Cognitive Functioning: An Underlying Mechanism of Age and Gender Differences in Self-Assessed Risk Tolerance among an Aging Population
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Data
2.2. Response Variable
2.3. Covariates
2.4. Data Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Descriptive Statistics
3.2. Pearson’s Correlation
3.3. Path Analysis
4. Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Grable, J.E. Risk tolerance. In Handbook of Consumer Finance Research; Jiao, J.J., Ed.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 1–20. [Google Scholar]
- Bucciol, A.; Zarri, L. The shadow of the past: Financial risk taking and negative life events. J. Econ. Psychol. 2015, 48, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yao, R.; Hanna, S.D.; Lindamood, S. Changes in financial risk tolerance, 1983–2001. Financ. Serv. Rev. 2004, 13, 249. [Google Scholar]
- Neelakantan, U. Estimation and impact of gender differences in risk tolerance. Econ. Inq. 2010, 48, 228–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Burton, E.L. The impact of risk aversion on economic development in Portugal. Perspect. Bus. Econ. 2015, 33, 27–38. [Google Scholar]
- Tavares, F.; Santos, E.; Tavares, V.; Ratten, V. The perception and knowledge of financial risk of the Portuguese. Sustainability 2020, 12, 8255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dohmen, T.; Falk, A.; Golsteyn, B.; Huffman, D.; Sunde, U. Risk attitudes across the life course. Econ. J. 2017, 127, F95–F116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blau, F.D.; Kahn, L.M. The US gender pay gap in the 1990s: Slowing convergence. Ilr Rev. 2006, 60, 45–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Roszkowski, M.J.; Grable, J. Gender stereotypes in advisors’ clinical judgments of financial risk tolerance: Objects in the mirror are closer than they appear. J. Behav. Financ. 2005, 6, 181–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Boyle, P.; Yu, L.; Buchman, A.; Bennett, D. Risk aversion is associated with decision making among community-based older persons. Front. Psychol. 2012, 3, 205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Grable, J.E. Financial risk tolerance and additional factors that affect risk taking in everyday money matters. J. Bus. Psychol. 2000, 14, 625–630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yao, R.; Sharpe, D.L.; Wang, F. Decomposing the age effect on risk tolerance. J. Socio-Econ. 2011, 40, 879–887. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hallahan, T.A.; Faff, R.W.; McKenzie, M.D. An empirical investigation of personal financial risk tolerance. Financ. Serv. Rev. 2004, 13, 57–78. [Google Scholar]
- Faff, R.; Hallahan, T.; McKenzie, M. Nonlinear linkages between financial risk tolerance and demographic characteristics. Appl. Econ. Lett. 2009, 16, 1329–1332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Booth, A.; Cardona-Sosa, L.; Nolen, P. Gender differences in risk aversion: Do single-sex environments affect their development? J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2014, 99, 126–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Borghans, L.; Heckman, J.J.; Golsteyn, B.H.; Meijers, H. Gender differences in risk aversion and ambiguity aversion. J. Eur. Econ. Assoc. 2009, 7, 649–658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cupples, S.; Rasure, E.; Grable, J. Educational achievement as a mediator between gender and financial risk tolerance: An exploratory study. Ewha J. Soc. Sci. 2013, 29, 151–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gibson, R.J.; Michayluk, D.; Van de Venter, G. Financial risk tolerance: An analysis of unexplored factors. Financ. Serv. Rev. 2013, 22, 23–50. [Google Scholar]
- Preacher, K.J.; Rucker, D.D.; Hayes, A.F. Addressing moderated mediation hypotheses: Theory, methods, and prescriptions. Multivar. Behav. Res. 2007, 42, 185–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Salthouse, T.A. The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychol. Rev. 1996, 103, 403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Van Hooren, S.; Valentijn, A.; Bosma, H.; Ponds, R.; Van Boxtel, M.; Jolles, J. Cognitive functioning in healthy older adults aged 64–81: A cohort study into the effects of age, sex, and education. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 2007, 14, 40–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCarrey, A.C.; An, Y.; Kitner-Triolo, M.H.; Ferrucci, L.; Resnick, S.M. Sex differences in cognitive trajectories in clinically normal older adults. Psychol. Aging 2016, 31, 166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Exel, E.; Gussekloo, J.; De Craen, A.; Bootsma-Van Der Wiel, A.; Houx, P.; Knook, D.; Westendorp, R. Cognitive function in the oldest old: Women perform better than men. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 2001, 71, 29–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Wahlin, Å.; MacDonald, S.W.; de Frias, C.M.; Nilsson, L.-G.; Dixon, R.A. How do health and biological age influence chronological age and sex differences in cognitive aging: Moderating, mediating, or both? Psychol. Aging 2006, 21, 318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kim, E.J.; Hanna, S.D.; Chatterjee, S.; Lindamood, S. Who among the elderly owns stocks? The role of cognitive ability and bequest motive. J. Fam. Econ. Issues 2012, 33, 338–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pak, T.-Y.; Chatterjee, S. Aging, overconfidence, and portfolio choice. J. Behav. Exp. Financ. 2016, 12, 112–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lee, T.M.; Chan, C.C.; Leung, A.W.; Fox, P.T.; Gao, J.-H. Sex-related differences in neural activity during risk taking: An fMRI study. Cereb. Cortex 2009, 19, 1303–1312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bannier, C.E.; Neubert, M. Gender differences in financial risk taking: The role of financial literacy and risk tolerance. Econ. Lett. 2016, 145, 130–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roszkowski, M.J.; Grable, J.E. Gender differences in personal income and financial risk tolerance: How much of a connection? Career Dev. Q. 2010, 58, 270–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cocco, J.F.; Gomes, F.J. Longevity risk, retirement savings, and financial innovation. J. Financ. Econ. 2012, 103, 507–529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Magendans, J.; Gutteling, J.M.; Zebel, S. Psychological determinants of financial buffer saving: The influence of financial risk tolerance and regulatory focus. J. Risk Res. 2017, 20, 1076–1093. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lucarelli, C.; Uberti, P.; Brighetti, G. Misclassifications in financial risk tolerance. J. Risk Res. 2015, 18, 467–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kochaniak, K.; Ulman, P. Risk-Intolerant but Risk-Taking—Towards a Better Understanding of Inconsistent Survey Responses of the Euro Area Households. Sustainability 2020, 12, 6912. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Variables | M | SD | Min | Max | Freq | % |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | 75.60 | 7.21 | 65.00 | 104.00 | - | - |
Gender | ||||||
Male | - | - | - | - | 3819 | 40.84 |
Female | - | - | - | - | 5531 | 59.16 |
Race | ||||||
White | - | - | - | - | 7594 | 81.22 |
African American | - | - | - | - | 1334 | 14.27 |
Others | - | - | - | - | 422 | 4.51 |
Ethnicity | - | - | - | - | ||
Hispanic | - | - | - | - | 835 | 8.93 |
Non-Hispanic | - | - | - | - | 8515 | 91.07 |
Marital status | ||||||
Single | - | - | - | - | 271 | 2.90 |
Married | - | - | - | - | 5402 | 57.78 |
Others | - | - | - | - | 3677 | 39.33 |
Education | ||||||
Less than high school | - | - | - | - | 1728 | 18.48 |
High school | - | - | - | - | 3401 | 36.37 |
College and above | - | - | - | - | 4221 | 45.14 |
Self-reported health status | ||||||
Excellent | - | - | - | - | 3365 | 35.99 |
Good | - | - | - | - | 3281 | 35.09 |
Fair | - | - | - | - | 2063 | 22.06 |
Poor | - | - | - | - | 641 | 6.86 |
Income | 60,659.45 | 108,263.80 | 0.00 | 4,033,928.00 | ||
Financial wealth | 169,141.80 | 589,532.70 | −299,865.00 | 1.58 × 107 | ||
Housing equity | 146,900.70 | 242,632.70 | −80,4561.90 | 7,627,460.00 | ||
Cognition score | 21.37 | 5.31 | 0.00 | 35.00 | ||
Risk tolerance | 5.59 | 2.77 | 0.00 | 10.00 |
Age | Gender | Cognition Score | Risk Tolerance | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Age | - | |||
Gender (Female = 1) | 0.014 | - | ||
Cognition score | −0.337 *** | 0.042 *** | - | |
Risk tolerance | −0.066 *** | −0.099 *** | 0.094 *** | - |
Outcomes | Predictors | β | 95% CI for β |
---|---|---|---|
Risk tolerance | Age | −0.024 * | [−0.047, −0.001] |
Gender (Female = 1) | −0.166 *** | [−0.207, −0.125] | |
Cognition score | 0.028 * | [0.003, 0.055] | |
Race | |||
White | −0.106 ** | [−0.174, −0.033] | |
Others | −0.100 | [−0.227, 0.036] | |
Ethnicity (Non-Hispanic = 1) | −0.152 ** | [−0.248, −0.057] | |
Marital status | |||
Married | 0.029 | [−0.101, 0.160] | |
Others | −0.025 | [−0.160, 0.103] | |
Education | |||
High school | 0.040 | [−0.032, 0.110] | |
College and above | 0.221 *** | [0.143, 0.286] | |
Self-reported health | |||
Excellent | 0.310 *** | [0.212, 0.410] | |
Good | 0.172 *** | [0.078, 0.268] | |
Fair | 0.023 | [−0.108, 0.128] | |
Income (in thousands) | 0.035 *** | [0.018, 0.050] | |
Financial wealth (in millions) | 0.002 | [−0.016, 0.018] | |
Housing equity (in thousands) | 0.004 | [−0.014, 0.024] | |
Cognition score | Age | −0.294 *** | [−0.312, −0.275] |
Gender (Female = 1) | 0.143 *** | [0.109, 0.176] | |
Education | |||
High school | 0.650 *** | [0.600, 0.701] | |
College and above | 0.921 *** | [0.871, 0.972] | |
Self-reported health | |||
Excellent | 0.548 *** | [0.469, 0.623] | |
Good | 0.445 *** | [0.363, 0.517] | |
Fair | 0.268 *** | [0.182, 0.346] | |
Income (in thousands) | 0.079 *** | [0.060, 0.095] |
β | 95% CI for β | Decision on Hypotheses | |
---|---|---|---|
Age → Cognition score → Risk tolerance | −0.008 * | [−0.016, −0.001] | Supports Hypothesis 1 |
Gender → Cognition score → Risk tolerance | 0.004 | [0.000, 0.008] | Does not support Hypothesis 2 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Sharma, M.; Chatterjee, S. Cognitive Functioning: An Underlying Mechanism of Age and Gender Differences in Self-Assessed Risk Tolerance among an Aging Population. Sustainability 2021, 13, 2361. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13042361
Sharma M, Chatterjee S. Cognitive Functioning: An Underlying Mechanism of Age and Gender Differences in Self-Assessed Risk Tolerance among an Aging Population. Sustainability. 2021; 13(4):2361. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13042361
Chicago/Turabian StyleSharma, Muna, and Swarn Chatterjee. 2021. "Cognitive Functioning: An Underlying Mechanism of Age and Gender Differences in Self-Assessed Risk Tolerance among an Aging Population" Sustainability 13, no. 4: 2361. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13042361
APA StyleSharma, M., & Chatterjee, S. (2021). Cognitive Functioning: An Underlying Mechanism of Age and Gender Differences in Self-Assessed Risk Tolerance among an Aging Population. Sustainability, 13(4), 2361. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13042361