4.1. Discussion
In this paper an experimental study aimed at detecting causal relationships between sustainable labeling (in the form of third-party labels or self-declared claim) and consumers’ perceptions (concerning product quality and company’s reliability), purchase intention, and willingness to pay, is presented. Furthermore, the role of another variable (the credibility of the sustainability message), considered to be a hypothetical mediator between the examined relationships, is taken into consideration.
Surprisingly, there are no significant relationships between TPLs and the dependent variables. Third-party labels, “alone”, do not make the difference. This finding is inconsistent with the results of various previous studies [
38,
41,
67], which argued that certifications delivered by independent third parties were perceived more favorably by consumers (than self-declarations) but reinforces Dekhili and Achabou [
34] conclusions, who found that self-declarations influenced consumer preferences in virtually the same way as independent certifications. Moreover, this is in line with the Ertz et al. study [
47], which showed that third-party environmental labeling alone did not significantly alter consumers’ evaluations of product quality, credibility, and their willingness to pay.
The reasons why the TPLs, although correctly associated with an external certification by the participants in this study, did not impact, as expected, on their perceptions (i.e., trust in product quality and corporate reliability), intentions, and willingness to pay might be better understood in light of some considerations:
Information implied by socio-environmental labels must be noticed [
38,
45], understood in their basic meaning/purpose, and perceived as credible by the customers. In other words, labels can act as effective signals only to the degree that consumers deem them both useful and credible [
68].
With reference to food products and especially to a special, “self-indulgent treat” product, like chocolate, other aspects (like the taste, price, brand, etc.) could maintain greater importance than socio-environmental attributes in the habitual purchasing path, followed by the consumer [
49].
Previous studies pointed out that labeling schemes were effective only if consumers were informed and aware of their meaning, characteristics, requirements, etc. [
69,
70,
71,
72,
73,
74]. If the labels are unknown or their meaning unclear, even the most sustainability-oriented consumer cannot use them [
71].
In this study, it was evident that even though participants were able to visually recognize the TPL’s (as the manipulation check confirmed), respondents showed an overall limited understanding of the labels’ purposes. This problem was not specifically related to the respondents’ selection adopted in the study, since it appeared in several other studies that showed that the overall knowledge on labeling and socio-environmental labels in general terms is limited [
70,
71], and varies across countries [
75,
76]. A European on-line survey of around 4000 consumers in six countries (UK, France, Germany, Spain, Sweden, and Poland) found that people had a limited understanding of the actual meaning of four selected labels (Fair Trade, Rainforest Alliance, Carbon Footprint, and Animal Welfare) and highlighted the existence of considerable differences across the six countries considered [
77]. Similar cross-country differences also emerged in other studies [
71,
72,
76], suggesting that the diversified level of prominence of sustainability issues on the public agenda can influence the salience of the concept, in the mind of consumers.
In addition to the national setting, other determinants of understanding of sustainability labels were found to be related to age (with younger respondents showing a higher level of understanding than the overall population) and levels of education, with higher levels of understanding for higher levels of education [
71].
In other words, the goal of sustainability labels is to reduce information asymmetry between the producer and the consumer regarding the socio-environmental attributes of a product, but this goal can be reached only if the consumer is motivated and sustains a cognitive effort in information processing and detailed decision-making. Highly educated individuals are the ones who are more likely to own the cognitive resources needed to sustain a high effort adoption process, which is required in relation to certification schemes [
73,
78], to understand the meaning of labels and to process it for the purpose of the purchase decision. However, when consumers fear the risk of being cheated [
21,
22] but also when they perceive a sensation of “information overload”, engendered by the increasing number of labeling schemes, substantial confusion might be generated in their mind [
79,
80], reducing their ability to decide or producing even negative reactions toward labels [
72,
73,
74].
Additional cognitive effort on the part of the consumer is therefore needed in these occasions, as well as when the label is poorly communicated and is not capable of being sufficiently self-explanatory. In this context, previous studies outlined [
71] that some labels communicated themselves fairly well even to respondents who had not seen them before, while others were not that able. Creating a self-explanatory label is a challenge, made even more complex by the limited space available on the packaging, which makes it difficult to communicate its meaning while grabbing consumers’ attention. The results from this study (
Section 3.2) confirm what already emerged in past literature [
49]—information related to the socio-environmental commitment of the producer is not the first element that the consumer considers when he has to buy a food product, especially a self-indulgent one like chocolate. Nevertheless, sustainability information becomes meaningful when other purchasing criteria (such as price and taste) are satisfied [
79], because in this case a private benefit, such as the pleasure of eating chocolate, is jointly obtained with a public one, like social-environmental protection [
74]. At these conditions, sustainability schemes act as a heuristic tool, provided that they are noticed and easily interpreted. From this point of view, we can understand the reasons why, in the present study, a self-declared claim, expressed in a textual, well-focused, and unambiguous way, proved to be more effective than third-party logos; being capable of transferring, with a minimum decoding effort, the intended message to the consumer, the textual message was more easily incorporated into consumers’ chains of comprehension, action, and response.
In fact, multiple regression revealed that self-declared claims, especially in the case of a formal tone of voice, played a particularly relevant role in influencing the dependent variables. Contrary to the expectations and denying the hypothesis H2b, when the self-declared claim was delivered through a more friendly copy, it turned out to be less effective than when it was conveyed with a more formal tone of voice.
The most incisive role of the formal claim can be explained first by considering that when confronted with socio-environmental issues, respondents were more sensitive to formal messages which appeared to be more reassuring (than a playful message winking at the consumer) and helped them to banish the specter of greenwashing [
21,
22], reducing the sensation of information overload [
79,
80]. Participants in this study had an even greater need for reassurance because they were exposed to the product packaging of an unknown (fictitious) brand, so they could not base their assessments on the previous trust or corporate reputation.
These are probably the main reasons why the friendly claim “alone” turned out to be less effective than the formal one in relation to all dependent variables, with the sole exception of the willingness to pay. This specific finding might be partially explained by considering that the consumer had little to lose, in an experimental condition, by rewarding the corporation and evaluating it positively for coming up with a “nice message” about sustainability. Consumers appreciation for an emotional copy claim (perceived as more “likable” or “enjoyable”) might have induced some of the interviewees to respond on the wave of the emotional impulse (exactly what the claim wanted to evoke) by declaring their willingness to pay a higher price. However, when it comes to evaluating the intention to purchase, the confidence in the quality of the product or the reliability of the company, the emotional wave tends to subside and the friendly claim turns out to be less effective than the formal one. In other words, a more emotional message can receive a premium price on the basis of an “affective” trust, which differs from the cognitive trust and does not necessarily convert into other consumer’s responses, like declared intention to buy or to activate word-of-mouth [
81].
Concerning the willingness to pay, the results obtained through multiple regression allow to almost completely confirm H3—the declared willingness to pay was maximum (€1.69) when the TPLs and self-declared claims were simultaneously displayed on the product packaging, and minimum (€1.24) when only the TPLs, without a self-declared claim (condition 4) appeared on the pack. More in detail, the highest willingness to pay (€1.69) was declared when the TPLs were combined with an informal self-declared one. When the TPLs were combined with a formal self-declared claim (condition 5), the consumer declared to be willing to pay a maximum price of €1.48, which was quite similar to the one declared in relation to a product packaging, showing only an informal claim without third-party labels (condition 3).
We can thus affirm that third-party labels and self-declared claims are both necessary—the former ones act as a guarantor but are not always self-explanatory, and can be insufficient when used “alone”; the latter ones are better at drawing the consumer’s attention to the sustainable benefits related to the product, conveying additional information that justifies the higher willingness to pay (that respondents declared when firm-level labels were combined with TPLs).
Finally, with respect to the hypothesized mediating role of the perceived credibility of the sustainability message (H4), our results highlight the mediating role of the perceived credibility with reference to the relationships between self-declared claims and customer perceptions/intentions and WTP. This confirms that the perceived credibility of the sustainability message is largely responsible for the significance of the relationships. This does not happen in the case of the third-party labels, in relation to which the problems arising from the limited levels of familiarity/understanding and credibility are already highlighted above.
4.2. Theoretical and Practical Implications
The present study examines the propensity of consumers to consider self-declared claims, in comparison with TPLs, in the case of an unknown brand. Moreover, it investigates multiple levels and forms of sustainability information, as well as multiple kinds of dependent variables of different nature (consumer’s perceptions, purchase intention, and willingness to pay).
Our findings question the conclusions that affirm the superiority of independent certifications and the untrustworthiness of a company’s self-declared claims [
38,
41,
67], and are in line with studies highlighting that sustainability third-party labels “alone” do not currently play a major role in consumers’ food choices [
71,
72,
82]. Our results confirm the efficacy of internal socio-environmental claims, not only in the case of a well-known brand—as outlined in the study of Dekhili and Achabou [
34]—but also with reference to an unknown one. A short, focused, clearly written sentence—stating the company’s sustainability engagement—can influence consumers’ perceptions and intentions more than TPLs, even when corporate reputation and brand image do not support consumer’s choice, playing their role of “cognitive shortcuts” and enabling the consumer to rapidly identify trustworthy companies, on the basis of previous knowledge.
Our experimental study provides some insights into the effects of the tone of voice that should be adopted to convey the self-declared claim, suggesting that a “formal” tone of voice is preferable, with respect to a more friendly one. A formal claim is more effective in influencing young consumers’ trust and their perception of corporate reliability, even because it has a stronger impact on the perceived credibility of the sustainability message, which as was observed, acted as a mediator between the self-declared claim and the dependent variables. For these reasons, especially if they cannot count on strong brand equity, companies should shun the temptation of acting on the emotional lever when defining the copy to articulate their sustainability claim, since young consumers seem to follow more the “cognitive” route, than the “affective” one, in addressing their evaluations/choices; the latter, more intuitive, only tended to affect the declared willingness to pay, without impacting the perceived credibility of the message as well as consumer trust, in a significant manner.
A further practical implication that does not directly concern individual companies is related to the need to implement actions aimed at improving consumer knowledge of the TPLs. Only a clearer understanding of the labels and of their meaning could increase their effectiveness. In this context, sustainability labels regularly face challenges in terms of the limited space available on the packaging that does not contribute to increasing their clarity. This issue could be tackled by the use of innovative IT solutions (e.g., through the use of barcode scanners, QR codes, etc.) to provide further information to consumers. Furthermore, when socio-environmental labels are not self-explanatory for consumers, they could be complemented by other tools (e.g., website and social media campaigns, leaflets, actions at the point of sale, etc.).
4.4. Limitations and Further Research
As with all empirical work, some limitations, offering guidance for further research, should be taken into account.
First, this research is referred to one specific product context, the food sector and future experiments should be conducted within other sectors to check whether the results carry over.
A second limitation is related to the artificial environment, an online “laboratory”, where the experiment was conducted. Though mitigated by presenting high-quality visual stimuli to participants, inevitably their choice situation obviously missed a variegated range of cues present in a natural setting. The fictitious product brand used in the study could represent another missing cue, as mentioned before. Although the research team deliberately chose to refer to an imaginary brand (to avoid possible bias due to respondents’ prior knowledge and attitudes towards a specific brand), brand knowledge can undeniably make a strong difference in determining consumer’s response to the marketing stimuli. In this context, the perceived credibility of the sustainable message might be influenced through consumers’ moral affective evaluation of the product/brand, which in turn, could be related to corporate reputation—what consumers already know and perceive about a brand, how (and if) they feel engaged in their relationship with the company, are all aspects that can strongly influence their perceptions/intentions toward the brand.
We already mentioned above the reason for choosing the sample used in the study. Although this might be viewed as a sample of convenience, younger people (millennials and post-millennials) are consumers that are more oriented toward sustainability, have a huge collective spending power, and are an increasingly important consumer group. Understanding their responses to sustainability labels is, therefore, an essential undertaking, even though these findings might not be generalizable to the population as a whole, or other countries.
Finally, both the sustainability labels included in the study, the external and the self-declared one, were respectively presented in only two variants. In addition, TPLs were presented in a graphic format (a logo), while self-declared claims were presented in a textual format. Future research could therefore explore a more variegated manipulation, considering other labels (or their different combinations) and further variations of the internal claim tone of voice, beside the two modes (“formal” and “friendly”) considered here. Further studies could also consider comparing the effectiveness of “textual” self-declared claims versus “only graphic” ones.