Next Article in Journal
Linking Perceived Corporate Social Responsibility and Employee Well-Being—A Eudaimonia Perspective
Next Article in Special Issue
Promoting STEAM Education in Primary School through Cooperative Teaching: A Design-Based Research Study
Previous Article in Journal
Green Taxes in Africa: Opportunities and Challenges for Environmental Protection, Sustainability, and the Attainment of Sustainable Development Goals
Previous Article in Special Issue
Developing Students’ Attitudes toward Convergence and Creative Problem Solving through Multidisciplinary Education in Korea
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Students’ Emotions and Engagement in the Emerging Hybrid Learning Environment during the COVID-19 Pandemic

by
Elizabeth Acosta-Gonzaga
1,* and
Elena Fabiola Ruiz-Ledesma
2
1
Sección de Estudios de Posgrado e Investigación (SEPI), Unidad Profesional Interdisciplinaria de Ingeniería y Ciencias Sociales y Administrativas (UPIICSA), Instituto Politécnico Nacional, Mexico City 07738, Mexico
2
Sección de Estudios de Posgrado e Investigación (SEPI), Escuela Superior de Cómputo (ESCOM), Instituto Politécnico Nacional, Mexico City 07738, Mexico
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sustainability 2022, 14(16), 10236; https://doi.org/10.3390/su141610236
Submission received: 28 June 2022 / Revised: 31 July 2022 / Accepted: 4 August 2022 / Published: 17 August 2022

Abstract

:
Due to the COVID-19 pandemic, classes in schools acquired a hybrid learning model. Students took their classes both in person and, at other times, remotely. However, students are currently facing situations that they are not familiar with after a period of two years of confinement due to the COVID-19 pandemic. This emerging model of learning, to which the students had to adapt, not only impacted on their emotions during learning but also influenced their perceptions of their abilities and skills in being able to perform adequately in a situation of uncertainty, which also influenced the degree of academic engagement that they had. This study applied the structural equation modeling technique, using PLS-SEM software, to a sample of 194 students. The results show that their self-efficacy to act in a situation of vulnerability was affected, which is why their negative emotions increased and their positive emotions decreased. This in turn influenced the degree of engagement and effort they invested in developing a school activity.

1. Introduction

During the pandemic, classes in schools were taught using a hybrid learning model, which involved students receiving classes that fluctuated between face-to-face and online periods. This model, which is called a hybrid [1], consists in dividing the total number of students into two blocks. The first block of students attended school to take their classes in person, while the second worked remotely. This hybrid model is an emerging adaptation that had to be implemented given the sanitary conditions [1].
This model is also called emergency remote education, according to Hodges et al. [2], as it was an emergency response to a situation in which the main measure was confinement; therefore, its effectiveness cannot be compared to other educational processes that were designed from the very beginning to be provided online [3]. The main difference is that e-learning or online education was created so that students could build their knowledge based on their individual learning experiences, supported by electronic media [4].
The hybrid learning model with which IPN higher education institutions have worked in Mexico City is an emerging adaptation in which the students of a group coexist simultaneously in different spaces [5]. For one week, half of the group studied in the physical classroom, and the other half was connected to classes in a virtual room; in the following week, the situation was reversed.
During this unexpected situation at the time of the COVID-19 pandemic, the students were affected academically, financially, physically, and psychologically [6].
Faced with the situations presented by this emerging hybrid environment, the students were caught up in a carousel of emotions, both positive and negative [7,8]. The negative emotions included anxiety [8,9] about the possibility of catching COVID-19 while traveling between home and school or at the school itself [10] and anger due to not being able to hear or take the entire class during the weeks in which they were learning remotely because of the failures of the school’s internet connection [9,10]. Another negative emotion that the students may have experienced was boredom during online classes due to the aforementioned technical problems related to the lack of a fast internet connection [11].
Fortunately, positive emotions also arose, such as the joy of obtaining a satisfactory grade or the hope of reaching a planned level of success and the pleasure of seeing their classmates in physical form again [12]. These emotions play a fundamental role in student motivation, learning, and performance [13,14,15,16] and, crucially, in the degree of engagement with their learning [17].
This emerging model of learning, to which the students had to adapt, not only impacted their emotions during learning but also influenced their perceptions of their abilities and skills in being able to perform adequately in a situation of uncertainty; we are all born with the need to feel effective in our environment [18].
This raises the following questions: to what extent did the hybrid model of learning affect the students’ efficacy in performing their schoolwork and what emotions prevailed? To what extent did self-efficacy and emotions affect school engagement?
Therefore, this study explored how the students’ degree of self-efficacy when performing in a situation of uncertainty may have influenced their emotions and their degree of engagement with their studies. This research is important as it provides empirical evidence that analyses the effects of the COVID-19 pandemic on the essential constructs for school success, such as academic feelings, self-efficacy, and academic engagement.

1.1. Academic Emotions, Engagement, and Self-Efficacy

Several theories have been proposed to study the interrelationships between motivation, engagement, and self-efficacy, and two of them have been given particular attention. The first is the control–value theory by Pekrun [19], which provides an approach to exploring emotions in achievement environments such as the academic context. This theory implies that students’ emotions can be affected by fostering their perceptions of competence and control. It is beneficial for schools because shaping educational environments properly can contribute to changing emotions in such a way that students know how to use them to their advantage.
The second theory is the expectancy–value theory of motivation by Eccles [20] and Pintrich [21], which analyzes the role of motivation in the performance of goals or tasks. The authors propose two main components as the foundation of this theory. The first plays a crucial role in determining school performance and refers to students’ beliefs about their ability to perform their schoolwork and whether they have control and are responsible for their performance. This component is called expectative and includes self-efficacy and beliefs about the control of their learning. The other component is called value and includes extrinsic and intrinsic motivation towards an objective and the value the student gives to a task.
The emotional state of students is directly related to the involvement that they have with their learning activities. Emotions are feelings towards a situation, thought, or person, which are distinguished by being real or unreal [22]. Among the positive emotions that students can experience are fun, enthusiasm, enjoyment, satisfaction, and vitality. Negative emotions can include boredom, frustration, anxiety, depression, or anger. Emotions during the learning process play a fundamental role in students’ motivation, learning, and school performance [13,14,15] and in the level of engagement that they show with their education [17]. In this study, positive emotions included enjoyment, enthusiasm, amusement, pride, and interest. Negative emotions included boredom, disinterest, frustration, sadness, and anxiety.
In defining school engagement, first Skinner and Belmont [23], then Fredricks et al. [24], and later Reschly and Christenson [25] found that academic engagement is a meta-construct that includes emotional, behavioral, and cognitive engagement. Regarding emotional engagement, the affective, positive, or negative responses that students manifest in the development of academic tasks are evident [24]. The opposite behavior to engagement is the lack of interest, disaffection, or detachment that students may have towards academic work. When a student presents this kind of behavior, they are capable of manifesting negative emotions such as frustration, boredom, anxiety, depression, or anger, as well as passivity, distraction, or mental disconnection [18].
Regarding behavioral engagement, González et al. [26] pointed out that this refers to the time, attention, and effort that students invest in their academic process, that is, in the resolution of assigned tasks and in their learning environment in general. On the other hand, Reschly and Christenson [25] and Reschly [27] commented that behavioral engagement is the level to which a student participates in academic, social, or extracurricular tasks involving attention, concentration, and persistence [18].
Cognitive engagement is the mental effort that students put into their learning process [26]. Student engagement is high when using sophisticated cognitive strategies, such as self-regulation or metacognition [28]. Some metacognitive actions involve learning to plan, monitor, and regulate learning, according to what was pointed out by Ramirez-Arellano et al. [29]. Planning allows students to have knowledge about the way in which the problem will be addressed, while monitoring helps students to understand the problem and monitor its evolution and finally regulate or control their learning [30].
Martin et al. [31] commented that one of the primary factors in understanding the level to which students have to get involved and take responsibility for their educational work is motivation. There are several authors who have analyzed this relationship and verified the effects of motivation on academic engagement [32]. In the case of Wang and Eccles [33], they confirmed that motivation affects student engagement, which they divided into emotional, behavioral, and cognitive engagement.
In the expectancy–value theory, [20,21,34] have proposed that within the motivation component, self-efficacy is an essential element in determining school performance and refers to the students’ beliefs about their ability to accomplish school tasks. In this regard, Sun et al. [35] found that there is also a positive relationship between self-efficacy as a component of motivation and academic performance.
Self-efficacy is defined as the confidence that a person has towards their ability to achieve an objective in a specific situation, and it is also considered fundamental for a student to achieve school goals and to be able to face the different situations that arise during their life [36].
In the school environment, the effects demonstrated by a student’s self-efficacy in meeting goals are crucial as the perception that the student has about their capacity and ability to solve the situations that arise is decisive [37]. Likewise, the control that students have over emotional reactions to success or failure is a strong predictor of effort and persistence in student activity [18].
It has been found that self-esteem, self-efficacy, and expectations are some of the elements that could be involved in student engagement, which in turn would have a considerable effect on the quality and level of engagement [38]. On the other hand, Skinner et al. [18] pointed out that every person innately has the need to feel effective within the environment in which they work, and the more that students, within their school environment, feel this sense of dominance, the better the quality of their involvement is.
Even today, inconsistent results are shown regarding the effects of self-efficacy and academic engagement in hybrid and face-to-face environments [39].
This section provides the theoretical foundation on which this research proposes a hypothetical model (see Figure 1) to study the relationships between self-efficacy, emotions, and academic engagement. The theoretical bases for each relationship are presented in the following section.

1.2. Related Work

This section presents previous research concerning the effects of self-efficacy and emotions on students’ engagement with their schoolwork.

Self-Efficacy and Its Relationship with Emotions and Academic Engagement

Within the academic field, it has been found that emotions during learning are positively related to motivation, learning strategies, and academic performance [40]. Other researchers have found that positive or negative emotions directly impact cognitive and learning strategies differently. For example, if students feel amused or curious about what they are learning (positive emotions), then they will use learning strategies such as self-regulation and critical thinking [14,40].
Similarly, it was found that fun (a positive emotion) is related to motivational beliefs and self-regulation strategies, which in turn were shown to have effects on the level of current and future engagement that the students demonstrated [17]. Another study also suggested that the effects of fun are essential for the adoption of online assessments for mathematics instruction [41].
Thus, positive academic emotions impel students to feel more engaged in schoolwork [42]. This was also observed in the study by Carmona-Halty et al. [43], who surveyed high school students in order to observe the effects of academic engagement and its role as a mediator between students’ emotions and performance. Their findings showed that academic engagement mediates the link between positive emotions and academic performance. In addition, the authors added that increasing students’ positive emotions is a challenge for educators and parents.
In contrast, negative emotions minimize the use of self-regulation strategies and promote external guidance/help [14,44,45].
Similarly, the research of Tze et al. [45] showed that when students feel bored it impacts on their motivation and learning strategies, with the greatest effect on their performance. Likewise, the study by Marchand and Gutierrez [15] demonstrated evidence of the strong negative effect of anxiety on the use of learning strategies.
Therefore, negative academic emotions could negatively on students’ achievements, making it more difficult for students to be engaged in their learning activities [46]. For example, in a longitudinal study on adolescents, Salmera et al. [47] analyzed the effects of self-efficacy as a component of personal resources and school burnout (as feelings of inadequacy) on academic engagement. Their results showed that self-efficacy had a positive relationship with academic engagement and a negative association with burnout. Additionally, one year after the beginning of this study, it was observed that academic engagement was negatively associated with school burnout.
King et al. [48], in a cross-sectional and longitudinal study, explored the effects of positive and negative emotions on academic engagement and its counterpart, academic disengagement. Their results proved that students with high levels of positive emotions were more engaged in their learning. Conversely, students with high levels of negative emotions reported higher disaffection. It was also observed that students who experienced positive emotions were more willing to strive for higher levels of engagement in their school.
Furthermore, in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic, through a longitudinal study Zhang et al. [49] explored the role of adaptability, academic emotions, and school engagement. Their results showed that academic engagement was positively correlated with positive emotions (such as enjoyment) and negatively correlated with negative emotions (such as anxiety, fear, and boredom).
Regarding motivation, it has also been found that there is a positive relationship between motivation and fun and a negative relationship between motivation and boredom [50]. Negative emotions decrease aspects related to intrinsic motivation. However, they increase extrinsic motivation, helping students in a certain sense, as this encourages them to avoid failure [14].
Another researcher, Pellas [51], who analyzed the effects that self-efficacy had on academic engagement in online education courses at a university, found that students who presented high levels of self-efficacy also reached high levels in the use of cognitive strategies and self-regulation. Likewise, it was found that self-efficacy was a significant predictor of the emotional engagement of those involved. As previously mentioned, it can be confirmed that self-efficacy in a given domain is essential to experiencing positive emotions, such as the enjoyment of that domain [17].
Likewise, other studies have found similar results, which explain that self-efficacy impacts on academic engagement [39] and learning performance [52]. Salmela-Aro and Upadyaya [47], when conducting a longitudinal study with adolescent students, also found that there is a positive linear relationship between self-efficacy and academic engagement; they point out that the level of self-efficacy that is attributed to a student influences the level of engagement and effort invested in developing an activity.
This was shown more clearly in the study by Heo et al. [39], who analyzed the relationship between self-efficacy and school engagement in students with and without symptoms of depression. The results showed a direct relationship between self-efficacy and school engagement in non-depressed students.
The research carried out by Borrachero et al. [53] analyzed the emotions of graduate teachers towards science teaching, finding that when teachers believed in their own ability to learn certain content, their positive emotions increased. Moreover, when teachers did not consider themselves capable of learning such content, their negative emotions increased.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Data Collection

A sample of 194 students from a higher education school was obtained, with ages ranging from 18 to 24. The students were pursuing a bachelor’s degree in computer science engineering. The surveys were anonymous and answered by Google Forms (Google, Mountain View, CA, USA). All the students were invited to participate in answering the questionnaire; they were informed of its objective and that the data collected would be used exclusively for research purposes. The students’ consent was requested to participate in the study, and all agreed to participate [54].
The questionnaire was distributed to the students on a regular day of classes; those in the physical classrooms answered on their mobile phones, and those taking classes online answered from home. It was applied at the end of March 2022, three months after starting their semester in the hybrid model, so that they could express their feelings about this model before returning to the face-to-face model; due to the sanitary conditions, they were to return to face-to-face classes in May 2022.
The sample size was five cases per variable observed in the low limit and ten cases per variable observed in the upper limit [55].

2.2. Instruments

The positive and negative emotions of the students were assessed using the Student Engagement and Disaffection in School (SED) instrument [18]. Positive emotions (PE) included enjoyment, enthusiasm, amusement, pride, and interest. Negative emotions (NE) included boredom, disinterest, frustration, sadness, and anxiety. The answers were evaluated using a 5-point Likert scale, where 1 meant totally disagree and 5 meant totally agree.
As the expectancy–value theory of motivation was included [20,21,34], we adopted the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ) instrument by Pintrich et al. [34,56]. This instrument includes self-efficacy (SE) as the motivational factor. Eight items were used and were evaluated with a 7-point Likert scale, where 1 was totally disagree and 7 was totally agree. Examples of self-efficacy for learning included “I’m certain I can understand the most difficult material presented in the readings for this course” and “I’m confident I can do an excellent job on the assignments and tests in this course”.
Both instruments, the SED and the MSLQ, have been validated previously in the educational context [29,57,58].
To assess learning engagement (LE), the Schreiner and Louis [59] Engaged Learning Index instrument was implemented, using ten items. This instrument also evaluated the responses using a 5-point Likert scale, where 1 meant totally disagree and 5 meant totally agree. This instrument has been validated in previous research, such as in the study by [39], for example. To use this instrument, translation into Spanish was carried out and it was reviewed by a group of academics and a professional translator. When all the professionals had given their approval, the instrument was applied at the school where this study took place. This instrument included items such as “I can usually find ways of applying what I’m learning in class to something else in my life” and “I feel energized by the ideas that I am learning in most of my classes”.
Likewise, as the instruments were measured with different Likert scales, all the factors were standardized [60].

2.3. Analysis

The proposed model was analyzed using the structural equation modeling (SEM) statistical technique, using the partial least squares method (PLS_SEM) and using SmartPLS version 3 software (SmartPLS GmbH, Oststeinbek Germany). This technique is particularly useful for small sample sizes and makes better predictions when compared to the covariance-based technique CB-SEM [61]. In addition, the PLS-SEM technique does not require statistical assumptions such as data normality and sample size; so, it is considered a non-parametric SEM technique. The PLS-SEM technique has been used for both confirmatory and predictive research [62].
Utilizing a PLS-SEM technique is a two-tier process. The first tier involves calculating the measurement model or outer model. This model can include reflective or formative indicator variables. The difference between the two measurement approaches lies in the causal priority between the constructs and their indicator variables. In this study, reflective measures were used as the indicator variables of each construct are competitive with each other and represent manifestations of the construct [63].
The second tier consists in estimating the relationship among the constructs in the structural model or inner model. The links between the constructs are hypothesized according to theoretical reasoning [64].
As the measurement model evaluates the quality of the relationships between indicator variables and their underlying constructs, there were some criteria to validate. The construct validity was verified using Cronbach’s alpha (A); for many researchers, values greater than 0.70 are considered acceptable [65].
The composite reliability (CR) and the average of variance extracted (AVE) evaluated the convergent validity; values greater than 0.7 are considered acceptable for CR and 0.5 for AVE [66].
The discriminant validity is verified when the values of the diagonal that represent the square root of the AVE values of each construct are greater than the values below them, which represent the values of the correlations between the constructs [67].
The indicator reliability shows how much of the indicator’s variation can be explained by the construct. Hulland [68] suggested that empirical studies can include loadings smaller than 0.4 in a PLS-SEM model or, as Hair et al. [66] mentioned, larger than 0.5.
The value of Cronbach’s alpha, CR, and AVE, as well as the Fornell–Larcker discriminant validity criterion (shown later), are indicators of the goodness of fit of the proposed model [69].
The structural model was estimated by assessing collinearity issues, the path coefficient significance level, the level of determination coefficient R2, the effect size f2, and the predictive relevance Q2.
If two or more constructs (as independent variables) of the structural model are highly related, there is a multicollinearity issue, and it is observed when the variance inflation factor (VIF) coefficient is higher than 5 [66]. The goodness of the path coefficients was tested by the bootstrapping technique and t-statistics [70].
The R2 reflects the level of the construct’s explained variance and can take values between 0 and 1. For social science research, the following thresholds are applied: weak (0.25), moderate (0.50), and substantial (0.75) [71].
The effect size f2 shows the change in R2 when a certain (exogenous) construct is omitted from the model. According to Cohen [72], the effect sizes of 0.02, 0.15, and 0.35 are small, medium, and large, respectively.
The Q2 value assesses the predictive accuracy of the structural model. Stone [73] and Geisser [74] suggest using the Stone–Geisser test. The predictive relevance should be positive and with values higher than zero [75].

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Statistics, Reliability, and Validity

The proposed model was analyzed using the structural equation technique, and the validity of the constructs was verified. The measurement model evaluated the construct reliability, convergent validity, discriminant validity, and indicator reliability. In order to achieve validity, five observed variables were removed from the learning engagement (LE) construct, and four observed variables from the negative emotions (NE) construct. No observed variables were removed from the self-efficacy (SE) and positive emotion (PE) constructs.
The observed variables and their factor loadings, including Cronbach’s alpha, composite reliability (CR), and the average variance extracted (AVE) of the constructs involved in the proposed model, are shown in Table 1.
The reliability of each construct (construct reliability) was evaluated using Cronbach’s alpha (A). Values greater than 0.70 were obtained; so, these values were considered acceptable. The convergent validity was also evaluated using the values of the composite reliability (CR) and the average variance extracted (AVE). Values greater than 0.7 for CR and 0.5 for AVE were observed; so, it was considered that construct validity was reached. The factor loadings show values higher than 0.4 [68], reaching indicator reliability.
Table 2 shows the discriminant validity of the measurement model, in which it is observed that the AVE values (on the diagonal) are greater than the values of the square root of the correlations between the constructs (the rest of the values). Therefore, discriminant validity was reached.

3.2. Causal Model or Structural Model

Figure 2 shows the results of the proposed model. These results demonstrate that the relationships between the constructs are statistically significant.
The results show that there is a direct and significant relationship between self-efficacy and academic engagement. Relationships between self-efficacy and positive emotions and self-efficacy and negative emotions were also observed. It was noticed that both emotions influence academic engagement. It was observed that the significance levels of the path coefficients were all significant (*** p < 0.001).
Hair [66] suggests that the VIF values should be lower than 5. The results show that the inner VIF values were NE (1.516), PE (1.399), and SE (1.443). Therefore, collinearity among the constructs was not an issue.
The determination coefficient R2 of the construct learning engagement was 0.58, which is considered medium; for the negative emotions, it was 0.256, and for the positive emotions, it was 0.195, both of which are considered small [71]. This means that PE, NE, and SE were able to explain more than half the variance in the learning engagement construct.
The effect sizes f2 of the structural model show the values of NE (0.407) and PE (0.202), which, according to Cohen [72], are large and medium, respectively. It was also observed that the SE construct had no effect on learning engagement as its value (0.015) was less than 0.02.
To evaluate the predictive accuracy of the structural model, the Q2 value of the endogenous constructs was used. The results show values of LE (0.289), NE (0.127), and PE (0.099), all of which are positive and higher than zero [75].

4. Discussion

During the health conditions imposed by the COVID-19 pandemic, in which an emerging hybrid teaching model was established, the manner in which self-efficacy influenced positive and negative emotions was analyzed, along with how these, in turn, have impacted the academic engagement of higher education students. Self-efficacy should be understood as the perception or judgement of students about their ability to complete school tasks.
Various studies have found the significant role that emotions play in the learning process [14,50,76,77]. The results show that self-efficacy has an impact on positive and negative emotions. As can be seen in Figure 2, there is a causal relationship between self-efficacy and negative emotions, which means that when students do not consider themselves capable of learning certain academic content, their negative emotions increase; that is, the low values of self-efficacy mean that students do not feel they have the skills and abilities to pass their subjects [53], which is why they feel anxious, stressed, distracted, and bored. This was also shown in the investigations by Sinatra and Taasoobshirazi [78] and Ramirez-Arellano et al. [29].
Additionally, it was found that there is a negative causal relationship between self-efficacy and the emotions of enjoyment, enthusiasm, and fun (positive). That is, when students do not believe in their own ability to learn the content of the subject, their positive emotions decrease, which could imply that students who feel less able to complete a school task have a decrease in their positive emotions and, therefore, that they might feel uninterested in the class [29]. As mentioned, it can be confirmed that self-efficacy in a given domain is essential to be able to experience positive emotions, such as the enjoyment of that domain [17].
What leads to the reflection on how the emerging hybrid teaching model has influenced the self-efficacy of the students during the uncertain conditions of the COVID-19 pandemic is the fact that they felt stressed, worried, and anxious about getting COVID-19 on their way from home to school and at the school itself. In the week during which students had to attend their online classes, feelings of anger or frustration were evident as a consequence of not being able to clearly hear their class due to failures caused by the school internet from which the teacher transmitted. Likewise, the students felt bored in their online classes due to the same technical problems caused by an inadequate internet signal. Therefore, it can be assumed that the described problems interfere with the skills that the students must have to be able to attend and complete all school activities, consequently increasing their negative emotions.
The results also show the effects of negative and positive emotions on academic engagement. Research has been conducted that confirms that emotions predict academic engagement [51] and school performance [14]. Emotions are an essential aspect of the academic career of students, and they can vary over time [79]. During the health emergency, the students’ emotions may have fluctuated due to the stress and anxiety caused by the environment of uncertainty about possible infections.
It has been proven that negative emotions are related to poor grades [80]; the fact that students experience emotions of stress, anxiety, worry, or boredom causes them to feel uncommitted to their learning, affecting their behavioral and cognitive engagement [81]. This was also observed in another study that showed that stress in students affected their degree of academic engagement [29]. Therefore, it is necessary to foster positive self-efficacy beliefs in students, which should be promoted to reduce negative emotions.
Conversely, if students feel enthusiastic and interested, these feelings cause a higher degree of engagement [53]. Therefore, it is vital to introduce motivating factors that promote enjoyment, enthusiasm, and fun in such a way that they manage to encourage a high degree of behavioral and cognitive engagement in students.
Other studies have found that self-efficacy influences the degree of academic engagement [39]. This research has also found a direct causal relationship between self-efficacy and academic engagement. When students show low levels of self-efficacy, this could imply that they are not very engaged with their homework and school activities. Conversely, high levels of self-efficacy have been shown to influence the degree of commitment of the students to their studies. Therefore, students who have a positive perception of the fact that they are capable of successfully carrying out their school activities will be more committed.
This was also observed in the research by Salmela-Aro and Upadyaya [47], who found a positive linear relationship between self-efficacy and academic engagement in adolescent students, pointing out that the level of self-efficacy that is attributed to a student influences their degree of engagement and effort invested in developing an activity. This implies that practices and academic activities that stimulate positive self-efficacy beliefs in students should be encouraged; this in turn will increase their motivation, performance, and skills when performing their school activities [58,82]. This encourages universities to apply support and tutoring models, especially for students in their first year at university, to develop skills such as autonomy, digital competence, and self-regulation [83].

5. Conclusions, Limitations, and Future Research

5.1. Conclusions

During the health emergency caused by the COVID-19 pandemic, universities had to implement an emerging hybrid teaching model, combining face-to-face classes and online classes. Within this new situation, students experienced feelings of uncertainty due to the fact that they were facing situations of possible contagion or technical problems due to the poor internet signal from the place where teachers transmitted classes. These problems affected the abilities and skills of the students in performing adequately in an uncertain situation. When students do not consider themselves capable of learning certain content, their negative emotions increase, and consequently, they feel anxious, stressed, distracted, and bored. Conversely, more motivated students feel more skillful in completing a school task; they feel interested in the class, enthusiastic, and even have a sense of fun. Emotions such as stress, anxiety, worry, and boredom cause students to feel less engaged in their learning. In response to this problem, the abilities and skills of students should be stimulated by providing the appropriate technical conditions and continuing to promote preventive measures to avoid contagion, all with the aim of reducing their negative emotions in order to help build positive self-efficacy beliefs in the students.
It can be concluded that the conditions of uncertainty in the hybrid learning model, which was implemented due to the health emergency, have affected the level of self-efficacy that is attributed to a student. This, in turn, has influenced the degree of engagement and the effort that students invest in developing an activity.

5.2. Limitations of the Research

The study had the limitation that it was only carried out in an IPN engineering school because the other IPN schools continued the semester online due to a large number of infections in the city. Therefore, a small sample size was obtained.

5.3. Future Research

As a continuation of this study, future research could focus on factors related to the work of tutoring the students who showed little engagement with their schoolwork by monitoring the tutoring for a semester and measuring the impact at the end of it.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, E.A.-G. and E.F.R.-L.; methodology, E.A.-G. and E.F.R.-L.; formal analysis, E.A.-G.; investigation, E.A.-G. and E.F.R.-L.; resources, E.A.-G. and E.F.R.-L.; writing—original draft preparation, E.A.-G.; writing—review and editing, E.A.-G. and E.F.R.-L.; supervision, E.A.-G. and E.F.R.-L.; funding acquisition, E.A.-G. and E.F.R.-L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Instituto Politécnico Nacional, grant number SIP20220747. The APC was funded by the Instituto Politécnico Nacional.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the Instituto Politécnico Nacional for their support of this research.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. O’Byrne, W.I.; Pytash, K.E. Hybrid and Blended Learning: Modifying Pedagogy across Path, Pace, Time, and Place. J. Adolesc. Adult Lit. 2015, 59, 137–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Hodges, C.B.; Moore, S.; Lockee, B.B.; Trust, T.; Bond, M.A. The Difference between Emergency Remote Teaching and Online Learning. 2020. Available online: http://hdl.handle.net/10919/104648 (accessed on 5 May 2022).
  3. Benítez-Amado, A. Las Universidades Como Organizaciones Adaptativas: El Contexto de la COVID 19 Como Revulsivo. Available online: https://www.universidadsi.es/las-universidades-como-organizaciones-adaptativas-el-contexto-de-la-covid-19-como-revulsivo/ (accessed on 7 May 2022).
  4. Tavangarian, D. Is E-Learning the Solution for Individual Learning? Electron. J. E-Learn. 2004, 2, 265–272. [Google Scholar]
  5. Alvarez, S.M. El Desafío de Repensar La Universidad En La Era Digital. Cuad. Univ. 2020, 13, 09–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Kecojevic, A.; Basch, C.H.; Sullivan, M.; Davi, N.K. The Impact of the COVID-19 Epidemic on Mental Health of Undergraduate Students in New Jersey, Cross-Sectional Study. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0239696. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Besser, A.; Flett, G.L.; Zeigler-Hill, V. Adaptability to a Sudden Transition to Online Learning during the COVID-19 Pandemic: Understanding the Challenges for Students. Scholarsh. Teach. Learn. Psychol. 2022, 8, 85–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Cao, W.; Fang, Z.; Hou, G.; Han, M.; Xu, X.; Dong, J.; Zheng, J. The Psychological Impact of the COVID-19 Epidemic on College Students in China. Psychiatry Res. 2020, 287, 112934. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. del Arco, I.; Flores, Ò.; Ramos-Pla, A. Structural Model to Determine the Factors That Affect the Quality of Emergency Teaching, According to the Perception of the Student of the First University Courses. Sustainability 2021, 13, 2945. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. López, E.P.; Atochero, A.V.; Rivero, S.C. Educación a distancia en tiempos de COVID-19: Análisis desde la perspectiva de los estudiantes universitarios. RIED Rev. Iberoam. De Educ. A Distancia 2021, 24, 331–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Alvárez, M. COVID-19 y Educación Superior: De Los Efectos Inmediatos al Día Después. Análisis de Impactos, Respuestas Políticas y Recomendaciones. Rev. Argent. De Educ. Super. 2020, 20, 156–158. [Google Scholar]
  12. Giannini, S. COVID-19 y Educación Superior: De Los Efectos Inmediatos al Día Después. Rev. Latinoam. Educ. Comp. RELEC 2020, 11, 1–57. [Google Scholar]
  13. Pekrun, R.; Goetz, T.; Titz, W.; Perry, R.P. Academic Emotions in Students’ Self-Regulated Learning and Achievement: A Program of Qualitative and Quantitative Research. Educ. Psychol. 2002, 37, 91–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Pekrun, R.; Goetz, T.; Frenzel, A.C.; Barchfeld, P.; Perry, R.P. Measuring Emotions in Students’ Learning and Performance: The Achievement Emotions Questionnaire (AEQ). Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2011, 36, 36–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Marchand, G.C.; Gutierrez, A.P. The Role of Emotion in the Learning Process: Comparisons between Online and Face-to-Face Learning Settings. Internet High. Educ. 2012, 15, 150–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Daniels, L.M.; Tze, V.M.C.; Goetz, T. Examining Boredom: Different Causes for Different Coping Profiles. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2015, 37, 255–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Chatzistamatiou, M.; Dermitzaki, I.; Efklides, A.; Leondari, A. Motivational and Affective Determinants of Self-Regulatory Strategy Use in Elementary School Mathematics. Educ. Psychol. 2015, 35, 835–850. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Skinner, E.; Furrer, C.; Marchand, G.; Kindermann, T. Engagement and Disaffection in the Classroom: Part of a Larger Motivational Dynamic? J. Educ. Psychol. 2008, 100, 765–781. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Pekrun, R. The Control-Value Theory of Achievement Emotions: Assumptions, Corollaries, and Implications for Educational Research and Practice. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2006, 18, 315–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Eccles, J. Expectancies, Values and Academic Behaviors. In Achievement and Achievement Motives; Spence, J.T., Ed.; Freeman: San Franciso, CA, USA, 1983; pp. 75–146. [Google Scholar]
  21. Pintrich, P.R. A Process-Oriented View of Student Motivation and Cognition. New Dir. Inst. Res. 1988, 1988, 65–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Pons, F.; Rosnay, M.D.; Cuisinier, F. Cognition and Emotion. In Learning and Cognition in Education; Aukrust, V.G., Ed.; Elsevier: Oxford, UK, 2011; pp. 78–84. [Google Scholar]
  23. Skinner, E.A.; Belmont, M.J. Motivation in the Classroom: Reciprocal Effects of Teacher Behavior and Student Engagement across the School Year. J. Educ. Psychol. 1993, 85, 571–581. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Fredricks, J.A.; Blumenfeld, P.C.; Paris, A.H. School Engagement: Potential of the Concept, State of the Evidence. Rev. Educ. Res. 2004, 74, 59–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Reschly, A.L.; Christenson, S.L. Jingle, Jangle, and Conceptual Haziness: Evolution and Future Directions of the Engagement Construct. In Handbook of Research on Student Engagement; Christenson, S.L., Reschly, A.L., Wylie, C., Eds.; Springer US: Boston, MA, USA, 2012; pp. 3–19. ISBN 978-1-4614-2018-7. [Google Scholar]
  26. González, A.; Rodríguez, Y.; Faílde, J.M.; Carrera, M.V. Anxiety in the Statistics Class: Structural Relations with Self-Concept, Intrinsic Value, and Engagement in Two Samples of Undergraduates. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2016, 45, 214–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Reschly, A.L. Interventions to Enhance Academic Engagement. In Student Engagement: Effective Academic, Behavioral, Cognitive, and Affective Interventions at School; Reschly, A.L., Pohl, A.J., Christenson, S.L., Eds.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2020; pp. 91–108. ISBN 978-3-030-37285-9. [Google Scholar]
  28. Sinatra, G.M.; Broughton, S.H.; Lombardi, D. Emotions in Science Education. In International Handbook of Emotions in Education; Pekrun, R., Linnembrink-García, L., Eds.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2014; pp. 415–436. [Google Scholar]
  29. Ramirez-Arellano, A.; Acosta-Gonzaga, E.; Bory-Reyes, J.; Hernández-Simón, L.M. Factors Affecting Student Learning Performance: A Causal Model in Higher Blended Education. J. Comput. Assist. Learn. 2018, 34, 807–815. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Greene, B.A. Measuring Cognitive Engagement With Self-Report Scales: Reflections From Over 20 Years of Research. Educ. Psychol. 2015, 50, 14–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Martin, A.J.; Yu, K.; Papworth, B.; Ginns, P.; Collie, R.J. Motivation and Engagement in the United States, Canada, United Kingdom, Australia, and China: Testing a Multi-Dimensional Framework. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 2015, 33, 103–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Sinatra, G.M.; Heddy, B.C.; Lombardi, D. The Challenges of Defining and Measuring Student Engagement in Science. Educ. Psychol. 2015, 50, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Wang, M.-T.; Eccles, J.S. School Context, Achievement Motivation, and Academic Engagement: A Longitudinal Study of School Engagement Using a Multidimensional Perspective. Learn. Instr. 2013, 28, 12–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Pintrich, P.R.; de Groot, E.V. Motivational and Self-Regulated Learning Components of Classroom Academic Performance. J. Educ. Psychol. 1990, 82, 33–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Sun, Z.; Xie, K.; Anderman, L.H. The Role of Self-Regulated Learning in Students’ Success in Flipped Undergraduate Math Courses. Internet High. Educ. 2018, 36, 41–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Pérez, M.C.; Molero, M. del M.; Barragán, A.B.; Martínez, Á.M.; Simón, M. del M.; Linares, J.J.G. Autoeficacia y Engagement en estudiantes de Ciencias de la Salud y su relación con la autoestima. Publicaciones 2018, 48, 161–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Bandura, A. Self-Efficacy Mechanism in Human Agency. Am. Psychol. 1982, 37, 122–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Griffiths, A.-J.; Sharkey, J.D.; Furlong, M.J. Student Engagement and Positive School Adaptation. In Handbook of Positive Psychology in Schools; Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group: New York, NY, US, 2009; pp. 197–211. ISBN 978-0-8058-6362-8. [Google Scholar]
  39. Heo, H.; Bonk, C.J.; Doo, M.Y. Influences of Depression, Self-Efficacy, and Resource Management on Learning Engagement in Blended Learning during COVID-19. Internet High. Educ. 2022, 54, 100856. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Pekrun, R.; Perry, R.P. Control-Value Theory of Achievement Emotions. In International Handbook of Emotions in Education; Educational psychology handbook series; Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group: New York, NY, USA, 2014; pp. 120–141. ISBN 978-0-415-89501-9. [Google Scholar]
  41. Acosta-Gonzaga, E.; Walet, N.R. The Role of Attitudinal Factors in Mathematical On-Line Assessments: A Study of Undergraduate STEM Students. Assess. Eval. High. Educ. 2018, 43, 710–726. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Reschly, A.L.; Huebner, E.S.; Appleton, J.J.; Antaramian, S. Engagement as Flourishing: The Contribution of Positive Emotions and Coping to Adolescents’ Engagement at School and with Learning. Psychol. Sch. 2008, 45, 419–431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Carmona-Halty, M.; Salanova, M.; Llorens, S.; Schaufeli, W.B. Linking Positive Emotions and Academic Performance: The Mediated Role of Academic Psychological Capital and Academic Engagement. Curr. Psychol. 2021, 40, 2938–2947. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Mega, C.; Ronconi, L.; De Beni, R. What Makes a Good Student? How Emotions, Self-Regulated Learning, and Motivation Contribute to Academic Achievement. J. Educ. Psychol. 2014, 106, 121–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Tze, V.M.C.; Klassen, R.M.; Daniels, L.M. Patterns of Boredom and Its Relationship with Perceived Autonomy Support and Engagement. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2014, 39, 175–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Putwain, D.; Sander, P.; Larkin, D. Academic Self-Efficacy in Study-Related Skills and Behaviours: Relations with Learning-Related Emotions and Academic Success. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 2013, 83, 633–650. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Salmela-Aro, K.; Upadyaya, K. School Burnout and Engagement in the Context of Demands–Resources Model. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 2014, 84, 137–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. King, R.B.; McInerney, D.M.; Ganotice, F.A.; Villarosa, J.B. Positive Affect Catalyzes Academic Engagement: Cross-Sectional, Longitudinal, and Experimental Evidence. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2015, 39, 64–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Zhang, K.; Wu, S.; Xu, Y.; Cao, W.; Goetz, T.; Parks-Stamm, E.J. Adaptability Promotes Student Engagement Under COVID-19: The Multiple Mediating Effects of Academic Emotion. Front. Psychol. 2021, 11, 633265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Putwain, D.W.; Pekrun, R.; Nicholson, L.J.; Symes, W.; Becker, S.; Marsh, H.W. Control-Value Appraisals, Enjoyment, and Boredom in Mathematics: A Longitudinal Latent Interaction Analysis. Am. Educ. Res. J. 2018, 55, 1339–1368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Pellas, N. The Influence of Computer Self-Efficacy, Metacognitive Self-Regulation and Self-Esteem on Student Engagement in Online Learning Programs: Evidence from the Virtual World of Second Life. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2014, 35, 157–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Diseth, Å.; Meland, E.; Breidablik, H.J. Self-Beliefs among Students: Grade Level and Gender Differences in Self-Esteem, Self-Efficacy and Implicit Theories of Intelligence. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2014, 35, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Borrachero, A.B.; Brígido, M.; Mellado, L.; Costillo, E.; Mellado, V. Emotions in Prospective Secondary Teachers When Teaching Science Content, Distinguishing by Gender. Res. Sci. Technol. Educ. 2014, 32, 182–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. British Educational Research Association, [BERA] Ethical Guidelines for Educational Research, Fourth Edition (2018). Available online: https://www.bera.ac.uk/publication/ethical-guidelines-for-educational-research-2018-online (accessed on 15 October 2020).
  55. Wang, J.; Wang, X. Structural Equation Modeling: Applications Using Mplus; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2012; ISBN 978-1-118-35631-9. [Google Scholar]
  56. Pintrich, P.R. A Manual for the Use of the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ); University of Michigan: Ann Arbor, MI, USA; Dept. of Education. Office of Educational Research and Improvement.Educational Resources Information Center: Washinton, DC, USA, 1991. [Google Scholar]
  57. Acosta-Gonzaga, E.; Ramirez-Arellano, A. Estudio comparativo de técnicas de analítica del aprendizaje para predecir el rendimiento académico de los estudiantes de educación superior. CienciaUAT 2020, 15, 63–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Acosta-Gonzaga, E.; Ramirez-Arellano, A. The Influence of Motivation, Emotions, Cognition, and Metacognition on Students’ Learning Performance: A Comparative Study in Higher Education in Blended and Traditional Contexts. SAGE Open 2021, 11, 21582440211027560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Schreiner, L.A.; Louis, M.C. The Engaged Learning Index: Implications for Faculty Development. J. Excell. Coll. Teach. 2011, 22, 5–28. [Google Scholar]
  60. Ennos, R.; Johnson, M. Statistical And Data Handling Skills in Biology; Pearson: London, UK, 2018; ISBN 1-292-13311-2. [Google Scholar]
  61. Terzis, V.; Economides, A.A. The Acceptance and Use of Computer Based Assessment. Comput. Educ. 2011, 56, 1032–1044. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Hair, J.F.; Hult, G.T.M.; Ringle, C.M.; Sarstedt, M. A Primer on Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM); Sage publications: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2021; ISBN 1-5443-9633-3. [Google Scholar]
  63. Bollen, K.A. Structural Equations with Latent Variables; Wiley Series in Probability and Mathematical Statistics; Applied Probability and Statistics Section; 1a; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 1989; ISBN 978-0-471-01171-2. [Google Scholar]
  64. Götz, O.; Liehr-Gobbers, K.; Krafft, M. Evaluation of Structural Equation Models Using the Partial Least Squares (PLS) Approach. In Handbook of Partial Least Squares; Springer Handbooks of Computational Statistics; Esposito Vinzi, V., Chin, W.W., Henseler, J., Wang, H., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2010; pp. 691–711. ISBN 978-3-540-32825-4. [Google Scholar]
  65. Bonett, D.G.; Wright, T.A. Cronbach’s Alpha Reliability: Interval Estimation, Hypothesis Testing, and Sample Size Planning. J. Organ. Behav. 2015, 36, 3–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Hair, J.F.; Black, W.C.; Babin, B.J.; Anderson, R.E.; Tatham, R.L. Multivariate Data Analysis, 6th ed.; Prentice Hall: Upper Saddle River, NJ, USA, 2006; Volume 5. [Google Scholar]
  67. Fornell, C.; Larcker, D.F. Evaluating Structural Equation Models with Unobservable Variables and Measurement Error. J. Mark. Res. 1981, 18, 39–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Hulland, J. Use of Partial Least Squares (PLS) in Strategic Management Research: A Review of Four Recent Studies. Strateg. Manag. J. 1999, 20, 195–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Garson, G.D. Partial Least Squares: Regression and Structural Equation Models; Statistical Associates Publishers: Asheboro, NC, USA, 2016; p. 63. [Google Scholar]
  70. Hair, J.F.; Hult, G.T.M.; Ringle, C.; Sarstedt, M. A Primer on Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM); Sage Publications: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2014; ISBN 978-1-4833-1097-8. [Google Scholar]
  71. Hair, J.F.; Sarstedt, M.; Pieper, T.M.; Ringle, C.M. The Use of Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling in Strategic Management Research: A Review of Past Practices and Recommendations for Future Applications. Long Range Plan. 2012, 45, 320–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Cohen, J. A Power Primer; Methodological issues and strategies in clinical research, 4th ed.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2016; p. 284. ISBN 978-1-4338-2091-5. [Google Scholar]
  73. Stone, M. Cross-Validatory Choice and Assessment of Statistical Predictions. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B Methodol. 1974, 36, 111–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Geisser, S. A Predictive Approach to the Random Effect Model. Biometrika 1974, 61, 101–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Chin, W.W. The Partial Least Squares Approach for Structural Equation Modeling. In Modern Methods for Business Research; Methodology for Business and Management; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1998; pp. 295–336. ISBN 0-8058-2677-7. [Google Scholar]
  76. Kim, C.; Park, S.W.; Cozart, J.; Lee, H. From Motivation to Engagement: The Role of Effort Regulation of Virtual High School Students in Mathematics Courses. J. Educ. Technol. Soc. 2015, 18, 261–272. [Google Scholar]
  77. Butz, N.T.; Stupnisky, R.H.; Pekrun, R. Students’ Emotions for Achievement and Technology Use in Synchronous Hybrid Graduate Programmes: A Control-Value Approach. Res. Learn. Technol. 2015, 23, 26097. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Sinatra, G.M.; Taasoobshirazi, G. The Self-Regulation of Learning and Conceptual Change in Science: Research, Theory, and Educational Applications. In Handbook of Self-Regulation of Learning and Performance, 2nd ed.; Educational psychology handbook series; Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group: New York, NY, USA, 2018; pp. 153–165. ISBN 978-1-138-90319-7. [Google Scholar]
  79. Wang, M.-T.; Chow, A.; Hofkens, T.; Salmela-Aro, K. The Trajectories of Student Emotional Engagement and School Burnout with Academic and Psychological Development: Findings from Finnish Adolescents. Learn. Instr. 2015, 36, 57–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Li, Y.; Lerner, R.M. Trajectories of School Engagement during Adolescence: Implications for Grades, Depression, Delinquency, and Substance Use. Dev. Psychol. 2011, 47, 233–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Skinner, E.A.; Kindermann, T.A.; Furrer, C.J. A Motivational Perspective on Engagement and Disaffection: Conceptualization and Assessment of Children’s Behavioral and Emotional Participation in Academic Activities in the Classroom. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 2009, 69, 493–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Jeong, J.S.; González-Gómez, D.; Cañada-Cañada, F.; Gallego-Picó, A.; Bravo, J.C. Effects of Active Learning Methodologies on the Students’ Emotions, Self-Efficacy Beliefs and Learning Outcomes in a Science Distance Learning Course. J. Technol. Sci. Educ. 2019, 9, 217–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. del Arco, I.; Silva, P.; Flores, O. University Teaching in Times of Confinement: The Light and Shadows of Compulsory Online Learning. Sustainability 2021, 13, 375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Hypothetical model of causal relationships.
Figure 1. Hypothetical model of causal relationships.
Sustainability 14 10236 g001
Figure 2. Resulting causal model *** p < 0.001.
Figure 2. Resulting causal model *** p < 0.001.
Sustainability 14 10236 g002
Table 1. Cronbach’s alpha, composite reliability, and average variance extracted of the proposed model.
Table 1. Cronbach’s alpha, composite reliability, and average variance extracted of the proposed model.
ConstructACRAVEObserved VariableFactor
Loadings
Self-Efficacy (SE)0.9080.9260.611SE10.742
SE20.809
SE30.814
SE40.696
SE50.820
SE60.688
SE70.849
SE80.819
Positive Emotions (PE)0.7790.8940.532PE10.517
PE20.690
PE30.787
PE40.841
PE50.769
Negative Emotions (NE)0.8650.8940.517NE10.793
NE20.814
NE30.634
NE40.621
NE50.823
NE60.754
NE70.619
NE80.658
Learning Engagement (LE)0.7610.8420.521LE10.749
LE20.511
LE30.814
LE40.696
LE50.797
Table 2. Discriminant validity.
Table 2. Discriminant validity.
Self-EfficacyPositive
Emotions
Negative
Emotions
Learning
Engagement
Self-Efficacy0.782−0.4410.5060.456
Positive Emotions 0.729−0.483−0.609
Negative Emotions 0.7190.697
Learning Engagement 0.722
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Acosta-Gonzaga, E.; Ruiz-Ledesma, E.F. Students’ Emotions and Engagement in the Emerging Hybrid Learning Environment during the COVID-19 Pandemic. Sustainability 2022, 14, 10236. https://doi.org/10.3390/su141610236

AMA Style

Acosta-Gonzaga E, Ruiz-Ledesma EF. Students’ Emotions and Engagement in the Emerging Hybrid Learning Environment during the COVID-19 Pandemic. Sustainability. 2022; 14(16):10236. https://doi.org/10.3390/su141610236

Chicago/Turabian Style

Acosta-Gonzaga, Elizabeth, and Elena Fabiola Ruiz-Ledesma. 2022. "Students’ Emotions and Engagement in the Emerging Hybrid Learning Environment during the COVID-19 Pandemic" Sustainability 14, no. 16: 10236. https://doi.org/10.3390/su141610236

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop