Next Article in Journal
When Culture Materializes: Societal Dynamics in Resilience of Social-Ecological Systems in the Case of Conch Management on Abaco, The Bahamas
Previous Article in Journal
Strategic Assessment and Economic Evaluation: The Case Study of Yanzhou Island (China)
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Nutrition Labeling Schemes and the Time and Effort of Consumer Processing

by
Manuel Alonso-Dos-Santos
1,*,
René Quilodrán Ulloa
1,
Álvaro Salgado Quintana
1,
Diego Vigueras Quijada
1 and
Pablo Farías Nazel
2
1
Administration Department, Universidad Católica de la Santísima Concepción, oncepción 4090541, Chile
2
Departamento de Administración, Facultad de Economía y Negocios, Universidad de Chile, Santiago 8330015, Chile
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sustainability 2019, 11(4), 1079; https://doi.org/10.3390/su11041079
Submission received: 23 December 2018 / Revised: 31 January 2019 / Accepted: 13 February 2019 / Published: 19 February 2019

Abstract

:
Guideline Daily Amount (GDA) and nutrition tables are the most used front-of-pack (FOP) nutrition labeling schemes in the world; however, they are hard to process considering the nutritional knowledge, effort, and time needed for interpretation. Consumers spend little time and effort evaluating food products. Consumers are selective, and FOP nutrition labeling schemes should be too. Recent studies have shown that warning messages—a new FOP nutrition labeling scheme—improves consumers’ ability to correctly identify less-healthy products. This study proposes that warning messages are also easier to process. Using eye-tracking, this study demonstrates that warning messages require less processing effort and time than GDA and nutrition tables. This study also shows that females process warning messages easier than males. Additionally, this study found no significant differences between physically active and inactive consumers in their processing of warning messages. The results are robust across product categories and brands.

1. Introduction

As so many foods are high in salt, saturated fat and/or sugar, eating habits do not always follow current dietary guidelines, although they do have a great environmental effect [1,2]. For example, an obese person produces one ton more of carbon emissions than a thin person [3]. Guideline Daily Amount (GDA) and nutrition tables (nutrition facts labels, nutrition information panels) are the most used front-of-pack (FOP) nutrition labeling schemes in the world; however, these are difficult to process for consumers considering the nutritional knowledge, effort and time needed for interpretation [4]. Both FOP nutrition labeling schemes take time and effort that would be a deterrent in real-life situations [5]. Grocery shopping usually entails low involvement with limited information search [6]. Consumers spend little time and effort evaluating food products [7]. Consumers are selective processors and FOP nutrition labeling schemes should be too [8,9].
In Chile (a country with a concerning number of overweight citizens), packaged foods have since June 2016 been required to include warning messages in the shape of a black octagon (reminiscent of a stop sign) on the FOP with the text “High in…” (“Alto en…”) to indicate when food products exceed certain levels of sugar, saturated fat, sodium or calories (see Figure 1) [10].
Recent studies in Uruguay and Brazil (both countries with concerning numbers of overweight citizens) have shown that warning messages (rather than other FOP nutrition labeling schemes such as GDA, the traffic-light system, etc.) improved consumers’ ability to correctly identify a less-healthy product [11,12,13,14]. Fifteen countries have expressed interest in the Chilean regulation and have requested information to consider the warning messages in their regulatory projects [10]. However, as of today no other country in the world uses warning messages. This should be enough to consider evaluating whether it is the best option.

1.1. Front-of-Pack (FOP) Nutrition Labeling Schemes and the Time and Effort of Consumer Processing

Product labels influence consumer processing [11]. Due to the variety of FOP nutrition labeling schemes, it should be expected that different FOP nutrition labeling schemes differ in their effects on consumer processing [12,13,14,15]. In the few seconds that consumers spend selecting food products they do not process all the information available on product labels [16]. Previous research has found that easier processing often results in greater liking of the product [17]. Metacognitive experience of differential processing ease, referred to as processing fluency, affects subsequent consumer judgments. In general, an increase in processing fluency leads to more-positive evaluations [18]. Therefore, an increase in processing fluency of FOP nutrition labeling schemes would lead to making healthy choices.
Warning messages could be easier to spot and interpret than GDA and nutrition tables for several reasons. First, warning messages include images (stop signs) and words (e.g., “high in sodium”). Redundant information simultaneously perceived through two channels (images and words) actually speeds up processing time [19]. Second, warning messages include a black octagon in the form of a stop sign. Bialkova and van Trijp [20] reported that consumer processing was faster and more accurate for labels with monochromatic rather than polychromatic color schemes. Third, warning messages include less information than GDA and nutrition tables. GDA and nutrition tables could be processed more globally and more abstractly than warning messages, the latter of which are processed more locally and concretely. Construal level theory suggests that local processing and concrete thinking accentuate both affective responses and fluency-based intuitive responses, which could lead to shorter processing times for warning messages [21]. Additionally, consumers experienced difficulties in understanding the nutritional information displayed on GDA and nutrition tables [4,5]. GDA and nutrition tables are difficult to understand for many, including children, and do not lend themselves to quick comparisons. Warning messages may be easier to process than GDA and nutrition tables. Hence:
Hypothesis 1.
Warning messages require less processing time and effort than GDA and nutrition tables.

1.2. The Role of Gender

Gender difference has been widely documented in prior literature [22]. Men are selective processors. In contrast, women are comprehensive processors who attempt to assimilate all the available information (e.g., nutrition information, brands, sizes, ingredients, prices) before making a judgment [23,24]. Consequently, it was hypothesized that consumer processing is faster triggered by warning messages in females than in males. Hence:
Hypothesis 2.
Females process warning messages easier than males.

1.3. The Role of Physical Activity

Warning messages could be equally efficient among physically active and inactive consumers. Previous studies have not found significant differences in the use and understanding of nutrition labels between gym and non-gym users [25,26]. Thus, one would expect no differences between physically active and inactive consumers in their processing times and efforts in reading warning messages. Therefore, the following hypothesis is proposed:
Hypothesis 3.
There are no significant differences between physically active and inactive consumers in their processing times and efforts in reading warning messages.

2. Methods

Following previous studies [16,27,28,29,30], this research used eye-tracking to measure the time and effort it takes to carry out psychological processes. A full factorial design in which product category (within subjects, random sequence: chocolate cookies, whole wheat bread, and Greek yogurt [27,29]), brand (between subjects: well-known and unknown brand) and FOP nutrition labeling schemes (between subjects: GDA, nutrition table and warning messages) were considered as factors was followed. After having completed the eye-tracking task, participants were asked to answer a questionnaire related to food habits and the participants’ levels of physical activity.

2.1. Sample

For convenience, the sample selection was conducted on a university campus in Chile. The fieldwork was performed in the technology research lab at the Universidad Católica de la Santísima Concepción during the first week of December in 2016. Chile is the country with the third highest rate of overweight people in the Americas, reaching 27.8% of the total Chilean population in 2014 [31]. The sample consisted of 90 men and 90 women with ages ranging from 18 to 61 (average age = 23.4, standard deviation = 8.9). In the present study 6 groups (balanced based on gender and age) of 30 consumers each were considered. Each consumer group was randomly assigned to an experimental condition. All of the participants had normal visual capacity and no known vision problems. All participants were duly informed of the test procedures and required to provide written informed consent prior to taking the test.

2.2. Procedures

Each product label was reproduced according to its actual size, in accordance with Chilean regulations (see Figure 2). Product labels were shown at random on a 24” Lenovo screen, with 1280 × 1024-pixel resolution. A preliminary trial was given to participants at the beginning of the experiment in order to achieve the subjects’ visual concentration before starting the actual test. Prior to each test, a distance of 65 centimeters was evaluated and measured between the participants and the screen.
The experiment used the EyeTribe system, which is considered to be a reliable tool used with Ogama software [32]. The system has a sampling rate of 60 Hz (similar to the Tobii pro system). The average precision is 0.5 degrees of visual angle with a spatial resolution of 0.1°. The device has latency under 20 milliseconds and permits 16 points in the calibration process. It allows horizontal and vertical head movements up to 75 centimeters. The EyeTribe system has proven to be a reliable system for eye-tracking measurements [33].
During the experiment, participants viewed each product label for nine seconds, alternating between a two-second-long black screen inserted in between product labels, as Gülçay and Cangöz [34] previously did in their study. At the end of the test, participants took an adapted survey about food habits [35,36] and their levels of physical activity [37]. There were no significant differences in the questionnaire responses between experimental conditions.

3. Results

The information obtained for each area of interest (in this study, the FOP nutrition labeling schemes) included the complete fixation time (CFT), the time to first fixation (TFF) and the number of fixations (NFs). Fixation count is related to the processing of information and can indicate difficulty in visual processing [16]. To examine the levels of consumer processing between FOP nutrition labeling schemes, a nonparametric analysis was conducted for CFT, TFF and NFs.
The average of TFF was 2.349 milliseconds (ms) for the warning message (nutrition table = 2.397 ms, GDA = 2.184 ms). The TFF was not significantly different between the different FOP nutrition labeling schemes, which may indicate that consumers do not feel more or less attraction to any specific FOP nutrition labeling scheme.
The results show that the CFT and NF indicators differed among the three FOP nutrition labeling schemes. Specifically, the mean of CFT was 1.028 ms for the warning message (nutrition table = 2.698 ms, GDA = 2.003 ms). The mean of NFs was 2.48 fixations for the warning message (nutrition table = 8.49, GDA = 6.56). The results are robust across product categories and brands (see Table 1). These results suggest that warning messages are easier to process than GDA and nutrition tables. Therefore, H1 is supported.
Figure 3 presents the average values of CFT for the three FOP nutrition labeling schemes. It is important to note that there were significant differences when comparing all pairs using Tukey-Kramer HSD. For example, there is a significant difference between GDA and the nutritional table (p-value < 0.05).
For the three indicators, females processed warning messages easier than males. To determine whether there is a difference in the consumer processing of warning messages based on gender, a nonparametric analysis was performed. Table 2 shows the average ranges and significance of the Wilcoxon/Mann–Whitney test. The results show that there were significant differences between men and women in two of the three indicators in regard to the warning message, specifically in terms of NFs and CFT. Therefore, H2 is supported.
In regard to the consumer processing of warning messages based on physical activity as stated by the participants, another nonparametric analysis was conducted for the three indicators. Table 3 shows the average ranges and significance of the Wilcoxon/Mann-Whitney test. Physically active consumers are people who engage in physical activity at least three times per week. The results show no significant differences between the subjects based on their stated levels of physical activity (active vs. inactive consumers). Therefore, H3 is supported.

4. Discussion

Previous studies have shown that warnings messages (rather than other FOP nutrition labeling schemes such as GDA, the traffic-light system, etc.) improved consumers’ ability to correctly identify a less-healthy product [11,12,13,14]. This study provides another advantage of warning messages, that warning messages are processed more easily than nutritional tables and GDA (the most-used FOP nutrition labeling scheme in the world [38]). The results show that NFs and CFT are less for warning messages than for nutrition tables and GDA, indicating that consumers require less effort and time to process warning messages than nutrition tables and GDA. These findings suggest that warning messages are easier to process. From an information processing perspective, warning messages have advantages over GDA and nutrition tables. Therefore, warning messages could be a consumer-friendly way of communicating nutrition information (vs. GDA and nutritional tables) [39] and may increase the likelihood that FOP nutrition labels are used by consumers [40].
The results also indicate differences between men and women, as found in prior studies [22,23,24]. The results show that women have significantly less NFs and CFT. According to the eye-tracking results, women processed warning messages easier than men. These results are encouraging, because women have a higher rate of obesity compared to men across countries [41]. Moreover, in most countries, women are the main shoppers of the food consumed by a household [42].
In regard to the influence of levels of physical activity, this study found no significant differences among physically active and inactive consumers in their processing of warning messages, which is consistent with previous studies [25,26]. This could mean that warning messages are equally efficient among physically active and inactive consumers, and therefore the implementation of warning messages has the potential to influence sustainable consumer decision-making among physically inactive consumers in the food domain.
Apart from the health risks linked to obesity, it also puts a significant social and economic burden on society [43]. Additionally, hyper-consumption and obesity have caused environmental problems. For example, since obese people eat more and are more likely to use cars than to walk, the obese population could increase the demand for food, fuel, etc. [1,2,3,44]. As of today, only Chile uses warning messages, but the results of this research suggest that more countries should use warning messages, which is consistent with previous findings [11,12,13,14]. The use of warning messages instead of other FOP nutrition labeling schemes is a cost-effective way to inform consumers of the quantity of sugar, saturated fat, sodium and calories in each product, as warning messages improve the capacity to recognize an unhealthy food. If countries incorporate warning messages into packaging they could help consumers choose healthier lifestyles and upgrade their ability to effectively interpret nutrition information. Therefore, more countries should contemplate the mandatory implementation of warning messages as the main FOP nutrition labeling scheme to achieve health improvements by changing eating behavioral attitudes and purchasing behavior. The impact of an easier FOP nutrition labeling scheme is related to preventing confusion because it ensures that customers can accurately select foods with lower levels of sugar, saturated fat, sodium or calories.
In Chile, food products use warning messages and this study shows that warning messages reduce the effort and time people spend at the store, and that consumers can make smart decisions because they comprehend the nutritional content in less time. As a consequence, consumers receive more information in a simple image. In countries where warning messages are mandatory, companies should consider product reformulation [45], i.e., changing certain ingredients to reduce warning messages on food packaging. The results also suggest that food products targeting women have a greater incentive to consider product reformulation because women process warning messages easier than men. In other countries, to improve the health of their consumers [46], companies could voluntarily implement warning messages for their less-healthy food products.
In regard to future research and the limitations of this study, caution should be exercised when interpreting the results of this experiment, primarily because the sample was selected out of convenience. Furthermore, the selected products did not represent a typical food basket and the FOP nutrition labeling schemes used in this study did not represent the full spectrum of FOP nutrition labeling schemes on an international scale. Therefore, future studies could include other samples (e.g., different demographics in other countries such as children [13], adolescents and older adults), other product categories (e.g., well-known unhealthy foods such as ice creams, frozen French fries and potato chips), and other FOP nutrition labeling schemes (e.g., traffic-light system and star-based labels [47]). Such studies could increase the generalizability of the results. Additionally, future studies could include other dependent variables such as the perception of product attributes, purchase intention, brand loyalty, word-of-mouth and so on, providing further insights on this issue. Because all variables were measured at the same day, this study did not completely cover the long-term effects of FOP nutrition labeling schemes. Therefore, further research using longitudinal data could complement this study.

5. Conclusions

This study was aimed at exploring consumer processing of a new FOP nutrition labeling scheme recently implemented in Chile: warning messages. For this purpose, an eye-tracking study comparing warning messages to GDA and nutrition tables was carried out. In line with previous research [16,27,28,29,30], this study used eye-tracking to measure the time and effort it takes to carry out psychological processes, which can help to gain insights about fundamental processes underlying FOP nutrition labeling schemes.
This study showed that warning messages meet the key objective of providing clear information that is easy for consumers to process. Warning messages are more efficient as they require less time and effort to process, especially in the case of women, and regardless of the subject’s level of physical activity. In summary, this study showed that warning messages are not only more effective in achieving consumers’ ability to correctly identify less-healthy products [11,12,13,14], but also that warning messages are more efficient because they require less time and effort to be processed by consumers. The research report in this paper provided a platform for managers and government bodies to develop strategies that will foster healthy eating decisions amongst consumers.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.A.D., R.Q.U., A.S.Q., D.V.Q. and P.F.; Methodology, M.A.D.; Software, M.A.D.; Validation, M.A.D.; Formal Analysis, M.A.D.; Investigation, M.A.D., R.Q.U., A.S.Q. and D.V.Q.; Writing—Original Draft Preparation, M.A.D., R.Q.U., A.S.Q., D.V.Q. and P.F.; Writing—Review & Editing, M.A.D., R.Q.U., A.S.Q., D.V.Q. and P.F.; Visualization, M.A.D.; Supervision, M.A.D.; Project Administration, M.A.D.

Funding

The authors have no funding to report.

Acknowledgments

Three anonymous reviewers greatly improved the quality of this manuscript and we thank them for their contributions.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Liu, Q.; Yan, Z.; Zhou, J. Consumer choices and motives for eco-labeled products in China: An empirical analysis based on the choice experiment. Sustainability 2017, 9, 331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Pfeiffer, C.; Speck, M.; Strassner, C. What leads to lunch—How social practices impact (non-) sustainable food consumption/eating habits. Sustainability 2017, 9, 1437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Edwards, P.; Roberts, I. Population adiposity and climate change. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2009, 38, 1137–1140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. De la Cruz-Góngora, V.; Torres, P.; Contreras-Manzano, A.; Jáuregui de la Mota, A.; Mundo-Rosas, V.; Villalpando, S.; Rodríguez-Oliveros, G. Understanding and acceptability by Hispanic consumers of four front-of-pack food labels. Intern. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2017, 14, 28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Draper, A.K.; Adamson, A.J.; Clegg, S.; Malam, S.; Rigg, M.; Duncan, S. Front-of-pack nutrition labelling: Are multiple formats a problem for consumers? Eur. J. Public Health 2013, 23, 517–521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Choi, S.; Lee, S.; Friske, W. The effects of featured advertising and package labeling on sustainability of cause-related marketing (CRM) products. Sustainability 2018, 10, 3011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Nenycz-Thiel, M.; Sharp, B.; Dawes, J.; Romaniuk, J. Competition for memory retrieval between private label and national brands. J. Bus. Res. 2010, 63, 1142–1147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Jacoby, J. Perspectives on information overload. J. Consum. Res. 1984, 10, 432–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Pieniak, Z.; Verbeke, W.; Scholderer, J.; Brunsø, K.; Olsen, S.O. European consumers’ use of and trust in information sources about fish. Food Qual. Prefer. 2007, 18, 1050–1063. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. DIPOL. Balance de gestión de la División de Políticas Públicas Saludables y Promoción 2014–2018; DIPOL: Santiago, Chile, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  11. Ares, G.; Varela, F.; Machin, L.; Antúnez, L.; Giménez, A.; Curutchet, M.R.; Aschemann-Witzel, J. Comparative performance of three interpretative front-of-pack nutrition labelling schemes: Insights for policy making. Food Qual. Prefer. 2018, 68, 215–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Arrúa, A.; Machín, L.; Curutchet, M.R.; Martínez, J.; Antúnez, L.; Alcaire, F.; Giménez, A.; Ares, G. Warnings as a directive front-of-pack nutrition labelling scheme: Comparison with the Guideline Daily Amount and traffic-light systems. Public Health Nutr. 2017, 20, 2308–2317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Lima, M.; Ares, G.; Deliza, R. How do front of pack nutrition labels affect healthfulness perception of foods targeted at children? Insights from Brazilian children and parents. Food Qual. Prefer. 2018, 64, 111–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Machín, L.; Cabrera, M.; Curutchet, M.R.; Martínez, J.; Giménez, A.; Ares, G. Consumer perception of the healthfulness of ultra-processed products featuring different front-of-pack nutrition labeling schemes. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 2017, 49, 330–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Grunert, K.G.; Fernández-Celemín, L.; Wills, J.M.; Storcksdieck genannt Bonsmann, S.; Nureeva, L. Use and understanding of nutrition information on food labels in six European countries. J. Public Health 2010, 18, 261–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  16. Oliveira, D.; Machín, L.; Deliza, R.; Rosenthal, A.; Walter, E.H.; Giménez, A.; Ares, G. Consumers’ attention to functional food labels: Insights from eye-tracking and change detection in a case study with probiotic milk. LWT Food Sci. Technol. 2016, 68, 160–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. King, D.; Janiszewski, C. The Sources and Consequences of the Fluent Processing of Numbers. J. Mark. Res. 2011, 48, 327–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Leonhardt, J.M.; Catlin, J.R.; Pirouz, D.M. Is your product facing the ad’s center? Facing direction affects processing fluency and ad evaluation. J. Advert. 2015, 44, 315–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Ding, W.; Marchionini, G.; Soergel, D. Multimodal surrogates for video browsing. In Proceedings of the Fourth ACM Conference on Digital Libraries, Berkeley, CA, USA, 11–14 August 1999; pp. 85–93. [Google Scholar]
  20. Bialkova, S.; van Trijp, H. What determines consumer attention to nutrition labels? Food Qual. Prefer. 2010, 21, 1042–1051. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Reimann, M.; Castaño, R.; Zaichkowsky, J.; Bechara, A. Novel versus familiar brands: An analysis of neurophysiology, response latency, and choice. Mark. Lett. 2012, 23, 745–759. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Kim, E.; Lee, S.; Lee, J. Reaching sustainable financial health: Gender differences in risk-taking patterns of financially excluded people. Sustainability 2018, 10, 1289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Miquel, M.-J.; Caplliure, E.-M.; Pérez, C.; Bigné, E. Buying private label in durables: Gender and other psychological variables. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 2017, 34, 349–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Putrevu, S. Exploring the origins and information processing differences between men and women: Implications for advertisers. Acad. Mark. Sci. Rev. 2001, 10, 1–14. [Google Scholar]
  25. Damian-Bastidas, N.; Chala-Florencio, R.J.; Chavez-Blanco, R.; Mayta-Tristan, P. Reading, use and interpretation of nutritional labels by fitness center users in Huancayo, Peru 2015. Nutr. Hosp. 2016, 33, 1410–1417. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  26. Wade, S.; Kennedy, O.B. Does gym use impact upon nutritional knowledge? Br. Food J. 2010, 112, 44–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Ares, G.; Mawad, F.; Giménez, A.; Maiche, A. Influence of rational and intuitive thinking styles on food choice: Preliminary evidence from an eye-tracking study with yogurt labels. Food Qual. Prefer. 2014, 31, 28–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Mawad, F.; Trías, M.; Giménez, A.; Maiche, A.; Ares, G. Influence of cognitive style on information processing and selection of yogurt labels: Insights from an eye-tracking study. Food Res. Int. 2015, 74, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Rebollar, R.; Lidón, I.; Martín, J.; Puebla, M. The identification of viewing patterns of chocolate snack packages using eye-tracking techniques. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015, 39, 251–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Varela, P.; Antúnez, L.; Silva Cadena, R.; Giménez, A.; Ares, G. Attentional capture and importance of package attributes for consumers’ perceived similarities and differences among products: A case study with breakfast cereal packages. Food Res. Int. 2014, 64, 701–710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. World Health Organization. World Health Statistics 2016: Monitoring Health for the SDGs Sustainable Development Goals; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  32. Voßkühler, A.; Nordmeier, V.; Kuchinke, L.; Jacobs, A.M. OGAMA (Open Gaze and Mouse Analyzer): Open-source software designed to analyze eye and mouse movements in slideshow study designs. Behav. Res. Methods 2008, 40, 1150–1162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Popelka, S.; Stachoň, Z.; Šašinka, Č.; Doležalová, J. EyeTribe tracker data accuracy evaluation and its interconnection with hypothesis software for cartographic purposes. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2016, 2016, e9172506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Gülçay, Ç.; Cangöz, B. Effects of emotion and perspective on remembering events: An eye-tracking study. J. Eye Mov. Res. 2016, 9, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Din, N.; Zahari, M.S.M.; Shariff, S.M. Customer Perception on Nutritional Information in Restaurant Menu. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 2012, 42, 413–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Pino, J.L.; Diaz, C.; Lopez, M.Á. Construcción y validación de un cuestionario para medir conductas y hábitos alimentarios en usuarios de la atención primaria de salud. Rev. Chil. Nutr. 2011, 38, 41–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Serón, P.; Muñoz, S.; Lanas, F. Nivel de actividad física medida a través del cuestionario internacional de actividad física en población chilena. Rev. Med. Chile 2010, 138, 1232–1239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Ducrot, P.; Méjean, C.; Julia, C.; Kesse-Guyot, E.; Touvier, M.; Fezeu, L.; Hercberg, S.; Péneau, S. Objective understanding of front-of-package nutrition labels among nutritionally at-risk individuals. Nutrients 2015, 7, 7106–7125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Siegrist, M.; Leins-Hess, R.; Keller, C. Which front-of-pack nutrition label is the most efficient one? The results of an eye-tracker study. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015, 39, 183–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Acton, R.B.; Vanderlee, L.; Roberto, C.A.; Hammond, D. Consumer perceptions of specific design characteristics for front-of-package nutrition labels. Health Educ. Res. 2018, 33, 167–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Global Health Observatory (GHO). 2015. Available online: https://www.who.int/gho/en/ (accessed on 18 February 2019).
  42. Olavarrieta, S.; Hidalgo, P.; Manzur, E.; Farías, P. Determinants of in-store price knowledge for packaged products: An empirical study in a Chilean hypermarket. J. Bus. Res. 2012, 65, 1759–1766. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. MacRae, R.; Szabo, M.; Anderson, K.; Louden, F.; Trillo, S. Empowering the citizen-consumer: Re-regulating consumer information to support the transition to sustainable and health promoting food systems in Canada. Sustainability 2012, 4, 2146–2175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Figueroa-García, E.; García-Machado, J.; Pérez-Bustamante Yábar, D. Modeling the social factors that determine sustainable consumption behavior in the community of Madrid. Sustainability 2018, 10, 2811. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Kanter, R.; Vanderlee, L.; Vandevijvere, S. Front-of-package nutrition labelling policy: global progress and future directions. Public Health Nutr. 2018, 21, 1399–1408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Shnayder, L.; Van Rijnsoever, F.J.; Hekkert, M.P. Motivations for Corporate Social Responsibility in the packaged food industry: an institutional and stakeholder management perspective. J. Clean. Prod. 2016, 122, 212–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Lundeberg, P.J.; Graham, D.J.; Mohr, G.S. Comparison of two front-of-package nutrition labeling schemes, and their explanation, on consumers’ perception of product healthfulness and food choice. Appetite 2018, 125, 548–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Warning messages in Chile.
Figure 1. Warning messages in Chile.
Sustainability 11 01079 g001
Figure 2. Fixation map with a rainbow color palette displaying the average fixation time on different warning messages.
Figure 2. Fixation map with a rainbow color palette displaying the average fixation time on different warning messages.
Sustainability 11 01079 g002
Figure 3. Average CFT by FOP nutrition labeling scheme.
Figure 3. Average CFT by FOP nutrition labeling scheme.
Sustainability 11 01079 g003
Table 1. Wilcoxon/Mann–Whitney test using front-of-pack (FOP) nutrition labeling schemes and type of brand (GDA: Guideline Daily Amount; CFT: Complete Fixation Time; TFF Time to First Fixation; NF Number of Fixations).
Table 1. Wilcoxon/Mann–Whitney test using front-of-pack (FOP) nutrition labeling schemes and type of brand (GDA: Guideline Daily Amount; CFT: Complete Fixation Time; TFF Time to First Fixation; NF Number of Fixations).
Type of BrandIndicatorFOP Nutrition Labeling SchemeAverage Rangep-Value
Well-known brandsCFTWarning message36.390.021 *
Nutrition table54.73
GDA47.2
TFFWarning message48.340.784
Nutrition table43.68
GDA45.9
NFWarning message30.950.000 *
Nutrition table57.83
GDA49.72
Unknown BrandsCFTWarning message26.730.000 *
Nutrition table63.53
GDA54.22
TFFWarning message45.860.641
Nutrition table51.76
GDA46.5
NFWarning message23.110.002 *
Nutrition table63.68
GDA57.7
* p < 0.05.
Table 2. Wilcoxon/Mann–Whitney test by gender.
Table 2. Wilcoxon/Mann–Whitney test by gender.
FOP Nutrition Labeling SchemeIndicatorGenderAverage Rangep-Value
Warning messageNFMen36.080.027 *
Women26.08
TFFMen32.40.544
Women29.64
CFTMen36.50.017 *
Women25.67
* p < 0.05.
Table 3. Wilcoxon/Mann–Whitney test by level of physical activity.
Table 3. Wilcoxon/Mann–Whitney test by level of physical activity.
FOP Nutrition Labeling SchemeIndicatorLevel of Physical ActivityAverage Rangep-Value
Warning messageCFTActive34.090.557
Inactive25.88
TFFActive32.210.486
Inactive31.38
NFActive33.980.544
Inactive26.19
* p < 0.05.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Alonso-Dos-Santos, M.; Quilodrán Ulloa, R.; Salgado Quintana, Á.; Vigueras Quijada, D.; Farías Nazel, P. Nutrition Labeling Schemes and the Time and Effort of Consumer Processing. Sustainability 2019, 11, 1079. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11041079

AMA Style

Alonso-Dos-Santos M, Quilodrán Ulloa R, Salgado Quintana Á, Vigueras Quijada D, Farías Nazel P. Nutrition Labeling Schemes and the Time and Effort of Consumer Processing. Sustainability. 2019; 11(4):1079. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11041079

Chicago/Turabian Style

Alonso-Dos-Santos, Manuel, René Quilodrán Ulloa, Álvaro Salgado Quintana, Diego Vigueras Quijada, and Pablo Farías Nazel. 2019. "Nutrition Labeling Schemes and the Time and Effort of Consumer Processing" Sustainability 11, no. 4: 1079. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11041079

APA Style

Alonso-Dos-Santos, M., Quilodrán Ulloa, R., Salgado Quintana, Á., Vigueras Quijada, D., & Farías Nazel, P. (2019). Nutrition Labeling Schemes and the Time and Effort of Consumer Processing. Sustainability, 11(4), 1079. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11041079

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop