The Relationship Between Connectedness to Nature and Pro-Environmental Behaviors: A Systematic Review
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Climate Change
1.2. Pro-Environmental Behavior Literature Background
1.3. Connectedness to Nature Literature Background
1.4. The Theoretical Relationship Between PEB and CN
2. Methods
2.1. Research Strategy
2.2. Description of the Studies
2.2.1. Date Distribution
2.2.2. Geographical Distribution
2.2.3. Sample Size
2.2.4. Proportion of Males and Females
2.2.5. Sampling Age
2.2.6. CN Measuring Instruments
2.2.7. PEB Measurement Instruments
2.3. Quality Assessment
3. Results
4. Discussion
4.1. Limitations of Included Studies
4.2. Limitations of This Study
4.3. Potential Impact
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Ref. | Sample Size | Gender Distribution | Age Distribution/Mean Age | Country | Connectedness to Nature Measure | Pro-Environmental Behavior Measure | Results |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
[113] | 360 | 31.9% M 68.1% F | 16–24 M = 20.11 | Arizona, USA | Connectedness to Nature Scale (CNS) [7] | A modified version of Pro-Environmental Behavior scale (PEB) [79]. | r = 0.34, p < 0.001 |
[75] | 688 | 49.6% M 50.4% F | 9–21 M = 15.42 | Canada and China | A shortened and adapted version of the instrument developed by Brügger et al. [66] | An adapted version of the General Ecological Behavior scale (GEB) [73]. | r = 0.42, p < 0.001 |
[98] | 973 | NR | NR NR | Granada, Spain | Connectedness to Nature Scale (CNS) [7] | In order to capture sustainable consumption (s.c.) they asked individuals to score the degree with which they perform several actions. In order to capture activism (a) they asked individuals to score the frequency with which they participate in demonstrations in support of the environment. | r(s.c.) = 0.36, p < 0.01; r(a) = 0.276, p < 0.01 |
[102] | 400 | NR | 18–75 NR | City of Thessaloniki, Greece. | Connectedness to Nature Scale (CNS) [7] | They used 10 behavioral items derived from previous studies, e.g., [73,81]. The authors considered two dimensions of pro-environmental behavior: environmental action (e.a.) and personal practices domains (p.p.). | r(p.p.) = 0.47, p < 0.01; r(e.a.) = 0.19, p < 0.01 |
[115] | 224 | 37.5% M 62.5% F | NR M = 23.64 | Brazil | Connectedness to Nature Scale (CNS) [7] | Self-reported PEB Scale [81] | r = 0.44, p < 0.01 |
[116] | 1068 | NR | 18–46 M = 21.12 | Granada, Spain | 14 items adapted from Mayer and Frantz [7] | Sixteen items, adapted from Binder, Blankenberg, and Guardiola [90], which asked participants to indicate how often they perform pro-environmental activities. | r = 0.40, p < 0.001 |
[101] | 150 | 42% M 58% F | NR M = 40.32 | Greece | Connectedness to Nature Scale (CNS) [7]; Iclusion of Nature in the Self scale (INS) [67] | They draw on the work of Kaiser and Wilson [73]; Alisat and Riemer [86] and Larson et al. [81] to develop a multi-dimensional measure of environmental behavior (22 items). The scale encompasses six potential behavior domains: civic actions (c.a.), policy support (p.s.), recycling (r.), transportation (t.), behavior in the household setting (b.h.s.) and consumerism (c.). | r(CNS,c.a.) = 0.24; r(CNS,p.s.) = 0.35; r(CNS,r.) = 0.438; r(CNS,t.) = 0.214; r(CNS,b.h.s.) = = 0.48; r(CNS,c.) = 0.384; r(INS,c.a.) = 0.334; r(INS,p.s.) = 0.419; r(INS,r.) = 0.333; r(INS,t.) = 0.248; r(INS,b.h.s.) = = 0.536; r(INS,c.) = 0.496; For all coefficients p < 0.01 |
[76] | 400 | 46% M 54% F | 9–12 M = 10 | Mexico | Children’s Affective Attitude Toward Nature Scale [64] | General Ecological Behavior Scale [74], adapted for use with children by Fraijo Sing et al. [78] | r = 0.46, p < 0.01 |
[35] | 76 | 25% M 75% F | 18–60 M = 33 | Australia | Connectedness to Nature Scale (CNS) [7] | Environmental Behavior scale [82]. | r = 0.36, p < 0.01 |
[34] | 113 | 55.8% M 44.2% F | 23–30 M = 26.54 | China | A Chinese version of the CNS [7] and a modified Chinese version of the computerized IAT based on that of Schultz and colleagues [117] was created to measure the RT (ms) needed to classify words associated with natural and built environments. | College Students’ Environmental Behaviors Questionnaire (CSEBQ) modified by Shen [83] from the Behavior-based Environmental Attitude scale [84] and a situational simulation experiment (s.s.e.). | r(CNS,CSEBQ) = 0.39, p < 001; r(IAT,s.s.e.) = 0.56, p < 0.001 |
[97] | 4960 | 48.6% M 51.4% F | 16–95 NR | England | The Nature Connection Index (NCI) [68] | Environment-related activities undertaken during the previous 12 months. The authors consider two types of pro-environmental behavior: household behaviors (h.b.) and nature conservation behaviors (n.c.b.). | r(NCI, h.b.) = 0.34, p < 0.001; r(NCI, n.c.b.) = = 0.19, p < 0.001 |
[118] | 97 | 61.9% M 38.1% F | 10–19 14.24 | Spain | Connectedness to Nature Scale (CNS) [7] | Pro-Environmental Behaviors Scale (PEBS) [80]. | r = 0.397, p < 0.001 |
[119] | 1251 | 51.3% M 48.7% F | NR NR | Canada | Krettenauer’s adapted version of the measure developed by Brügger and colleagues [66] | Environmental Questionnaire used by [65]. | r = 0.53, p < 0.01 |
[61] | 489 | NR | 18–40 NR | Pakistan | The scale of CN was adapted from Perrin and Benassi [62] | The scale of PB was adapted from Robertson and Barling [85]. | r = 0.57, p < 0.05 |
[100] | 212 | NR | 18–35 NR | India | Connectedness to Nature Scale (CNS) [7] | Environment Action Scale [86]. The study investigates two types of behavior: participatory action (p.a.) and leadership action (l.a.). | University students: r(CN, p.a.) = 0.227, p < 0.01; r(CN, l.a.) = −0.058; not significant. Professionals: r(CN, p.a.) = 0.243, p < 0.05; r(CN, l.a.) = 0.265, p < 0.01 |
[120] | 423 | 38.3% M 61.7% F | NR NR | New Zealand | A 12-item version of Brügger et al. [66] Disposition to Connect With Nature Scale. | The General Ecological Behavior Scale [73,74] | r = 0.48, p < 0.001 |
[121] | 352 | 48.6% M 51.4% F | 4–12 M = 7.63 | Australia | Connection to Nature Index (CNI) [64] | Connection to Nature Index (CNI) [64]. | r = 0.46, p < 0.001 |
[43] | 307 | 44% M 56% F | NR NR | Australia | Seven items derived from Gosling and Williams [70] | Six Pro-Environmental Behavior items based on Tonge et al. [91,92,93]. | r = 0.323, p < 0.001 |
[104] | 4700 | NR | 18–75 NR | Sweden | Three item from the “Outdoor Recreation in Change” National Survey [72]. Participants were asked to respond to three items following the question: “To be in nature usually makes me feel or experience”: (i) A heightened sense about the interplay of nature, that everything is connected. (ii) A feeling that the city is dependent on the surrounding nature. (iii) A feeling that all people, including myself, are united and a part of nature. | Six questions from the “Outdoor Recreation in Change” National Survey [72]. Participants were asked to respond to a list of behavioral items following the question: “What of the following do you do for environmental reasons”: (i) I choose to walk, ride the bicycle or use public transportation instead of going by car. (ii) I collect and separate household waste. (iii) I eat organically produced food. (iv) I purchase green eco label products. (v) I reduce my speed when driving. (vi) I choose the train over air travel. | r(i) = 0.09; r(ii) = 0.11; r(iii) = 0.16; r(iv) = 0.17; r(v) = 0.20; r(vi) = 0.13 p < 0.008 for all coefficients. |
[122] | 159 | 3.8% M 96.2% F | NR M = 25.56 | Italy | A single graphical item from Schultz [22]. Participants were asked to choose one of the seven figures presented which better represented their relationship with nature. | A self-reported measure of general PEBs composed of 14 item from [27,87]. | r = 0.52, p < 0.001 |
[123] | 185 | 33% M 67% F | 18–48 M = 21.5 | USA | CNS [7] | Pro-Environmental Behavior Scale [80]. | r = 0.38, p < 0.001 |
[63] | 245 | 48.2% M 51.8% F | 17–56 M = 19.69 | Spain | Seven items based on Mayer and Frantz’s scale [7] | Seven items of the Student Environmental Behavior Scale [94] and an additional item [95]. | r = 0.62, p < 0.01 |
[124] | 430 | 41.9% M 58.1% F | 20–59 M = 31.2 | Italy | Connectedness to Nature Scale [7] | Pro-environmental behaviors’ questionnaire (adapted from Markle [80] and Menardo [88]). To this were added four questions on the purchase of products, another question on environmental citizenship and one on separate waste collection. | r = 0.28, p < 0.001 |
[125] | 589 | 36.7% M 63.3% F | 18–65 M = 30.06 | Philippines | Connectedness to Nature Scale [7] | General Measure of Ecological Behavior [74]. | r = 0.40, p < 0.01 |
[71] | 78 | 23% M 77% F | NR NR | USA | Five items [71] | Seven items [71]. | r = 0.42, p < 0.0001 |
[96] | 382 | 98% M 2% F | 20–82 41.4 | Serbia | Inclusion of Nature in the Self Scale [67] | The EB in this study was measured by the question: “Have you changed your behavior due to environmental reasons?”. | r = 0.13, p < 0.01 |
[126] | 211 | 27% M 73% F | NR 26.57 | England | The short form of the nature relatedness scale [69] | A measure of the frequency of engaging in 22 Pro-Environmental Behaviors [89]. | r = 0.59, p < 0.001 |
[127] | N1 = 54 N2 = 299 | 33.3% MN1 66.7% FN1 39.5% MN1 60.5% FN1 | NR N1 NR N2 MN1 = 36.15 MN2 = 43.3 | USA | Connectedness to Nature Scale (CNS) [7] | General Ecological Behavior Scale (GEBS) [73] in N1 and Pro-Environmental Behavior Scale (PEB) [79] in N2. | r(CNS,GEBS) = 0.38, p < 0.01; r(CNS,PEB) = 0.43, p < 0.001 |
[77] | 296 | 40.9% M 59.1% F | 9–12 10.42 | Mexico | Connection to Nature Index (CNI) [64] | Eleven items from the General Ecological Behavior Scale of Kaiser [74] adapted for use with children by Fraijo Sing et al. [78]. | r = 0.49, p < 0.001 |
Ref. | Main Findings | Limitations | Risk of Biases |
---|---|---|---|
[113] | Statistically controlling mindfulness, Connectedness to Nature is significantly and positively associated with pro-environmental behavior. Connectedness to nature mediates the relationship between mindfulness and pro-environmental behavior. | The study was aimed only at university students. The research examined daily Pro-Environmental Behaviors (e.g., recycling, the use of reusable shopping bags), and therefore, the generalization of these findings to other types of Pro-Environmental Behaviors should be implemented with caution. It is important to note that the results are correlative, so the data cannot support causal relationships. The research was carried out in Arizona, it would be desirable to replicate it in other countries. | Sampling bias due to convenience sampling technique (non-probability sampling). Participants self-reported the average frequency of their engagement in 17 daily Pro-Environmental Behaviors, but self-reports may contain recall errors, in addition to distortions that can occur due to bias of social desirability. |
[75] | Connectedness to nature is significantly and positively associated with pro-environmental behavior in both Canada and China. | Drawing is correlative, so it does not allow causal inferences. Only two cultural groups were compared, so the question remains whether the cultural differences documented in the study are unique for the two countries or represent more general factors applicable to a wider spectrum of cultures. | Sampling of convenience and non-representative. While the authors focused on distinguishing collectivism–individualism, there might be other factors that produced the differences between the Canadian and Chinese samples. Measures are self-reported, so biases of social recall and desirability are possible. |
[98] | There is a significant and positive correlation between Connectedness to Nature and two types of pro-environmental behavior: sustainable consumption and activism. | The sample is adequate for statistical analysis, but a larger sample size would have been ideal. Because we work with correlative data, causality cannot be affirmed. The study was conducted in Spain, it would be desirable to replicate it in other contexts. | Sampling of convenience and non-representative. The environmental activism indicator leaves room for improvement: it is based on participation in pro-environmental events and could be enriched by including other dimensions of activism. Data are self-reported, so it can be distorted due to recall biases and social desirability. |
[102] | There is a correlation between Connectedness to Nature and two dimensions of pro-environmental behavior: personal practices and environmental action. | It would be useful to replicate the study also in other socio-cultural contexts. The study has a correlational nature, so cause and effect inferences cannot be made. | Only self-reported environmental behavior was measured, not real behavior, which raises a concern about the bias of social desirability over possible recall errors. |
[116] | Pro-environmental behavior mediates the link between Connectedness to Nature and food adhesion, fully explaining why people who feel more connected to nature report greater adherence in the short term and are more likely to continue their diet in the future. A high Connectedness to Nature predicts a high PEB index. | The participants are all university students. The study has a correlative nature, which undermines the possibility of making causal inferences. Many variables have been evaluated with binary responses. There are additional factors that can play a role in vegetarian dietary adherence, for example, it would be useful to consider constructs such as sensitivity to disgust, moral foundations, other relevant values, and self-control when considering meat consumption. The sample was placed in a geographical area where many people follow a Mediterranean diet, this justifies intercultural validation, in order to demonstrate the reliability of the effects observed outside Spain. | The authors based themselves on what was self-reported by the participants, so there may be distortions due to recall errors and biases of social desirability. Sampling of convenience. |
[115] | The results suggest that current experiences of adults in nature have a positive effect on their PEB, this effect is partially explained by the Connectedness to Nature. Connectedness to nature and pro-environmental behavior are positively associated. | The participants are all university students. Most of them are women. It would be useful to replicate the study in other developing and developed countries so as to allow a generalization of the results. The study is correlative, so no causal explanations can be made. | Sampling of convenience. The data were self-reported by the participants, so there is a risk of recall errors and distortions due to bias of social desirability. |
[101] | The results indicate that pro-environmental behavior is a multidimensional construct. There is a positive correlation between Connectedness to Nature and six domains of pro-environmental behavior: civic actions; policy support; recycling; transportation choices; behavior in the home; and sustainable consumption. | Since the predictive power of connection, the ecological worldview, and environmental concern have never been examined with respect to multiple behavioral domains, The conclusions of this study are provisional and further validation of the results is essential to verify their replicability. It is also critical to examine other psychological antecedents that predict environmental behavior versus those reported in this study, such as norms, earnings, hedonic motivations, and contextual factors (e.g., status, comfort, behavioral opportunities) that previous research has highlighted. This study was carried out in Greece, the results should also be replicated in different socio-cultural contexts. The study is correlative, so no causal conclusions can be drawn. | Sampling of convenience. The measurement of pro-environmental behavior used was constructed at-hoc by the authors, which increases the risk of potential bias. This study is based on self-reported measures, so social desirability could be a limiting factor that influences responses, as well as potential recall errors. |
[76] | There was a moderately positive correlation between the children’s place of residence and PEB, as well as the Connectedness to Nature and PEB. | The narrow age range does not allow generalizations. It would be important to replicate this research in other countries to allow generalization. The study is correlative, so no causal conclusions can be drawn. | Convenience sampling. Self-reported measurements, therefore possible errors due to social desirability bias and memory bias. |
[35] | Connectedness to nature is positively correlated with Pro-Environmental Behaviors. This relationship remained positive and significant even by controlling social desirability. Connectedness to nature explains 10% of the variance in pro-environmental behavior. | The participants are all graduates in psychology. Most of the participants are females. The use of the Environmental Behavior scale of Schultz et al. [82] may be a limitation. The scale consists of only 10 behaviors, six of which are related to the recycling or reuse of articles. This limited number of behaviors may have contributed to the low score as participants may have adopted behaviors not included in the scale. Because the scale measures engagement in past behaviors, participants may not be able to properly remember how often they have engaged in behavior, if at all. The extent of the effects observed is relatively small. While the relationships are significant, little variance in pro-environmental behavior has been explained by Connectedness to Nature (only 10%). The research was conducted in only one country, it would be desirable to replicate it in other countries. The study is correlative, so cause and effect relationships cannot be inferred. | Sampling of convenience. Self-reported responses are therefore potentially affected by distortions due to social desirability bias and mnemonic bias. |
[34] | The results showed that the explicit Connectedness to Nature was positively correlated with intentional environmental behaviors, while the implicit connectedness was positively correlated with spontaneous environmental behaviors. Therefore, it is the implicit Connectedness to Nature that can really predict pro-environmental behavior in real-life situations. | The age range is rather narrow. The participants are all university students. The study was carried out in China, it would be useful to replicate it in other contexts in order to advance generalizations. As a correlation study, no causal statements can be made. The structure of the plastic bag test to measure spontaneous environmental behavior is binary, not continuous. Therefore, measurement could be simplified, necessitating the development of a more elaborate instrument in future studies. | Convenience sampling. Self-reported data, then possibly suffering from recall errors and bias of social desirability. |
[97] | The authors found a positive correlation between Connectedness to Nature and two types of pro-environmental behavior: Household PEB and Nature Conservation PEB. | The study is correlative, so no cause-and-effect inferences can be advanced. Authors acknowledge that they know little about the quality of contact with nature in the measures used. The study was conducted in England, so it would be useful to replicate it in other countries so that we can generalize the results. The size of the observed effects is small, indicating that natural factors explain only a limited amount of variance in the results. | The data are self-reported, so it may be affected by biases of social desirability and memory. |
[118] | Results show that the highest scores on Connectedness to Nature are correlated to higher scores on pro-environmental behavior, life satisfaction, beliefs on environmental behavior, and knowledge of the circular economy. | A limit is the sample size, which has been shown to be a limitation by authors in studies with a similar sample size. The age range is narrow. The lack of control over strange variables, such as household socio-economic data, limits the generalization of results. The study was conducted in Spain, it would be desirable to replicate it in other countries. The study is correlative, so it is not possible to come to conclusions about cause-effect relationships. | Convenience sampling. Measurements are self-reported and, therefore influenced by biases of social desirability and recall. |
[119] | Connectedness to nature is a strong predictor of pro-environmental behavior, higher levels of Connectedness to Nature are associated with higher levels of pro-environmental behavior. Results show that Connectedness to Nature mediates significantly the relationship between age and PEB, so that, with advancing age, their connection levels increase, which, in turn, leads to an increase in PEB levels. | The study is correlative, so it is not possible to make causal inferences. The questionnaire of this study failed to measure a number of important variables, in particular, the amount of time spent in nature by the participants was not measured, similarly, individual occupations, infrastructure levels in their cities, and levels of industrialization near their homes have not been measured. The research was carried out in Canada, it would be useful to replicate it in other socio-cultural contexts in order to generalize the results. | Pro-environmental behavior was measured using a series of items, taken from different tools, there may have been biases in the choice of items by the authors. The data have been self-reported, so it could be distorted due to social desirability bias and mnemonic bias. |
[61] | Results show that Connectedness to Nature and pro-environmental behavior are significantly and positively correlated. In addition, pro-environmental behavior and Connectedness to Nature are significant mediators in the relationship between social responsibility and financial performance. | The current work has been conducted only in four cities of Pakistan, future researchers should include more cities from other geographical locations in order to have a better generalization. Pro-environmental behavior could also be explained by other variables that have been neglected in this research. The study is correlative, no causal inferences can be made. | Convenience sampling. Self-reported measurements are therefore potentially affected by social desirability and memory biases. |
[100] | There is a weak positive correlation between Connectedness to Nature and participatory actions in Indian university students. There is a weak positive correlation between Connectedness to Nature and participatory actions in Indian professionals. There is a weak positive correlation between Connectedness to Nature and leadership actions in Indian professionals. | The study was conducted in India, so to generalize the results the same research should be replicated in other socio-cultural contexts. The research has a correlational nature, therefore it does not allow us to conclude causal explanations. | Convenience sampling. Self-reported responses, therefore possibly affected by memory biases and social desirability. |
[120] | Connectedness to nature had the strongest direct positive association with PEB. The participation in tree planting had statistically significant indirect relationships with PEB through two routes. The first indirect association was through Connectedness to Nature and the second indirect association was through Connectedness to Nature, the use of nature for psychological restoration, and environmental attitudes. | Individuals who particularly appreciate nature or have more financial resources may be more likely to live in greener areas and participate in plantation projects than those who are not so interested in nature or have less economic resources, the lack of causal assignment creates a threat to internal validity. The study was conducted with residents of Wellington City, a city that, atypically, has an abundance of green space. Future research examining residents in other urban areas with less green space and more limited opportunities to engage with nature would strengthen confidence in our findings. The study, being correlative, does not allow to make cause-effect inference. | The results are based on self-reported data, which creates a threat to validity (i.e., measurements may not access what we intend to measure). Sampling of convenience. |
[121] | The age of participants is significantly and negatively correlated with both the time spent in nature and the children’s Connectedness to Nature, which means that older students tended to spend less time in nature. Socio-economic conditions were shown to have a significant and negative correlation with Connectedness to Nature. Pro-environmental behaviors were found to be significantly and positively correlated with Connectedness to Nature and the time spent in nature. As expected, there was also a significant and positive correlation between Connectedness to Nature and time in nature. | The narrow age range does not allow generalizing results to adults. The drawing is correlative, so no causal conclusions can be drawn. The study was carried out in Australia, it should be replicated in other countries to make possible a generalization. The measure of the amount of time spent in nature simply asked if the children had spent time outdoors when they were at home and at school. It is not a measure of the type of activity, the type of activity in nature is likely to be an important factor to explore. | The data are self-reported, so it may be affected by the bias of social desirability and memory. Convenience sampling. |
[43] | An individual’s sense of identity as an environmentalist or having a Connectedness to Nature was shown to influence pro-environmental behavior, as the study found a significant positive relationship between these two dimensions. Personal standards have a positive effect on the Pro-Environmental Behaviors used in this study. | The study is limited to the context of Western culture, it would be desirable to validate the results in a different cultural context. The study has a correlational nature, so no causal conclusions can be drawn. | The measures used in this study are based on an individual’s response rather than measuring or witnessing actual behavior, as such, there is potential for respondents to provide socially desirable responses. The answers may also be affected by mnemonic bias. Non-probability sampling (convenience sampling) was used. |
[104] | Positive correlations were found between Connectedness to Nature and 6 items of pro-environmental behavior: I choose to walk, bicycle and mass transit instead of the car; I sort household waste; I eat organically grown food; I purchase environmentally friendly products; I drive at slower speeds while driving a car; I choose to take the train over air travel. | The research was conducted in only one country, so it would be useful to replicate it in other countries in order to advance generalizations. The study is correlative therefore it does not allow causal inference. Connectedness to nature was measured by reference to only one question. | The answers are self-reported, so they may be affected by social desirability bias, plus recall errors. |
[122] | The results of the analysis show a significant and positive correlation between Connectedness to Nature and pro-environmental behavior. | The majority of the sample is made up of university students. In addition, there is an overwhelming majority of females compared to males. The study is of a correlational nature, therefore causal relationships cannot be inferred. The effect size of the correlation is small, so little variance is explained. The study was conducted in only one country, it would be desirable to replicate it in other countries to increase the generalizability of the results. | Convenience and non-representative sampling. PEB was measured using a self-reported measure, so information can be distorted due to recall bias and social desirability bias. |
[123] | There was a medium-sized positive correlation between the CNS and the PEBS | The external validity of the current study is tenuous, as the data were collected from a relatively small sample of college students in rural, politically conservative Idaho. The design is cross-sectional and correlational. The study was carried out in only one country, so the results are not generalizable. | Convenience and non-representative sampling. PEB measurement is self reported, so there are possible biases of recall and social desirability. |
[63] | A significant relationship between CN and PEB has been found, which is wide and positive. | The sample size is sufficient to estimate coefficients with appropriate power, but, because it is nevertheless limited, particularly concerning the external validity of results, future research should employ larger and, instead of university students, more diverse samples. The studied relationships should furthermore be addressed in other groups, such as children, since, for instance, little research has been conducted on the effects of nature connectedness on children’s behavior. The cross-sectional character of the study does not allow to establish causality and directionality of effects. The study should be replicated in other countries to increase their generalizability. | Sampling bias due to convenience sampling technique. Measures are self-reported, therefore affected by potential bias (recall and social desirability). |
[124] | The total pro-environmental behavior was positively correlated with Connectedness to Nature. | Most of the participants were women, therefore future studies with a more heterogeneous sample are necessary. The transversal nature of the study does not allow us to verify the causality of behavior, but only the correlation link between the variables investigated. Consequently, a longitudinal study may provide further details. Finally, to increase cultural generalizability, the study should be replicated in other contexts. | The data were collected through an online survey that presents some problems related to sampling, for example, the bias of self-election. Replicating the study using both an online and offline setting, would lead to more valid and reliable conclusions. Furthermore, the limits of self-report scales, such as the bias of social desirability, in which the participant could give answers considered more acceptable. Future studies should implement direct observation approaches of various Pro-Environmental Behaviors, although they present other difficulties. |
[125] | The relationships between the variables are in the expected direction, showing a significant and positive correlation between Connectedness to Nature and ecological behavior. | First, the results are primarily correlations. Future research may consider conducting experimental studies to demonstrate causal relations on the impact of nature relatedness on ecological behavior. Second, this research does not claim generalizability as participants were mostly adolescents and early adults. Future studies may consider examining the hypotheses in representative samples, especially in children as they will be the generation who will face the consequences of environmental degradation. This study did not include culturally relevant variables. For instance, norms may contribute to Filipinos’ ecological behavior. The study was carried out in the Philippines, to increase the generalizability it should be replicated in other countries. | Snowball sampling (not probabilistic). Self-reported measures lead to possible biases of recall and social desirability. |
[71] | High correlations were found between attitudes, connection to nature, and environmentally responsible behaviors. | The sample size is small. Most participants are women. The study is correlative, so no causal statements can be made. To increase cultural generalizability, the study should be replicated. | Non-probability sampling technique (convenience sampling). Self-reported measurements with potential biases of recall and social desirability. Measurements of Connectedness to Nature and pro-environmental behavior used were constructed at-hoc by the authors, which increases the risk of potential bias. |
[96] | The study shows a significant and positive correlation between Connectedness to Nature and environmental behavior. | Most of the participants are men and are all farmers. The sample of this study was highly homogenous in terms of cropping systems, examining other cropping systems and the differences between them is left for future studies. The study does not allow causal inferences. The study was carried out in Serbia, in order to increase its generalizability it should be replicated in other contexts. | Measures are self-reported, therefore impacted by recall bias and social desirability bias. |
[126] | The correlation between nature relatedness and PEB is significant and positive. | Most of the sample consists of women. The use of an online methodology introduced a lack of experimental control in regard to data collection. The study is correlational, therefore it does not allow to advance cause-effect claims. The study was conducted in one state only, to increase the generalizability it would be advisable to replicate the study in other countries. | Measures are self-reported and therefore have potential biases. Sampling of convenience. |
[127] | In both samples, nature connectedness was positively correlated with ecological behavior, | Data derived from different sample populations is needed to determine the generalizability of these findings. For example, the present study’s samples were predominately White, reflecting a lack of racial and ethnic diversity that should be remedied in future studies. The study has a correlational nature. The study should be replicated in different countries to increase cultural generalizability. | Convenience sampling. Measures used are self-reported and impacted by recall and social desirability biases. |
[77] | Connectedness to nature is positively and significantly related to pro-ecological behavior. | The number of participants, their age, and the fact that they lived in the same city; altogether, makes it impossible to conclude that the sample is representative of the Mexican population aged 9 to 12 years. In addition, in spite of differences that may exist between socio-demographic data such as sex, age, and school grade, the results were not compared based on those characteristics. The study was conducted in one state only, to increase the generalizability it would be advisable to replicate the study in other countries. Finally, the correlational research design could also be considered as a limitation, given its restrictions compared with experimental studies. | Convenience sampling. The use of self-report scales in the measurement of variables presents disadvantages compared to other, more objective data-collection techniques. |
Topic | No. | Item | Location Where Item Is Reported |
---|---|---|---|
TITLE | |||
Title | 1 | Identify the report as a systematic review. | Page 1 |
ABSTRACT | |||
Abstract | 2 | See the PRISMA 2020 for Abstracts checklist | Page 1 |
INTRODUCTION | |||
Rationale | 3 | Describe the rationale for the review in the context of existing knowledge. | Page 2–3 |
Objectives | 4 | Provide an explicit statement of the objective(s) or question(s) the review addresses. | Page 1 |
METHODS | |||
Eligibility criteria | 5 | Specify the inclusion and exclusion criteria for the review and how studies were grouped for the syntheses. | Page 4 |
Information sources | 6 | Specify all databases, registers, websites, organizations, reference lists and other sources searched or consulted to identify studies. Specify the date when each source was last searched or consulted. | Page 4 |
Search strategy | 7 | Present the full search strategies for all databases, registers, and websites, including any filters and limits used. | Page 4 |
Selection process | 8 | Specify the methods used to decide whether a study met the inclusion criteria of the review, including how many reviewers screened each record and each report retrieved, whether they worked independently, and if applicable, details of automation tools used in the process. | Page 4 |
Data collection process | 9 | Specify the methods used to collect data from reports, including how many reviewers collected data from each report, whether they worked independently, any processes for obtaining or confirming data from study investigators, and if applicable, details of automation tools used in the process. | Page 4 |
Data items | 10a | List and define all outcomes for which data were sought. Specify whether all results that were compatible with each outcome domain in each study were sought (e.g., for all measures, time points, analyses), and if not, the methods used to decide which results to collect. | Page 4 |
10b | List and define all other variables for which data were sought (e.g., participant and intervention characteristics, funding sources). Describe any assumptions made about any missing or unclear information. | Appendix A Table A1 | |
Study risk of bias assessment | 11 | Specify the methods used to assess risk of bias in the included studies, including details of the tool(s) used, how many reviewers assessed each study and whether they worked independently, and if applicable, details of automation tools used in the process. | Page 4 |
Effect measures | 12 | Specify for each outcome the effect measure(s) (e.g., risk ratio, mean difference) used in the synthesis or presentation of results. | Page 4 |
Synthesis methods | 13a | Describe the processes used to decide which studies were eligible for each synthesis (e.g., tabulating the study intervention characteristics and comparing against the planned groups for each synthesis (item 5)). | Page 4 |
13b | Describe any methods required to prepare the data for presentation or synthesis, such as handling of missing summary statistics, or data conversions. | Page 4 | |
13c | Describe any methods used to tabulate or visually display results of individual studies and syntheses. | Page 4 | |
13d | Describe any methods used to synthesize results and provide a rationale for the choice(s). If meta-analysis was performed, describe the model(s), method(s) to identify the presence and extent of statistical heterogeneity, and software package(s) used. | Page 4 | |
13e | Describe any methods used to explore possible causes of heterogeneity among study results (e.g., subgroup analysis, meta-regression). | Page 4 | |
13f | Describe any sensitivity analyses conducted to assess robustness of the synthesized results. | Page 4 | |
Reporting bias assessment | 14 | Describe any methods used to assess risk of bias due to missing results in a synthesis (arising from reporting biases). | Page 4 |
Certainty assessment | 15 | Describe any methods used to assess certainty (or confidence) in the body of evidence for an outcome. | Page 4 |
RESULTS | |||
Study selection | 16a | Describe the results of the search and selection process, from the number of records identified in the search to the number of studies included in the review, ideally using a flow diagram. | Page 5 and Figure 1 |
16b | Cite studies that might appear to meet the inclusion criteria, but which were excluded, and explain why they were excluded. | Page 5 | |
Study characteristics | 17 | Cite each included study and present its characteristics. | Appendix A—Table A1 |
Risk of bias in studies | 18 | Present assessments of risk of bias for each included study. | Appendix A—Table A2 |
Results of individual studies | 19 | For all outcomes, present, for each study: (a) summary statistics for each group (where appropriate) and (b) an effect estimate and its precision (e.g., confidence/credible interval), ideally using structured tables or plots. | Appendix A—Table A1 |
Results of syntheses | 20a | For each synthesis, briefly summarize the characteristics and risk of bias among contributing studies. | Page 7–9 |
20b | Present results of all statistical syntheses conducted. If meta-analysis was performed, present for each the summary estimate and its precision (e.g., confidence/credible interval) and measures of statistical heterogeneity. If comparing groups, describe the direction of the effect. | Page 7–9 | |
20c | Present results of all investigations of possible causes of heterogeneity among study results. | Page 4 | |
20d | Present results of all sensitivity analyses conducted to assess the robustness of the synthesized results. | Page 4 | |
Reporting biases | 21 | Present assessments of risk of bias due to missing results (arising from reporting biases) for each synthesis assessed. | Page 4 |
Certainty of evidence | 22 | Present assessments of certainty (or confidence) in the body of evidence for each outcome assessed. | Page 4 |
DISCUSSION | |||
Discussion | 23a | Provide a general interpretation of the results in the context of other evidence. | Page 9–10 |
23b | Discuss any limitations of the evidence included in the review. | Page 10 | |
23c | Discuss any limitations of the review processes used. | Page 11 | |
23d | Discuss implications of the results for practice, policy, and future research. | Page 11 | |
OTHER INFORMATION | |||
Registration and protocol | 24a | Provide registration information for the review, including register name and registration number, or state that the review was not registered. | the review was not registered |
24b | Indicate where the review protocol can be accessed, or state that a protocol was not prepared. | the protocol was internal and thus cannot be accessed | |
24c | Describe and explain any amendments to information provided at registration or in the protocol. | No amendments | |
Support | 25 | Describe sources of financial or non-financial support for the review, and the role of the funders or sponsors in the review. | Page 12 |
Competing interests | 26 | Declare any competing interests of review authors. | Page 11 |
Availability of data, code and other materials | 27 | Report which of the following are publicly available and where they can be found: template data collection forms, data extracted from included studies, data used for all analyses, analytic code, any other materials used in the review. | Page 11 |
CN Measure | Reliability | Validity | Ref |
---|---|---|---|
Connectedness to Nature Scale (CNS) [7] | Good | Excellent | [35,98,101,102,113,115,116,118,123,124,125,127] |
Chinese version of the CNS [128] | Acceptable | Average | [34] |
7 items based on Mayer and Frantz’s scale [7] | Good | Average | [63] |
13-item version of CNS adapted by [62] | Acceptable | Good | [61] |
12-item short version of the Disposition to Connect With Nature Scale [66] | Good | Average | [120] |
Adapted version of the Disposition to Connect With Nature Scale [65] | Good | Good | [75,119] |
Connection to Nature Index (CNI) [64] | Good | Excellent | [76,77,121] |
Inclusion of Nature in the Self scale (INS) [67] | Not available | Poor | [96,101,122] |
Nature Connection Index (NCI) [68] | Excellent | Excellent | [97] |
6-item short form of the Nature Relatedness Scale [69] | Good | Excellent | [126] |
3 items extrapolated from the article published by [70] | Good | Average | [43] |
5 items built Ad Hoc by [71] | Not available | Poor | [71] |
Three item from the national survey “Outdoor Recreation in Change” by [72] | Good | Average | [104] |
PEB Measure | Reliability | Validity | Ref |
---|---|---|---|
Pro-Environmental Behaviors Scale (PEBS) [80] | Acceptable | Excellent | [118,123] |
A modified version of Pro-Environmental Behavior scale (PEB) [79] | Excellent | Average | [113,127] |
Pro-environmental behaviors’ questionnaire (adapted from [80,88]). To this were added six questions. | Good | Good | [124] |
The scale of PB was adapted from [85] | Good | Average | [61] |
Sustainable consumption index [98] | Acceptable | Poor | [98] |
Ad Hoc questions [98] | Not available | Poor | [98] |
10 behavioral items derived from previous studies, e.g., [73,81] | Excellent | Good | [102] |
Self-reported PEB Scale [81] | Questionable | Average | [115] |
16 items, adapted from Binder, Blankenberg, and Guardiola [90] | Not available | Average | [116] |
Authors developed a multi-dimensional measure of environmental behavior [101] | Acceptable—Good | Good | [101] |
General Ecological Behavior Scale [73,74] | Acceptable | Good | [120,125,127] |
General Ecological Behavior Scale [73,74], adapted for use with children by [78] | Acceptable | Average | [76,77] |
An adapted version of the General Ecological Behavior scale (GEB) [73] | Acceptable | Average | [75] |
Environmental Behavior scale [82] | Questionable—acceptable | Good | [35] |
College Students’ Environmental Behaviors Questionnaire (CSEBQ) modified by Shen [83] from the Behavior-based Environmental Attitude scale [84] and a situational simulation experiment (s.s.e.) | Acceptable | Good | [34] |
Environment-related activities undertaken during the previous 12 months [97] | Not available | Poor | [97] |
Environmental Questionnaire used by [65] | Good | Excellent | [119] |
Environment Action Scale [86]. | Excellent | Excellent | [100] |
Connection to Nature Index (CNI) [64] | Good | Excellent | [121] |
Six Pro-Environmental Behavior items based on [91,92,93] | Excellent | Good | [43] |
Six questions from the “Outdoor Recreation in Change” National Survey [72]. | Questionable | Average | [104] |
A self-reported measure of general PEBs composed of 14 item from [27,87] | Acceptable | Average | [122] |
Seven items of the Student Environmental Behavior Scale [94] and an additional item [95] | Acceptable | Average | [63] |
7 items Ad Hoc [71] | Not available | Poor | [71] |
Ad Hoc question [96] | Not available | Poor | [96] |
A measure of the frequency of engaging in 22 Pro-Environmental Behaviors [89] | Good | Poor | [126] |
References
- Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change (IPCC). Climate Change 2022: Impacts, Adaptation and Vulnerability. Contribution of Working Group II to the Sixth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change; Pörtner, H.-O., Roberts, D.C., Tignor, M.M.B., Poloczanska, E.S., Mintenbeck, K., Alegría, A., Craig, M., Langsdorf, S., Löschke, S., Möller, V., et al., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2022. [Google Scholar]
- Bilotta, N.; Botti, F. Paving the Way for Greener Central Banks. In Current Trends and Future Developments around the Globe; Edizioni Nuova Cultura for Istituto Affari Internazionali (IAI): Rome, Italy, 2022. [Google Scholar]
- Atwoli, L.; Erhabor, G.E.; Gbakima, A.A.; Haileamlak, A.; Ntumba, J.-M.K.; Kigera, J.; Laybourn-Langton, L.; Mash, R.; Muhia, J.; Mulaudzi, F.M.; et al. COP27 Climate Change Conference: Urgent Action Needed for Africa and the World. Lancet Oncol. 2022, 23, 1486–1488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Masud, M.M.; Akhtar, R.; Afroz, R.; Al-Amin, A.Q.; Kari, F.B. Pro-Environmental Behavior and Public Understanding of Climate Change. Mitig. Adapt. Strateg. Glob. Change 2015, 20, 591–600. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Axelrod, L. Balancing Personal Needs with Environmental Preservation: Identifying the Values That Guide Decisions in Ecological Dilemmas. J. Soc. Issues 1994, 50, 85–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pruneau, D.; Doyon, A.; Langis, J.; Vasseur, L.; Ouellet, E.; McLaughlin, E.; Boudreau, G.; Martin, G. When Teachers Adopt Environmental Behaviors in the Aim of Protecting the Climate. J. Environ. Educ. 2006, 37, 3–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mayer, F.S.; Frantz, C.M. The Connectedness to Nature Scale: A Measure of Individuals’ Feeling in Community with Nature. J. Environ. Psychol. 2004, 24, 503–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kurisu, K. What Are Pro-Environmental Behaviors (PEBs)? In Pro-Environmental Behaviors; Kurisu, K., Ed.; Springer: Tokyo, Japan, 2015; pp. 1–26. ISBN 978-4-431-55834-7. [Google Scholar]
- Hong, Z.; Park, I.K. The Effects of Regional Characteristics and Policies on Individual Pro-Environmental Behavior in China. Sustainability 2018, 10, 3586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wall, G. Barriers to Individual Environmental Action: The Influence of Attitudes and Social Experiences. Can. Rev. Sociol. Rev. Can. Sociol. 1995, 32, 465–489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ryland, E. Gaia Rising: A Jungian Look at Environmental Consciousness and Sustainable Organizations. Organ. Environ. 2000, 13, 381–402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hardin, G. The Tragedy of the Commons. Science 1968, 162, 1243–1248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ajzen, I. The Theory of Planned Behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 1991, 50, 179–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hines, J.M.; Hungerford, H.R.; Tomera, A.N. Analysis and Synthesis of Research on Responsible Environmental Behavior: A Meta-Analysis. J. Environ. Educ. 1987, 18, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hungerford, H.R.; Volk, T.L. Changing Learner Behavior Through Environmental Education. J. Environ. Educ. 1990, 21, 8–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Allen, J.B.; Ferrand, J.L. Environmental Locus of Control, Sympathy, and Proenvironmental Behavior: A Test of Geller’s Actively Caring Hypothesis. Environ. Behav. 1999, 31, 338–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stern, P.C.; Dietz, T.; Abel, T.; Guagnano, G.A.; Kalof, L. A Value-Belief-Norm Theory of Support for Social Movements: The Case of Environmentalism. Hum. Ecol. Rev. 1999, 6, 81–97. [Google Scholar]
- Chernev, A.; Blair, S. Doing Well by Doing Good: The Benevolent Halo of Corporate Social Responsibility. J. Consum. Res. 2015, 41, 1412–1425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barragan-Jason, G.; Loreau, M.; de Mazancourt, C.; Singer, M.C.; Parmesan, C. Psychological and Physical Connections with Nature Improve Both Human Well-Being and Nature Conservation: A Systematic Review of Meta-Analyses. Biol. Conserv. 2023, 277, 109842. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Whitburn, J.; Linklater, W.; Abrahamse, W. Meta-Analysis of Human Connection to Nature and Proenvironmental Behavior. Conserv. Biol. 2020, 34, 180–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Soga, M.; Gaston, K.J. Extinction of Experience: The Loss of Human–Nature Interactions. Front. Ecol. Environ. 2016, 14, 94–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schultz, P.W. Inclusion with Nature: The Psychology Of Human-Nature Relations. In Psychology of Sustainable Development; Schmuck, P., Schultz, W.P., Eds.; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 2002; pp. 61–78. ISBN 978-1-4615-0995-0. [Google Scholar]
- Nisbet, E.K.; Zelenski, J.M.; Murphy, S.A. The Nature Relatedness Scale: Linking Individuals’ Connection with Nature to Environmental Concern and Behavior. Environ. Behav. 2009, 41, 715–740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tam, K.-P. Dispositional Empathy with Nature. J. Environ. Psychol. 2013, 35, 92–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perkins, H.E. Measuring Love and Care for Nature. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010, 30, 455–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davis, J.L.; Green, J.D.; Reed, A. Interdependence with the Environment: Commitment, Interconnectedness, and Environmental Behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 2009, 29, 173–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dutcher, D.D.; Finley, J.C.; Luloff, A.E.; Johnson, J.B. Connectivity with Nature as a Measure of Environmental Values. Environ. Behav. 2007, 39, 474–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kals, E.; Schumacher, D.; Montada, L. Emotional Affinity toward Nature as a Motivational Basis to Protect Nature. Environ. Behav. 1999, 31, 178–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clayton, S.; Opotow, S. Identity and the Natural Environment: The Psychological Significance of Nature; The MIT Press: Cambridge, MT, USA, 2003; ISBN 978-0-262-27046-5. [Google Scholar]
- Kellert, S.R.; Heerwagen, J.; Mador, M. Biophilic Design: The Theory, Science and Practice of Bringing Buildings to Life; John Wiley &Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2011; ISBN 9780470163344. [Google Scholar]
- Seddon, N.; Chausson, A.; Berry, P.; Girardin, C.A.J.; Smith, A.; Turner, B. Understanding the Value and Limits of Nature-Based Solutions to Climate Change and Other Global Challenges. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2020, 375, 20190120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marsh, H.W. The Structure of Academic Self-Concept: The Marsh/Shavelson Model. J. Educ. Psychol. 1990, 82, 623–636. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tauber, P.G. An Exploration of the Relationships among Connectedness to Nature, Quality of Life, and Mental Health-ProQuest. Master’s Thesis, Utah State University, Logan, UT, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Geng, L.; Xu, J.; Ye, L.; Zhou, W.; Zhou, K. Connections with Nature and Environmental Behaviors. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0127247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pereira, M.; Forster, P. The Relationship Between Connectedness to Nature, Environmental Values, and Pro-Environmental Behaviours. Reinvention Int. J. Undergrad. Res. 2015, 8. [Google Scholar]
- Wilson, E.O. Biophilia; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1984; ISBN 978-0-674-07442-2. [Google Scholar]
- Kellert, S.R.; Wilson, E.O. The Biophilia Hypothesis; Island Press: Cambridge, DC, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Klassen, M.J. Connectedness to Nature: Comparing Rural and Urban Youths’ Relationships with Nature; Library and Archives Canada = Bibliothèque et Archives Canada: Ottawa, ON, Canada, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Ashmore, R.D.; Deaux, K.; McLaughlin-Volpe, T. An Organizing Framework for Collective Identity: Articulation and Significance of Multidimensionality. Psychol. Bull. 2004, 130, 80–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martin, C.; Czellar, S. Where Do Biospheric Values Come from? A Connectedness to Nature Perspective. J. Environ. Psychol. 2017, 52, 56–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Olivos, P.; Aragonés, J.I.; Amérigo, M. The connectedness to nature scale and its relationship with environmental beliefs and identity. Int. J. Hisp. Psychol. 2011, 4, 5–19. [Google Scholar]
- Dong, X.; Liu, S.; Li, H.; Yang, Z.; Liang, S.; Deng, N. Love of Nature as a Mediator between Connectedness to Nature and Sustainable Consumption Behavior. J. Clean. Prod. 2020, 242, 118451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pearce, J.; Huang, S.S.; Dowling, R.K.; Smith, A.J. Effects of Social and Personal Norms, and Connectedness to Nature, on pro-Environmental Behavior: A Study of Western Australian Protected Area Visitors. Tour. Manag. Perspect. 2022, 42, 100966. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pennisi, L.; Lackey, N.Q.; Holland, S.M. Can an Immersion Exhibit Inspire Connection to Nature and Environmentally Responsible Behavior? J. Interpret. Res. 2017, 22, 35–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pasca, L. Pride and Guilt as Mediators in the Relationship between Connection to Nature and Pro-Environmental Intention. Clim. Change 2022, 175, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Melson, G.F. Why the Wild Things Are: Animals in the Lives of Children. In Why the Wild Things Are; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2009; ISBN 978-0-674-04092-2. [Google Scholar]
- Shepard, P. (Ed.) The Others: How Animals Made Us Human; Island Press: Cambridge, DC, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Stilgoe, J.R. Gone Barefoot Lately? Am. J. Prev. Med. 2001, 20, 243–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Louv, R. Last Child in the Woods: Saving Our Children from Nature-Deficit Disorder; Algonquin Books: Chapel Hill, NC, USA, 2005; ISBN 978-1-56512-391-5. [Google Scholar]
- Schultz, P.W. New Environmental Theories: Empathizing with Nature: The Effects ofPerspective Taking on Concern for Environmental Issues. J. Soc. Issues 2000, 56, 391–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vining, J.; Merrick, M.S.; Price, E.A. The Distinction between Humans and Nature: Human Perceptions of Connectedness to Nature and Elements of the Natural and Unnatural. Hum. Ecol. Rev. 2008, 15, 1–11. [Google Scholar]
- Festinger, L.; Riecken, H.W.; Schacher, S. When Prophecy Fails; University of Minnesota Press: Minneapolis, MN, USA, 1956. [Google Scholar]
- Cialdini, R.B.; Brown, S.L.; Lewis, B.P.; Luce, C.; Neuberg, S.L. Reinterpreting the Empathy–Altruism Relationship: When One into One Equals Oneness. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1997, 73, 481–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Raymond, C.M.; Brown, G.; Robinson, G.M. The Influence of Place Attachment, and Moral and Normative Concerns on the Conservation of Native Vegetation: A Test of Two Behavioural Models. J. Environ. Psychol. 2011, 31, 323–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bruni, C.M.; Schultz, P.W. Implicit Beliefs about Self and Nature: Evidence from an IAT Game. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010, 30, 95–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bruni, C.M.; Chance, R.C.; Schultz, P.W.; Nolan, J.M. Natural Connections: Bees Sting and Snakes Bite, But They Are Still Nature. Environ. Behav. 2012, 44, 197–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Restall, B.; Conrad, E. A Literature Review of Connectedness to Nature and Its Potential for Environmental Management. J. Environ. Manag. 2015, 159, 264–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Moher, D.; Liberati, A.; Tetzlaff, J.; Altman, D.G.; The PRISMA Group. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses: The PRISMA Statement. Ann. Intern. Med. 2009, 151, 264–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bramer, W.M.; Rethlefsen, M.L.; Kleijnen, J.; Franco, O.H. Optimal Database Combinations for Literature Searches in Systematic Reviews: A Prospective Exploratory Study. Syst. Rev. 2017, 6, 245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Higgins, J.; Thomas, J.; Chandler, J.; Cumpston, M.; Li, T.; Page, M.J.; Welch, V.A. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions, 2nd ed.; John Wiley & Sons, Incorporated: Newark, NJ, USA, 2019; ISBN 978-1-119-53662-8. [Google Scholar]
- Hongxin, W.; Khan, M.A.; Zhenqiang, J.; Cismaș, L.-M.; Ali, M.A.; Saleem, U.; Negruț, L. Unleashing the Role of CSR and Employees’ Pro-Environmental Behavior for Organizational Success: The Role of Connectedness to Nature. Sustainability 2022, 14, 3191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perrin, J.L.; Benassi, V.A. The Connectedness to Nature Scale: A Measure of Emotional Connection to Nature? J. Environ. Psychol. 2009, 29, 434–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Apaolaza, V.; Paredes, M.R.; Hartmann, P.; Barrutia, J.M.; Echebarria, C. How Does Mindfulness Relate to Proenvironmental Behavior? The Mediating Influence of Cognitive Reappraisal and Climate Change Awareness. J. Clean. Prod. 2022, 357, 131914. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cheng, J.C.-H.; Monroe, M.C. Connection to Nature: Children’s Affective Attitude Toward Nature. Environ. Behav. 2012, 44, 31–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krettenauer, T. Pro-Environmental Behavior and Adolescent Moral Development. J. Res. Adolesc. 2017, 27, 581–593. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brügger, A.; Kaiser, F.G.; Roczen, N. One All? Eur. Psychol. 2011, 10, 324–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wesley, S.P. The structure of environmental concern: Concern for self, other people, and the biosphere. J. Environ. Psychol. 2001, 21, 327–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hunt, A.; Stewart, D.; Richardson, M.; Hinds, J.; Bragg, R.; White, M.; Burt, J. Monitor of Engagement with the Natural Environment: Developing a Method to Measure Nature Connection across the English Population (Adults and Children); 2017; Available online: https://publications.naturalengland.org.uk/file/6167023385575424 (accessed on 14 April 2025).
- Nisbet, E.K.; Zelenski, J.M. The NR-6: A New Brief Measure of Nature Relatedness. Front. Psychol. 2013, 4, 813. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gosling, E.; Williams, K.J.H. Connectedness to Nature, Place Attachment and Conservation Behaviour: Testing Connectedness Theory among Farmers. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010, 30, 298–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Iverson, A. Do Adults Who Attend a Family Nature Event Have Correlations Between Environmental Responsible Behaviors, Connection to Nature, and Environmental Attitudes? Bachelor’s Thesis, Department of Environmental Studies, Delhi, India, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Romild, U. En Studie Om Fritidsaktiviteter Och Naturupplevelser; Mittuniversitet: Östersund, Sweden, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Kaiser, F.G.; Wilson, M. Goal-Directed Conservation Behavior: The Specific Composition of a General Performance. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2004, 36, 1531–1544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaiser, F.G. A General Measure of Ecological Behavior. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1998, 28, 395–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krettenauer, T.; Wang, W.; Jia, F.; Yao, Y. Connectedness with Nature and the Decline of Pro-Environmental Behavior in Adolescence: A Comparison of Canada and China. J. Environ. Psychol. 2020, 71, 101348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duron-Ramos, M.F.; Collado, S.; García-Vázquez, F.I.; Bello-Echeverria, M. The Role of Urban/Rural Environments on Mexican Children’s Connection to Nature and Pro-Environmental Behavior. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Barrera-Hernández, L.F.; Sotelo-Castillo, M.A.; Echeverría-Castro, S.B.; Tapia-Fonllem, C.O. Connectedness to Nature: Its Impact on Sustainable Behaviors and Happiness in Children. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fraijo Sing, B.S.; Corral Verdugo, V.; Tapia Fonllem, C.; García Vázquez, F. Adaptation and Testing of a Scale of Orientation to Sustainability among Sixth Graders. Rev. Mex. Investig. Educ. 2012, 17, 1091–1117. [Google Scholar]
- Whitmarsh, L.; O’Neill, S. Green Identity, Green Living? The Role of pro-Environmental Self-Identity in Determining Consistency across Diverse pro-Environmental Behaviours. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010, 30, 305–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Markle, G.L. Pro-Environmental Behavior: Does It Matter How It’s Measured? Development and Validation of the Pro-Environmental Behavior Scale (PEBS). Hum. Ecol. 2013, 41, 905–914. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Larson, L.R.; Stedman, R.C.; Cooper, C.B.; Decker, D.J. Understanding the Multi-Dimensional Structure of pro-Environmental Behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 2015, 43, 112–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schultz, P.W.; Gouveia, V.V.; Cameron, L.D.; Tankha, G.; Schmuck, P.; Franěk, M. Values and Their Relationship to Environmental Concern and Conservation Behavior. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 2005, 36, 457–475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shen, L.-J. Character and Relationship Research on Undergraduates’ Environmental Values, Attitudes and Behaviors. Master’s Thesis, Shanxi University, Taiyuan, China, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Kaiser, F.G.; Oerke, B.; Bogner, F.X. Behavior-Based Environmental Attitude: Development of an Instrument for Adolescents. J. Environ. Psychol. 2007, 27, 242–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Robertson, J.L.; Barling, J. Greening Organizations through Leaders’ Influence on Employees’ pro-Environmental Behaviors. J. Organ. Behav. 2013, 34, 176–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alisat, S.; Riemer, M. The Environmental Action Scale: Development and Psychometric Evaluation. J. Environ. Psychol. 2015, 43, 13–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaiser, F.G.; Wölfing, S.; Fuhrer, U. Environmental attitude and ecological behaviour. J. Environ. Psychol. 1999, 19, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Menardo, E.; Brondino, M.; Pasini, M. Adaptation and Psychometric Properties of the Italian Version of the Pro-Environmental Behaviours Scale (PEBS). Environ. Dev. Sustain. 2020, 22, 6907–6930. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Whitmarsh, L.; Capstick, S.; Nash, N. Who Is Reducing Their Material Consumption and Why? A Cross-Cultural Analysis of Dematerialization Behaviours. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. A Math. Phys. Eng. Sci. 2017, 375, 20160376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Binder, M.; Blankenberg, A.-K.; Guardiola, J. Does It Have to Be a Sacrifice? Different Notions of the Good Life, pro-Environmental Behavior and Their Heterogeneous Impact on Well-Being. Ecol. Econ. 2020, 167, 106448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tonge, J.; Ryan, M.M.; Moore, S.A.; Beckley, L.E. The Effect of Place Attachment on Pro-Environment Behavioral Intentions of Visitors to Coastal Natural Area Tourist Destinations. J. Travel Res. 2015, 54, 730–743. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kim, A.K.; Airey, D.; Szivas, E. The Multiple Assessment of Interpretation Effectiveness: Promoting Visitors’ Environmental Attitudes and Behavior. J. Travel Res. 2011, 50, 321–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Halpenny, E.A. Pro-Environmental Behaviours and Park Visitors: The Effect of Place Attachment. J. Environ. Psychol. 2010, 30, 409–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Markowitz, E.M.; Goldberg, L.R.; Ashton, M.C.; Lee, K. Profiling the “Pro-Environmental Individual”: A Personality Perspective. J. Personal. 2012, 80, 81–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Panno, A.; Carrus, G.; Maricchiolo, F.; Mannetti, L. Cognitive Reappraisal and Pro-Environmental Behavior: The Role of Global Climate Change Perception. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2015, 45, 858–867. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Despotović, J.; Rodić, V.; Caracciolo, F. Farmers’ Environmental Awareness: Construct Development, Measurement, and Use. J. Clean. Prod. 2021, 295, 126378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martin, L.; White, M.P.; Hunt, A.; Richardson, M.; Pahl, S.; Burt, J. Nature Contact, Nature Connectedness and Associations with Health, Wellbeing and pro-Environmental Behaviours. J. Environ. Psychol. 2020, 68, 101389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ibáñez-Rueda, N.; Guillén-Royo, M.; Guardiola, J. Pro-Environmental Behavior, Connectedness to Nature, and Wellbeing Dimensions among Granada Students. Sustainability 2020, 12, 9171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aromataris, E.; Lockwood, C.; Porritt, K.; Pilla, B.; Jordan, Z. JBI Manual for Evidence Synthesis-JBI Global Wiki. Available online: https://jbi-global-wiki.refined.site/space/MANUAL (accessed on 7 April 2025).
- Dubey, A.; Shukla, J. Relationship between Connectedness to Nature and Pro-Environmental Behaviour of University Students and Professional in India. J. Adv. Res. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2020, 3, 19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gkargkavouzi, A.; Halkos, G.; Matsiori, S. A Multi-Dimensional Measure of Environmental Behavior: Exploring the Predictive Power of Connectedness to Nature, Ecological Worldview and Environmental Concern. Soc. Indic. Res. 2019, 143, 859–879. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gkargkavouzi, A.; Paraskevopoulos, S.; Matsiori, S. Assessing the Structure and Correlations of Connectedness to Nature, Environmental Concerns and Environmental Behavior in a Greek Context. Curr. Psychol. 2021, 40, 154–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gignac, G.E.; Szodorai, E.T. Effect Size Guidelines for Individual Differences Researchers. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2016, 102, 74–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beery, T.H.; Wolf-Watz, D. Nature to Place: Rethinking the Environmental Connectedness Perspective. J. Environ. Psychol. 2014, 40, 198–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brick, C.; Sherman, D.K.; Kim, H.S. “Green to Be Seen” and “Brown to Keep down”: Visibility Moderates the Effect of Identity on pro-Environmental Behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 2017, 51, 226–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, C.; Geng, L.; Rodríguez-Casallas, J.D. How and When Higher Climate Change Risk Perception Promotes Less Climate Change Inaction. J. Clean. Prod. 2021, 321, 128952. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pong, V.; Tam, K.-P. Relationship between Global Identity and Pro-Environmental Behavior and Environmental Concern: A Systematic Review. Front. Psychol. 2023, 14, 1033564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Balundė, A.; Perlaviciute, G.; Truskauskaitė-Kunevičienė, I. Sustainability in Youth: Environmental Considerations in Adolescence and Their Relationship to Pro-Environmental Behavior. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 582920. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lindenberg, S.; Steg, L. Normative, Gain and Hedonic Goal Frames Guiding Environmental Behavior. J. Soc. Issues 2007, 63, 117–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Steg, L.; Bolderdijk, J.W.; Keizer, K.; Perlaviciute, G. An Integrated Framework for Encouraging Pro-Environmental Behaviour: The Role of Values, Situational Factors and Goals. J. Environ. Psychol. 2014, 38, 104–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spangenberger, P.; Geiger, S.M.; Freytag, S.-C. Becoming Nature: Effects of Embodying a Tree in Immersive Virtual Reality on Nature Relatedness. Sci. Rep. 2022, 12, 1311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sneed, J.C.; Deringer, S.A.; Hanley, A. Nature Connection and 360-Degree Video: An Exploratory Study with Immersive Technology. J. Exp. Educ. 2021, 44, 378–394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barbaro, N.; Pickett, S.M. Mindfully Green: Examining the Effect of Connectedness to Nature on the Relationship between Mindfulness and Engagement in pro-Environmental Behavior. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2016, 93, 137–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Arendt, F.; Matthes, J. Nature Documentaries, Connectedness to Nature, and Pro-Environmental Behavior. Environ. Commun. 2016, 10, 453–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosa, C.D.; Profice, C.C.; Collado, S. Nature Experiences and Adults’ Self-Reported Pro-Environmental Behaviors: The Role of Connectedness to Nature and Childhood Nature Experiences. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1055. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Krizanova, J.; Rosenfeld, D.L.; Tomiyama, A.J.; Guardiola, J. Pro-Environmental Behavior Predicts Adherence to Plant-Based Diets. Appetite 2021, 163, 105243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schultz, P.W.; Shriver, C.; Tabanico, J.J.; Khazian, A.M. Implicit Connections with Nature. J. Environ. Psychol. 2004, 24, 31–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Solano-Pinto, N.; Garrido, D.; Gértrudix-Barrio, F.; Fernández-Cézar, R. Is Knowledge of Circular Economy, Pro-Environmental Behavior, Satisfaction with Life, and Beliefs a Predictor of Connectedness to Nature in Rural Children and Adolescents? A Pilot Study. Sustainability 2020, 12, 9951. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Anderson, D.J.; Krettenauer, T. Connectedness to Nature and Pro-Environmental Behaviour from Early Adolescence to Adulthood: A Comparison of Urban and Rural Canada. Sustainability 2021, 13, 3655. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Whitburn, J.; Linklater, W.L.; Milfont, T.L. Exposure to Urban Nature and Tree Planting Are Related to Pro-Environmental Behavior via Connection to Nature, the Use of Nature for Psychological Restoration, and Environmental Attitudes. Environ. Behav. 2019, 51, 787–810. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dellavia, L. The Impact of Time Outdoors on Pro-Environmental Behaviours as a Function of Child and Teacher Connectedness to Nature. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Adelaide, Adelaide, Australia, 2021. [Google Scholar]
- Molinario, E.; Lorenzi, C.; Bartoccioni, F.; Perucchini, P.; Bobeth, S.; Colléony, A.; Diniz, R.; Eklund, A.; Jaeger, C.; Kibbe, A.; et al. From Childhood Nature Experiences to Adult Pro-Environmental Behaviors: An Explanatory Model of Sustainable Food Consumption. Environ. Educ. Res. 2020, 26, 1137–1163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Diessner, R.; Genthôs, R.; Praest, K.; Pohling, R. Identifying with Nature Mediates the Influence of Valuing Nature’s Beauty on Proenvironmental Behaviors. Ecopsychology 2018, 10, 97–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zamò, E. Comportamenti Pro-Ambientali: Il Ruolo Del Genere e Delle Caratteristiche Individuali in Relazione All’ambiente. Bachelor’s Thesis, Padua University, Padova, Italy, 2022. [Google Scholar]
- Aruta, J.J.B.R. Connectedness to Nature Encourages, but Materialism Hinders, Ecological Behavior in the Philippines: The Higher Order and Second-Order Factors of Environmental Attitudes as Viable Mediating Pathways. Ecopsychology 2021, 13, 114–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilkie, S.; Trotter, H. Pro-Environmental Attitudes, pro-Environmental Behaviours and Nature-Relatedness: Differences Based on Place Preference. Eur. Rev. Appl. Psychol. 2022, 72, 100705. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hanley, A.W.; Bettmann, J.E.; Kendrick, C.E.; Deringer, A.; Norton, C.L. Dispositional Mindfulness Is Associated with Greater Nature Connectedness and Self-Reported Ecological Behavior. Ecopsychology 2020, 12, 54–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tam, K.-P. Concepts and Measures Related to Connection to Nature: Similarities and Differences. J. Environ. Psychol. 2013, 34, 64–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Guazzini, A.; Valdrighi, G.; Fiorenza, M.; Duradoni, M. The Relationship Between Connectedness to Nature and Pro-Environmental Behaviors: A Systematic Review. Sustainability 2025, 17, 3686. https://doi.org/10.3390/su17083686
Guazzini A, Valdrighi G, Fiorenza M, Duradoni M. The Relationship Between Connectedness to Nature and Pro-Environmental Behaviors: A Systematic Review. Sustainability. 2025; 17(8):3686. https://doi.org/10.3390/su17083686
Chicago/Turabian StyleGuazzini, Andrea, Giulia Valdrighi, Maria Fiorenza, and Mirko Duradoni. 2025. "The Relationship Between Connectedness to Nature and Pro-Environmental Behaviors: A Systematic Review" Sustainability 17, no. 8: 3686. https://doi.org/10.3390/su17083686
APA StyleGuazzini, A., Valdrighi, G., Fiorenza, M., & Duradoni, M. (2025). The Relationship Between Connectedness to Nature and Pro-Environmental Behaviors: A Systematic Review. Sustainability, 17(8), 3686. https://doi.org/10.3390/su17083686