Next Article in Journal
Research on the Pounding Response and Pounding Effect of a Continuous Rigid-Frame Bridge with Fabricated Super-High Piers Connected by Grouting Sleeves
Next Article in Special Issue
Sustainability Trends in Humanitarian Architecture Research: A Bibliometric Analysis
Previous Article in Journal
Analysis of the Impact of Remanufacturing Process Innovation on Closed-Loop Supply Chain from the Perspective of Government Subsidy
Previous Article in Special Issue
Narrative-Based Disaster Learning Programmes Simultaneously Improve People’s Disaster Awareness Scores, Willingness to Pay and Settlement Preferences
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Place Identity and Traumatic Experiences in the Context of Wildfires

1
Department of Psychology, Fernando Pessoa Canarias University, 35450 Santa María de Guía, Spain
2
PhD Student of Health Sciences Programme, University of Castilla-La Mancha, 02071 Albacete, Spain
3
Faculty of Labour Relations and Human Resources, University of Castilla-La Mancha, 02071 Albacete, Spain
4
PU in Social and Environmental Psychology, CHROME-Unîmes Université, 30021 Nîmes, France
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sustainability 2022, 14(18), 11332; https://doi.org/10.3390/su141811332
Submission received: 7 June 2022 / Revised: 21 August 2022 / Accepted: 7 September 2022 / Published: 9 September 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Post-disaster Recovery from a Sustainability Perspective)

Abstract

:
Climate change represents a threat to life; as such, it is associated with psychological disorders. The subjective perceptions of life impacts from different traumatic experiences develop understanding and the enable predictions of future consequences. This psychological impact also tends to increase the risk perception of climate change and the intention to prevent it. Greater emphasis on place identity can promote resilience and prevent psychological distress. The aim of this descriptive cross-sectional study is to describe the ontological life impact of fires, based on socio-demographic variables, risk perception, self-perceived resistance (SPR), and place identity. The sample consisted of 210 residents of areas affected by forest fires in Gran Canaria (Spain), who answered a questionnaire consisting of multiple scales and an assessment of the traumatic experience through the VIVO (Spanish initials of Ontological Vital Impact Assessment) questionnaire. The clustering of areas according to SPR and neighbourhood was considered a new variable, referred to as territorial resistance. This variable was useful in describing the different profiles of ontological life impacts and risk perceptions. The ontological life impact of the extreme experience differed between unaffected and affected people. Feeling that they had been judged for the occurrence was associated with lower psychological adjustment and a greater perception of control over the risk. Control also increased with place identity and the number of experiences. Emphasising risk, recognising the local knowledge of residents, and including them in decision-making and future action plans contributes to a sense of community, and thus, can improve coping.

1. Introduction

Climate change has significantly increased the frequency and severity of natural disasters [1], including the number and intensity of wildfires. According to the Ministry for Ecological Transition and the Demographic Challenge (MITECO), in 2021, Spain recorded 2914 forest fires (>1 ha). In particular, the fire that occurred in 2019 on the island of Gran Canaria was the worst forest fire in Spain since 2012. More than 10,000 people were evacuated and a total of 10 municipalities were affected; over 40 days, more than 10,000 ha burned. Traces of the fire are still visible in the midlands. These are areas that live in constant risk, with small fires occurring every year, as well a high frequency of alerts for extreme weather conditions throughout the year.
The impact of natural disasters is significant, considering the multiple dangers they pose to the population, including post-traumatic stress disorder and depression [2,3,4,5]. In such extreme situations, psychological coping mechanisms are often not available, and contingency plans do not always include a specific mental health approach. An increased risk of suicide has been identified, which may be related to a loss of identity due to changes in living environments and forms of life [6]. Exposed people feel that not only is their physical integrity at risk, but they also fear losing their homes and environments [7]. In fact, feeling a loss of place is one of the most important impacts of these events, impacting psychological well-being and jeopardizing the ability to cope with environmental changes [8,9]. The prevalence and persistence of these disorders varies from victim to victim: in some cases, they decrease with the return to normality; however, in other cases, they continue to be present even years after the event, especially in those victims who are most vulnerable financially or lack social support [10]. The adoption of a clinical symptomatology perspective in the approach to trauma, leaving out the impacts on identity and worldview in the affected population, may explain why studies on the long-term consequences of disasters have been inconclusive to date.
Clinical psychology traditionally defines trauma as a psychological wound that exceeds the usual coping mechanisms [11], which can be generated by different situations, such as natural disasters, wars, sexual abuse, and interpersonal violence. These events or situations are experiences in which a person’s physical or psychological integrity may be threatened, changing the living conditions of the person or the people in their environment, resulting in the person questioning their reality and that of their environment [12,13]. This imbalance does not always have to subsequently lead to negative or destructive consequences for well-being. There are other perspectives in which trauma is conceptualised from a positive point of view, where the victim is able to respond resiliently to the traumatic experience and adapt to the changes that the situation has brought about [14]. Pérez-Sales et al. [12] called these circumstances that disrupt the balance of people in their environment “extreme experiences”, and validated the VIVO questionnaire (Spanish initials of Ontological Vital Impact Assessment) as a tool to analyse the impact of extreme experiences on worldviews, identity, and basic beliefs from a multi-dimensional perspective. This questionnaire is composed of 10 conceptual blocks (Worldviews; Attitude Towards the World; View of Human Beings; Coping; Impact of Past Situations; Emotions; Telling the Experience; Consequences; Social Support; Identity), with 35 subscales in total.
Although the nature of a disaster and the associated environmental factors, such as the severity of the hazard, the number of deaths, and material vulnerability, can help to understand its differential impacts on the population, other subjective variables, such as the subjective explanation of the experience, attributions of responsibility, or the perception of loss of control, are essential for understanding the psychological consequences [15]. The subjective perception of the life impact of an event facilitates understanding of the differential response to trauma, as well as predicting subsequent consequences [13], and will be determined by multiple psychosocial elements, such as the loss of control experienced by displaced persons or their emotions. Self-perceived resistance (SPR) [13] measures the intersection of subjective experience and the threatening characteristics of extreme situations. It involves the interaction of a perception of physical life threat (PT) and perception of life Impact (PI) variables, with four categories: non-affected, vulnerable, survivor, and resistant. If a person reports low PT and low PI, they are considered to be non-affected; however, if this person considers that the experience had a significant impact on their life (high PI) but a low PT, they are considered as vulnerable. If the affected person perceives a high PT and low PI, this person is perceived as resistant. Finally, if both PT and PI are reported as high, the person is considered as a survivor. A study of more than 700 people who had experienced natural disasters [5] revealed that ‘Survivor’ and ‘Vulnerable’ participants had higher post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) scores than ‘Non-affected’ participants, as measured by the PTSD Checklist—Civilian Version (PCL-C). In terms of the dimensions, the ‘Survivor’ group scored more highly than the ‘Non-affected’ and “Resistant” groups in re-experiencing and activation, as well as the “Vulnerable” group compared with the “Non-affected” group.
Natural disasters represent a threat to quality of life, health and, potentially, life itself. People bring meaning to the places they inhabit, and form attachments and feelings of belonging to them. It is they who, through their daily experiences, have made the place their home. When people are affected by traumatic events, such as forest fires, eruptions, floods, or earthquakes, there is a loss of place of residence, as well as a social fracturing that alters collective narratives and interrupts their life trajectory [16,17]. Nonetheless, victims of natural disasters tend to experience greater post-traumatic growth, especially those who have established a strong connection to the place [18] and those communities that are empowered to participate in the reconstruction of an affected area. In this sense, resilience to natural disasters could be seen as a process focused in place [19].
Being a victim of a catastrophe associated with climate change can mean increased susceptibility and perceptions of risk [20]. Adequately understanding how people perceive risk is necessary to guide disaster risk management, improve emergency responses, and encourage people to prevent disasters [21,22]. Risk perception can be defined as a subjective evaluation of a risk [23]; therefore, it is different for each person and is influenced by factors such as the qualitative characteristics of the risk (e.g., voluntariness or control), socio-demographic characteristics, or knowledge of associated risks (e.g., climate change). Victims of a natural disaster may have different narratives about the same event [24]. The past experience of hazard-affected residents increases their risk perception and preparedness intentions [25]. In the study by Bernardo et al. [26], participants affected by the wildfires were more afraid and considered themselves more knowledgeable about the risks than those who were not affected. Knowledge and awareness influence preparedness for extreme events [27]. Due to the different perceptions that residents and government agencies responsible for risk communication might have, it is recommended to engage the community from the beginning [28]. In addition, creating a sense of social cohesion and incorporating knowledge about the landscape in fire-prone areas can facilitate planning and management [29].
The connection to place stimulates adaptation strategies that consider local conditions and increases resilience [30]. This bond may have a mediating effect between risk perception and coping [31]. Individuals with a high degree of place attachment underestimate risk, whereas unattached individuals perceive future hazards as more threatening. However, contradictory results are reported in the literature on the relationship between place attachment and risk perception [32]; for example, it has also been found that greater linkages to place increase the perception of risk [26,33].
The psychological impact of an hazard event, such as a fire, also tends to increase the perceived risk of climate change [32] and influences the intention to prevent it, as well as leading to a re-evaluation of identity and worldview [20]. Even so, few studies have explored the relationship between experiencing a specific natural disaster, such as a wildfire, the perception of climate change, and its impacts on identity; however, as far back as 1991 (cited in [34]), Feitelson published an article in Global Environmental Change proposing the importance of place attachment and identity in understanding human responses to climate change. Greater emphasis on place can promote resilience and prevent psychological distress in climate-related events [18].
The aim of this study was to describe the ontological life impact of fires, based on socio-demographic variables, risk perception, perceived resistance, and place identity. Our specific objectives were:
  • To analyse place identity in terms of socio-demographic variables, including age, gender, and place of residence;
  • To link local and island place identity with the risk perception of climate change and wildfire;
  • To describe the vital ontological impact of the extreme experience (VIVO) as a function of self-perceived resistance (SPR) and place of residence;
  • To analyse the relationship between place identity, VIVO, and SPR;
  • To describe the risk perception of climate and wildfire as a function of SPR and analyse its relationship with the differential vital ontological impact.
The proposed hypotheses that correspond to each of the objectives above are as follows:
Hypothesis 1 (H1).
There is higher place identity with increasing age, even more so in those residing in more affected municipalities [18] and who experienced a high number of evacuations [35].
Hypothesis 2 (H2).
Greater place identity is associated with a greater risk perception of climate change [34] and wildfire risk [26].
Hypothesis 3 (H3).
The place of residence combined with SPR determines a differential ontological life impact [5].
Hypothesis 4 (H4).
The place of residence is also expected to link greater long-term psychological adjustment to greater local identity [18].
Hypothesis 5 (H5).
Experience (SPR and VIVO) will increase the perception of fire risk [36] and climate change risk [37].

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

A total of 210 participants took part in this study. The sample was obtained from areas affected by the last major wildfire in Gran Canaria, who were either evacuated or confined and lived there at the moment of its occurrence. This island forms part of the Canary Islands archipelago (Spain), located in the Atlantic Ocean off the Moroccan coast (Figure 1).
A convenience sampling approach was used, due to the specificity of the sample, keeping the percentage of participants per neighbourhood similar to the number of affected inhabitants in each area (Figure 2). The average length of residence in the neighbourhood was 38.7 years, and that on the island was 45.2 years. A proportion of 60% of the participants were men; 40% were women. No participant was identified with another gender. The average age was 48.4 years. According to the declared socio-economic level, most participants were classified as low (34.8%) or medium (65.2%), and most of them had children (65.7%) and animals (67.1%).

2.2. Instruments

The questionnaire was divided into 6 sections, consisting of a total of 156 items. A total of 11 questions were used to explore the main socio-demographic variables, such as gender or years of residence in the neighbourhood. We decided to use ‘neighbourhood’, instead of ‘town of residence’, because the distribution of residents in Gran Canaria means that the same town could have neighbourhoods several kilometres away from each other which would be unequally affected by a wildfire. In addition, it is common for the inhabitants of the region to choose this name to refer to their place of residence.
Place Identity Scale. The three identity scale items developed by Hernández et al. [38] were applied in two contexts: neighbourhood and island. The island adaptation, translated into Portuguese, was used by Bernardo et al. [26] with good psychometric properties (in this study, neighbourhood identity α = 0.95; island identity α = 0.94).
Risk perception. Perceptions of climate change risk and fire risk were assessed independently. Participants were asked to rate each of the risks on a 7-point semantic differential scale, according to 9 risk perception attributes (voluntariness of risk, immediacy of effect, knowledge for those exposed and for science, control over risk, newness, number of people affected, dread and severity of the consequences). This scale was based on the psychometric paradigm [23] and the risk characteristics were Puy’s Spanish adaptation [39] of the scale developed Fischhoff et al. [40].
Information about the experience of the fire. Participants selected the answers that best described their situation from a set of 10 possible responses. For example: ‘I had to be evacuated because of the fire’, ‘My house was partially or totally damaged’, and/or ‘I was indirectly affected by the environmental damage’. The number of evacuations suffered over time was also queried.
Self-Perceived Resistance [13]. This is an indicator based on the interaction of PT and PI. As described above, it classified the population into four groups: non-affected, vulnerable, survivor, and resistant.
VIVO questionnaire [12]. Divided into two parts, 72 items were directed to the general population and 43 items were directed to the victims, in which they responded to questions regarding the fire experienced. This questionnaire was detailed in the Introduction.

2.3. Procedure

Both an online and a paper questionnaire were used to facilitate the participation of people with different socio-demographic characteristics. Snowball sampling was performed, with two main phases: The first was the identification of potential participants on the internet following the method and considerations described by Eiroa-Orosa [41], through an advanced search of Twitter posts using a combination of the hashtags generated during the fire {(#ifvalleseco OR #ifartenara OR #ifcumbregc OR #ifgrancanaria OR #iftejeda) lang:es until: 25 September 2019 since: 10 August 2019}. This social platform was chosen because it is the most frequently used to follow fires in real time. Many tweets are posted with images, affected areas, reactions, etc., enabling us to identify those who were affected and contact them through private messages. Twitter is useful for assessing the damage caused by a natural disaster, and can be used to predict the economic impacts [42]. Potential participants were also reached through forums and publications in social groups in the affected areas, as well as by emails sent to the neighbourhood associations. Subsequently, face-to-face sampling was conducted in all affected locations, in order to identify affected people who do not have an internet presence, requesting the participation of neighbours in randomly selected streets and houses. Participants were informed of our objectives and the possible risks of the research, and were asked at the end for informed consent regarding whether they agreed to participate freely. In the online version, which did not include identification data in the consent form, only ticking an acceptance box allowed the content of the questionnaire to be viewed. The questionnaire required approximately 20 min to complete, although it took up to 50 min when conducted with elderly people who were assisted when completing it. Responses were collected in the period between 24 July and 30 October 2021.
The database was prepared in an Excel file. Descriptive statistics were calculated and SPSS Statistics software (version 25.00. IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA) was used for hypothesis testing. The tests used were correspondence analysis, Pearson correlations, ANOVA for mean differences in place identity, and MANOVA in the case of VIVO and risk perception variables. Finally, post hoc analysis using the Scheffé test for significant differences was conducted.

3. Results

3.1. Place Identity and Socio-Demographic Variables

First, we analysed the differences in local place identity and island place identity, according to the principal socio-demographic characteristics. Table 1 presents the descriptive statistics. Using Student’s t-tests, no significant differences were found between men and women, in terms of either local identity (t = −0.03; ns) or island identity (t = −0.04; ns). Place of residence showed differences in local place identity when assessed by ANOVA (F = 9.21; p < 0.00), with an adequate observed power (1.00) but a small effect size (0.27). Island identity exhibited no significant differences (F = 1.58; p > 0.05).
Pearson correlations were used to analyse the relationships between the island and local identities, age, years of residence, and the number of evacuations. The local and island identities were significantly correlated (r = 0.188; p < 0.01); both were also significantly correlated with years of residence on the island (r = 0.355; p < 0.00 and r = 0.200; p < 0.005, respectively). Length of residence in the neighbourhood only correlated with local identity (r = 0.427; p < 0.00). Age and the number of evacuations also correlated only with local identity (r = 0.294; p < 0.00 and r = 0.228; p < 0.005, respectively).

3.2. Local and Island Place Identity and Risk Perception of Climate Change and Fire Risk

Five of the nine climate change risk perception attributes were significantly correlated with local identity; in particular, the knowledge of people exposed was positively correlated, whereas voluntariness, control, catastrophism, and severity were inversely correlated. Island identity was only inversely correlated with fear. As for fire risk perceptions, significant correlations were only found between local identity and control. The correlation coefficients are shown in Table 2.

3.3. Description of the Vital Ontological Impact (VIVO) as a Function of Self-Perceived Resistance (SPR) and Place of Residence

Based on the responses given regarding the level of perceived threat and life impact, participants were classified according to the four SPR categories: non-affected (16.7%); vulnerable (52.4%); resistant (7.6%); and survivor (23.3%). In terms of their perceptions of physical life threat (PT) and life impact (PI), 7.1% of the participants indicated that the experience had not affected them vitally; 61.9% indicated that it had affected them at the time, but not in the present; 25.2% responded that there were still aspects of the experience that affected them very much; and finally, 5.7% reported that the event had changed their view of life. In terms of threat, 1.9% considered it mild, 24.3% deemed it moderate, 89.9% perceived it as severe, and 11% believed it to be extreme. Simple correspondence analysis was carried out by combining the municipality and perceived resistance variables (Figure 3).
Three groups were differentiated according to self-perceived resistance. Inhabitants of Gáldar, Tejeda, and Artenara were classified as survivor–resistant (high PT and moderate–high PI). Inhabitants of Agaete, San Bartolomé, San Mateo, La Aldea, and Moya were classified as vulnerable (low PT, high PI). Finally, inhabitants of Valleseco were considered non-affected (low PT and PI). The clustering of areas according to SPR created by the correspondence analysis was considered as a new variable, referred to as territorial resistance in the following analyses.
MANOVA of the 10 main VIVO blocks in the three groups was carried out, and the results indicated that the interaction was significant (p < 0.000), with adequate power (0.998) and a high effect size (η2 = 0.12). Table 3 presents the results, showing which blocks showed significant differences.
Statistically significant differences were observed in the Consequences block. In the post hoc tests, it was observed that the non-affected group scores were significantly lower than those of the vulnerable group. There were also differences in the Social Support block. Post hoc tests indicated that the vulnerable group scores were significantly higher than those of the non-affected and survivor–resistant groups. Finally, differences were also observed in the Identity block, with the non-affected group scoring lower than the vulnerable and survivor–resistant groups. Figure 4 shows the VIVO profile, including the scores in the subscales of the blocks where significant differences were observed, for three subjects with extreme scores in each of the groups. Therefore, MANOVA was applied to determine which subscales of the block best explained the differences (Table 4).
The Wilks’ lambda values of the MANOVA test results for the subscales in all the blocks were significant. In the Consequences block, significant differences with adequate power were found in the subscales of sensitivity/insensitivity with others and acceptance of chance, with the post hoc tests showing that the non-affected group scores were lower than those of the vulnerable group. In the Social Support block, there was a significant difference in the blaming the victim subscale. Post hoc tests indicated differences between the non-affected and vulnerable groups and vulnerable and survivor–resistant groups, with vulnerable scoring significantly more highly. Finally, the significant differences that reached adequate power in the Identity block were in the future and hope and victimhood subscales. In the former, according to the post hoc tests, non-affected group scores were significantly lower than those of the vulnerable and survivor–resistant groups. Finally, differences in victimhood were found in the non-affected group, which scored lower than those in the vulnerable group.

3.4. Place Identity, Vital Ontological Impact, and Territorial Resistance

The ANOVA test of place identity by territorial resistance indicated significant differences (Table 5). The relationship between place identity and ontological life impact was explored by Pearson correlation coefficient analysis (Table 6).
Statistically significant differences were observed in local identity, with a medium effect and adequate power. The post hoc Scheffé results indicated that the non-affected group scored lower in local identity than the vulnerable and survivor–resistant groups. Correlations with the main VIVO blocks (Table 6) were only significant between local identity and the Identity block (r = 0.137; p < 0.05).

3.5. Experience and Risk Perception

To explore the relationship between self-perceived resistance and neighbourhood, as a result of differences in wildfire exposure, a novel territorial resistance variable was used. A mean difference analysis was performed on scores of the risk attributes of climate change risk as well as fire risk with respect to territorial resistance. The Wilks’ lambda statistics of both MANOVA tests were significant, with adequate power and high effect sizes (η2 > 0.12); see Table 7 and Table 8.
Post hoc tests indicated that the non-affected group scored significantly more highly in catastrophism and significantly lower in severity than vulnerable and survivor–resistant groups.
The attributes that showed differences between the three groups, with moderate effect sizes and adequate power, were voluntariness, knowledge of exposed persons, and control. Post hoc tests indicated significant differences between the non-affected and vulnerable groups, with the former scoring more highly. The vulnerable group scored significantly lower than the survivor–resistant group. Finally, the non-affected group scored significantly more highly than the survivor–resistant group.
Finally, the differences in risk perception with respect to the ontological life impact subscales were analysed in terms of the correlations. Table 9 shows that the VIVO subscales were correlated with each other, and that the sensitivity/insensitivity with others was inversely correlated with the catastrophic climate change risk perception attribute. Acceptance of chance was also inversely correlated with that attribute, as well as with the fire risk perception attribute of voluntariness. Blaming the victim was inversely correlated with willingness, knowledge, and control of fire risk. The future subscale was inversely correlated with both attributes of climate change risk perception; finally, victimhood was inversely correlated with fire risk voluntariness and climate change catastrophism.
In terms of correlation with the number of evacuations experienced and age, age was inversely correlated with the control (r = −0.149; p < 0.05) and voluntariness (r = −0.166; p < 0.05) of fire risk perception, as well as the severity (r = −0.347; p < 0.01) and catastrophism (r = −0.308; p < 0.01) of climate change. The number of evacuations experienced was only correlated with the control (r = −0.142; p < 0.05) of fire risk perception and the severity (r = −0.176; p < 0.05) of climate change.

4. Discussion

According to the proposed hypotheses, local place identity would increase with the age and years of residence in the neighbourhood of the participants. It would also differ depending on the municipality of residence, increasing in areas that experienced more evacuations and decreasing in areas less affected by fires. Therefore, risk situations can increase the connection with a place [18].
No relationship was found between island identity and fire risk perception, and only local place identity was correlated with the control attribute. Residents with a stronger local place identity perceived the fire risk to be more controllable. Attachment to place provides a sense of security to residents [31,43], who are unlikely to leave the place despite the high risk, instead engaging in preventive activities [26,44]. Previous studies which had found that an increase in the perception of risk was not a determinant of mitigation [45] did not explore the different attributes that comprise risk. The results of this study demonstrated that identity can provide a sense of control, and it would be interesting to test whether it is a determinant of mitigation.
Five of the nine climate change risk perception attributes were significantly correlated with local identity. People who had higher place identity perceived themselves as more knowledgeable and evaluated risk as being more controllable, voluntary, and individual, as well as less severe. Rooted local identities influence the perceptions of climate change risk, and are relevant for awareness and adaptation [34]; however, the literature has still insufficiently explored this question.
Using perceived resistance to relate the subjective perception of an experience to its psychological impact has been effective in previous studies [5,13]. In this study, the classification of the sample into three groups according to self-perceived resistance and the municipality of residence was operationally useful in describing the different profiles of risk perception and ontological life impact. Furthermore, these groups differed in their local identity, but not in their island identity. The survivor–resistant group, comprising those with the highest physical threat, were the most frequently identified and lived in the most-affected locations, in terms of losses, according to official data. The values of subsidies to compensate damage to homes, farms, and the closure of businesses were highest in Tejeda, Gáldar, and Artenara (Quesada. 13 August 2020). The lowest amount spent was in Valleseco, coinciding with the group classified as non-affected and the least locally identified. The vulnerable group (low physical threat and high vital impact) experienced an intermediate level of losses.
Ecological and landscape losses must be included with the direct and indirect economic losses and the emotional impact of wildfires on the people affected, which sometimes remains for decades. Rodríguez-Carreras et al. [46] reported how most of those affected by fires almost two decades ago still expressed feelings of consternation, frustration, and powerlessness. In this study, the ontological life impacts of the extreme experience differed, according to the perceived resistance. In particular, significant differences were found between the non-Affected and vulnerable groups. Therefore, life influences appear to be more influential than physical threats, in agreement with the study by Pérez-Sales et al. [13]. According to the results in the acceptance of chance subscale, those in the vulnerable group asked themselves more about why it had happened to them, corresponding to a higher impact in the subscale. They tended to blame the victim, felt that they had been judged for the event, and considered that the experience had distanced them from others, according to the subscale. Finally, in the Identity block, differences were found in all subscales, but only those in future and victimhood were significant, with an adequate effect size. Affected persons view the future with more pessimism and hopelessness, and recognise themselves as victims as an important part of their identity. The non-affected hold more hope for the future, and effectively do not refer to themselves as victims.
Non-affected participants who experienced the wildfire also significantly differed in their perceptions of climate change, in the attributes of catastrophism and severity. Affected individuals considered climate change to be less severe and more individual; the same relationship as that found with greater local identity and with the future subscale. Perceptions of the catastrophic power of a risk and the severity of a hazard are associated with protective behaviours [47]. Emphasising local manifestations of the climate crisis and its connections to individual livelihoods can help to engage individuals in mitigation actions and moderate the risk of extreme events [48]. For this, it is also necessary that the population considers that they can do something to improve the future, which they currently conceive with hopelessness and pessimism.
In relation to the perceptions of fire risk, those affected consider that they have more knowledge and that the risk is more controllable than those not affected. This higher perception of control was also associated with the feeling of being judged and a higher place identity, as well as age and the number of evacuations experienced. Previous studies have found that the experience of residents is a determinant of perceived risk, providing a better understanding and knowledge of risk [49]. It has also been associated with the intention to prepare [25]. However, feeling more control can cause people to engage in risky situations, such as burning or operating forbidden machinery on high-temperature alert days. Other studies have found that those affected by wildfires who are aware of the likelihood of risk tend not to undertake the recommended protective actions, because they do not consider them effective or relevant to them [48]. It has also been found that people exposed to a constant risk may perceive it as more controllable, due to experience [50], in line with the psychometric paradigm that associates high risk familiarity and control with a better perception of risk [23,40]. Years of exposure to wildfires, with proper evacuation protocols that prevent fatalities, lead to the consideration of fire as an old and known hazard, thus contributing to a sense of control [51]. On the other hand, control has also been related to the voluntariness of risk [52] (cited in [50]). The results of this study also suggested a correlation between voluntariness and control.
Disaster preparedness education for adults is based on their initiative and motivation. Emphasising risk, recognising the local knowledge of residents, and including them in decision-making and future action plans contributes to a sense of community [46]. This can help them feel listened to, rather than judged, and improves the coping and psychological impacts described in terms of their worldview and others. In addition, providing a space for them to share testimonies about their experience can also improve their psychological adjustment. Previous studies on fires have found that vicarious experience through the testimonies of local victims can also increase awareness and prevention actions in the general population [51].
This study provides arguments that justify the need to include people exposed to risk in management and prevention plans. However, it is important to consider certain limitations of the study. First, non-probabilistic sampling was used, which may limit the representativeness of the sample. Moreover, not having previous measures of place identity or assessing it two years after the fire did not allow us to determine whether it had been affected by the event. However, previous studies have demonstrated that identity is a construct of connection to place which is more stable than attachment; therefore, it is more difficult to modify in a short period of time [53]. During the last few years, the global population has been affected by the COVID-19 pandemic, which may have affected their responses to the questionnaire regarding traumatic experiences, especially in terms of perceiving the future with more pessimism. Finally, some studies have indicated that an experience may only increase the risk perception for a short period of time [51]; thus, it may be that this perception had diminished during the two years since the wildfire.

5. Conclusions

Residents of rural areas must learn to coexist with fire, knowing the risks to which they are exposed and participating in fire prevention [54]. Collective actions in environmental management are fundamental for adaptation and environmental conservation. To reduce vulnerability to fire risk, management policies must include analysis not only of the landscape, but also the inhabitants, incorporating their perceptions, psychological barriers, and the means to strengthen their interactions [1]. Planning strategies collaboratively considering the livelihoods, community conflicts, and land-uses of residents reduces the probability of risk propagation and threats to people [46].
Acting on climate change not only requires addressing environmental or climate issues, but also influences all aspects related to the exposed people. Experiencing a weather-related natural disaster can induce people to re-evaluate their vulnerability, as well as their worldview and their view of others. It also has an influence on their perceptions of climate change, and may increase the intention of adopting adaptive behaviours. However, studies have typically only focused on linkages to residential places, and it is probable that place identity for recreational spaces (e.g., natural spaces) is also relevant in understanding disaster prevention behaviours [55]. Further studies should consider this aspect, as well as exploring how the perceptions of life impact experiences influence perceived control and how this relates to coping strategies, including the role that place identity has in the perception of control.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, P.d.J., P.O.-J. and O.N.; Formal analysis, P.d.J.; Investigation, P.d.J.; Methodology, P.d.J. and P.O.-J.; Supervision, P.O.-J.; Writing—original draft, P.d.J.; Writing—review & editing, P.O.-J. and O.N. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The protocol complies with the ethical considerations of the Declaration of Helsinki (1964), Washington (2002), and the American Psychological Association (2002), and was approved by the Bioethics Committee of the Fernando Pessoa Canarias University (Act Nª02/2021).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all the subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The database of the research is available on request.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the psychology graduates of the class of 2021 (UFPC) who assisted in identifying participants; we also thank all the affected persons who responded to the questionnaire.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Sapiains, R.; Ugarte, A.M.; Aldunce, P.; Marchant, G.; Romero, J.A.; González, M.E.; Inostroza-Lazo, V. Local Perceptions of Fires Risk and Policy Implications in the Hills of Valparaíso, Chile. Sustainability 2020, 12, 4298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Boscarino, J.A.; Hoffman, S.N.; Adams, R.E.; Figley, C.R.; Solhkhah, R. Mental Health Outcomes among Vulnerable Residents after Hurricane Sandy: Implications for Disaster Research and Planning. Am. J. Disaster Med. 2014, 9, 107–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Dodd, W.; Scott, P.; Howard, C.; Scott, C.; Rose, C.; Cunsolo, A.; Orbinski, J. Lived Experience of a Record Wildfire Season in the Northwest Territories, Canada. Can. J. Public Health 2018, 109, 327–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Jermacane, D.; Waite, T.D.; Beck, C.R.; Bone, A.; Amlôt, R.; Reacher, M.; Kovats, S.; Armstrong, B.; Leonardi, G.; James Rubin, G.; et al. The English National Cohort Study of Flooding and Health: The Change in the Prevalence of Psychological Morbidity at Year Two. BMC Public Health 2018, 18, 330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Olivos-Jara, P.; Eiroa-Orosa, F.J.; Pérez-Sales, P.; Vergara Sánchez, M.; Barbero-Val, E. Impacto sobre la visión del mundo, los otros y de sí mismo en supervivientes de catástrofes de la naturaleza. In Cómo Afrontar una Catástrofe: Percepción de Riesgo y Factores Psicosociales de la Adaptación; Olivos-Jara, P., Navarro, O., Loureiro, A., Eds.; Ediciones de la Universidad de Castilla-La Mancha: Cuenca, Spain, 2020; pp. 117–136. ISBN 978-84-9044-364-4. [Google Scholar]
  6. Clayton, S. Climate Change and Mental Health. Curr. Environ. Health Rep. 2021, 8, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Haney, T.J. Move out or Dig in? Risk Awareness and Mobility Plans in Disaster-Affected Communities. J. Contingencies Crisis Manag. 2019, 27, 224–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Marshall, N.; Adger, W.N.; Benham, C.; Brown, K.; Curnock, M.I.; Gurney, G.G.; Marshall, P.; Pert, P.L.; Thiault, L. Reef Grief: Investigating the Relationship between Place Meanings and Place Change on the Great Barrier Reef, Australia. Sustain. Sci. 2019, 14, 579–587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Phillips, C.; Murphy, C. Solastalgia, Place Attachment and Disruption: Insights from a Coastal Community on the Front Line. Reg. Environ. Chang. 2021, 21, 46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Benevolenza, M.A.; DeRigne, L. The Impact of Climate Change and Natural Disasters on Vulnerable Populations: A Systematic Review of Literature. J. Hum. Behav. Soc. Environ. 2019, 29, 266–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Gutierrez, N.A.; Campos Medina, L.; Ropert Lackington, T.; Sharim Kovalskys, D. The Transition from Protagonism to Refusal: The Double Trauma in a Post-Fire Resettlement in Valparaiso, Chile. Scr. Nova-Rev. Electron. Geogr. Y Cienc. Soc. 2020, 24, 636. [Google Scholar]
  12. Pérez-Sales, P.; Eiroa-Orosa, F.J.; Olivos, P.; Barbero-Val, E.; Fernández-Liria, A.; Vergara, M. Vivo Questionnaire: A Measure of Human Worldviews and Identity in Trauma, Crisis, and Loss—Validation and Preliminary Findings. J. Loss Trauma 2012, 17, 236–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Pérez-Sales, P.; Vergara-Campos, M.; Eiroa-Orosa, F.J.; Olivos-Jara, P.; Fernández-Liria, A.; Barbero-Val, E.; Galán-Santamarina, A. Perceived Resistance to Experiences of Trauma and Crisis: A Study Comparing Multiple Life Events. Traumatology 2022, 28, 109–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Poseck, B.V.; Carbelo, B.C. Vecina La experiencia traumática desde la psicología positiva: Resiliencia y crecimiento postraumático. Pap. Del Psicólogo 2006, 27, 40–49. [Google Scholar]
  15. Afifi, W.A.; Felix, E.D.; Afifi, T.D. The Impact of Uncertainty and Communal Coping on Mental Health Following Natural Disasters. Anxiety Stress Coping 2012, 25, 329–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Clemente, M.; Salvati, L. “Interrupted” Landscapes: Post-Earthquake Reconstruction in between Urban Renewal and Social Identity of Local Communities. Sustainability 2017, 9, 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Olivos Jara, P.; Navarro Carrascal, Ó.; Loureiro, A. Cómo Afrontar una Catástrofe: Percepción de Riesgo y Factores Psicosociales de la Adaptación; Ediciones de la Universidad de Castilla-La Mancha: Cuenca, Spain, 2020; ISBN 978-84-9044-364-4. [Google Scholar]
  18. Jansen, S.J.T. Place Attachment, Distress, Risk Perception and Coping in a Case of Earthquakes in the Netherlands. J. Hous. Built Environ. 2020, 35, 407–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Alexander, D.E. Resilience and Disaster Risk Reduction: An Etymological Journey. Nat. Hazards Earth Syst. Sci. 2013, 13, 2707–2716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Sattler, D.N.; Lousi, U.; Graham, J.M.; Latu, V.; Johnson, J.; Helu, S.L. Climate Change in Tonga: Risk Perception and Behavioral Adaptation. In Managing Climate Change Adaptation in the Pacific Region; Climate Change Management; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2020; pp. 273–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Fernandez, G.; Tun, A.M.; Okazaki, K.; Zaw, S.H.; Kyaw, K. Factors Influencing Fire, Earthquake, and Cyclone Risk Perception in Yangon, Myanmar. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 2018, 28, 140–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Guardiola-Albert, C.; Díez-Herrero, A.; Cuervo-Arango, M.A.; Bodoque, J.M.; García, J.A.; Naranjo-Fernández, N.; Aroca-Jiménez, E. Analysing Flash Flood Risk Perception through a Geostatistical Approach in the Village of Navaluenga, Central Spain. J. Flood Risk Manag. 2020, 13, e12590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Slovic, P. Perception of Risk. Science 1987, 236, 280–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Antronico, L.; Coscarelli, R.; De Pascale, F.; Di Matteo, D. Climate Change and Social Perception: A Case Study in Southern Italy. Sustainability 2020, 12, 6985. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Zhang, K.; Parks-Stamm, E.J.; Ji, Y.; Wang, H. Beyond Flood Preparedness: Effects of Experience, Trust, and Perceived Risk on Preparation Intentions and Financial Risk-Taking in China. Sustainability 2021, 13, 13625. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Bernardo, F.; Santos, L.; Dias, D.; Rodrigues, M. Risk Experience, Emotions, Place Identity, and Coping Strategies in People Affected by an Unexpected Fire (Experiencia de Riesgo, Emociones, Identidad de Lugar y Estrategias de Afrontamiento En Personas Afectadas Por Un Incendio Inesperado). Psyecology 2020, 11, 130–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Salah, P.; Sasaki, J. Knowledge, Awareness, and Attitudes toward Tsunamis: A Local Survey in the Southern Coast of Iran. Sustainability 2021, 13, 449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Jensen, O.; Ong, C. Collaborative Action for Community Resilience to Climate Risks: Opportunities and Barriers. Sustainability 2020, 12, 3413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Marey-Perez, M.; Loureiro, X.; Corbelle-Rico, E.J.; Fernández-Filgueira, C. Different Strategies for Resilience to Wildfires: The Experience of Collective Land Ownership in Galicia (Northwest Spain). Sustainability 2021, 13, 4761. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Madhuri; Tewari, H.R.; Bhowmick, P.K. Ingenuity of Skating on Marshy Land by Tying a Pot to the Belly: Living with Flood Is a Way of Life. Environ. Dev. Sustain. 2015, 17, 1287–1311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Greer, A.; Binder, S.B.; Thiel, A.; Jamali, M.; Nejat, A. Place Attachment in Disaster Studies: Measurement and the Case of the 2013 Moore Tornado. Popul. Environ. 2020, 41, 306–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Guillard, M.; Navarro, O.; Fleury-Bahi, G. Flooding Experience and Assessment of Climate Change: Implication of Psychological Distance, Risk Perception and Place Attachment/Experiencia de Inundaciones y Evaluación Del Cambio Climático: Implicaciones de La Distancia Psicológica, La Percepción de Riesgo y El Apego al Lugar. Psyecology 2019, 10, 287–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Muñoz-Duque, L.A.; Arroyave, O. Percepción Del Riesgo y Apego al Lugar En Población Expuesta a Inundación: Un Estudio Comparativo. Pensam. Psicológico 2017, 15, 79–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Devine-Wright, P. Think Global, Act Local? The Relevance of Place Attachments and Place Identities in a Climate Changed World. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2013, 23, 61–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Maldonado, L.; Kronmüller, E.; Crocco, I.G. Place Attachment in Post-Disaster Areas: The Case of the Reoccupation of Chaiten City, Chile (Apego al Lugar En Áreas Post-Desastre: El Caso de La Reocupación de La Ciudad de Chaitén, Chile). Psykhe 2020, 29, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Brenkert-Smith, H.; Jahn, J.L.S.; Vance, E.A.; Ahumada, J. Resistance and Representation in a Wildland–Urban Interface Fuels Treatment Conflict: The Case of the Forsythe Ii Project in the Arapaho-Roosevelt National Forest. Fire 2020, 3, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Tam, K.; Leung, A.K.-y.; Clayton, S. Research on Climate Change in Social Psychology Publications: A Systematic Review. Asian J. Soc. Psychol. 2021, 24, 117–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Hernández, B.; Carmen Hidalgo, M.; Salazar-Laplace, M.E.; Hess, S. Place Attachment and Place Identity in Natives and Non-Natives. J. Environ. Psychol. 2007, 27, 310–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Puy, A. A Psychometric Study on the Social Perception of Risks. Int. J. Soc. Psychol. 1995, 10, 173–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Fischhoff, B.; Slovic, P.; Lichtenstein, S.; Read, S.; Combs, B. How Safe Is Safe Enough? A Psychometric Study of Attitudes towards Technological Risks and Benefits. Policy Sci. 1978, 9, 127–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Eiroa-Orosa, F.J. Cuestionarios psicológicos e investigación en Internet: Una revisión de la literatura. An. Psicol. 2008, 24, 9. [Google Scholar]
  42. Kryvasheyeu, Y.; Chen, H.; Obradovich, N.; Moro, E.; Van Hentenryck, P.; Fowler, J.; Cebrian, M. Rapid Assessment of Disaster Damage Using Social Media Activity. Sci. Adv. 2016, 2, e1500779. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Swapan, M.S.H.; Sadeque, S. Place Attachment in Natural Hazard-Prone Areas and Decision to Relocate: Research Review and Agenda for Developing Countries. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 2021, 52, 101937. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Navarro, O.; Restrepo-Ochoa, D.; Muñoz-Duque, L.A.; Zapa-Perez, K.; Ameline, A.; Mercier, D.; Fleury-Bahi, G. Determinants of Coping Strategies in Two Types of Natural Hazards: Flash Floods and Costal Flooding. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 2020, 46, 101514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Schulte, S.; Miller, K.A. Wildfire Risk and Climate Change: The Influence on Homeowner Mitigation Behavior in the Wildland–Urban Interface. Soc. Nat. Resour. 2010, 23, 417–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Rodríguez-Carreras, R.; Úbeda, X.; Francos, M.; Marco, C. After the Wildfires: The Processes of Social Learning of Forest Owners’ Associations in Central Catalonia, Spain. Sustainability 2020, 12, 6042. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Aragonés, J.-I.; Talayero, F.; Olivos, P. Percepción del riesgo de la gripe A (H1N1) desde el “paradigma psicométrico”. Rev. Psicol. Soc. 2010, 25, 271–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Titko, M.; Ristvej, J. Assessing Importance of Disaster Preparedness Factors for Sustainable Disaster Risk Management: The Case of the Slovak Republic. Sustainability 2020, 12, 9121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Oubennaceur, K.; Chokmani, K.; Lessard, F.; Gauthier, Y.; Baltazar, C.; Toussaint, J.-P. Understanding Flood Risk Perception: A Case Study from Canada. Sustainability 2022, 14, 3087. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Hahm, S.; Knuth, D.; Kehl, D.; Schmidt, S. The Impact of Different Natures of Experience on Risk Perception Regarding Fire-Related Incidents: A Comparison of Firefighters and Emergency Survivors Using Cross-National Data. Saf. Sci. 2016, 82, 274–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Mccaffrey, S. Thinking of Wildfire as a Natural Hazard. Soc. Nat. Resour. 2004, 17, 509–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Twigger-Ross, C.L.; Breakwell, G.M. Relating Risk Experience, Venturesomeness and Risk Perception. J. Risk Res. 1999, 2, 73–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Ruiz, C.; Hernández, B. Emotions and Coping Strategies during an Episode of Volcanic Activity and Their Relations to Place Attachment. J. Environ. Psychol. 2014, 38, 279–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Cerezal, J.C.S.; Borges, J.N. Retos de la Gestión Forestal y Ambiental en las Islas Canarias en el Siglo XXI: Libro Homenaje: Ingeniero de Montes Don Isidoro Sánchez García; Colegio de Ingenieros de Montes: Madrid, Spain, 2020; ISBN 978-84-09-24958-9. [Google Scholar]
  55. Wallis, A.; Fischer, R.; Abrahamse, W. Does Place Visualisation Increase Household Preparedness for Natural Hazard Events? A Longitudinal Intervention. J. Environ. Psychol. 2021, 73, 101556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Location of the Canary Islands.
Figure 1. Location of the Canary Islands.
Sustainability 14 11332 g001
Figure 2. Sample location and percentages.
Figure 2. Sample location and percentages.
Sustainability 14 11332 g002
Figure 3. Correspondence analysis plot.
Figure 3. Correspondence analysis plot.
Sustainability 14 11332 g003
Figure 4. VIVO profiles of three representative participants from each group on the subscales of the significantly differing blocks.
Figure 4. VIVO profiles of three representative participants from each group on the subscales of the significantly differing blocks.
Sustainability 14 11332 g004
Table 1. Socio-demographic characteristics.
Table 1. Socio-demographic characteristics.
VariablesLocal Place IdentityIsland Place Identity
NMSDNMSD
GenderMen1264.971.141265.570.64
Women844.981.16845.530.83
MunicipalityValleseco574.191.21575.690.65
Agaete334.531.19335.460.76
Gáldar155.470.70155.600.66
La Aldea215.490.76215.490.69
Moya105.600.58105.630.48
Artenara195.630.58195.630.57
Tejeda335.420.97335.570.65
San Mateo125.171.07125.560.63
San Bartolomé105.500.55104.871.41
Total 2104.971.142105.550.72
N, number of participants; M, mean; SD, standard deviation.
Table 2. Pearson correlation coefficients between local identity, island identity, and risk perceptions of climate change and fire.
Table 2. Pearson correlation coefficients between local identity, island identity, and risk perceptions of climate change and fire.
Risk PerceptionLocal IdentityIsland Identity
Climate changeVoluntariness−0.194 **−0.122
Immediacy0.041−0.004
Knowledge0.213 **0.084
Knowledge science0.0930.031
Control−0.163 *0.046
Newness0.101−0.036
Catastrophism−0.219 **0.004
Dread0.062−0.167 *
Severity−0.195 **−0.008
FireVoluntariness−0.0370.076
Immediacy0.0110.109
Knowledge0.0490.125
Knowledge science0.009−0.012
Control−0.182 **0.033
Newness0.0010.106
Catastrophism−0.043−0.101
Dread−0.044−0.055
Severity0.0730.145 *
Note: **, p < 0.01; *, p < 0.05.
Table 3. VIVO differences by territorial resistance group.
Table 3. VIVO differences by territorial resistance group.
VIVO Block123MANOVA
MSDMSDMSDFSigη21 − βPost hoc
Worldviews2.260.492.200.572.150.550.530.59
Attitude towards the World2.650.462.740.382.660.401.580.21
View of Human Beings2.520.522.690.512.650.521.750.18
Coping3.030.623.070.702.980.690.340.80
Impact of Past Situations2.360.452.320.532.300.470.180.84
Emotions2.380.882.800.882.670.793.970.020.040.711 < 2
Telling the Experience3.110.583.100.643.110.590.020.99
Consequences1.930.652.330.672.210.765.960.010.560.871 < 2
Social Support2.180.552.430.472.190.66.310.000.060.892 > 1
2 > 3
Identity2.270.672.550.622.60.624.650.010.040.791 < 2
1 < 3
Note: 1, non-affected; 2, vulnerable; 3, survivor–resistant; M, mean; SD, standard deviation; Sig, significance level; F, Fisher’s F-distribution; η2, partial eta squared (effect size); 1 − β, observed power.
Table 4. Differences in means between subscales corresponding to the Consequences, Social Support, and Identity blocks.
Table 4. Differences in means between subscales corresponding to the Consequences, Social Support, and Identity blocks.
BlockSubscale123MANOVA
MSDMSDMSDFSigη21 − βPost hoc
ConsequencesSensitivity/insensitivity with others1.870.492.240.762.140.735.020.010.050.811 < 2
Capacity to feed affection1.420.761.420.671.500.680.230.80
Acceptance of chance2.491.273.331.453.031.505.910.000.060.871 < 2
Social SupportSocial Support1.900.631.890.731.870.740.090.920.000.06
Blaming the victim2.400.832.970.512.520.8811.110.000.100.991 < 2
2 > 3
IdentityFuture and hope1.710.571.990.582.000.584.830.010.050.801 < 2
1 < 3
Identity changes2.740.833.040.723.080.753.610.030.030.66
Change in priorities2.081.082.150.992.470.942.690.070.030.53
Victimhood2.550.913.030.862.850.914.890.010.050.801 < 2
Note: 1, non-affected; 2, vulnerable; 3, survivor–resistant; M, mean; SD, standard deviation; Sig, significance level; F, Fisher’s F-distribution; η2, partial eta squared (effect size); 1 − β, observed power.
Table 5. Difference in means of place identity by territorial resistance.
Table 5. Difference in means of place identity by territorial resistance.
Place Identity123ANOVA
MSDMSDMSDFSigη21 − βPost hoc
Local4.191.215.091.055.490.8125.780.000.201.001 < 2
1 < 3
Island5.690.655.430.825.590.632.410.09
Note: 1, non-affected; 2, vulnerable; 3, survivor–resistant; M, mean; SD, standard deviation; Sig, significance level; F, Fisher’s F-distribution; η2, partial eta squared (effect size); 1 − β, observed power.
Table 6. Pearson correlation coefficients of place identity and VIVO blocks.
Table 6. Pearson correlation coefficients of place identity and VIVO blocks.
VIVO BlockLocalIsland
Worldviews−0.133−0.134
Attitude towards the World0.131−0.061
View of Human Beings0.0390.009
Coping−0.098−0.114
Impact of Past Situations−0.0810.117
Emotions−0.017−0.084
Telling the Experience0.009−0.044
Consequences0.059−0.096
Social Support−0.026−0.074
Identity0.137 *−0.006
Note: *, p < 0.05.
Table 7. Mean differences in attributes of climate change risk perception.
Table 7. Mean differences in attributes of climate change risk perception.
Attributes123ANOVA
MSDMSDMSDFSigη21 − βPost hoc
Voluntariness4.771.534.121.614.281.603.930.020.04
Immediacy4.131.684.001.594.141.480.140.87
Knowledge3.341.673.351.513.931.723.370.040.030.63
Knowledge science2.551.612.651.582.941.641.000.37
Control4.381.573.61.453.751.354.020.020.040.71
Newness3.891.703.641.554.011.850.990.37
Catastrophism5.141.464.171.514.241.819.180.000.080.981 > 2
1 > 3
Dread3.271.643.981.413.991.814.090.020.040.72
Severity4.021.354.241.374.361.399.930.000.090.981 < 2
1 < 3
Note: 1, non-affected; 2, vulnerable; 3, survivor–resistant; M, mean; SD, standard deviation; Sig, significance level; F, Fisher’s F-distribution; η2, partial eta squared (effect size); 1 − β, observed power.
Table 8. Mean differences in attributes of fire risk perception.
Table 8. Mean differences in attributes of fire risk perception.
Attributes123ANOVA
MSDMSDMSDFSigη21 − βPost hoc
Voluntariness4.371.793.441.813.742.014.690.010.040.791 > 2
Immediacy2.611.462.671.433.331.723.820.020.040.69
Knowledge2.821.482.331.443.231.975.780.000.050.872 < 3
Knowledge science2.161.082.31.292.521.451.560.21
Control4.161.423.771.273.321.345.110.010.050.821 > 3
Newness5.531.385.451.594.941.723.180.040.030.60
Catastrophism5.041.504.881.584.771.701.000.37
Dread5.511.305.61.085.231.462.150.12
Severity4.471.124.350.964.71.231.600.20
Note: 1, non-affected; 2, vulnerable; 3, survivor–resistant; M, mean; SD, standard deviation; Sig, significance level; F, Fisher’s F-distribution; η2, partial eta squared (effect size); 1 − β, observed power.
Table 9. Pearson correlation coefficient.
Table 9. Pearson correlation coefficient.
123456789
1. Sensitivity/insensitivity with others1
2. Acceptance of chance0.337 **1
3. Blaming the victim0.182 **0.317 **1
4. Future0.431 **0.598 **0.300 **1
5. Victimhood0.213 **0.639 **0.315 **0.640 **1
6. Severity CC−0.105−0.121−0.106−0.139 *−0.1351
7. Catastrophic CC−0.180 **−0.179 *−0.123−0.159 *−0.155 *0.503 **1
8. Voluntariness Fire−0.061−0.167 *−0.143 *−0.090−0.186 **0.1250.168 *1
9. Knowledge Fire−0.008−0.112−0.236 **0.021−0.0430.109−0.0100.339 **1
10. Control IF0.011−0.005−0.175 *−0.0400.0240.1070.1270.184 **0.260 **
Note: **, p < 0.01; *, p < 0.05.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

de Jesús, P.; Olivos-Jara, P.; Navarro, O. Place Identity and Traumatic Experiences in the Context of Wildfires. Sustainability 2022, 14, 11332. https://doi.org/10.3390/su141811332

AMA Style

de Jesús P, Olivos-Jara P, Navarro O. Place Identity and Traumatic Experiences in the Context of Wildfires. Sustainability. 2022; 14(18):11332. https://doi.org/10.3390/su141811332

Chicago/Turabian Style

de Jesús, Peter, Pablo Olivos-Jara, and Oscar Navarro. 2022. "Place Identity and Traumatic Experiences in the Context of Wildfires" Sustainability 14, no. 18: 11332. https://doi.org/10.3390/su141811332

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop