = 0.80 ) indicates that there is a 20% probability of accepting the null hypothesis when it is false [67].

3. Results

We start the analysis by comparing the results from our sample to the sample from the “Resclima study” [48]. We use the answers from the pretest of our MOOC sample and compare the score for all 32 statements in the questionnaire. The purpose of this comparison is to establish a relation between the SR in our MOOC sample, as measured with the questionnaire, and the reference study.
Figure 1 shows the score for each question where 4 corresponds to a correctly classified statement and 1 to an incorrectly identified statement. The results are grouped according to four areas which correspond to the four basic questions used to design the MOOC: “What is Climate Change? (Physical processes)”, “What are its causes? (Causes)”, “What are its consequences? (Consequences)” and “What can we do? (Responses)”. Comparison of results of the MOOC with the previous study reveals a similar overall trend in the average score of participants, although participants of the MOOC generally have a higher average score.
Figure 2 shows the same comparison, but now we show the overall average and the grouped average of the four previously mentioned areas, where the average in the four areas represent the variable dependent “knowledge in SR”. When comparing the average score of each of the four areas between the MOOC and the “Resclima study” it became clear that participants of the MOOC scored higher in the areas of physical processes (3.21 vs. 2.66) and causes (3.15 vs. 2.81) while scores for the areas of consequences and responses are very similar.
The results of the pretest are an indication of the initial SR of Climate Change, where low scores suggest the prevalence of misconceptions such as the erroneous idea that the hole in the ozone layer is a consequence of Climate Change. The initial comparison with the previous study excludes any major peculiarity of our sample.
After this initial comparison, we focus on the MOOC group and the evolution of the SR. Once the students completed the pretest, they started the MOOC course. The pace of the course was one module per week and after the sixth week (the last) the posttest was submitted. This test was mandatory to finish the course and all the people who performed the posttest finished the course.
As a first result we show the comparisons between the pre- and posttest. One of the main objectives of this study was to find out if participants of the MOOC improved their knowledge on concepts related to Climate Change given the characteristics of the sample. Figure 3 shows the comparison between the average score of the pre- and posttest of the MOOC sample. The average score of the pretest was 3.22 and after the MOOC course participants reached an average score of 3.40. This is a clear and significant improvement in the average score. The average score includes all 32 items and all participants who finished the MOOC. As we show below, this level of improvement is similar across many different subsets of age, gender, educational level and country of origin.
After the hypothesis contrast, significant advances (p minor value) of the dependent variable knowledge in SR were observed. The values of this variable were calculated using the average scores of the four areas of knowledge and the difference between the average scores of the pre- and posttest has an effect size of 0.46 (Table 3).
A more detailed view of the improvement of the average score of the posttest in the separated areas is shown in Figure 4. A significant advance in all areas of knowledge was observed but some differences exist (Table 4). Comparing all four different areas, we observed that the Areas 3 (Causes) and 4 (Responses) show a relatively higher improvement compared to Areas 1 (Definitions) and 2 (Consequences). The effect size value ( d = 0.68 ) for Area 3 (Causes) explains a moderate difference [65] (Figure 4). The reasons behind this asymmetric improvement are discussed in the next section.
We also analyzed the results for different subsamples of the group of participants based on the demographic data collected during the first questionnaire. This analysis allows us to find a relation between demographics and the MOOC format.
The first analysis compares teachers versus non-teachers. Within these groups, we selected the ages where they might be active (26–65) and compared their scores against the rest of the population. Figure 5 shows the results of this comparison and a larger improvement for the teachers was observed even though their initial score is lower compared to the rest. In the discussion below, we present some arguments that could explain these observations.
Finally, we analyzed the sample by looking into the country of origin of the participants. Based on the data of the country of origin of the participants, we divided the group into two subgroups: (1) participants from Spain; and (2) participants from Latin America. Figure 6 shows the pre- and posttest results. A very similar improvement of the overall average score was observed in both groups, although their initial score is different. The results indicate that there are significant differences between both groups (Spain/Latin America), being Latin America the one that reaches a higher level of performance in knowledge in SR (posttest: Z = 2.18 ; p = 0.29 ), in the variable score in the posttest ( M d n E s = 3.19 and M d n L a t = 3.46 ).

4. Discussion

The main objective of this paper is to see if the SR on Climate Change can be improved by means of MOOCs on the basic concepts of Climate Change. This question is extremely relevant since SRs are considered a main driver for climate action [68,69] and misconceptions in this SR may be the reason behind the slow response of society despite the urgency indicated by scientific evidence [68,70]. Moreover, our study could provide more support to the hypothesis that education is one of the main tools for Climate Change mitigation and adaptation, as suggested by the UNFCC [69,71].
Comparison of the results of the questionnaire from our MOOC sample and the sample used in the “Resclima study” [48] reveals a similar general trend, ruling out the fact that our MOOC sample could have some important peculiarities that make it very different from the USC sample. This means that the SR in our sample is, despite small differences, similar to the SR of the USC sample. A possible explanation for a higher initial score in the MOOC group could be the fact that participants enrolled in the MOOC start with a higher interest in these topics as their enrollment is voluntarily. The two categories where we observed bigger differences between samples are causes and responses, confirming a slightly better initial knowledge on the topic by MOOC students.
The SR consist of all relevant ideas and practices associated with an object [68]. Some authors have suggested that nature could be this object [72]. In this situation, SR could be the ideas and practices that society has in connection with a natural phenomenon. Other authors [73] have suggested that SRs of Climate Change contain scientific information but most of it reaches society through media or other mediators which could simplify, reduce or interpret them based on multiple variables or interests. Our hypothesis is that Education is an effective way to transfer scientific knowledge which is useful to improve a distorted SR. Courses on scientific knowledge have been suggested as a good method to increase SR of environmental issues [74,75] but also in other disciplines such as psychology [76] or mathematics [77]. We propose to evaluate the improvement of SR on Climate Change measuring student’s competence through the identification of the scientific veracity of a series of statements, which are classified into four areas of knowledge related to the design of MOOC materials: physical processes, consequences, causes and responses to Climate Change [58]. To this aim, we investigated the MOOC sample with the pre- and post questionnaires.
The most interesting finding of our study is the improvement of the SR after taking the MOOC course (Figure 3).This improvement can be seen in the overall average score and is consistent along all four areas of knowledge (Figure 4). When looking more into detail to the improvement in the different areas, a higher improvement was observed in the areas of causes and responses. A higher improvement in the area of causes was expected since a significant part of the course was dedicated to a detailed description of the scientific evidence that explains the mechanisms behind Climate Change. In the case of Responses, the improvement is remarkable given the relatively small importance of this section compared to the science sections. These findings support the idea that a course that is largely based on scientific knowledge has the power to indirectly improve the understanding of non-scientific aspects of the issue, for example mitigation and adaptation, which have been included in the area of responses during the questionnaire.
Moreover, a high score in the areas of responses and causes could mean that, although students understood the consequences of Climate Change, they did not make a connection with the causes and therefore did not see the links to their own behavior. A higher improvement in the area response (mitigation and adaptation) could imply that participants have established a connection between the observed changes in the Climate and the impact of our behavior as a society. Although they may know the consequences, they might not know strategies for mitigation and adaptation; again, the MOOC improves this situation. This is interesting as the MOOC seems to even the scores along the four categories, which means a more equilibrated representation.
The rest of the observations on the relative improvement between different groups might be explained in terms of the fitness of the MOOC format for different groups. In the first place, the advantages of using a MOOC compared to other more traditional teaching methods, such as face-to-face classes, should be pointed out. The improvement of results shows that the MOOC format has produced a positive and quantifiable change in the students SR on Climate Change. The advantages of a MOOC [78] are obvious: it is a didactic tool that can be accessed from anywhere in the world and can be adapted to different learning rhythms and schedule needs, because the students can access the material when they want and as many times as they want. Although the MOOC format offers the possibility of raising doubts and questions to the teachers, this possibility was not used very often by students in our MOOC. This suggest that the improvement of knowledge was achieved through the materials provided in the MOOC, although other sources may be consulted during the course.
The MOOC generated improvement in the average score of the posttest in all age groups and, when we compared teachers and non-teachers (Figure 5), we observed a higher improvement among the teachers even though they started with a lower initial score. This may be explained by the fact that the teachers might have an initial interest in the topic and can make better use of the tools that the MOOC provides. Additionally, teachers are used to taking short courses as part of their professional development and therefore might be better prepared to take advantage of the contents of the MOOC.
The average score of the results of the pretest (Figure 6) of Latin American participants (M = 3.25 of a total of 275) is higher than that of the Spanish participants in the MOOC (M = 3.16 of a total of 175), which means a greater awareness of this issue among Latin Americans. This might be due to some differences in the mandatory school curriculum. This is interesting, and at the same time alarming, as Spain is one of the countries which will be most affected by the consequences of Climate Change [79]. Nevertheless, the fact that all students, regardless of their nationality, improved their average score (Figure 6) proves that small language barriers between the different countries did not affect the learning process and demonstrates the usefulness of a course prepared in Spanish, a language spoken by more than five hundred million people. This is why we can highlight the importance of creating educational materials in Spanish on this topic.

5. Conclusions

The Social Representation of Climate Change in society is an important driver for Climate action. A distorted or incomplete SR may be the main cause for the observed apathy even though we are facing the most important threat of our times. The challenge to contain the impacts of Climate Change will require large transformations of society which need to be addressed now. In this paper, we go beyond previous studies that focus on how the SR of Climate Change is formed. The main objective of this paper is to see if the SR of Climate Change can be improved by means of knowledge-based learning about the basic science of Climate Change. Moreover, our study provides a clear demonstration of how education can become a fundamental tool for Climate Change mitigation and adaptation, as suggested by the UNFCC.
For this, we used a well tested questionnaire to measure misconceptions in the SRs of Climate Change in a group of students of a knowledge-based MOOC. The MOOC on the science of Climate Change, developed at the Universidad de Salamanca, explains in simple terms the definition of Climate Change, its causes and consequences and what we can do. We applied this questionnaire before and after the course to see how these misconceptions evolve. To analyze the results, we grouped them into four areas developed in the MOOC: definitions, causes, consequences and response.
We observed an improvement in all categories for the people who finished the course. In particular, the categories of causes and responses show the best improvement. This is an important result as it shows how, for this particular group, a MOOC based on knowledge can improve the SR in particular in the response category which describes how much people know about the mitigation and adaptation, which is needed to mobilize society to contain this important problem.
The results also allowed us to test the efficiency of the MOOC platform among different groups. We observed small but significant differences in some groups within the sample. Teachers performed better after the course than non-teachers and people from Latin America showed a better improvement than Spanish students, although the margin is small. These results allowed us to think that the MOOC might be an efficient way to put education at the center of mitigation and adaptation strategies. Its characteristics allow a fast and broad access to important information which can improve the SR and perhaps lead to action for climate.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, C.R. and P.M.; methodology, F.M.; formal analysis, E.F.; investigation, A.-M.B., M.A.F., P.H., L.D., D.C., S.A.-S.; data curation, K.M.B., A.G.-V.; writing—original draft preparation, C.R.; writing—review and editing, A.-M.B., L.D., D.C., S.A.-S.; visualization, E.F.

Funding

This research was funded by the project 2017/00287/001 of the Fundación Biodiversidad del Ministerio de Agricultura, Pesca, Alimentación y Medio Ambiente (España).

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge MediaLab at the Universidad de Salamanca for its help in the development of this work.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Abbreviations

The following abbreviations are used in this manuscript:
SRSocial Representation
CCClimate Change
MOOCMassive Online Open Course
IPCCIntergovernmental Panel on Climate Change

Appendix A

Here, we present all the statements of the questionnaire. They are grouped according to the four sections that we have defined for our analysis.
Table A1. Statements of the questionnaire, grouped according to the four sections that we have defined for our analysis.
Table A1. Statements of the questionnaire, grouped according to the four sections that we have defined for our analysis.
Area 1. Physical processes related to CC
4.The polar hole of the ozone layer causes the melting of the poles.
7.The acid rain is one of the causes of climate change.
12.If it were not the greenhouse effect, there would be no life as we know it.
16.Climate change is a consequence of the hole in the ozone layer.
19.The greenhouse effect occurs when gases retain part of the radiation reflected by the Earth’s surface.
20.Sea level is increasing due to the expansion of water by the rise of temperature.
27.The CO2 causes the destruction of the ozone layer.
29.According to Earth’s climatic history, there have been oscillations between colder and warmer periods.
Area 2. Consequences of CC
2.A warmer planet will expand the area of incidence of tropical diseases.
3.The increased temperatures will favour the concurrence of extreme weather events (cyclones, hurricanes, floods, etc.)
6.Skin cancers will increase as a result of climate change.
10.All countries will suffer climate change.
11.The greenhouse effect puts in risk life in the Earth.
15.Climate change will increase the number of earthquakes and tsunamis.
21.The climate change will decrease the rainfall in my country.
22.The rising temperatures will affect all regions of the planet alike.
25.Climate change will exacerbate problems of desertification in the Iberian Peninsula.
30.Many islands and coastal areas will be submerged due to climate change.
Area 3. Causes of CC
1.The greenhouse effect is a natural phenomenon.
8.Most of greenhouse gases present in the atmosphere come from natural sources.
9.The CO2 is the main gas responsible for climate change.
11.The increase in meat consumption contributes to climate change.
13.Every time coal, oil or gas is used, we contribute to climate change.
17.Climate change is caused by human activity.
18.Climate change is the result of natural climatic variability.
23.The CO2 is a natural component of the atmosphere.
28.There is scientific consensus when considering human activity as the main cause of climate change.
31.The greenhouse effect is caused by human activity.
Responses to CC
5.If we stop emitting greenhouse gases, we will not be affected by the climate change.
24.If we stop emitting greenhouse gases, we will be less vulnerable to the climate change.
26.The climate change would be reduced if we planted more trees.
32.Replacing private transport by the public is one of the most effective measures to address the climate change.

References

  1. Pachauri, R.K.; Allen, M.R.; Barros, V.R.; Broome, J.; Cramer, W.; Christ, R.; Church, J.A.; Clarke, L.; Dahe, Q.; Dasgupta, P.; et al. Climate Change 2014: Synthesis Report. Contribution of Working Groups I, II and III to the Fifth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change; IPCC: Geneva, Switzerland, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  2. Stocker, T. Climate Change 2013: The Physical Science Basis: Working Group I contribution to the Fifth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK; New York, NY, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  3. Field, C.B. Climate Change 2014—Impacts, Adaptation and Vulnerability: Regional Aspects; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  4. National Aeronautics and Space Administration, NASA. Global Climate Change: Evidence. 2008. Available online: https://climate.nasa.gov/evidence/ (accessed on 25 September 2019).
  5. Walther, G.R.; Post, E.; Convey, P.; Menzel, A.; Parmesan, C.; Beebee, T.J.; Fromentin, J.M.; Hoegh-Guldberg, O.; Bairlein, F. Ecological responses to recent climate change. Nature 2002, 416, 389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Vitousek, P.M.; Mooney, H.A.; Lubchenco, J.; Melillo, J.M. Human domination of Earth’s ecosystems. Science 1997, 277, 494–499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Chapin, F.S., III; Zavaleta, E.S.; Eviner, V.T.; Naylor, R.L.; Vitousek, P.M.; Reynolds, H.L.; Hooper, D.U.; Lavorel, S.; Sala, O.E.; Hobbie, S.E.; et al. Consequences of changing biodiversity. Nature 2000, 405, 234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Pecl, G.T.; Araújo, M.B.; Bell, J.D.; Blanchard, J.; Bonebrake, T.C.; Chen, I.C.; Clark, T.D.; Colwell, R.K.; Danielsen, F.; Evengård, B.; et al. Biodiversity redistribution under climate change: Impacts on ecosystems and human well-being. Science 2017, 355, eaai9214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Ramirez, F.; Rodriguez, C.; Seoane, J.; Figuerola, J.; Bustamante, J. How will climate change affect endangered Mediterranean waterbirds? PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0192702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Foden, W.B.; Young, B.E.; Akçakaya, H.R.; Garcia, R.A.; Hoffmann, A.A.; Stein, B.A.; Thomas, C.D.; Wheatley, C.J.; Bickford, D.; Carr, J.A.; et al. Climate change vulnerability assessment of species. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Clim. Chang. 2019, 10, e551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Hoegh-Guldberg, O.; Bruno, J.F. The impact of climate change on the world’s marine ecosystems. Science 2010, 328, 1523–1528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Hallegatte, S.; Rozenberg, J. Climate change through a poverty lens. Nat. Clim. Chang. 2017, 7, 250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Sellers, S.; Ebi, K.L.; Hess, J. Climate Change, Human Health, and Social Stability: Addressing Interlinkages. Environ. Health Perspect. 2019, 127, 045002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Cerri, C.E.P.; Cerri, C.C.; Maia, S.M.F.; Cherubin, M.R.; Feigl, B.J.; Lal, R. Reducing Amazon deforestation through agricultural intensification in the Cerrado for advancing food security and mitigating climate change. Sustainability 2018, 10, 989. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Gleick, P.H. Water, drought, climate change, and conflict in Syria. Weather Clim. Soc. 2014, 6, 331–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Balbus, J.; Berry, P.; Brettle, M.; Jagnarine-Azan, S.; Soares, A.; Ugarte, C.; Varangu, L.; Prats, E.V. Enhancing the sustainability and climate resiliency of health care facilities: A comparison of initiatives and toolkits. Revista Panamericana Salud Pública 2016, 40, 174–180. [Google Scholar]
  17. Black, R.; Bennett, S.R.; Thomas, S.M.; Beddington, J.R. Climate change: Migration as adaptation. Nature 2011, 478, 447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Agnew, R. Dire forecast: A theoretical model of the impact of climate change on crime. In Transnational Environmental Crime; Routledge: Abingdon, UK, 2017; pp. 97–118. [Google Scholar]
  19. Cattaneo, C.; Beine, M.; Fröhlich, C.J.; Kniveton, D.; Martinez-Zarzoso, I.; Mastrorillo, M.; Millock, K.; Piguet, E.; Schraven, B. Human migration in the era of climate change. Rev. Environ. Econ. Policy 2019, 13, 189–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Chersich, M.F.; Swift, C.P.; Edelstein, I.; Breetzke, G.; Scorgie, F.; Schutte, F.; Wright, C.Y. Violence in hot weather: Will climate change exacerbate rates of violence in South Africa? S. Afr. Med. J. 2019, 109, 447–449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. United Nations. Paris Agreement; United Nations Treaty Collection; United Nations: New York, NY, USA, 2016; Chapter XXVII 7. [Google Scholar]
  22. Westerhoff, L.; Sheppard, S.R.; Iype, D.M.; Cote, S.; Salter, J. Social mobilization on climate change and energy: An evaluation of research projects in British Columbia, Canada. Energy Res. Soc. Sci. 2018, 46, 368–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Sheppard, S.; Iype, D.M.; Cote, S.; Salter, J. Special Report–A Synthesis of PICS-Funded Social; The Pacific Institute for Climate Solutions: Victoria, BC, Canada, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  24. Van Kerkhoff, L.; Lebel, L. Linking knowledge and action for sustainable development. Annu. Rev. Environ. Resour. 2006, 31, 445–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Jodelet, D. El movimiento de retorno al sujeto y el enfoque de las representaciones sociales. Cultura Representaciones Sociales 2008, 3, 32–63. [Google Scholar]
  26. Abric, J.C. Les représentations sociales: Aspects théoriques. In Pratiques sociales et représentations; Presses Universitaires de France: Paris, France, 1994; Volume 2, pp. 11–37. [Google Scholar]
  27. Jodelet, D. La representación Social: Fenómenos, Concepto y Teoría; Psicología Social II; Moscovici, S., Ed.; Paidós: Barcelona, Spain, 1986; pp. 469–494. [Google Scholar]
  28. Moscovici, S. La representación social: Un concepto perdido. Psicoanálisis Imagen Público 1979, 2, 27–44. [Google Scholar]
  29. Centro de Investigaciones Sociológicas. Barómetro Noviembre 2018. Avance de resultados (Estudio n° 3231). 2018. Available online: http://datos.cis.es/pdf/Es3231mar_A.pdf (accessed on 25 September 2019).
  30. Bello Benavides, L.O.; Meira-Cartea, P.Á.; González Gaudiano, É.J. Representaciones sociales sobre cambio climático en dos grupos de estudiantes de educación secundaria de España y bachillerato de México. Revista Mexicana de Investigación Educativa 2017, 22, 505–532. [Google Scholar]
  31. Meira-Cartea, P.Á. Problemas ambientales globales y educación ambiental: Una aproximación desde las representaciones sociales del cambio climático. Revista Integra Educativa 2013, 6, 29–64. [Google Scholar]
  32. Meira-Cartea, P.Á.; Arto-Blanco, M. La representación del cambio climático en la sociedad española. De la conciencia a la acción. Seguridad y Medio Ambiente 2008, 1, 1689–1699. [Google Scholar]
  33. Cartea, P.Á.M.; Blanco, M.A.; Hernández, F.H.; da Cunha, L.I.; Castiñeiras, J.J.L. La Respuesta de la Sociedad Española Ante el Cambio Climático; Fundación MAPFRE, Aldine Editorial: Madrid, Spain, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  34. Meira-Cartea, P.Á.; Arto-Blanco, M. La Sociedad ante el Cambio Climático. Conocimientos, Valoraciones y Comportamientos en la Población Española; MAPFRE: Santiago de Compotela, Spain, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  35. Meira-Cartea, P.Á. Representaciones sociales del cambio climático en la sociedad española: Una lectura para comunicadores. In Medios de Comunicación y Cambio Climático. Actas de las Jornadas Internacionales. Coord. por Rosalba Mancinas Chávez; Rogelio Fernández Reyes (dir.); Facultad de Comunicación de la Universidad de Sevilla: Sevilla, Spain, 2013; pp. 59–90. [Google Scholar]
  36. Blanco, M.A. El cambio climático narrado por alumnos de educación primaria y secundaria: Propuesta de análisis para dibujos y textos. In Investigar para Avanzar en Educación Ambiental; Organismo Autónomo Parques Nacionales: Madrid, Spain, 2010; p. 11. [Google Scholar]
  37. Meira-Cartea, P.Á.; Arto-Blanco, M. Representaciones del cambio climático en estudiantes universitarios en España: Aportes para la educación y la comunicación. Educar em Revista 2014, 3, 15–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Arto-Blanco, M.; Meira-Cartea, P.Á.; Gutiérrez-Pérez, J. Climate literacy among university students in Mexico and Spain: influence of scientific and popular culture in the representation of the causes of climate change. Int. J. Glob. Warm. 2017, 12, 448–467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Gaudiano, E.G.; CARTEA, P.M. Educación, comunicación y cambio climático. Resistencias para la acción social responsable. Trayectorias 2009, 11, 6–38. [Google Scholar]
  40. Meira, P.; Arto, M.; Barba, M. Conoce y Valora el Cambio Climático. Propuestas para Trabajar en Grupo; MAPFRE: Madrid, Spain, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  41. Fernández-Reyes, R.; Piñuel Raigada, J.L.; Vicente-Mariño, M. La cobertura periodística del cambio climático y del calentamiento global en El País, El Mundo y La Vanguardia. Revista Latina de Comunicación Social 2015, 70, 122–140. [Google Scholar]
  42. Mariño, M.V.; Torrico, D.V. Presencia y funciones del discurso científico en la cobertura informativa y cinematográfica del cambio climático. Prisma Social Revista de Investigación Social 2014, 12, 120–152. [Google Scholar]
  43. Working Group II, IPCC (Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change). Climate Change 2014: Impacts, Adaptation, and Vulnerability. Part A: Global and Sectorial Aspects. Contribution of Working Group II to the Fifth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK; New York, NY, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  44. Smith, E.K.; Mayer, A. A social trap for the climate? Collective action, trust and climate change risk perception in 35 countries. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2018, 49, 140–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Karl, T.R.; Melillo, J.M.; Peterson, T.C.; Hassol, S.J. Global Climate Change Impacts in the United States; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  46. Roldán, A.E.; Pérez, J.G.; Arto-Blanco, M.; Meira-Cartea, P.Á. La representación social del cambio climático en el alumnado universitario español de Ciencias e Ingeniería. Enseñanza de las Ciencias: Revista de Investigación y Experiencias Didácticas 2017, Núm. Extra, 1765–1770. [Google Scholar]
  47. García-Vinuesa, A.; Cartea, P.Á.M.; Blanco, M.A.; Bisquert, K.M. Social representations of climate change in a group of college students from the University of Santiago de Compostela: Common culture vs. scientific culture. Eco-Thinking 2019, 1. [Google Scholar]
  48. Meira-Cartea, P.A.; Gutiérrez-Pérez, J.; Arto-Blanco, M.; Escoz-Roldán, A. Influence of academic education vs. common culture on the climate literacy of university students/Formación académica frente a cultura común en la alfabetización climática de estudiantes universitarios. Psyecology 2018, 9, 301–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Pongiglione, F. The key role of causal explanation in the climate change issue. THEORIA. Revista de Teoría, Historia y Fundamentos de la Ciencia 2012, 27, 175–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Yuan, L.; Powell, S. MOOCs and Open Education: Implications for Higher Education; JISC CETIS: Bolton, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  51. Hansen, J.D.; Reich, J. Democratizing education? Examining access and usage patterns in massive open online courses. Science 2015, 350, 1245–1248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Kaplan, A.M.; Haenlein, M. Higher education and the digital revolution: About MOOCs, SPOCs, social media, and the Cookie Monster. Bus. Horizons 2016, 59, 441–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Campbell, D.T.; Stanley, J.C. Experimental and Quasi-Experimental Designs for Research. In Ravenio Books; Cengage Learning: Boston, MA, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  54. Awareness and Training on Climate Change for Primary and Secondary Teachers. Available online: https://miriadax.net/web/concienciacion-y-capacitacion-en-materia-de-cambio-climatico-para-profesores-de-primaria-y-secunda0 (accessed on 1 September 2019).
  55. Blas, E.S. Pre-Experimental Designs in Psychology and Education: A Conceptual Review. Liberabit 2013, 19, 133–141. [Google Scholar]
  56. Van Dalen, D.; Meyer, W. Manual de Técnicas de Investigación; Buenos Aires Editorial; Ediciones Paidós: Barcelona, Spain, 1971. [Google Scholar]
  57. Morales, P. Medición de Actitudes en Psicología y Educación. Tercera edición Revisada; Universidad Pontificia Comillas: Madrid, Spain, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  58. Teijón, P.H.; Sánchez, S.A.; Sevilla, M.I.A.; Ballegeer, A.M.; Fernández, D.C.; Martín, M.L.D.; Izquierdo-Álvarez, V.; López, S.L.; Gómez, J.M.S.; Méndez, C.R. A MOOC on the Science of Climate Change for primary and secondary teachers in Spanish. In Proceedings of the Sixth International Conference on Technological Ecosystems for Enhancing Multiculturality, Salamanca, Spain, 24–26 October 2018; pp. 525–529. [Google Scholar]
  59. Fernández Vítores, D. El español: una lengua viva. Informe 2018, 2018, 72. [Google Scholar]
  60. Kesim, M.; Altınpulluk, H. A theoretical analysis of MOOCs types from a perspective of learning theories. Procedia-Soc. Behav. Sci. 2015, 186, 15–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Merrill, M. First Principles of Instruction: Identifying and Designing Effective, Efficient and Engaging Instruction; Pfeiffer: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  62. Swain, M.; Lapkin, S. Focus on Form through Collaborative Dialogue: Exploring Task Effects; Researching Pedagogic Tasks; Routledge: Abingdon, UK, 2013; pp. 109–128. [Google Scholar]
  63. Guo, P.J.; Kim, J.; Rubin, R. How video production affects student engagement: An empirical study of MOOC videos. In Proceedings of the First ACM Conference on Learning@Scale Conference, Atlanta, GA, USA, 4–5 March 2014; pp. 41–50. [Google Scholar]
  64. Margaryan, A.; Bianco, M.; Littlejohn, A. Instructional quality of massive open online courses (MOOCs). Comput. Educ. 2015, 80, 77–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences; Routledge: Abingdon, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  66. Grissom, R.J.; Kim, J.J. Effect Sizes for Research: Univariate and Multivariate Applications; Routledge: Abingdon, UK, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  67. Lipsey, M.W. Design Sensitivity: Statistical Power for Experimental Research; Sage: Newcastle Upon Tyne, UK, 1990; Volume 19. [Google Scholar]
  68. Fischer, A.; Peters, V.; Neebe, M.; Vávra, J.; Kriel, A.; Lapka, M.; Megyesi, B. Climate change? No, wise resource use is the issue: Social representations of energy, climate change and the future. Environ. Policy Gov. 2012, 22, 161–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Whitmarsh, L.; Seyfang, G.; O’Neill, S. Public engagement with carbon and climate change: To what extent is the public ‘carbon capable’? Global Environ. Chang. 2011, 21, 56–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Lorenzoni, I.; Nicholson-Cole, S.; Whitmarsh, L. Barriers perceived to engaging with climate change among the UK public and their policy implications. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2007, 17, 445–459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Ashe, J.W.; Van Lierop, R.; Cherian, A. The Role of the Alliance of Small Island States (AOSIS) in the Negotiation of the United Nations Framework Convention on Climate Change (UNFCCC); Natural Resources Forum; Wiley Online Library: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 1999; Volume 23, pp. 209–220. [Google Scholar]
  72. Buijs, A.E.; Arts, B.J.; Elands, B.H.; Lengkeek, J. Beyond environmental frames: the social representation and cultural resonance of nature in conflicts over a Dutch woodland. Geoforum 2011, 42, 329–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Meira Cartea, P.Á. Comunicar el Cambio Climático: Escenario Social y Líneas de Acción; Ministerio de Medio Ambiente y Medio Rural y Marino: Madrid, Spain, 2007.
  74. Chawla, L.; Cushing, D.F. Education for strategic environmental behavior. Environ. Educ. Res. 2007, 13, 437–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Bradley, J.C.; Waliczek, T.M.; Zajicek, J.M. Relationship between environmental knowledge and environmental attitude of high school students. J. Environ. Educ. 1999, 30, 17–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Morant, N. Social representations and professional knowledge: The representation of mental illness among mental health practitioners. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2006, 45, 817–838. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. De Abreu, G.; Gorgorió, N. Social Representations and Multicultural Mathematics Teaching and Learning; Working Group 10 Mathematics Education in Multicultural Settings, Ed.; Department of Education, University of Cyprus: Larnaca, Cyprus, 2007; p. 1559. [Google Scholar]
  78. Chengjie, Y. Challenges and changes of MOOC to traditional classroom teaching mode. Can. Soc. Sci. 2015, 11, 135. [Google Scholar]
  79. Greiving, S.; Fleischhauer, M.; Lindner, C.; Lückenkötter, J.; Peltonen, L.; Juhola, S.; Niemi, P.; Vehmas, J.; Davoudi, S.; Achino, E.; et al. ESPON Climate—Climate Change and Territorial Effects on Regions and Local Economies; Appl. Res. Proj.; ESPON: Plaza Bristol, CT, USA, 2013; Volume 1. [Google Scholar]
Scheme 1. The Social Representation of Climate Change is made of three main components: The information, the structure and the attitude. The main idea of the paper is to act on the information component through a knowledge-based MOOC. By improving this component, we will reach an improved SR closer to the scientific perspective. As the SR is the main driver for mitigation and adaptation, it is expected to also improve these areas.
Scheme 1. The Social Representation of Climate Change is made of three main components: The information, the structure and the attitude. The main idea of the paper is to act on the information component through a knowledge-based MOOC. By improving this component, we will reach an improved SR closer to the scientific perspective. As the SR is the main driver for mitigation and adaptation, it is expected to also improve these areas.
Sustainability 11 06317 sch001
Figure 1. Comparison of scores between the original sample of the “Resclima study” [48] (blue line labelled USC) and the pretest of the MOOC sample (orange line). Both samples contain all the available populations. The questions are grouped according to the four basic areas used to design the MOOC: physical processes, causes, consequences and responses. On the horizontal axis, we present the number of the statement in the questionnaire grouped according to the four areas that we have designed in our study. On the vertical axis, the average score in a given question.
Figure 1. Comparison of scores between the original sample of the “Resclima study” [48] (blue line labelled USC) and the pretest of the MOOC sample (orange line). Both samples contain all the available populations. The questions are grouped according to the four basic areas used to design the MOOC: physical processes, causes, consequences and responses. On the horizontal axis, we present the number of the statement in the questionnaire grouped according to the four areas that we have designed in our study. On the vertical axis, the average score in a given question.
Sustainability 11 06317 g001
Figure 2. This figure presents a comparison between our pretest MOOC sample (orange bars) and the “Resclima study” [48] (blue bars). The bars at the top show a comparison of the averages over all statements. The rest of the bars show the comparison of the scores in each of the four areas that we have designed for our study.
Figure 2. This figure presents a comparison between our pretest MOOC sample (orange bars) and the “Resclima study” [48] (blue bars). The bars at the top show a comparison of the averages over all statements. The rest of the bars show the comparison of the scores in each of the four areas that we have designed for our study.
Sustainability 11 06317 g002
Figure 3. Scores for dependent variable knowledge in SR (average over the four areas).
Figure 3. Scores for dependent variable knowledge in SR (average over the four areas).
Sustainability 11 06317 g003
Figure 4. Averaged scores between pre- and posttest in the four areas that we have used in our study. The areas of causes and responses show the highest improvements.
Figure 4. Averaged scores between pre- and posttest in the four areas that we have used in our study. The areas of causes and responses show the highest improvements.
Sustainability 11 06317 g004
Figure 5. Improvement in teacher (at the working-age, 25–65 years) vs. non-teachers.
Figure 5. Improvement in teacher (at the working-age, 25–65 years) vs. non-teachers.
Sustainability 11 06317 g005
Figure 6. Improvement by origin: Spain or Latin America.
Figure 6. Improvement by origin: Spain or Latin America.
Sustainability 11 06317 g006
Table 1. Demographics of the sample.
Table 1. Demographics of the sample.
Pretest Participants530
ParticipantsFrequencyPercentage
Spain20238.1
Latin America31258.9
Others163.0
Gender
Females27251.3
Male25848.7
Occupation
Primary Teacher244.5
Secondary Teacher10519.8
University Teacher5710.8
Others Teacher5410.2
No Teacher13725.8
Student15328.9
Studies
Basic Education101.9
Secondary Education509.4
Vocational Training Media122.3
Vocational Training High6211.7
Uiversity Education39674.7
Table 2. Description of the contents of the MOOC.
Table 2. Description of the contents of the MOOC.
ModulesContents
0. Welcome
1. A changing climate. A scientific perspectiveWeather and climate and their study methods. Paleoclimatology: climate changes through the Earth History. Relationship between climate and humans.
2. Evidence of Climate ChangeClimate variability and Climate Change. Evidences of Climate Change: observations from the Atmosphere, Hydrosphere, Lithosphere and Cryosphere.
3. The functioning of Climate Change.Introduction to the physical-chemical processes that cause Climate Change. The energy balance. The greenhouse effect. The Carbon cycle. The impact of the Climate Change on the Biosphere and ecosystems.
4. Human activity as a cause of Climate Change.Impact of human activity on the planet. Origin of greenhouse gas emissions. The energy sector: basic concepts, consumption patterns. Impact of the food sector.
5. Future scenariosThe mathematics of the Climate Change. Computational models. Future possible scenarios.
6. What can we do from education?The socio-economic aspects of Climate Change. International agreements. Possible mitigation and adaptation strategies. The role of education. Legislative competencies and curricula related to Climate Change.
Table 3. Comparative analysis of pretest and posttest in Knowledge in SR.
Table 3. Comparative analysis of pretest and posttest in Knowledge in SR.
nMSDP25MedP75Hypothesis Contrast
ZK-S pMWU pd1- β
Knowledge in SRPretest4633.210.392.983.183.42−5.47<0.001<0.0010.470.99
Posttest2363.400.433.083.383.76
Table 4. Comparative analysis of pretest and posttest in the areas of knowledge in SR.
Table 4. Comparative analysis of pretest and posttest in the areas of knowledge in SR.
nMSDP25MedP75Hypothesis Contrast
ZK-S pMWU pd1- β
Area 1Pretest5163.210.492.883.253.62−4.03<0.001<0.0010.290.88
Posttest2563.360.533.003.383.88
Area 2Pretest5053.340.442.603.403.10−2.440.005<0.0010.200.50
Posttest2483.430.442.803.433.50
Area 3Pretest4963.150.433.003.103.70−8.42<0.001<0.0010.681.00
Posttest2463.440.423.103.403.90
Area 4Pretest5113.110.592.753.003.50−4.75<0.001<0.0010.360.98
Posttest2533.320.583.003.254.00

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Ferrari, E.; Ballegeer, A.-M.; Fuertes, M.A.; Herrero, P.; Delgado, L.; Corrochano, D.; Andrés-Sánchez, S.; Marc Bisquert, K.; Garcia-Vinuesa, A.; Meira, P.; et al. Improvement on Social Representation of Climate Change through a Knowledge-Based MOOC in Spanish. Sustainability 2019, 11, 6317. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11226317

AMA Style

Ferrari E, Ballegeer A-M, Fuertes MA, Herrero P, Delgado L, Corrochano D, Andrés-Sánchez S, Marc Bisquert K, Garcia-Vinuesa A, Meira P, et al. Improvement on Social Representation of Climate Change through a Knowledge-Based MOOC in Spanish. Sustainability. 2019; 11(22):6317. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11226317

Chicago/Turabian Style

Ferrari, Enzo, Anne-Marie Ballegeer, Miguel Angel Fuertes, Pablo Herrero, Laura Delgado, Diego Corrochano, Santiago Andrés-Sánchez, Kylyan Marc Bisquert, Antonio Garcia-Vinuesa, Pablo Meira, and et al. 2019. "Improvement on Social Representation of Climate Change through a Knowledge-Based MOOC in Spanish" Sustainability 11, no. 22: 6317. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11226317

APA Style

Ferrari, E., Ballegeer, A. -M., Fuertes, M. A., Herrero, P., Delgado, L., Corrochano, D., Andrés-Sánchez, S., Marc Bisquert, K., Garcia-Vinuesa, A., Meira, P., Martinez, F., & Ruiz, C. (2019). Improvement on Social Representation of Climate Change through a Knowledge-Based MOOC in Spanish. Sustainability, 11(22), 6317. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11226317

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop