Next Article in Journal
Silage Fermentation and In Vitro Degradation Characteristics of Orchardgrass and Alfalfa Intercrop Mixtures as Influenced by Forage Ratios and Nitrogen Fertilizing Levels
Next Article in Special Issue
Parents’ Views on Family Resiliency in Sustainable Remote Schooling during the COVID-19 Outbreak in Finland
Previous Article in Journal
Social Factors Key to Landscape-Scale Coastal Restoration: Lessons Learned from Three U.S. Case Studies
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

“Depression from Overestimation”: Income, Perception Bias and Children’s Mental Health in China’s Rural Households

1
School of Management, Zhejiang University, 866 Yuhangtang Rd, Hangzhou 310058, China
2
China Academy for Rural Development (CARD), Zhejiang University, 866 Yuhangtang Rd, Hangzhou 310058, China
3
School of Public Affairs, Zhejiang University, 866 Yuhangtang Rd, Hangzhou 310058, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sustainability 2020, 12(3), 870; https://doi.org/10.3390/su12030870
Submission received: 28 November 2019 / Revised: 18 January 2020 / Accepted: 19 January 2020 / Published: 23 January 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Sustainability of Families and Child Welfare)

Abstract

:
The mental health of rural children is closely related to their household characteristics, with household income level as one of the important influencing factors. In general, improvement in household income level is deemed to play an important role in promoting children’s mental health. However, the impact and mechanism of household income status perception bias on children’s mental health due to changes in the structure of household expenditure are under studied. On the basis of the perspective of household income status perception bias, we constructed a representative behavior household model of income status perception bias and a three-wave panel. We adopted the data from Chinese household tracking surveys in 2012, 2014, and 2016 to empirically analyze the mechanism and channel of household income status perception bias on children’s mental health. Results reveal that: (1) A significant negative correlation exists between household income level and income status perception bias, and poor households are likely to have income status perception bias. (2) A significant positive correlation exists between income status perception bias of poor households and their gift-giving expenditure, whereas a negative correlation exists between income status perception bias and expenditure for children’s education. The more the poor households overestimate their income status, the more inclined they are to increase their gift spending and reduce expenditure for children’s education, thereby changing the structure of family expenditure. (3) A significant negative correlation exists between poor household income status perception bias and the mental health status of their children, whereas a positive correlation exists between household expenditure for children’s education and children’s mental health status. That is, the more that poor households overestimate their relative income status, the greater the mental pressure on children. Finally, the reduction of expenditure on children’s education by rural households is an effective channel through which income status perception bias among poor households affects children’s mental pressure.

1. Introduction

Since the Reform and the implementation of the Opening up Policy, China’s economy has experienced rapid development and has undergone structural transformation. These changes have resulted in profound changes in the social environment. Issues, such as rising childcare costs, shortage of quality education resources, and further increases in employment pressure, have become increasingly prominent in relation to children’s education and human capital creation. These phenomena have led Chinese children and their families to face an increasingly competitive environment for growth. Pressure on character shaping, interpersonal relationship building, learning ability development, and living environment adaptation has multiplied [1]. According to the latest data released by the National Health Commission in 2018, approximately 30 million children under the age of 17 in China suffer from various emotional disorders and learning and behavioral problems caused by mental pressure. (The data sources are available online at: http://www.nhc.gov.cn/wjw/zccl/201805/93bd24e3199c4bd9bfae5ff6a258bcdb.shtml). Furthermore, according to the World Health Organization, global psychological barriers in children will increase by more than 50% in the next 20 years and will become one of the main causes of illness, disability, and death [2]. Therefore, studying the influencing factors of children’s mental health is of great theoretical and practical significance for relieving children’s mental pressure, promoting children’s health, and improving the rate of return of education.
Rural areas have received the hardest hit for children’s psychological problems due to structural contradictions caused by unbalanced urban and rural development. On the one hand, a large number of left-behind children (The “left-behind children” refers to children who live in the place of hukou registration and live together with one or no parents, as he or she or both have migrated [3]) are recognized in rural China. According to China’s 1% National Population Sample Survey in 2015, there are more than 45.01 million of left-behind children, accounting for approximately 34.6% of the total rural children [4]. Therefore, the lack of parental care, adequate family, and parent–child education has rendered rural left-behind children vulnerable to anxiety and depression [5,6]. On the other hand, the level of general economic development in rural areas is low compared with that of cities. That is, low-income households cannot provide sufficient food, medical care, education, and entertainment facilities for children. As a result, children in poor rural households score lower than normal children in terms of health, cognitive ability, individual development, learning achievement, and happiness [7]. In addition, parents in some rural households are more prone to unstable emotions such as depression, anxiety and anger due to poverty. Family disharmony and family breakdown are relatively common in low-income households. Parents lack patience, tolerance, trust, and encouragement in raising children. Frequent punishments aggravate psychological pressure on children and seriously affect the mental health of children in rural China [8,9].
To date, the majority of existing research investigated the factors affecting the mental health of rural children on the basis of external environmental characteristics, children’s individual characteristics, parental characteristics, and children’s household characteristics. External environmental characteristics include economic inequality and social environment disorder in poverty-stricken areas, gender and social status discrimination, air and water pollution, medical quality differences and access methods, and regional financial credit levels [10,11,12,13,14]. Individual characteristics of children include gender, age, health status, academic performance, and years of schooling [15,16,17,18]. Moreover, parental characteristics include education level, health status, marital status, and effective parent–child time [19,20,21]. Children’s household characteristics include household size and structure, income and expenditure, asset structure, and family atmosphere [15,22,23]. Among these factors, family income levels are generally considered one of the most important factors affecting children’s mental health. However, existing research only focuses on objective conditions, such as actual income level of households, and ignores the subjective cognition and evaluation of household members especially the income level and economic status of heads of households and financial supervisors [24]. Therefore, we take the perspective of subjective understanding of the income status of rural households. Our systematic study of the impact of rural households’ perspective of subjective understanding of the income status on children’s mental health and its transmission mechanism can effectively remedy the shortcomings of the existing literature and provide a new economic explanation for the causes of rural children’s mental health problems. On the basis of the abovementioned literature review and empirical facts, we select mental pressure on rural children as the research object.
Applying the perspective of household income status perception bias, a representative behavior rural household model of perceived income bias is constructed for theoretical analysis, based on which we put forward several hypotheses to be tested. The paper adopts the “China Family Panel Studies” (CFPS) data in 2012, 2014, and 2016 to construct a three-wave panel (Supplementary Material). It empirically analyzes the mechanism and channel of a household’s income status perception bias affecting mental pressure among children. Finally, it provides corresponding discussion and suggestions based on the empirical analysis results.
The remainder of this paper is organized as follows. Section 2 presents the literature review that systematically summarizes and categorizes the existing studies on perception bias and children’s mental health. Section 3 constructs a representative behavior model of income perception bias, explains empirical facts through theoretical analysis, and provides hypotheses for empirical research. Section 4 discusses the empirical strategy and measurement model. Section 5 introduces data sources, variable descriptions, and descriptive statistics. Section 6 provides empirical results and analysis. Section 7 provides a conclusion and discussion.

2. Literature Review

2.1. Effect of Household Characteristics on Children’s Mental Health

Studies on children’s mental health problems have shown that the characteristic variables associated with children and their parents and families are the main factors affecting their mental health. Children’s gender, age, health status, and academic level may be causes of their mental health problems. Among the variables, the influence of children’s gender on mental health has yet to reach a consensus because the majority of studies posit that gender does not significantly interfere with children’s mental health [16]. The effect of children’s age on their mental health level is relatively complex and are mostly realized through other factors. For example, Davis et al. (2010) [15] found that a family’s economic situation caused mental pressure on children before the age of 12. The younger a child is, the greater the mental pressure on the child is [15]. The impact of academic performance on children’s mental pressure depends on the growth environment, training mode, and policy national conditions of children in different regions. In the current context of China’s huge competitive pressure on educational resources and emphasis on exam-oriented education, families pay excess attention to children’s academic performance. This situation leads poorly performing children to face great psychological pressure [18]. However, many studies abroad have yet to find a significant correlation between academic level, academic achievement, and children’s mental health [17].
Parents’ education level, health status, marital status, and effective parent–child time influence children’s mental health. Parents’ academic qualifications and health status are often related to employment and household income. As such, parents with low education level and poor health tend to have relatively low income and a high probability of unemployment. This tendency not only leads to a decrease in the material investment of parents to children but also increases the likelihood of serious negative impact on the formation of children’s opinion on life and values, which leads to increased mental pressure [15,21]. Family relationships with loose marital status are generally characterized by quarrels, conflicts, indifference, and lack of concern for family members. An unstable and harsh marriage can lead to behavioral disorders and increased symptoms of depression among children [19,25]. In addition, long-term effective parent–child time is the main feature of parental recognition and care for children, which will effectively alleviate children’s mental pressure [19]. However, the risk of mental health problems for rural left-behind children will significantly increase due to the lack of parent–child time that results from the migration of their parents [26,27,28].
The household factors that affect children’s mental health can be summarized as two aspects: the demographic structure and economic conditions of the household. Demographic structure of the household will affect the distribution of household resources: Blake (1981) [29] argued that a “dilution” effect occurs in the household, that is, the more the children, the less resources each child obtains. Therefore, for households with limited resources, the more the children, the greater individual mental pressure each child experiences [29]. In the social context of rural China, the oldest or youngest child in general gains more household resources, such that their mental pressure may be less [30]. Economic conditions refer to the impact of the overall income and expenditure level of a household on the access of children’s resources. Low-income households are subject to lower resources endowments, and their relatively low growth investment in children (i.e., education, medical & health, and entertainment) makes it is difficult for the children to acquire basic material conditions for growth [31]. Furthermore, to improve their overall income level, most adults in low-income households opt to work longer, resulting in children who lack necessary mental support, such as companionship, communication, and family interaction, and it is easier for children to develop bad living habits in the stage of character development [32]. Therefore, children in low-income households are more likely to be lonely and depressed and have a greater negative impact on mental health in the long run [22,23]. What’s more, not only general labor market and income characteristics affect adversely children’s mental health but they can also affect the latter through decreased health care services consumption and unhealthy life style habits [33,34,35].

2.2. Perception Bias and Its Impact on Behavior

In terms of perception bias, the “D–K effect” proposed by Kruger and Dunning (1999) [36] is an internationally accepted theory. It states that individual behaviors may produce an assessment that is inconsistent with the actual situation in one aspect of themselves, and this erroneous estimation of one’s tendency is called “perception bias.” The causes and manifestations of individual perception bias vary. For example, individuals with low ability are likely to overestimate themselves because they lack the correct measure of their abilities and sufficient resources to improve their cognitive level. In contrast, individuals with high ability mostly underestimate themselves because of factors, such as cognitive level, interpersonal relationships, and personality traits that cause them to mistakenly overestimate others’ abilities. Perception bias has been confirmed by an increasing number of empirical studies. For example, students with poor grades tend to have optimistic expectations for their test results [37]. Students who fail driver’s license exams are more likely to overestimate their driving skills than those who pass the exam [38]. Similarly, chess players will overestimate their predictive ability in the game, whereas those who are low ability will be more likely to overestimate their own level, which lead to prediction errors [39].
Perception bias has an impact on many individual behaviors. Research in economics focuses on risk behavioral decision analysis. Entrepreneurs do not show a strong risk preference when making entrepreneurial decisions, but perception bias leads to a low perception of risk and options when starting a business [40]. In venture capital research, Zacharakis and Shepherd (2001) [41] found that venture capitalists often overestimate their decision-making level compared with the average person. As such, perception bias often has a certain negative impact on the accuracy of risk investment decisions [41]. In addition, research on perception bias and its impact on behavior and decision making is widespread in areas such as consumption, credit, financial asset valuation, corporate or household financial decisions, and poverty perception [42,43,44,45].
In different decision-making situations, various types of household expenditures that result from decision-making behaviors are also affected by perception bias. Among them, perception bias of household income status largely affects the expenditure structure of households [46]. A representative household’s expenditure categories mainly include food, tobacco, alcohol, clothing, housing, daily necessities, transportation, communication, education, culture, entertainment, health care, and other basic supplies. The consumption of clothing, food, housing, travel and daily necessities is relatively fixed rigid expenditure types, so it is not easy for household to adjust significantly [47]. However, other more flexible expenditure types may change due to adjustments in household income and spending decisions. For example, under the peer-effect and status concern, households are likely to choose households with equal or higher income status as a reference and then increase gift-giving expenditure due to concerns about their social status [48]. However, under the influence of perception bias of income status, if a rural household overestimates its income status within nearby areas, then the level of gift-giving expenditure will be higher than that determined by the actual income of the household [49]. Expenditure on children’s education will be further compressed in the case of poor households with low income and difficulty in reducing fixed expenditures. Therefore, children are likely to face greater pressure in terms of academic performance and getting along with peers, which will affect their mental health [50].

3. Theoretical Framework and Hypothesis

On the basis of the “D–K effect” proposed by Kruger and Dunning (1999) [36], low-income rural households tend to overestimate their income levels due to their low cognitive abilities. Furthermore, under the influence of perceptual bias, low-income rural households tend to be closer to high-income rural households in terms of household consumption due to the “demonstration effect” (The “demonstration effect” of consumption indicates that consumers compare with other consumers when they consume and intend to exceed others in the same social class at the level of consumption.) of consumer behavior; and the household gift-giving expenditure represents its “Face” in the nearby area, thus under the action of the above two effects, the gift-giving expenditure increases due to the comparison among households in the same area and this phenomenon is particularly common in rural China [51,52]. However, this phenomenon is different between normal rural household and poor rural household: (1) The income limitation of poor household makes they do not have enough resources to invest in human capital and social capital, thus the limitation of knowledge level and social skills results in relatively low perception which makes poor rural households more likely to generate income status perception biases and thus overestimate the income status, and increase gift-giving spending under the influence of income status perception biases; conversely, normal households are less likely to experience bias in income status perception, and the income status perception bias has lower influence on their gift-giving expenditure [48,49,50,51,52]. (2) Normal rural households have higher incomes, more adequate savings, and excellent asset allocation, so even if they increase gift-giving expenditure, they may not need to reduce other flexible expenditure; however, poor rural households, especially those below the poverty line, of which the income and savings can only meet basic living needs, once the gift-giving expenditure rises, other expenditures will inevitably be reduced, thus under the circumstance that the rigid expenditures cannot be changed, poor rural households can only reduce flexible expenditures, especially children’s education expenditure which has a large share in the flexible expenditures [47,49,50,51,52]. When children’s education expenditures cannot be guaranteed, children in poor rural households will face tremendous pressure in academic, social, recreational and personality building, which will negatively affect their mental health [50]. Therefore, poor rural household’s perception bias of real income status will change their consumption expectation and expenditure structure, making household gift-giving expenditure increase and the human capital (education investment) of children will be reduced, which ultimately reduces the mental health level of children.
To deepen the understanding of empirical facts and provide theoretical basis for empirical research, we construct a simple but general theoretical model. The model aims to explain the likelihood that the income perception bias of rural households will have an impact on its expenditure structure and children’s psychological pressure. We assume that the utility function of a representative actor (rural household) is:
U   ( x ,   e )
where x represents rural household expenditure, which is assumed to be the sum of children’s human capital investment (H) and gift-giving expenditure (r), x = H + r. In addition, e represents rural household income. Therefore, Equation (1) can be rewritten as U (H, r, e).
We use Equations (2) and (3) to depict rural households’ willingness to maximize their utility and budget constraints under general circumstances:
max H ,   r ,   e U   ( H ,   r ,   e )
s . t .   e     ( q H   +   w r )   =   0
where H and r are defined similar to those in Equation (1); q represents the cost of human capital investment, w represents the cost of gift-giving expenditure, and households’ income return rate is set to 1. To ensure that the above optimization problem has a unique solution, assume that the household’s utility function U (H, r, e) is a strictly concave function, and “ H * , r * , e * ” are the equilibrium solution of the optimization problem.
According to the above relevant background and literature review, we refer to the perception bias model constructed by Loewenstein (2003) [53], which assumes existence probability P ( P     [0,1]), such that the representative actor (rural household) utility function is rewritten as:
U ˜   ( x ,   e   |   e )   =   ( 1     P ) · U   ( H ,   r ,   e )   +   P · U   ( H ,   r ,   e )
To a certain extent, the utility of rural households ( U ˜ ) is the weighted average of real utility U (H, r, e) and income perception bias utility U (H, r, e′) with a weight of probability (P) due to the influence of income perception bias (e′). Let P = p(e) and P / e     0 , that is, probability P is the minus function of rural household income (e). In particular, if the rural household does not have income perception bias, when P = 0, then U ˜ ( x ,   e | e ) = U (H, r, e). Based on the cited assumptions, the income perception bias of rural households will be incorporated in their utility function. We use U 0 as the coreference of the income perception bias utility function U (H, r, e′) and assume that utility function has a limited range of values. Thus, the optimization problem of the representative actor (rural household) can be changed to:
max H ,   r ,   e [ 1     p ( e ) ] · U   ( H ,   r ,   e )   +   p ( e ) · U 0
s . t .    e     ( q H   +   w r )   =   0
The equilibrium solution of the optimization problem is H * * , r * * , e * * . In combination with the analysis results of the abovementioned theories, Proposition 1 can be obtained as follows.
Proposition 1.
e *   >   e * * and e   >   e * * , if ( H * * , r * * , e * * ) ≠ ( H * , r * , e * ).
Proposition 1 shows that if optimization problems (Equations (2) and (5)) have different solutions under the same constraints (Equation (3)), then households with relatively low income are likely to have income perception bias and tend to overestimate their income level due to the influence of perception bias. Combining Proposition 1 with the solutions of optimization problems (Equations (2) and (5)), the following Lemmas can be obtained.
Lemma 1.
H *   >   H * * and r *   r * * .
Lemma 1 indicates that rural households with low income are likely to increase gift-giving expenditure and reduce human capital investment in children under the influence of income perception bias. If children’s psychological pressure function in this situation is assumed to be “F = f (H)” and has the first order condition “ F / H   <   0 ”, then Lemma 2 can be obtained as follows.
Lemma 2.
F *   =   f   ( H * )   <   f   ( H * * )   =   F * * .
Lemma 2 indicates that children’s psychological pressure will increase as rural households with low income reduce their investment in human capital under the influence of income perception bias.
In summary (The proofs of Proposition 1, Lemma 1, and Lemma 2 are shown in Appendix A), on the basis of the theoretical model provided in this section and combined with the existing literature, we propose the following hypotheses:
Hypothesis 1 (H1).
Poor rural households are likely to have income status perception bias and tend to overestimate their income level. That is, the lower their income is, the easier for rural households to overestimate their income levels.
Hypothesis 2a (H2a).
Poor rural households’ income status perception bias will affect the structure of household expenditures. Poor rural households who overestimate their income status will increase their gift-giving expenditure.
Hypothesis 2b (H2b).
Poor rural households’ income status perception bias will affect the structure of household expenditure. Poor rural households who overestimate their income status will reduce their expenditure for children’s education.
Hypothesis 3 (H3).
Poor rural households’ income status perception bias will increase psychological pressure on their children by reducing human capital investment in children, which will lead to a reduction in their mental health level.

4. Empirical Strategies and Econometric Models

According to the research hypothesis, our empirical strategy design aims to test whether low-income rural households with income status perception bias will overestimate their income level, then change the structure of household expenditure, which ultimately affects the mental health of children. Therefore, we will test each hypothesis and conduct an empirical analysis, based on which we build econometric models. Considering endogenous problems caused by the unobservable effect existed in different individuals and years in the micro panel of rural households and children, which may lead to biased estimation results, we establish a two-way fixed effect model to test the abovementioned hypotheses in Section 3 based on the research of Rockoff (2004), Zhou and Yu (2011), and Lu (2012) [27,54,55] (In the empirical study in this paper, there is basically no reverse causality between dependent and independent variables, the endogenous problem mainly comes from the omission of variables. The missing variables include external environmental factors such as regional social ethos and economic development level, factors such as individual personality traits in rural households, time effects such as political systems and policies in certain years, and rural households’ social capital and peer effect such as peer pressure. The two-way fixed effects model provided in this article can strip the influence of the first three unobservable effects; however, peer effect is related to social networks and individual spillover effects, which are difficult to handle through technical means. However, in the context of this article, the endogenous problem caused by missing peer effect will not systematically overestimate the results of the sample, especially the sample of the poor rural households, and because we have fully controlled covariates at different levels and several robustness tests have been performed, the possibility that the final estimation result is disturbed by endogenous problems that lead to inconsistent estimation is relatively low).
To test Hypothesis 1, we construct the following two-way fixed effect regression equation:
Bias fkt   =   α   +   β 1 Income fkt   +   δ   X fkt   +   φ fm   +   λ kt   +   ϵ fkt
where the dependent variable Bias fkt indicates the perception bias of rural household f living in county k in year t. The core independent variable Income fkt indicates the income of household f in county k in year t. β 1 measures the effect of household income level on household income status perception bias. The vector matrix X fkt represents a set of control variables that may have an impact on the perception bias of income status, including a series of rural household characteristic variables. We adopt those variables to eliminate the interference of other factors on the estimation of core independent variables. φ f m denotes individual fixed effects by controlling for the endogenous problem on the estimated results caused by unobservable factors that vary only with the individual characteristic but not with time (It mainly controls for the fixed effects of long-term individual and household characteristics, and its meaning is similar across equations). λ kt indicates the county–year fixed effect by controlling for the endogenous problem on the estimated results caused by unobservable factors that vary only with county and time but not with household and individual characteristics [56] (It mainly controls for the fixed effects of external environment characteristics that vary with county and time, and its meaning is similar across equations). ϵ fkt represents the random error term of the regression equation. According to Hypothesis 1, the lower the household income level is, the higher the income status perception bias is. Therefore, we expect a negative correlation between the two variables. The estimated coefficient β 1 of the core independent variables Income fkt should be significantly negative.
Further, we conduct the following regression equation to test Hypothesis 2a:
Gift fkt   =   α   +   β 2 Bias fkt   +   δ   X fkt   +   φ fm   +   λ kt   +   ϵ fkt
where the dependent variable Gift fkt indicates the gift-giving expenditure of household f living in county k in year t. The core independent variable Bias fkt represents the income status perception bias of household f living in county k in year t. Furthermore, β 2 measures the effect of perception bias on household gift spending. The meanings of other variables in Equation (7) are the same as those in Equation (6). According to Hypothesis 2a, the higher the income status perception bias of poor rural households, the more their gift-giving expenditures. Therefore, we expect a positive correlation between them. The estimated coefficient β 1 of the core independent variables Bias fkt should be significantly positive.
Hypothesis 2b is tested using the following regression Equation (8):
Edu _ cost ifkt   =   α   +   β 3 Bias fkt   +   δ   X fkt   +   θ   X ikt   +   φ fm   +   λ kt   +   ϵ ifkt
where the dependent variable Edu _ cost ifkt indicates the expenditure for education for child i in household f living in county k for year t. The core independent variable Bias fkt represents income status perception bias, and β 3 measures its effect on expenditure for children’s education. The vector matrices X fkt and X ikt represent a set of control variables that may have an impact on household’s expenditure for children’s education, the former includes a series of household characteristic variables, the latter includes a series of individual characteristic variables of children. We adopted these variables to eliminate the interference of other factors on the estimation of the core independent variables. The meanings of other variables in Equation (8) are the same as those in Equations (6) and (7). According to Hypothesis 2b, the higher the income status perception bias of poor rural households, the lower their expenditure for children’s education. Therefore, we expect a negative correlation between them. The estimated coefficient β 3 of the core independent variables Bias fkt should be significantly negative.
Hypothesis 3 is tested using the following regression Equation (9):
MH ifkt   =   α   +   β 4 Bias fkt   +   β 5 Edu _ cost ifkt +   δ   X fkt   +   θ   X ikt   +   μ   X ifkt   +   φ fm   +   λ kt   +   ϵ ifkt
where dependent variable MH ifkt indicates the mental health of child i for household f living in county k and year t. The core independent variable Bias fkt represents income status perception bias, and β 4 measures its effect on children’s mental health. Independent variable Edu _ cost ifkt represents expenditure for children’s education, and β 5 measures its effect on children’s mental health. The vector matrices X fkt , X ikt and X ifkt represent a set of control variables that may have an impact on children’s mental health, including a series of rural household characteristic variables, individual characteristic variables of children and individual characteristic variables of children’s parents, respectively. We adopt these variables to eliminate the interference of other factors on the estimation of the core independent variables. The meanings of other variables in Equation (9) are the same as those in Equations (7) and (8). According to Hypothesis 3, when there is only one independent variable Bias fkt in the regression, the higher the income status perception bias of poor rural households, the lower their children’s mental health, if we use mental pressure to represent the children’s mental health level, then there will be a positive correlation between income status perception bias and children’s mental health and a significantly positive coefficient of β 4 ; however, the more the poor rural households spend on their children’s education, the higher their children’s mental health level, and we expect a negative correlation between expenditure for children’s education and children’s mental health and the estimated coefficient β 5 of Edu _ cost ifkt should be significantly negative. However, the estimated coefficient β 4 of the independent variables Bias fkt may be non-significant after Edu _ cost ifkt is added into the regression.

5. Data Sources and Descriptive Statistics

Combined with the abovementioned summarized research hypotheses, empirical strategies, and econometric models, the sample data adopted in the empirical research in this paper should comprehensively cover the income, relative income status, and subjective perception of rural households in all regions of China, structure of household expenditure, and relevant individual and household characteristic variables. The CFPS database was established by the China Social Survey Center of Peking University, and it aims to collect and analyze the individual and household data of more than 16,000 households in more than 600 counties in 25 provinces of China. The database is intended to comprehensively examine China’s social and economic changes and support academic research and policy decisions. The CFPS database covers the samples and variables required for this study (A detailed introduction to CFPS data and data acquisition methods can be found at: http://opendata.pku.edu.cn/dataverse/CFPS). Up to now, the CFPS database has been updated with individual tracking data in 2010, 2012, 2014 and 2016. We use the three-wave panel composed of the survey data in 2012, 2014, and 2016 for empirical research because of the large systematic difference in the design of the questionnaire of household expenditure between the 2010 survey and the latter three periods surveys. Finally, 3688 effective sample observations are obtained with data cleaning (The three-wave panel data contain 25,701 observations, and valid observations are retained after excluding samples missing from the rural samples and psychological stress-related data).
This study mainly investigates the influence of income status perception bias of rural households on the mental pressure experienced by children. Three core variables should be tested, namely, household income and subjective perception, mental health status of children, and household expenditure structure. The main research objects are children under the age of 15 in rural households. As the core dependent variable, the mental health of children in rural households was measured by the Kessler Mental Distress Scale (K6 Scale) [57]. The K6 scale is a widely used mental pressure test scale worldwide. In the CFPS questionnaire, the K6 scale contains six questions answered by children as follows. (If the child is unable to answer independently due to young age or other special reasons, then the investigator will assist and guide his or her guardian). In the past month, “how frequently do you feel that you can’t do anything?” “How often do you feel nervous,” “How frequently do you feel uncomfortable and unable to keep calm?” “How frequently do you feel hopeless for the future?” “How often do you feel unable to do anything,” and “how often do you think you’re failing?” Each of these items has a score range of 0–4. The larger the score is, the greater the mental pressure and the lower the mental health level. A total score of more than 13 for the six items indicates that the child may be in a substantial state of psychological depression. In addition, two important variables of rural household social spending and expenditure for children’s education can also be directly obtained from the CFPS database.
The questions in the CFPS questionnaire about household financial income and expenditure are answerable by the person who is “most familiar with family finances.” Thus, this article assumes that “the person who is most familiar with family finances” refers to household financial supervisors, and we use their income status perception to represent the entire household. The measurement of a household’s subjective income status perception is derived from the question “what level of income does your income belong to locally” in the questionnaire, which divides households’ subjective income into five levels arranged from low to high. Correspondingly, we divide the actual income of rural households within the same county/district into five levels arranged from low to high. Finally, the overestimation of the household’s income status is measured by the abovementioned individual household income perception minus its corresponding actual income. We use it as the proxy variable for the income status perception bias.
According to the literature review, the factors affecting children’s mental health include children’s individual, household, and parental characteristics and external environment. Therefore, we also control for the relevant variables in combination with empirical strategies and measurement models. Among them, the control variables of children’s individual characteristics mainly include age, current education, physical health status, and academic performance. The control variables for household characteristics mainly include economic status, age of other household members, health status, number of children, and whether the child is the first child. The parental characteristics control variables mainly include parents’ education, marital status, and effective parent–child time. Other external environmental factors are controlled for by county–year fixed effects in the panel two-way fixed effect model. Table 1 reports the definitions and descriptions of the variables required for empirical research.
In addition, this study aims to investigate the differences between poor and non-poor households and further test whether the impact of rural household income perception bias on children’s mental health is heterogeneous. Thus, the total sample is divided into the poor and non-poor samples according to the income level of rural households. On the basis of the results, we use the sample of the non-poor rural households as a control group for empirical demonstration. The poor group sample is divided according to household per capita net income standard published in the “China Rural Poverty Monitoring Report.” Rural households with per capita net income of less than 2300 CHY (2012), 2800 CHY (2014), and 3000 CHY (2016) in the three waves of the samples are categorized into the poor sample group. Table 2 reports the descriptive statistics of all relevant variables for both sample groups. From the descriptive statistical results of the sample, mental pressure on children in poor households, income status perception bias, and total number of children in the family are significantly higher than those in non-poor households under the condition of uncontrolled two-way fixed effect. Variables, such as expenditure on children’s education, children’s academic performance, household disposable income, household per capita expenditure, and parental marital status, are significantly lower than those of non-poor households. After controlling for the two-way fixed effect, the gap between poor and non-poor households has narrowed. However, the level of psychological pressure and income status perception bias, which are the core variables of poor households, remain significantly higher than those of non-poor households. However, expenditure for children’s education and per capita disposable income of poor households are significantly lower than those of non-poor rural households. In addition, the gift-giving expenditure of poor households also becomes significantly higher than that of non-poor households. Thus, no significant difference in other variables was observed between the two groups. (Table A1 in Appendix A provides a Pearson correlation coefficient matrix of the correlation among the variables in this research, which can be used as a priori judgments of the empirical study).

6. Empirical Analysis

6.1. Effect of Household Income Level on Household Income Status Perception Bias

First, we use the panel two-way fixed effect model in regression (Equation (6)) to test Hypothesis 1. The dependent variable is household income status perception bias, which is established by its self-estimated income minus actual income. The independent variable is household income, which is described by the absolute logarithm value of household income and household relative income ranking in its county. Table 3 provides the empirical results of the effect of household income level on household income status perception bias. All regression models in the table control for household–individual and county–year fixed effects. To avoid heteroscedasticity problem in random error terms, we employ the White heteroscedasticity robust standard error to improve the estimation coefficients. Columns (3) and (4) are baseline regression, which represents the results of the effect of the log income and relative income ranking on income status perception bias, respectively. Columns (1) and (2) portray the contrast of baseline regression, of which the econometric models did not control for household characteristic variables. Comparing the baseline and contrast regressions, the estimated results of the regression model remain robust.
Table 3 shows a significant negative correlation between household income level and income status perception bias (p < 0.01) regardless of whether the independent variable is log income or income ranking. Furthermore, this result is unaffected by whether or not the household characteristic variables are controlled for. The estimation coefficient of the log income variable in Column (3) depicts that, after controlling for the related household characteristic variable and two-way fixed effect, every 10% increases in household income will lead to a degree of 0.02 decreases of household income status perception bias. The estimation coefficient of the income ranking variable in Column (4) shows every 1 level increase in household income ranking after controlling for the related household characteristic variable and two-way fixed effect. That is, the degree of household income status perception bias will be reduced by approximately 0.1. The empirical results imply that the higher the income level is, the lower the degree of income status perception bias. Conversely, the lower the income level is, the poorer the household is, and the more likely it is to overestimate the relative status of their income. This tendency leads to income status perception bias. This result not only provides supporting evidence for Kruger and Dunning’s research but also verifies the relationship between household income level and perceived bias of household income status as proposed in Hypothesis 1 [36].

6.2. Effect of Household Income Status Perception Bias on Household Expenditure Structure

On the basis of testing Hypothesis 1, we further test Hypothesis 2a by conducting a regression (Equation (7)), which is the effect of household income status perception bias on household gift-giving expenditure. Table 4 presents the regression results. According to the relevant documents of Section 5, to explore the impact of household income status perception bias on household gift-giving expenditure we divide the sample into two groups, namely, non-poor (normal) and poor households, we use the regression results of the poor households sample as baseline and that of the non-poor households sample as contrast. Regression controls for the household–individual and county–year fixed effects. White’s heteroscedasticity robust standard error is used to improve the estimation efficiency. Columns (3) and (4) in Table 4 are listed as the baseline results of controlling for the household characteristic variables, and Columns (1) and (2) are the contrast result without controlling for the household characteristic variables. Columns (1) and (3) are listed as the regression results of the non-poor group samples, whereas Columns (2) and (4) are listed for the opposite group samples.
The regression results in Column (4) in Table 4 indicate a significant positive correlation between income status perception bias of poor households and gift-giving expenditure (p < 0.01). This result is unaffected by whether or not the household control variables are controlled for. The estimated coefficient of the income status perception bias shows that, after controlling for the related household characteristic variables and two-way fixed effects, the income status perception bias of poor households will increase by an average of 180 CHY per year, which is equivalent to 10% of the per capita expenditure of poor households per year (According to Table 2, the per capita expenditure of the sample households in the poverty group is 1886 CHY). As a control group, the regression coefficients of Column (2) also showed similar results. The abovementioned regression results imply that the more households overestimate their income status, the more inclined they are to increase their family gift-giving expenditure. Combined with theory of peer effect and status anxiety [48], the hypothesis that income status perception bias of poor households affects their expenditure structure by increasing their gift-giving expenditure is verified [48].
Correspondingly, the estimated coefficient of “perception bias” in Columns (1) and (3) in Table 4 is non-significant, and the coefficient symbols are inverted when the control variables are added. This finding shows that the perception bias of household income status has no significant impact on households’ gift-giving expenditure for the non-poor sample. On the one hand, normal households have high income levels, and their income status perception bias is weak, which is inadequate so as to affect households’ gift-giving expenditure. On the other hand, even if the perception bias of income status is generated, most normal households lack incentives to adjust their gift-giving expenditure because of stable social relations and economic conditions.
Table 5 reports the empirical results for testing Hypothesis 2b. For this part, we also divide the sample into two groups, namely, non-poor (normal) and poor households. Regression Equation (8) is used to estimate the impact of household income status perception bias on household expenditure for children’s education. Columns (3) and (4) in Table 5 are listed as the baseline result of controlling for the household characteristic variables. Columns (1) and (2) denote the contrast result without controlling for household characteristic variables, and Columns (1) and (3) are listed as the regression results of the non-poor group samples. Columns (2) and (4) are listed as regression results of the poor group samples. All regressions control for the household–individual and county–year fixed effects and use the White heteroscedasticity robust standard error to improve the estimation efficiency.
The regression results under Column (5) in Table 5 show a significant negative correlation between income status perception bias and expenditure for children’s education among poor households (p < 0.01). This result is robust regardless of whether or not the control variables are added. Every unit increase in perceived income status bias of poor households will reduce the annual expenditure for children’s education by more than 400 CHY on average. This value is equivalent to 20% of the annual per capita expenditure of poor rural households (According to Table 2, the per capita expenditure of the sample households in the poverty group is 1886 CHY) after controlling for related household and children characteristic variables and two-way fixed effects. As the control group, the regression coefficients of Column (2) also showed similar results. The cited regression results imply that the more that households overestimate their income status, the more inclined they are to reduce their expenditure for children’s education. The estimated coefficient in Columns (1) and (3) in Table 5 is non-significant, which indicates that household income status perception bias does not significantly affect expenditure for children’s education for the non-poor group. On the one hand, the reason is that normal households have high income levels, and the income status perception bias is weak. On the other hand, even if income status perception bias occurs under the circumstances that high-income households attach great importance to education and human capital investment, the majority of normal households will also be inclined to increase expenditure for children’s education.

6.3. Effect of Household Perception Bias of Income Status on Children’s Mental Health

Finally, the regression in Equation (9) is used to investigate the effect of household income status perception bias on children’s mental health. The regression adopts the White variance robust standard error and controls for household–individual and county–year fixed effects. Table 6 reports the estimated results. Columns (5)–(8) depict the baseline regression results, where Columns (5)–(8) represent the non-poor and poor group samples, respectively. Among them, Columns (5) and (7) are listed as the result of reduced form estimation to measure the total effect of household income status perception bias on children’s mental health. Columns (6) and (8) add “expenditure for children’s education” as a covariant to examine whether such expenditure is an ideal indirect channel through which household income status perception bias affects children’s mental health. Columns (1)–(4) are in contrast to the baseline regression, of which econometric models without control variables are established. The comparison of the two results shows that the regression model estimation results remain robust.
The regression results of Column (7) in Table 6 show a significant negative correlation between household income status perception bias and children’s mental health (p < 0.01). After controlling for the related variables of household, children, and their parents as well as two-way fixed effects, every unit income status perception bias will lead to an average of 6.4-point-increase in children’s mental stress (The total score of K6 is 24. The higher the score is, the greater the mental pressure is. See Section 4 for details). After adding expenditure for children’s education as a covariate in Column (8), the coefficient of income status perception bias remains significant but is reduced to 5.9. The regression coefficient of expenditure for children’s education was significantly negative, which indicates a significant positive correlation between expenditure for children’s education and children’s mental health (p < 0.01). Every unit income status perception bias will lead to an average of 5.9 points increase in children’s mental stress. If households spend 1000 CHY more on children’s education every year, the average mental stress of children will be reduced by 4.7 points. This result means that nearly 30% of children from poor households will become mentally stressed if their families overestimate their income status. However, more than 37% of depressed children may recover if expenditure for children’s education increases by 1000 CHY. As the control group, Columns (2) and (4) also showed similar results, and the regression results of non-poor household samples were non-significant, which are also less affected by other control variables. When expenditure for children’s education is added as a covariant, the regression coefficient of perception bias decreases, which indicates that expenditure for children’s education will weaken the negative impact of certain income perception bias on children’s mental health. Therefore, the reduction of expenditure for children’s education is an effective channel through which poor household income perception bias increases mental pressure on children. Based on cited analysis, Hypothesis 3 can be basically verified.

6.4. Robustness Checks

(1) Using different poverty recognition standards to divide the poor and non-poor group samples. In contrast to the national standards provided in the “China Rural Poverty Monitoring Report,” Columns (1)–(3) in Table 7 report the results of the panel two-way fixed effect regression for (relatively) poor households identified by the standard of lower-than-median income per capita disposable income of rural households in each county/district. The correlation regression coefficients are the same as those of the baseline regression results in Table 4, Table 5 and Table 6, which also support the relevant hypothesis and indicate that the original model and results remain robust.
(2) Proportion of gift-giving expenditure and expenditure for children’s education in the total expenditure per capita of the household is adopted to enter the regression. Household gift-giving expenditure and expenditure for children’s education in Table 4 and Table 5 are replaced by their respective share of total expenditure, and the panel two-way fixed effect regression is conducted. Columns (4)–(5) in Table 7 report the results. The regression results and analysis are consistent with the baseline regression in Table 4 and Table 5, which indicate that the original model and results are robust.
In addition, we also carry out robustness checks as followings: (1) we use different clustering levels of Newey-West robust standard errors instead of the White heteroscedasticity robust standard errors in baseline estimation, the results are consistent with that in our main text, indicating that the original model and results remain robust (see Table A6 in Appendix A); (2) in addition to the two-way fixed effect model in the main text, we also use methods such as pooled OLS regression and multiple choice Probit models for alternative estimates, the final results are consistent with that in the main text, indicating that the original model and results remain robust (see Table A7 in Appendix A); (3) we conducted an exclusive test on the structure of rural household expenditure by running the regression where rigid expenditures such as expenditure of food, clothing, transportation, communication, housing and daily necessities are dependent variables as well as income status perception bias is independent variable, the obtained regression coefficients were not significant, indicating the rationality of theoretical analysis, hypothesis formulation and empirical logic in this paper (see Table A8 in Appendix A).

7. Conclusions and Discussion

The mental health of rural children is closely related to their household characteristics, with household income level as one of the important influencing factors. In general, improvement in household income level is deemed to play an important role in promoting children’s mental health. However, the impact and mechanism of household income status perception bias on children’s mental health due to changes in the structure of household expenditure are under studied. We construct a theoretical model of the representative household model based on perception bias theory using the difference between households’ self-rated income ranking and actual income ranking to measure their income status perception bias level. Furthermore, we adopt the CFPS 2012, 2014, and 2016 household survey data to construct a three-wave panel for empirical research. The effect of household income status perception bias on children’s mental health and its mechanism channel are also explored.
Results show (1) a significant negative correlation between household income level and income status perception bias, and poor households are likely to have income status perception bias. That is, the higher the income level is, the less likely it is to overestimate income status. On the contrary, the lower the income level is, the poorer the household is. Thus, they poor households more likely to overestimate their relative status of income, which results in income status perception bias. (2) A significant positive correlation exists between income status perception bias of poor households and their gift-giving expenditure. The more that poor households overestimate their relative income status, the more inclined they are to increase their gift-giving expenditure, thereby changing the structure of household expenditure. Specifically, non-poor households do not appear to experience a similar phenomenon. (3) A significant negative correlation exists between poor households’ income status perception bias and expenditure for children’s education. The more that poor households overestimate their relative income status, the more inclined they are to reduce their expenditure for children’s education. However, non-poor rural households do not appear to experience a similar phenomenon. (4) Income status perception bias among poor households is negatively correlated with children’s mental health level, whereas expenditure for children’s education is positively correlated with children’s mental health level. The more that poor households overestimate their relative income status, the greater the children’s mental health level in their households. The reduction of household expenditure on children’s education is a channel through which poor rural households’ income perception bias increases children’s mental pressure.
On the basis of these conclusions, the government and relevant departments are encouraged to further promote the development of rural regional economy, raise the income level of rural households, ensure that rural households have unlimited necessary expenditures for living, and fundamentally reduce the negative impact of perception bias. Increasing efforts on precise poverty alleviation and focusing on supporting poor households to steadily eliminate poverty and increase income are important. Moreover, narrowing the income gap among rural households and timely and effective publicity and education about reasonable expenditure on household consumption at the village and community levels are measures that can be taken. Furthermore, enhancing the ability for rational arrangement and utilization sourcing of poor households, improving the awareness of expenditure structure, and enhancing the cognitive ability of poor households can reduce the level of perception bias. Especially, raising awareness among poor households about the importance of children’s education can effectively guarantee expenditure on children’s education, improve the level of education in rural areas, and increase the necessary mental education and counseling services. Providing further policy support and financial subsidies to poor households can help guarantee a conducive environment for the good mental health of children and improve the level of mental health of children in poor rural areas.
This study provides a new story in the unique economic and cultural context of China, the world’s largest developing country, for children’s mental health-related research. This study focuses on the special group of rural households, especially poor rural households, and provides explanations from new perspectives such as perception bias and household expenditure structure, enriching the conclusions of existing research and complementing the latest literature on the causes and influence mechanisms of children’s mental health [34,35,58]. In addition, the policy implications derived from this study is of great significance to improve the mental health of children from poor rural households in developing countries and regions, and helps to promote the sustainability of families and child welfare.
Admittedly, this study still has the following limitations: (1) the short panel and relatively small number of valid samples makes the data not comprehensive enough, resulting in the lack of research on long-term effects and sample heterogeneity in this study; (2) this study only analysis one channel for D-K effect and demonstration effect to influence the mental health of children from poor rural household, lacking the research on other influence channels and mechanisms; (3) although the data and empirical strategies we adopted maximize the control of variables and unobservable effects that may interfere with the estimation results, the endogenous problems caused by missing variables and selective biases still constitute a potential threat to the results of this study. In the future, on the basis of improved data, we will further investigate whether income perception bias affects children’s mental health through other channels, and incorporate heterogeneity analysis such as different cultural frameworks and regional differences; in addition, we will focus on the solutions for endogeneity problem such as finding suitable instrumental variables or conducting research through random controlled trials or quasi-natural experiments.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/2071-1050/12/3/870/s1.

Author Contributions

Data curation, M.C.; Formal analysis, M.C. and J.D.; Funding acquisition, C.Y.; Methodology, J.D. and M.C.; Software, J.D. and M.C.; Writing—original draft, M.C.; Writing—review & editing, J.D. and C.Y. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was funded by the National Natural Science Foundation Item, National Natural Science Foundation of The People’s Republic of China [grant number 71773113].

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Proof of Proposition 1.
Optimality implies:
U ( H * * ,   r * * ,   e * * )     U ( H * ,   r * ,   e * )
( 1     p   ( e * ) )   U   ( H * ,   L * ,   e * )   +   p   ( e * )   U 0     1     p   e * *   U   H * * ,   r * * ,   e * *   +   p   e * *   U 0
Then we have
( 1     p   ( e * ) )   U   ( H * * ,   r * * ,   e * * )   +   p   ( e * ) U 0     1     p   e * *   U   H * * ,   r * * ,   e * *   +   p   e * *   U 0
p   ( e * ) [   U 0     U   ( H * * ,   r * * ,   e * * ) ]     p   ( e * * ) [   U 0     U   ( H * * ,   r * * ,   e * * ) ]
[ p   ( e * )     p   ( e * * ) ] [   U 0     U   ( H * * ,   r * * ,   e * * ) ]     0
Since U 0     U   ( H * * ,   r * * ,   e * * ) > 0, therefore U   ( H * * ,   r * * ,   e )   >   U   ( H * * ,   r * * ,   e * * )   , and then must be p   ( e * ) p   ( e * * ) and e   <   e * * . So, if the solution set ( H * * , r * * , e * * ) ≠ ( H * , r * , e * ), by the fact that there is a unique maximum, all of the inequalities above become strict. Finally, we have e * * < e * and e   <   e * * . □
Proof of Lemma 1.
Follows directly from the objective functions and budget constraints in Equations (2), (3) and (5). □
Proof of Lemma 2.
Follows directly from the assumption of psychological pressure function F. □
Table A1. Pearson correlation coefficient matrix.
Table A1. Pearson correlation coefficient matrix.
1234567891011121314151617
11
2−0.135 ***1
30.0280.179 ***1
4−0.016−0.0020.0341
5−0.060 ***0.189 ***0.0160.0031
6−0.045 ***0.191 ***0.019−0.0220.413 ***1
7−0.028 *−0.005−0.058 **−0.008−0.053 ***−0.0131
80.059 ***0.002−0.033−0.034 *−0.023−0.034 *01
9−0.081 ***0.171 ***0.033−0.0040.306 ***0.152 ***−0.015−0.032 *1
10−0.079 ***0.171 ***0.134 ***−0.128 ***0.0270.0210.0160.0210.075 ***1
11−0.0140.177 ***0.208 ***0.0060.036**0.0200.0240.00800.034 **0.213 ***1
12−0.089 ***0.022−0.052 **0.0130.0020.032 *0.042 **0.035 *0.005−0.011−0.102 ***1
130.065 ***−0.019−0.046 *−0.032 *0.007−0.0190.095 ***0.059 ***−0.012−0.048 ***−0.060 ***0.219 ***1
140.046 ***−0.305 ***−0.0360.020−0.116 ***−0.066 ***0.019−0.021−0.362 ***−0.152 ***−0.110 ***0.0190.0031
15−0.047 ***0.039 **0.050 *0.068 ***−0.011−0.012−0.037 **−0.029−0.0200.044 ***0.062 ***−0.016−0.042 **0.044 ***1
160.0060.040 **0.0280.0050.005−0.007−0.0220.016−0.0130.0140.046 ***−0.177 ***−0.057 ***0.0110.0191
170.012−0.066 ***0.030−0.023−0.0160.041 **0.031 *−0.0280.020−0.017−0.035 **0.120 ***0.041 **0.040 **−0.054 ***−0.167 ***1
Note: 1—Children’s mental health; 2—Education expenditure for children; 3—Household gift-giving expenditure; 4—Perception bias of household income status; 5—Children’s age; 6—Children’s education; 7—Children’s physical health status; 8—Children’s academic performance; 9—Whether the child is the first child; 10—Household income; 11—Household expenditure; 12—Household members’ age; 13—Household members’ physical health status; 14—Number of children in household; 15—Parents’ education; 16—Parents’ marital status; 17—Effective parent-child time. * p < 0.1; ** p < 0.05; *** p < 0.01.
Table A2. Effect of household income level on household income status perception bias (full version).
Table A2. Effect of household income level on household income status perception bias (full version).
VariablesPerception Bias
(1)(2)(3)(4)
Income −0.198 *** −0.174 ***
(0.066) (0.0651)
Income ranking −0.099 *** −0.0908 ***
(0.029) (0.0307)
Household members’ age 0.01150.0103
(0.0116)(0.0117)
Household members’ health −0.00145−0.00261
(0.0668)(0.0666)
Household expenditure −0.00167 **−0.000880 **
(0.000819)(0.000391)
No. of children 0.8450.840
(0.906)(0.933)
Household-individual FEYesYesYesYes
County-year FEYesYesYesYes
Observations3621362735123517
R20.2550.2570.3040.307
Note: White heteroscedasticity robust standard errors in brackets; * p < 0.1; ** p < 0.05; *** p < 0.01.
Table A3. Effect of perception bias of household income status on its gift-giving expenditure (full version)
Table A3. Effect of perception bias of household income status on its gift-giving expenditure (full version)
VariablesGift-Giving Expenditure
Non-PoorPoorNon-PoorPoor
(1)(2)(3)(4)
Perception bias−89.802300.543 ***84.830179.718 ***
(374.600)(0.000)(482.452)(0.000)
Household members’ age −279.2 *265.6 ***
(166.1)(0.0192)
Household members’ health 1501 *274.3 ***
(899.0)(0.0471)
Household expenditure 0.451 **−0.105 ***
(0.211)(00175)
No. of children −15335581 ***
(1915)(468.2)
Household-individual FEYesYesYesYes
County-year FEYesYesYesYes
Observations27628801366441
R20.2550.2570.3040.307
Note: White heteroscedasticity robust standard errors in brackets; * p < 0.1; ** p < 0.05; *** p < 0.01.
Table A4. Effect of perception bias of household income status on education expenditure (full version)
Table A4. Effect of perception bias of household income status on education expenditure (full version)
VariablesEducation Expenditure for Children
PoorNon-PoorPoorNon-Poor
(1)(2)(3)(4)
Perception bias70.127−188.032 ***144.879−444.461 ***
(102.100)(53.521)(111.035)(0.000)
Household members’ age 18.26−83.65 **
(34.55)(40.302)
Household members’ health 105.780.49 **
(105.7)(38.990)
Household expenditure 0.03840.00382 *
(0.0364)(0.00211)
No. of children −2293 ***−3035 ***
(709.7)(602.3)
Children’s age 596.6 **532.2 ***
(268.2)(295.4)
Children’s education 193.7258.1 ***
(442.0)(0.001)
Children’s physical health −14.56−143.0 ***
(59.70)(0.003)
Children’s academic performance 237.0 **238.1 ***
(120.6)(0.000)
Whether the child is the first child −1451 **0.000
(617.2)(.)
Household-individual FEYesYesYesYes
County-year FEYesYesYesYes
Observations27628801366441
R20.5120.8250.6310.903
Note: White heteroscedasticity robust standard errors in brackets; * p < 0.1; ** p < 0.05; *** p < 0.01.
Table A5. Effect of perception bias of household income status on education expenditure (full version)
Table A5. Effect of perception bias of household income status on education expenditure (full version)
VariablesChildren’s Mental Health
Non-PoorPoorNon-PoorPoor
(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)(6)(7)(8)
Perception bias0.04750.02993.509 ***2.765 ***0.06860.06236.433 ***5.910 ***
(0.0726)(0.0730)(0.476)(0.526)(0.0902)(0.0900)(0.475)(0.000)
Expenditure for children’s education −0.00783 −0.00304** 0.0478 −0.00466 ***
(0.0588) (0.00127) (0.121) (0.000)
Household members’ age 0.01400.0122−0.912−0.221
(0.0271)(0.0263)(0.899)(0.364)
Household members’ health 0.1460.137−2.845−3.027
(0.152)(0.153)(2.539)(2.924)
Household expenditure 0.000750 **0.000734 **−0.0006.98 ***−0.000347 ***
(0.000)(0.000)(0.000)(0.000)
No. of children 0.2680.4210.1270.096
(0.972)(0.978)(0.363)(0.139)
Children’s age −2.021 ***−0.8382.6741.585 *
(0.463)(0.753)(2.276)(0.478)
Children’s education 0.3090.305−5.250−3.748 *
(0.455)(0.462)(4.007)(1.978)
Children’s physical health 0.185 **0.185 **−2.891 **−2.574 **
(0.0765)(0.0771)(2.653)(1.245)
Children’s academic performance 0.436 *0.413*−1.950 ***−1.436 ***
(0.223)(0.226)(0.0008)(0.0009)
Whether the child is the first child −0.749−0.6670.0000.000
(1.113)(1.115)(.)(.)
Parents’ educational −0.0761−0.0746−2.716 ***−2.744 ***
(0.173)(0.172)(0.000)(0.000)
Parents’ marital status −0.369−0.612−1.352 ***−1.245 ***
(0.872)(0.933)(0.007)(0.002)
Effective parent–child time 0.3520.357−10.97 ***−8.446 ***
(0.827)(0.833)(0.00978)(0.0149)
Household-individual FEYesYesYesYesYesYesYesYes
County-year FEYesYesYesYesYesYesYesYes
Observations2113208565464714321416455451
R20.6970.7050.9350.9450.7550.7560.9930.998
Note: White heteroscedasticity robust standard errors in brackets; * p < 0.1; ** p < 0.05; *** p < 0.01.
Table A6. Robustness checks of diverse clustering levels.
Table A6. Robustness checks of diverse clustering levels.
VariablesTotal SamplePoverty Sample
Perception biasGift-Giving ExpenditureEducation ExpenditureChildren’s Mental Health
(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)(6)
Panel I: Clustering at county level
Income −0.174 **
(0.0742)
Income ranking −0.0908 ***
(0.0339)
Perception bias 179.718 ***−444.5 ***6.433 ***5.910 ***
(0.000)(0.000)(0.449)(0.000)
Education expenditure −0.00466 ***
(0.000)
Observations35123517441437455451
R20.3040.3070.3100.8960.9930.998
Panel II: Clustering at household level
Income −0.174 ***
(0.0666)
Income ranking −0.0908 ***
(0.0313)
Perception bias 179.718 ***−444.5 ***6.433 ***5.910 ***
(0.000)(0.000)(0.485)(0.000)
Education expenditure −0.00466 ***
(0.000)
Observations35123517441441455451
R20.3040.3070.3170.9360.9930.998
Household controlsYesYesYesYesYesYes
Children’s characteristics controlsNoNoNoYesYesYes
Parents’ characteristics controlsNoNoNoNoYesYes
Household-individual FEYesYesYesYesYesYes
County-year FEYesYesYesYesYesYes
Note: Newey-West robust standard errors in brackets; * p < 0.1; ** p < 0.05; *** p < 0.01. Household control variables include household members’ age, self-rated health, household expenditure, and household’s children number. Children characteristics include children’s age, education, physical health status, academic performance, and whether the child is the first child in the household.
Table A7. Robustness checks of diverse specifications.
Table A7. Robustness checks of diverse specifications.
VariablesTotal SamplePoverty Sample
Perception BiasGift-Giving ExpenditureEducation ExpenditureChildren’s Mental Health
(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)(6)
Panel I: OLS estimation
Income −0.268 ***
(0.0232)
Income ranking −0.106 ***
(0.0118)
Perception bias 268.0 ***−7.4580.697 ***0.721 ***
(30.180)(71.39)(0.0991)(0.0992)
Education expenditure 0.00357
(0.0425)
Observations35123517353441455451
R20.1670.1670.1630.2070.1480.147
Panel II: Probit model estimation
Income 0.0973 ***
(0.0190)
Income ranking 0.0590 ***
(0.00965)
Perception bias 0.313 ***0.225 *
(0.0418)(0.125)
Education expenditure −0.0215 **
(0.012)
Observations35123517455451
Pseudo R20.1450.1310.1900.183
Household controlsYesYesYesYesYesYes
Children’s characteristics controlsNoNoNoYesYesYes
Parents’ characteristics controlsNoNoNoNoYesYes
Note: White heteroscedasticity robust standard errors in brackets; * p < 0.1; ** p < 0.05; *** p < 0.01. Household control variables include household members’ age, self-rated health, household expenditure, and household’s children number. Children characteristics include children’s age, education, physical health status, academic performance, and whether the child is the first child in the household.
Table A8. Effect of perception bias on rigid expenditure of poor household
Table A8. Effect of perception bias on rigid expenditure of poor household
VariablesFood Clothing Housing/Daily Necessities Transportation
(1)(2)(3)(4)
Perception bias50.15−7.506−145.7−29.62
(213.8)(14.85)(124.9)(42.05)
Household controlsYesYesYesYes
Household-individual FEYesYesYesYes
County-year FEYesYesYesYes
Observations822819824820
R20.8910.9550.9980.947
Note: White heteroscedasticity robust standard errors in brackets; * p < 0.1; ** p < 0.05; *** p < 0.01. Household control variables include household members’ age, self-rated health, household expenditure, and household’s children number.

References

  1. Yue, J.L.; Fan, X. Child Care Policy in China: Review, Reflection and Reconstruction. Soc. Sci. China 2018, 9, 92–111, 206. [Google Scholar]
  2. Theme Group of the International Liaison Department of the Central Committee of the Chinese Youth Research Center. Special Investigation on the Tenth Anniversary of the Implementation of the United Nations World Program of Behavior for Youth to the Year 2000 and Beyond (1995–2004): China Youth Development Report. China Youth Study 2005, 11, 4–29. [Google Scholar]
  3. Duan, C.; Lyu, L.; Guo, J.; Wang, Z. Survival and Development of Left-Behind Children in Rural China: Based on the Analysis of the Sixth Census Data. Popul. J. 2013, 35, 37–49. [Google Scholar]
  4. Population Census Office under the State Council. Department of Population and Employment Statistics National Bureau of Statistics; Data on the 2015 1% National Population Sampling Survey Data; China Statistics Press: Beijing, China, 2016.
  5. Chan, K.W.; Ren, Y. Children of migrants in China in the twenty-first century: Trends, living arrangements, age-gender structure, and geography. Eurasian Geogr. Econ. 2018, 59, 133–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Duan, C.R.; Zhou, F.L. A Study on Children Left Behind. Popul. Res. 2005, 1, 29–36. [Google Scholar]
  7. Brooks-Gunn, J.; Duncan, G.J.; Maritato, N. Poor Families, Poor Outcomes: The Well-Being of Children and Youth; Conseq. Grow. Up Poor; Duncan, G.J., Brooks-Gunn, J., Eds.; Russell Sage Foundation: New York, NY, USA, 1997; pp. 1–17. [Google Scholar]
  8. Petti, T.A.; Leviton, L.C. Re-Thinking Rural Mental Health Services for Children and Adolescents. J. Public Health Policy 1986, 7, 58–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Crane, J. The Epidemic Theory of Ghettos and Neighborhood Effects on Dropping out and Teenage Childbearing. Am. J. Soc. 1991, 96, 1226–1259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Evans, G.W. The Environment of Childhood Poverty. Am. Psychol. 2004, 59, 77–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Schellenberg, J.A.; Victora, C.G.; Mushi, A.; De Savigny, D.; Schellenberg, D.; Mshinda, H.; Bryce, J.; Tanzania IMCI MCE Baseline Household Survey Study Group. Inequities among the Very Poor: Health Care for Children in Rural Southern Tanzania. Lancet 2003, 361, 561–566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Devoe, J.E.; Krois, L.; Stenger, R. Do Children in Rural Areas Still Have Different Access to Health Care? Results from a Statewide Survey of Oregon’s Food Stamp Population. J. Rural Health 2009, 25, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. You, J.; Han, W.C.; Feng, Y. Effects of Formal Microfinance on Health and Nutrition Level of Rural Children. Chin. Rural Econ. 2014, 3, 58–72. [Google Scholar]
  14. Braga, A.L.; Saldiva, P.H.; Pereira, L.A.; Menezes, J.J.; Conceição, G.M.; Lin, C.A.; Zanobetti, A.; Schwartz, J.; Dockery, D.W. Health Effects of Air Pollution Exposure on Children and Adolescents in São Paulo, Brazil. Pediatr. Pulmonol. 2015, 31, 106–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Davis, E.; Sawyer, M.G.; Lo, S.K.; Priest, N.; Wake, M. Socioeconomic Risk Factors for Mental Health Problems in 4–5-year-old Children: Australian Population Study. Acad. Pediatr. 2010, 10, 41–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Reiss, F. Socioeconomic Inequalities and Mental Health Problems in Children and Adolescents: A Systematic Review. Soc. Sci. Med. 2013, 90, 24–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Mcleod, J.D.; Uemura, R.; Rohrman, S. Adolescent Mental Health, Behavior Problems, and Academic Achievement. J. Health Soc. Behav. 2012, 53, 482–497. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Yang, S.H.; Wang, Z.Y.; Xue, J.W. Correlation of psychological stress and academic achievement among students. Chin. J. Sch. Health 2016, 5, 704–707. [Google Scholar]
  19. Dadds, M.R.; Sanders, M.R.; Morrison, M.; Rebgetz, M. Childhood Depression and Conduct Disorder: II. An Analysis of Family Inter-Action Patterns in the Home. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 1992, 101, 505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Patterson, G.R. Coercion as a Basis for Early Age of Onset for Arrest. J. Mccord Coerc. 1995, 81–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Cohen, S.; Janickideverts, D.; Chen, E.; Matthews, K.A. Childhood Socioeconomic Status and Adult Health. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2010, 1186, 37–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. World Health Organization. The World Health Report 2001 Mental Health: New Understanding, New Hope; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  23. Dearing, E. Psychological Costs of Growing up Poor. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2010, 1136, 324–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Dahl, G.B.; Lochner, L. The Impact of Family Income on Child Achievement: Evidence from the Earned Income Tax Credit. Am. Econ. Rev. 2012, 102, 1927–1956. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Lengua, L.J.; Kovacs, E.A. Bidirectional Associations between Temperament and Parenting and the Prediction of Adjustment Problems in Middle Childhood. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 2005, 26, 21–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Zhou, M.; Murphy, R.; Tao, R. Effects of Parents’ Migration on the Education of Children Left Behind in Rural China. Popul. Dev. Rev. 2014, 40, 273–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Lu, Y. Education of Children Left Behind in Rural China. J. Marriage Fam. 2012, 74, 328–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  28. Normile, D. One in Three Chinese Children Faces an Education Apocalypse. An Ambitious Experiment Hopes to Save Them. Science 2017, 21. Available online: https://www.sciencemag.org/news/2017/09/one-three-chinese-children-faces-education-apocalypse-ambitious-experiment-hopes-save (accessed on 25 July 2018). [CrossRef]
  29. Blake, J. Family Size and the Quality of Children. Demography 1981, 18, 421–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Mechoulan, S.; Wolff, F.C. Intra-household allocation of family resources and birth order: Evidence from France using sibling’s data. J. Popul. Econ. 2015, 28, 937–964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Hua, Q.C.; Kaiser, A.P. Behavior Problems of Preschool Children from Low-Income Families: Review of the Literature. Top. Early Child. Spec. Educ. 2003, 23, 188–216. [Google Scholar]
  32. Li, Q.; Zang, W.B. The Health of Left-Behind Children in Rural China. China Econ. Q. 2010, 1, 341–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Hoffmand, R.; Kröger, H.; Pakpahan, E. Pathways between socioeconomic status and health: Does health selection or social causation dominate in Europe? Adv. Life Course Res. 2018, 36, 23–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Pearce, A.; Dundas, R.; Whitehead, M.; Taylor-Robinson, D. Pathways to inequalities in child health. Arch. Dis. Child. 2019, 104, 998–1003. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  35. Roy, A.L.; Isaia, A.; Li-Grining, C.P. Making meaning from money: Subjective social status and young children’s behavior problems. J. Fam. Psychol. 2019, 33, 240–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Kruger, J.; Dunning, D. Unskilled and unaware of it: How difficulties in recognizing one’s own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1999, 77, 1121–1134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Ehrlinger, J.; Johnson, K.; Banner, M.; Dunning, D.; Kruger, J. Why the unskilled are unaware: Further explorations of (absent) self-insight among the incompetent. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 2008, 105, 98–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Mynttinsen, S.; Sundstrom, A.; Vissers, J.; Koivukoski, M.; Hakuli, K.; Keskinen, E. Self-Assessed Driver Competence among Novice Drivers—A Comparison of Driving Test Candidate Assessments and Examiner Assessments in a Dutch and Finnish Sample. J. Saf. Res. 2009, 40, 301–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Park, Y.J.; Santos-Pinto, L. Overconfidence in tournaments: Evidence from the field. Theory Decis. 2010, 69, 143–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Simon, M.; Houghton, S.M.; Aquino, K. Cognitive Biases, Risk Perception, and Venture Formation: How Individuals Decide to Start Companies. J. Bus. Ventur. 2000, 15, 113–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Zacharakis, A.L.; Shepherd, D.A. The Nature of Information and Overconfidence on Venture Capitalists’ Decision Making. J. Bus. Ventur. 2001, 16, 311–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Asparouhova, E.; Bossaerts, P.; Eguia, J.; Zame, W. Cognitive Biases, Ambiguity Aversion and Asset Pricing in Financial Markets. Swiss Finance Inst. Res. Pap. 2009, 9–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Laing, G.K. Impact of Cognitive Biases on Decision Making by Financial Planners: Sunk Cost, Framing and Problem Space. Int. J. Econ. Finance 2010, 2, 11–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Bertrand, M.; Morse, A. Information Disclosure, Cognitive Biases, and Payday Borrowing. J. Finance 2011, 66, 1865–1893. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Mahmood, T.; Yu, X.; Klasen, S. Do the Poor Really Feel Poor? Comparing Objective Poverty with Subjective Poverty in Pakistan. Soc. Indic. Res. 2018, 142, 543–580. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Yang, B.Y. Cognition of Social Status, Income Gap and Consumption Expenditure of Chinese Residents: Based on China Family Tracking Survey CFPS Survey Data. J. Commer. Econ. 2018, 14, 45–47. [Google Scholar]
  47. Zhao, W.F. Health impact, Family Expenditure Structure and Households’ Income. J. Zhongnan Univ. Econ. Law 2017, 3, 115–124. [Google Scholar]
  48. Chen, X.; Kanbur, R.; Zhang, X. Peer Effects, Risk Pooling, and Status Seeking: What Explains Gift Spending Escalation in Rural China? CEPR Discuss. Pap. 8777 2012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Chen, Y.; Gu, H.Y.; Shi, Q.H. Gifts Become Heavy Burdens: An Economic Analysis of Rural Humanity and Courtesy Behavior. Consum. Econ. 2005, 6, 60–63. [Google Scholar]
  50. Zou, W.; Zheng, H. Why Children from Poor Families Don’t Read: Risk, Intergenerational Transfer of Human Capital and Poverty Trap. Econ. Perspect. 2014, 6, 16–31. [Google Scholar]
  51. Duesenberry, J.S. Income, Saving and the Theory of Consumer Behavior; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1949. [Google Scholar]
  52. Zhou, G.S.; Ma, G.R. Does Gift Expenditure Reduce Normal Consumption: Evidence from CFPS 2010. Zhejiang Soc. Sci. 2015, 3, 15–26, 156. [Google Scholar]
  53. Loewenstein, G.; O’Donoghue, T.; Rabin, M. Projection Bias in Predicting Future Utility. Q. J. Econ. 2003, 118, 1209–1248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Rockoff, J.E. The impact of individual teachers on student achievement: Evidence from panel data. Am. Econ. Rev. 2004, 94, 247–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Zhou, B.; Yu, L. The Impact of Agricultural Technology Application on Farmers’ Income: A Case Study of tracing Observation of Farmers in Jiangxi Province. Chin. Rural Econ. 2011, 1, 49–57. [Google Scholar]
  56. Wang, J. The Economic Impact of Special Economic Zones: Evidence from Chinese Municipalities. J. Dev. Econ. 2013, 101, 133–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Kessler, R.C.; Barker, P.R.; Colpe, L.J.; Epstein, J.F.; Gfroerer, J.C.; Hiripi, E.; Howes, M.J.; Normand, S.L.T.; Manderscheid, R.W.; Walters, E.E.; et al. Screening for Serious Mental Illness in the General Population. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 2003, 60, 184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Bøe, T.; Dearing, E.; Stormark, K.M.; Zachrisson, H.D. Subjective Economic Status in Adolescence: Determinants and Associations with Mental Health in the Norwegian Youth Hordaland Study. J. Fam. Econ. 2018, 39, 323–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Table 1. Description of variables.
Table 1. Description of variables.
Definition of VariablesVariable NamesVariable Descriptions
Core independent and dependent variablesChildren’s mental healthChildren’s mental pressure measured using the K6 scale
Expenditure for children’s educationHousehold’s per capita educational expenditure for children (unit: CHY)
Household gift-giving expenditureHousehold’s per capita gift-giving expenditure (unit: CHY)
Perception bias of household income statusHousehold financial supervisor’s household income perception–actual income perception
Household income Household’s per capita disposable income (unit: CHY)
Children’s individual characteristics control variablesChildren’s ageChildren’s age during the survey year
Children’s education Children’s current grade: Nursery = 1; Kindergarten = 2; Primary school = 3; Junior high school = 4; High school/Technical school/Vocational school = 5; Junior college = 6
Children’s physical health statusChildren’s frequency of hospital visits for illness in the past year
Children’s academic performanceCurrent grade ranking of children’s academic performance: Top 10% = 1; 11–25% = 2; 26–50% = 3; 51–75% = 4; Bottom 24% = 5
Whether the child is the first childYes = 0; No = 1
Household characteristics control variablesHousehold expenditureHousehold’s per capita expenditure (unit: CHY)
Household members’ ageThe age of household members except children in the survey year
Household members’ physical healthHousehold members’ self-rated health scores
Number of children in the householdThe number of children in the household during the survey year
Parents’ characteristics control variablesParents’ educational attainment Highest educational attainment of children’s parents: Illiterate/Semi-illiterate = 1; Primary school = 2; Junior high school = 3; High school/Technical school/Vocational school = 4; junior college = 5; Bachelor’s = 6; Master’s = 7; Doctor = 8
Parents’ marital statusDivorced = 0; Married = 1
Effective parent-child timeTotal length of time the father or mother spent with the child within one year: less than 6 months = 0; 6 months or more = 1
Table 2. Descriptive statistics.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics.
Variable NameNon-PoorPoorNon-Poor–Poor
ObsMeansS.D.ObsMeansS.D.Diff. (S.E.)Cond. Diff. (S.E.)
Children’s mental health27967.6983.5718928.0813.719−0.382 ***(0.142)−0.224 ***(0.078)
Expenditure for children’s education27621860285488013372442523.2 ***(98.59)160.8 ***(58.43)
Household gift-giving expenditure11893320463236929756250344.4(351.8)−257.8 *(151.2)
Perception bias of household income status2796−0.02931.9598920.6661.675−0.695 ***(0.0672)−0.497 ***(0.149)
Household income 25778993113158441886132067107 **(506.2)1274(792.4)
Children’s age279612.571.72389212.541.6840.0301(0.0651)0.000157(0.0058)
Children’s education 26033.4530.7128023.4290.7180.0240(0.0289)0.0266(0.0750)
Children’s physical health status27261.0882.2508671.0022.5260.0857(0.0960)−0.235(0.210)
Children’s academic performance24133.6271.9337573.4891.9160.138 *(0.0800)−0.178(0.180)
Whether the child is the first child2747922272338801132762.28090 ***(140.4)6246 ***(447.6)
Household expenditure279646.1011.2689245.6711.880.426(0.451)0.562(0.667)
Household members’ age27963.1381.2268923.1571.291−0.0185(0.0490)0.0899(0.0982)
Household members’ physical health27961.7860.9038922.0121.060−0.226 ***(0.0394)0.0214(0.0360)
Number of children in the household27960.8660.3408920.8340.3720.0322 **(0.0140)0.0413(0.0418)
Parents’ educational attainment 27242.9101.4028533.0222.042−0.112(0.0749)−0.103(0.176)
Parents’ marital status27960.9220.2688920.9000.3000.0218 *(0.0112)0.0131(0.0121)
Effective parent-child time27960.08660.2818920.1050.307−0.0188(0.0116)0.0340(0.0207)
Note: Standard errors in brackets; * p < 0.1; ** p < 0.05; *** p < 0.01.
Table 3. Effect of household income level on household income status perception bias.
Table 3. Effect of household income level on household income status perception bias.
VariablesPerception Bias
(1)(2)(3)(4)
Income −0.198 *** −0.174 ***
(0.066) (0.065)
Income ranking −0.099 *** −0.091 ***
(0.029) (0.031)
Household controlsNoNoYesYes
Household-individual FEYesYesYesYes
County-year FEYesYesYesYes
Observations3621362735123517
R20.2550.2570.3040.307
Note: White heteroscedasticity robust standard errors in brackets; * p < 0.1; ** p < 0.05; *** p < 0.01. Household control variables include household members’ age, self-rated health, household expenditure, and number of children in the household. Due to space limitations, please refer to Table A2 in Appendix A for full version including estimated coefficients of control variables.
Table 4. Effect of household income status perception bias on household gift-giving expenditure.
Table 4. Effect of household income status perception bias on household gift-giving expenditure.
VariablesGift-Giving Expenditure
Non-PoorPoorNon-PoorPoor
(1)(2)(3)(4)
Perception bias−89.802300.543 ***84.830179.718 ***
(374.600)(0.000)(482.452)(0.000)
Household controlsNoNoYesYes
Household–individual FEYesYesYesYes
County–year FEYesYesYesYes
Observations27628801366441
R20.2550.2570.3040.307
Note: White heteroscedasticity robust standard errors in brackets; * p < 0.1; ** p < 0.05; *** p < 0.01. Household control variables include household members’ age, self-rated health, household expenditure, and number of children in the household. Due to space limitations, please refer to Table A3 in Appendix A for full version including estimated coefficients of control variables.
Table 5. Effect of household income status perception bias on household expenditure for education.
Table 5. Effect of household income status perception bias on household expenditure for education.
VariablesExpenditure for Children’s Education
Non-PoorPoorNon-PoorPoor
(1)(2)(3)(4)
Perception bias70.127−188.032 ***144.879−444.461 ***
(102.100)(53.521)(111.035)(0.000)
Household controlsNoNoYesYes
Children’s characteristics controlsNoNoYesYes
Household–individual FEYesYesYesYes
County–year FEYesYesYesYes
Observations27628801366441
R20.5120.8250.6310.903
Note: White heteroscedasticity robust standard errors in brackets; * p < 0.1; ** p < 0.05; *** p < 0.01. Household control variables include household members’ age, self-rated health, household expenditure, and number of children in the household. Children’s characteristics include children’s age, education, physical health status, academic performance, and whether the child is the first child in the household. Due to space limitations, please refer to Table A4 in Appendix A for full version including estimated coefficients of control variables.
Table 6. Effect of household perception bias of income status on children’s mental health.
Table 6. Effect of household perception bias of income status on children’s mental health.
VariablesChildren’s Mental Health
Non-PoorPoorNon-PoorPoor
(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)(6)(7)(8)
Perception bias0.04750.02993.509 ***2.765 ***0.06860.06236.433 ***5.910 ***
(0.0726)(0.0730)(0.476)(0.526)(0.0902)(0.0900)(0.475)(0.000)
Education expenditure −0.00783 −0.00304 ** 0.0478 −0.00466 ***
(0.0588) (0.00127) (0.121) (0.000)
Household controlsNoNoNoNoYesYesYesYes
Children’s characteristics controlsNoNoNoNoYesYesYesYes
Parents’ characteristics controlsNoNoNoNoYesYesYesYes
Household–individual FEYesYesYesYesYesYesYesYes
County–year FEYesYesYesYesYesYesYesYes
Observations2113208565464714321416455451
R20.6970.7050.9350.9450.7550.7560.9930.998
Note: White heteroscedasticity robust standard errors in brackets; * p < 0.1; ** p < 0.05; *** p < 0.01. Household control variables include household members’ age, self-rated health, household expenditure, and number of children in the household. Children’s characteristics include children’s age, education, physical health status, academic performance, and whether the child is the first child in the household. Parents’ characteristics include parents’ educational attainment, marital status, and effective parent–child time. Due to space limitations, please refer to Table A5 in Appendix A for full version including estimated coefficients of control variables.
Table 7. Robustness checks.
Table 7. Robustness checks.
VariablesRelative Poverty SampleProportion of Total Expenditure
Gift-Giving ExpenditureEducation ExpenditureChildren’s Mental HealthGift-Giving ExpenditureEducation Expenditure
(1)(2)(3)(4)(5)
Perception bias47.943 ***−214.926 ***3.676 ***0.00843 ***−0.0300 ***
(0.000)(0.000)(0.549)(0.000)(0.000)
Education expenditure −0.00398 ***
(0.000)
Household controlsYesYesYesYesYes
Children’s characteristics controlsNoYesYesNoYes
Parents’ characteristics controlsNoNoYesNoNo
Household–individual FEYesYesYesYesYes
County–year FEYesYesYesYesYes
Observations486513529486437
R20.7240.6980.8990.9760.953
Note: White heteroscedasticity robust standard errors in brackets; * p < 0.1; ** p < 0.05; *** p < 0.01. Household control variables include household members’ age, self-rated health, household expenditure and number of children in the household. Children’s characteristics include children’s age, education, physical health status, academic performance, and whether the child is the first child in the household. Parents’ characteristics include parents’ educational attainment, marital status, and effective parent–child time.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Dang, J.; Cheng, M.; Ye, C. “Depression from Overestimation”: Income, Perception Bias and Children’s Mental Health in China’s Rural Households. Sustainability 2020, 12, 870. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12030870

AMA Style

Dang J, Cheng M, Ye C. “Depression from Overestimation”: Income, Perception Bias and Children’s Mental Health in China’s Rural Households. Sustainability. 2020; 12(3):870. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12030870

Chicago/Turabian Style

Dang, Jingqi, Mingda Cheng, and Chunhui Ye. 2020. "“Depression from Overestimation”: Income, Perception Bias and Children’s Mental Health in China’s Rural Households" Sustainability 12, no. 3: 870. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12030870

APA Style

Dang, J., Cheng, M., & Ye, C. (2020). “Depression from Overestimation”: Income, Perception Bias and Children’s Mental Health in China’s Rural Households. Sustainability, 12(3), 870. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12030870

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop