Next Article in Journal
Association of Urinary Potassium Excretion with Blood Pressure Variability and Cardiovascular Outcomes in Patients with Pre-Dialysis Chronic Kidney Disease
Previous Article in Journal
Protective Effect of Lactiplantibacillus plantarum 1201 Combined with Galactooligosaccharide on Carbon Tetrachloride-Induced Acute Liver Injury in Mice
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Impact of Household Chaos and Dietary Intake on Executive Function in Young Children

1
Department of Human Development and Family Studies, University of Illinois, Urbana, IL 61801, USA
2
Division of Nutritional Sciences, University of Illinois, Urbana, IL 61801, USA
3
Department of Food Science and Human Nutrition, University of Illinois, Urbana, IL 61801, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2021, 13(12), 4442; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13124442
Submission received: 2 November 2021 / Revised: 8 December 2021 / Accepted: 9 December 2021 / Published: 12 December 2021
(This article belongs to the Section Pediatric Nutrition)

Abstract

:
Children’s executive functions (EFs) emerge over time and can be shaped by household environments and dietary intake. However, there is a lack of knowledge about how these factors influence EFs in children aged 18–24 months. This study tested a model exploring the relations between parent-reported dietary intake, household chaos, and child EF. The sample consisted of 294 families participating in the STRONG Kids2 birth cohort study of nutrition and child health. Caregivers completed the Food Frequency Questionnaire (FFQ), the Confusion, Hubbub, and Order Scale (CHAOS), and the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function®-Preschool Version (BRIEF-P) to assess model variables. Regression analyses revealed a significant and independent association between assorted snacks and processed foods and two EF subscales. There were also significant associations between household chaos and each EF subscale. There was no significant moderation effect. These findings suggest that family households characterized by dysregulation are associated with children’s EF difficulties during early childhood and that the role of unhealthy dietary intake in child EF should be explored further. Future longitudinal studies that include multi-method approaches are needed to document the mechanisms through which household chaos impacts child EF over time.

1. Introduction

Executive function (EF) consists of an array of higher-order cognitive skills that have been associated with various forms of optimal functioning [1]. EF abilities involve processes that govern thoughts and behaviors, which can influence how children react to situations and create relationships across contexts. In addition to genetic contributions [2,3,4,5], dietary intake and household environment factors have been shown to influence children’s EF abilities. For example, the overall caregiving environment and household are thought to be critical in promoting the development of children’s EFs [6,7,8,9]. In addition, research has shown that young children who eat more healthy foods and fewer snack foods or processed meats tend to have more optimal EF abilities [10,11]. With respect to household environments, the literature suggests that higher levels of household chaos (settings high in noise and with fewer routines) is associated with lower executive function abilities, such as limited ability to focus or control one’s emotions [12,13], and may indirectly predict behavioral regulation in children [14].
Although these aspects of a child’s environment may influence EF development, there is less known about the interplay between dietary intake and household environment factors as influences on children’s EF. To address this gap in the literature, we tested a model examining parent-reported dietary intake and child EF, and household chaos was tested as a moderator.

1.1. Executive Function

EF encompasses general-purpose control mechanisms that are often linked to the brain’s prefrontal cortex (PFC). The literature shows that EF may regulate and influence the dynamics of cognition, actions, and create meaning for various ages [1,15]. These processes develop throughout childhood and may play a role in a child’s social, emotional, and physical interactions and relationships [16,17]. The frontal lobes, specifically in the PFC, send and receive information from major sensory and motor control regions, and the PFC is an essential brain structure that oversees and manages neural systems located in cortical and subcortical areas. The PFC continually monitors activities in the cortical and subcortical regions while sending signals to execute certain behaviors [18]. Thus, these signals can influence an individual’s executive function abilities and behaviors.
The literature presents three core dimensions of EF: Inhibitory Control, Working Memory, and Cognitive Flexibility or set-shifting. These processes can control goal-directed actions and responses to complex or significant situations [19,20]. They may consist of reasoning, problem-solving, and organizing within a child’s life [15,21,22,23]. These EF abilities and skills are crucial for success in school and social groups, cognitive development, and physical and mental health [3]. The ability to alter behavioral responses, shift attention, control emotions or feelings, plan or organize, and use one’s working memory all fall within EF’s dimensions. Inhibitory control includes the ability to control one’s emotions, attention, and behaviors while doing what may be appropriate or needed in a situation [3]. Shift refers to the ability to move from one situation or aspect of a problem to another. Emotional control involves difficulties in emotional expression and a child’s ability to control one’s emotions and feelings. Planning/organizing refers to the child’s ability to manage current or future tasks in various situations. Lastly, working memory involves the capacity to hold information in memory for goals or plans. Collectively, these aspects reflect the broader construct of EF.

1.2. Dietary Intake and Children’s EF

Nutrition plays an essential role in a child’s healthy development, including EF capacities. Researchers have found evidence suggesting potential bidirectional links between EF and dietary intake. Riggs et al. (2010) found that in fourth-grade students (Mage = 9.4), EF proficiency was negatively related to snack food intake. They discussed how youth with enhanced cognitive abilities and emotional control skills might be better at inhibiting the cognitive and emotional rewards that may come with snack food. They also explain that youth with stronger working memory skills may have more goals to eat healthier foods [24]. This study illustrates that cognition may impact dietary intake on multiple levels. The literature also shows that creative thinking and working memory may be affected by added sugar and dietary fiber. For example, in preadolescent children, added sugar intake was negatively associated with tests of creative thinking, and dietary fiber was positively associated with overall creative thinking [25]. Additionally, researchers found that executive cognition-function in fourth-grade children was negatively associated with high-calorie snack food intake and positively associated with fruit and vegetable intake [26].
A recent systematic review exploring young and old adolescents and older children found a positive association between healthy food intake and EF, including whole grains, fish, fruits, and vegetables. In addition, less nutritious food, including snack foods, sugar-sweetened beverages, and processed meats, were inversely related to EF [10]. The studies measured various dimensions of EF, including inhibition, working memory, and attention and planning. When explicitly examining inhibition in adolescents, researchers found a positive association between inhibitory problems, poor decision-making, and intake of sweet drinks and snack food [27]. In older children and young adolescents, intake of mixed grains was beneficial for cognitive performance [28], and poorer diet quality was associated with worse cognition [29]. In addition, increased fish intake, using a cluster-randomized cross-over trial, explained increases in reading and inattention performance in third- and fourth-grade children (aged 8–11 years) [30].
Overall, study findings suggest positive associations between healthy food intake and children’s EF abilities, and negative associations between snack, processed, or more unhealthy food intake and children’s EF abilities. However, most studies were conducted with older children and adolescent samples and focused on school-related executive function tasks. The current study focuses on a younger population (18–24 months), which can help researchers and community members understand the effect of dietary intake on children’s EFs at a younger age when these skills are rapidly developing. These findings might also shed light on how influential the caregivers can be, primarily when they are making critical food-related decisions for their children.

1.3. Household Chaos

Household chaos describes an environment that is high in noise and crowding and low in regularity and routines [14]. Researchers have found that household chaos is associated with various outcomes, such as behavior problems, limited attentional focusing, reduced ability to understand and act in certain social situations, and reduced accuracy and efficiency in cooperative parent-child tasks [12]. Additionally, researchers have found a direct association between higher levels of household chaos and poor performances on tasks that are related to core dimensions of executive functioning, such as inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility, working memory, and effortful control [20,31,32,33,34]. Higher levels of household chaos may also interfere with a child’s proficiency levels, creating a lack of control within their environment [35]. Furthermore, household disorganization may also negatively influence working memory, attention shifting, and other forms of inhibitory control [36], and a lack of routine has often been associated with poor performance on EF tasks in kindergartners [37]. Lastly, a recent meta-analysis found evidence that household chaos is significantly and negatively associated with child executive functioning [31].
Furthermore, positive home environments (low family conflict, high family cohesion, and low household chaos) have been associated with healthier food-related behaviors. More negative home environments (high family conflict, low family cohesion, and increased household chaos) have been associated with more unhealthy food-related behaviors [38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45].
Taken together, prior research has demonstrated that both unhealthy dietary intake in children and chaotic living environments may impact a variety of children’s EF skills. However, there is less examination of how these factors, together, influence EF skills in young children. Dysregulated environments may exacerbate the effects of unhealthy dietary intake on EF, or low household chaos may buffer these effects. Examining the interplay between household chaos and dietary intake and their influence on early EF skills can help shed light on modifiable factors in the environment and diet that can promote developing EF capacities.

1.4. Goal of the Present Study

This study aimed to examine the relation between children’s dietary intake, household chaos, and EF abilities. Because these children were 18–24 months of age, we wanted to explore food groups and dietary intake patterns when caregivers are the primary source of food availability. The study also aimed to examine whether household chaos moderates these associations. First, we examined the association between six dietary intake patterns and children’s EFs. We hypothesized that children with a healthier dietary intake would have higher EF scores. Second, we determined whether household chaos moderated the association between dietary intake and EF subscales. We hypothesized that lower household chaos would buffer children from the effects of a less healthy dietary intake.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

The data in this analysis are from families participating in the STRONG Kids2 (SK2) longitudinal birth cohort study in the Midwestern United States. It is designed to examine multi-level predictors of weight trajectories, dietary habits, and family relationships over the first seven years of life [46]. As part of the more extensive study, caregivers completed surveys regarding their household chaos, child EF behaviors, child dietary intake, and demographics for both the caregiver and child. This study was approved by the University Institutional Review Board. Participant characteristics can be found in Table 1.

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Executive Function

EF was assessed using the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Preschool Version (BRIEF-P) [17]. This survey assesses multiple dimensions of EF, including inhibition, shifting, emotional control, working memory, and planning/organizing. Parents are asked to complete this questionnaire based on how often each behavior has been an issue in their child’s life during the last six months, and these items are ranged from 1 (never) to 3 (often). Example statements include: “is easily overwhelmed or overstimulated by typical daily activities”, “gets out of control more than playmates”, and “talks or plays too loudly.” Five subscales (listed above), three broad index scores (Inhibitory Self-Control, Flexibility, and Emergent Metacognition), and one global composition score are also obtained from the BRIEF-P. Two validity scales (Inconsistency and Negativity) can also be acquired. The Inhibitory Self-Control Index (ISCI) combines the inhibit and emotional control subscales, the Flexibility Index (FI) is a sum of the shift and emotional control subscales, and the Emergent Metacognition Index (EMI) incorporates the working memory and plan/organize subscales. The global composition score (GEC) consolidates all subscales to receive a total score. Higher scores on the BRIEF-P indicate worse performance on EF abilities. For this study, the ISCI, FI, and EMI indexes will be used for all analyses. Adequate internal reliability was demonstrated for the ISCI (Cronbach’s α = 0.92), FI (Cronbach’s α = 0.88), and EMI subscales (Cronbach’s α = 0.93).

2.2.2. Dietary Intake

Child dietary intake was assessed using a child block of the Food Frequency Questionnaire (FFQ) developed by Nutrition Quest [47]. The FFQ measures the intake of fruits, vegetables, fats, proteins, and dairy in children 2 to 7 years of age. Parents complete the 90-item questionnaire in response to their child’s “usual eating habits in the past six months”. Example items include banana, broccoli, beef, and butter. Questions are rated on an 8-point Likert scale. The scale consists of the following: never, once per month, 2–3 times per month, once per week, twice per week, 3–4 times per week, 5–6 times per week, and every day. The food list was developed from NHANES III dietary recall data, and the USDA Nutrient Database for Standard Reference was also used for nutrient assessment. Based on previous literature, food groups were created to combine related food types [48,49]. This method was used to analyze food groups or profiles instead of specific nutrients specifically. There were 23 food groups, and a principal component analysis was conducted to reduce the dietary intake data. The results revealed six components from the 23 food groups (see description below and Table 2).

2.2.3. Household Chaos

Household chaos and environmental levels were assessed using the Confusion, Hubbub, and Order Scale (CHAOS) [50]. The questionnaire consists of 15 statements surrounding their household environment and chaos. Each question is on a 4-point Likert scale, ranging from “very much like your home” to “not at all like your own home”. Example questions include: “It’s a real zoo in our home”, “Our home is a good place to relax”, and “First thing in the day, we have a regular routine at home”. CHAOS measures two categories of household chaos. The first is routines and organization, and the second facet is disorganization, confusion, and noise. A single score is obtained by summing the items with the highest possible score of 60. A higher score indicates a higher level of chaos within the home or environment. Adequate internal reliability was demonstrated for household chaos (Cronbach’s α = 0.86).

2.2.4. Data Analysis

The Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS; IBM Corp, Armonk, NY, USA) version 27.0 was used for data analysis, and the statistical significance was set at p ≤ 0.05. A PCA was conducted to examine food groups further and create dietary intake model variables. To test associations among all study variables, including demographic variables, bivariate parametric correlations were conducted. Then, a series of multiple regressions were conducted to test the hypothesis that dietary intake and household chaos have a unique and combined impact on executive function. For the moderation analysis, interaction terms were created by multiplying the dietary intake variables and household chaos scores.

3. Results

3.1. Preliminary Analyses

Preliminary analyses were conducted to determine whether demographic variables were associated with executive function variables. Child gender, family income, and parent race/ethnicity were not associated with any EF outcome variables (all p’s > 0.05). Since there were no significant correlations, no demographic variables were used in further analyses.

3.2. FFQ Analyses

To create dietary intake food groups, a principal components analysis (PCA) was conducted. A PCA with varimax rotation was performed using responses to the food groups formed from the FFQ. Six components with Eigenvalues > 1.00 collectively accounted for 57.365% of the variance. A factor loading of 0.4 was used, which was consistent with previous studies [51,52,53,54]. All food groups loaded on a factor using this cutoff. Components can be found in Table 2.
Component 1, Assorted Snacks and Processed Foods, explained 13.93% of the variance and included Savory Snacks, Refined Carbohydrates, Fried Foods, Processed Meats, Mixed Foods, Condiments, and Butter/Margarine. Component 2, Assorted Vegetables, Fruit, and Fish, explained 11.53% of the variance and included Vegetables, Starchy Foods, Legumes, Fruit, and Fish/Seafood. Component 3, Fruit Juice and Sweet Items, contained 100% Fruit Juice, Sweet Beverages, and Sweet Foods. This explained 8.98% of the variance. Component 4, Assorted Proteins, included Poultry, Red Meats, and Eggs, and it explained 8.76% of the variance. Component 5, Grains and Nuts, explained 7.80% of the variance, and contained items directly related to Grains and Peanuts/Nuts. Lastly, Component 6, Assorted Dairy and Water, included Yogurt, Dairy, and Water and explained 6.37% of the variance. Adequate internal reliability was demonstrated for Component 1 (Cronbach’s α = 0.76), Component 2 (Cronbach’s α = 0.65), and Component 3 (Cronbach’s α = 0.55). We used a cutoff of 0.55 due to our sample size and to have adequate reliability for each component [55,56,57]. Therefore, Component 4 (Cronbach’s α = 0.51), Component 5 (Cronbach’s α = 0.47), and Component 6 (Cronbach’s α = 0.26) were not used in the final analyses.

3.3. Bivariate Correlations

Dietary Intake and Child EF

Bivariate associations between all model variables are depicted in Table 3. Assorted Snacks and Processed Foods were positively and significantly associated with household chaos and each EF subscale (all p’s < 0.05). Intake of Assorted Vegetables, Fruit, and Fish was negatively and significantly associated with household chaos r (331) = −0.12, p = 0.03, and no EF subscales. Fruit Juice and Sweet Items were positively and significantly associated with household chaos and all EF subscales (all p’s < 0.05). Assorted Snacks and Processed Foods, and Fruit Juice and Sweet Items were the only subscales used in the final analyses.

3.4. Regression Analysis

Dietary Intake and Child EF

In the second analysis, dietary intake, household chaos, and the interaction term were entered as predictor variables, and each EF index was tested separately as the dependent variable. Regarding the Assorted Snacks and Processed Foods subscale, results revealed that the whole model accounted for about 20% of the variance (Adjusted R2 = 0.20). For the EF ISCI index, household chaos (β = 0.41, p ≤ 0.000) and the Assorted Snacks and Processed Foods subscale (β = 0.13, p = 0.02) were both significant predictors; however, the interaction variable was not significant (see Table 4). Similar results were found for the EMI index (Table 5). However, for the EF FI index, the Assorted Snacks and Processed Foods subscale was not a significant predictor in the model (Table 6). Regarding the Fruit Juice and Sweet Items subscale, results revealed that the whole model accounted for about 18% of the variance (Adjusted R2 = 0.18). For the EF ISCI index, only household chaos (β = 0.41, p ≤ 0.000) was a significant predictor of EF abilities (see Table 7). Similar results were found for EMI and FI indexes (Table 8 and Table 9).

4. Discussion

This study examined associations between dietary intake, household chaos, and EF in children 18–24 months of age. In the final analyses, a significant association between dietary intake and EF for the Assorted Snacks and Processed Foods component was obtained, with higher intake related to lower shift and emotional control abilities, as well as lower working memory and planning and organizing abilities. No other independent associations with other dietary intake variables were observed. Contrary to our hypotheses, results also indicated that household chaos does not modify dietary intake and EF associations but instead has an independent effect on EF. Additionally, household chaos was significantly associated with all three BRIEF-P indexes in the final models. The association between household chaos and poorer EF is consistent with previous literature [12,58,59]. Because household chaos describes an environment that is high in noise and crowding and low in regularity and routines [14], this environment may distract, limit, or alter a child’s EF abilities.
The current findings can contribute to the larger literature in multiple ways. First, we found independent associations between household chaos and child EF, and this highlights how even at a young age (18–24 months) household chaos might impact children’s EF abilities. They may not understand the signals around them when environments are noisy or disorganized, and the lack of routine and regularity might influence their attentional and emotional regulation. Second, these findings can help researchers, policymakers, and families understand how household chaos might influence children’s dietary intake and EF abilities. Informing other scholars, individuals, and policymakers about this phenomenon can help families develop more routines and healthier lifestyles and provide knowledge about how external influences can impact children’s executive functions. Lastly, these results can inform future studies that set out to examine how household chaos may affect other dimensions of EF using various methodologies, designs, and more diverse samples.
With respect to dietary intake, our hypotheses were partially correct regarding associations between Assorted Snacks and Processed Foods and worse performance in all EF subscales. We also saw a similar pattern of relations at a correlational level between the Fruit Juice and Sweet Items subscale and all EF subscales. This indicates that various types of food may impact a child’s performance in EF-related tasks. Researchers found, using NHANES data, that nearly all children aged 2 to 5 years old consumed a snack on a specific day, with 62% occurring in the morning, 84% in the afternoon, and 72% in the evening. They found that these snacks accounted for 28% of total energy intake, and many of these snacks also included beverages consumed within a specific day [60]. These findings further illustrate that snacks and certain beverages are prominent within young children and how we need to be more aware of how influential snacks and beverages can be to a young child’s diet and lifestyle. Consistent with previous research [10,24,26,27,29], diet quality may influence a child’s or adolescent’s performance in EF-related tasks, and this may further influence their cognitive abilities. This warrants further investigation to understand how diet quality affects younger children’s developing EF capacities. Many caregivers assist with feeding when children are young, so future researchers need to examine further how this relationship and assistance might influence a child’s dietary intake.
There are also limitations to this study. The sample was not diverse with respect to race/ethnicity and income. Therefore, the findings are not generalizable, and diverse family contexts and relationships should be examined in the future. No causal inferences can be made regarding the discovered associations based on the correlational design, highlighting the need for longitudinal and experimental methodologies. Future research should incorporate multiple measures of dietary intake, EF, and household chaos to understand these results further.
Despite these limitations, the current findings are novel and contribute to the literature on EF in children aged 18 to 24 months. For example, they further support the notion that the nature of the household environment, specifically household chaos, may influence young children’s developing EF capacities. As such, preventions focused on activities and support for parents to establish healthy routines and lower unhealthy food intake in their children might help mitigate EF problems. Furthermore, the data suggest that children’s unhealthy dietary intake may also be related to the household environment, and these associations warrant future study. These factors might impact a child and family on multiple levels and have implications for children’s developing EF capacities.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, S.I. and K.B.; formal analysis, S.I. and K.B.; funding acquisition, S.M.D., B.F. and K.B.; investigation, S.I. and K.B.; methodology, S.I., S.M.D., B.F. and K.B.; project administration, S.I. and K.B.; supervision, S.M.D., B.F. and K.B.; validation, S.I. and K.B.; writing—original draft, S.I.; writing—review and editing, S.I., S.M.D., B.F. and K.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by grants from the National Dairy Council to Sharon Donovan and Barbara H. Fiese (CoPI’s), the Gerber Foundation to Sharon Donovan, the Christopher Family Foundation to Sharon Donovan and Kelly K. Bost, Hatch ILLU 793-390 from the USDA National Institute of Food and Agriculture to Kelly K. Bost, Hatch ILLU 793-330 from the U.S. Department of Agriculture to Barbara H. Fiese, Kelly K. Bost, and Margarita Teran-Garcia, and the National Institutes of Health DK107561 to Sharon Donovan. The STRONG Kids 2 Team includes Kelly Bost, Sharon Donovan, Soo-Yeun Lee, Brent McBride, Margarita Teran-Garcia, and Barbara H. Fiese.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign. The most recent protocol number is 13448, and the date of approval is 26 October 2021.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the STRONG Kids 2 Birth Cohort study participants and the STRONG Kids 2 Birth Cohort project Team, including Barbara Fiese, Sharon Donovan, Kelly Bost, Soo-Yeun Lee, Brent McBride, and Margarita Teran-Garcia.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Garon, N.; Bryson, S.E.; Smith, I.M. Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychol. Bull. 2008, 134, 31–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Barnes, J.J.; Dean, A.J.; Nandam, L.S.; O’Connell, R.G.; Bellgrove, M.A. The molecular genetics of executive function: Role of monoamine system genes. Biol. Psychiatry 2011, 69, e127–e143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Diamond, A. Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 135–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Gilbert, S.J.; Burgess, P.W. Executive function. Curr. Biol. 2008, 18, R110–R114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Greene, C.M.; Braet, W.; Johnson, K.A.; Bellgrove, M.A. Imaging the genetics of executive function. Biol. Psychol. 2008, 79, 30–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Bernier, A.; Beauchamp, M.H.; Carlson, S.M.; Lalonde, G. A secure base from which to regulate: Attachment security in toddlerhood as a predictor of executive functioning at school entry. Dev. Psychol. 2015, 51, 1177–1189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Bernier, A.; Carlson, S.M.; Deschênes, M.; Matte-Gagné, C. Social factors in the development of early executive functioning: A closer look at the caregiving environment. Dev. Sci. 2012, 15, 12–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Diamond, A.B.; Barnett, S.W.; Thomas, J.; Munro, S. Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science 2007, 318, 1387–1388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  9. Fay-Stammbach, T.; Hawes, D.J.; Meredith, P. Parenting influences on executive function in early childhood: A review. Child Dev. Perspect. 2014, 8, 258–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Cohen, J.F.W.; Gorski, M.T.; Gruber, S.A.; Kurdziel, L.B.F.; Rimm, E.B. The effect of healthy dietary intake on executive cognitive functioning in children and adolescents: A systematic review. Br. J. Nutr. 2016, 116, 989–1000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  11. Isaacs, E.; Oates, J. Nutrition and cognition: Assessing cognitive abilities in children and young people. Eur. J. Nutr. 2008, 47, 4–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Dumas, J.E.; Nissley, J.; Nordstrom, A.; Smith, E.P.; Prinz, R.J.; Levine, D.W. Home chaos: Sociodemographic, parenting, interactional, and child correlates. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 2005, 34, 93–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Pike, A.; Iervolino, A.C.; Eley, T.C.; Price, T.S.; Plomin, R. Environmental risk and young children’s cognitive and behavioral development. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2006, 30, 55–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Vernon-Feagans, L.; Willoughby, M.; Garrett-Peters, P. Predictors of behavioral regulation in kindergarten: Household chaos, parenting, and early executive functions. Dev. Psychol. 2016, 52, 430–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Miyake, A.; Friedman, N.P. The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2012, 21, 8–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Anderson, P. Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychol. 2002, 8, 71–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Isquith, P.K.; Crawford, J.S.; Espy, K.A.; Gioia, G.A. Assessment of executive function in preschool-aged children. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 2005, 11, 209–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Funahashi, S.; Andreau, J.M. Prefrontal cortex and neural mechanisms of executive function. J. Physiol. Paris 2013, 107, 471–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  19. Hughes, C.; Devine, R.T. For better or for worse? Positive and negative parental influences on young children’s executive function. Child Dev. 2019, 90, 593–609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Hughes, C.; Ensor, R. How do families help or hinder the development of executive function? New Dir. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2009, 123, 35–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Collins, A.; Koechlin, E. Reasoning, learning, and creativity: Frontal lobe function and human decision-making. PLoS Biol. 2012, 10, e1001293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Lehto, J.E.; Juujärvi, P.; Kooistra, L.; Pulkkinen, L. Dimensions of executive functioning: Evidence from children. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 2003, 21, 59–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Lunt, L.; Bramham, J.; Morris, R.G.; Bullock, P.R.; Selway, R.P.; Xenitidis, K.; David, A.S. Prefrontal cortex dysfunction and ‘Jumping to Conclusions’: Bias or deficit? J. Neuropsychol. 2012, 6, 65–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Riggs, N.R.; Spruijt-Metz, D.; Sakuma, K.L.; Chou, C.P.; Pentz, M.A. Executive cognitive function and food intake in children. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 2010, 42, 398–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Hassevoort, K.M.; Lin, A.S.; Khan, N.A.; Hillman, C.H.; Cohen, N.J. Added sugar and dietary fiber intake are associated with creativity in preadolescent children. Nutr. Neurosci. 2020, 23, 791–802. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Riggs, N.R.; Spruijt-Metz, D.; Chou, C.P.; Pentz, M.A. Relationships between executive cognitive function and lifetime substance use and obesity-related behaviors in fourth grade youth. Child Neuropsychol. 2012, 18, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Ames, S.L.; Kisbu-Sakarya, Y.; Reynolds, K.D.; Boyle, S.; Cappelli, C.; Cox, M.G.; Dust, M.; Grenard, J.L.; Mackinnon, D.P.; Stacy, A.W. Inhibitory control effects in adolescent binge eating and intake of sugar-sweetened beverages and snacks. Appetite 2014, 81, 180–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Chung, Y.C.; Park, C.H.; Kwon, H.K.; Park, Y.M.; Kim, Y.S.; Doo, J.K.; Shin, D.H.; Jung, E.S.; Oh, M.R.; Chae, S.W. Improved cognitive performance following supplementation with a mixed-grain diet in high school students: A randomized controlled trial. Nutrition 2012, 28, 165–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Haapala, E.A.; Eloranta, A.M.; Venäläinen, T.; Schwab, U.; Lindi, V.; Lakka, T.A. Associations of diet quality with cognition in children-the physical activity and nutrition in children study. Br. J. Nutr. 2015, 114, 1080–1087. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  30. Sorensen, L.B.; Damsgaard, C.T.; Dalskov, S.M.; Petersen, R.A.; Egelund, N.; Dyssegaard, C.B.; Stark, K.D.; Andersen, R.; Tetens, I.; Astrup, A.; et al. Diet-induced changes in iron and n-3 fatty acid status and associations with cognitive performance in 8-11-year-old Danish children: Secondary analyses of the Optimal Well-Being, Development and Health for Danish Children through a Healthy New Nordic Diet School Meal Study. Br. J. Nutr. 2015, 114, 1623–1637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Andrews, K.; Atkinson, L.; Harris, M.; Gonzalez, A. Examining the effects of household chaos on child executive functions: A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 2020, 147, 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Brown, D.D.; Weatherholt, T.N.; Burns, B.M. Understanding parent reports of children’s attention behaviors: Role of children’s attention skills, temperament, and home environment. J. Early Child. Infant Psychol. 2010, 6, 41–58. [Google Scholar]
  33. Chen, N.; Deater-Deckard, K.; Bell, M.A. The role of temperament by family environment interactions in child maladjustment. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2014, 42, 1251–1262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  34. Hur, E.; Buettner, C.K.; Jeon, L. Parental depressive symptoms and children’s school-readiness: The indirect effect of household chaos. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2015, 24, 3462–3473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Evans, G.W.; Stecker, R. Motivational consequences of environmental stress. J. Environ. Psychol. 2004, 24, 143–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Berry, D.; Blair, C.; Willoughby, M.; Garrett-Peters, P.; Vernon-Feagans, L.; Mills-Koonce, W.R.; Family Life Project Key Investigators. Household chaos and children’s cognitive and socio-emotional development in early childhood: Does childcare play a buffering role? Early Child. Res. Q. 2016, 34, 115–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  37. Martin, A.; Razza, R.A.; Brooks-Gunn, J. Specifying the links between household chaos and preschool children’s development. Early Child Dev. Care 2012, 182, 1247–1263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Anderson, S.E.; Keim, S.A. Parent-child interaction, self-regulation, and obesity prevention in early childhood. Curr. Obes. Rep. 2016, 5, 192–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  39. Berge, J.M.; Wall, M.; Larson, N.; Forsyth, A.; Bauer, K.W.; Neumark-Sztainer, D. Youth dietary intake and weight status: Healthful neighborhood food environments enhance the protective role of supportive family home environments. Health Place 2014, 26, 69–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Cyril, S.; Halliday, J.; Green, J.; Renzaho, A.M.N. Relationship between body mass index and family functioning, family communication, family type and parenting style among African migrant parents and children in Victoria, Australia: A parent-child dyad study. BMC Public Health 2016, 16, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  41. Halliday, J.A.; Palma, C.L.; Mellor, D.; Green, J.; Renzaho, A.M.N. The relationship between family functioning and child and adolescent overweight and obesity: A systematic review. Int. J. Obes. 2014, 38, 480–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Martin-Biggers, J.; Quick, V.; Zhang, M.; Jin, Y.; Byrd-Bredbenner, C. Relationships of family conflict, cohesion, and chaos in the home environment on maternal and child food-related behaviours. Matern. Child Nutr. 2018, 14, e12540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  43. Renzaho, A.M.N.; Kumanyika, S.; Tucker, K.L. Family functioning, parental psychological distress, child behavioural problems, socio-economic disadvantage and fruit and vegetable intake among 412 year-old Victorians, Australia. Health Promot. Int. 2011, 26, 263–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  44. Renzaho, A.M.N.; Dau, A.; Cyril, S.; Ayala, G.X. The influence of family functioning on the intake of unhealthy foods and beverages among 1- to 12-y-old children in Victoria, Australia. Nutrition 2014, 30, 1028–1033. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Smith, J.D.; Montaño, Z.; Dishion, T.J.; Shaw, D.S.; Wilson, M.N. Preventing weight gain and obesity: Indirect effects of the family check-up in early childhood. Prev. Sci. 2015, 16, 408–419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Fiese, B.H.; Musaad, S.; Bost, K.K.; McBride, B.A.; Lee, S.Y.; Teran-Garcia, M.; Donovan, S.M. The Strong Kids 2 birth cohort study: A cell-to-society approach to dietary habits and weight trajectories across the first 5 years of life. Curr. Dev. Nutr. 2019, 3, nzz007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Nutrition Quest. Block Questionnaire for Ages 2-7—Kids 2-7 FFQ. 2014. Available online: http://www.nutritionquest.com/assessment/list-of-questionnaires-and-screeners/ (accessed on 18 July 2021).
  48. Berding, K.; Holscher, H.D.; Arthur, A.E.; Donovan, S.M. Fecal microbiome composition and stability in 4- to 8-year-old children is associated with dietary patterns and nutrient intake. J. Nutr. Biochem. 2018, 56, 165–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Leong, C.; Taylor, R.W.; Haszard, J.J.; Fleming, E.A.; Tannock, G.W.; Szymlek-Gay, E.A.; Cameron, S.L.; Yu, R.; Carter, H.; Chee, K.L.; et al. Relative validity and reproducibility of a food frequency questionnaire to assess nutrients and food groups of relevance to the gut microbiota in young children. Nutrients 2018, 10, 1627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  50. Matheny, A.P.; Wachs, T.D.; Ludwig, J.L.; Phillips, K. Bringing order out of chaos: Psychometric characteristics of the Confusion, Hubbub, and Order Scale. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 1995, 16, 429–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Korkalo, L.; Vepsäläinen, H.; Ray, C.; Skaffari, E.; Lehto, R.; Hauta-alus, H.; Nissinen, K.; Meinilä, J.; Roos, E.; Erkkola, M. Parents’ reports of preschoolers’ diets: Relative validity of a food frequency questionnaire and dietary patterns. Nutrients 2019, 11, 159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  52. Niedzwiedzka, E.; Wadolowska, L.; Kowalkowska, J. Reproducibility of a non-quantitative food frequency questionnaire (62-item ffq-6) and pca-driven dietary pattern identification in 13–21-year-old females. Nutrients 2019, 11, 2183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  53. Northstone, K.; Ness, A.R.; Emmett, P.M.; Rogers, I.S. Adjusting for energy intake in dietary pattern investigations using principal components analysis. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2008, 62, 931–938. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  54. Northstone, K.; Emmett, P.M. Are dietary patterns stable throughout early and mid-childhood? A birth cohort study. Br. J. Nutr. 2008, 100, 1069–1076. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  55. Cortina, J.M. What is coefficient alpha? An examination of theory and applications. J. Appl. Psychol. 1993, 78, 98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Taber, K.S. The Use of Cronbach’s Alpha When Developing and Reporting Research Instruments in Science Education. Res. Sci. Educ. 2018, 48, 1273–1296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Nehring, A.; Nowak, K.H.; zu Belzen, A.U.; Tiemann, R. Predicting students’ skills in the context of scientific inquiry with cognitive, motivational, and sociodemographic variables. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2015, 37, 1343–1363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Wachs, T.D. Nature of relations between the physical and social microenvironment of the two-year-old child. Early Dev. Parent. 1993, 2, 81–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Wachs, T.D.; Camli, O. Do ecological or individual characteristics mediate the influence of the physical environment upon maternal behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 1991, 11, 249–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Shriver, L.H.; Marriage, B.J.; Bloch, T.D.; Spees, C.K.; Ramsay, S.A.; Watowicz, R.P.; Taylor, C.A. Contribution of snacks to dietary intakes of young children in the United States. Matern. Child Nutr. 2018, 14, e12454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Table 1. Descriptive Statistics for Model Variables.
Table 1. Descriptive Statistics for Model Variables.
N%MSD
Child gender
Male14249.0
Female14851.0
Caregiver monthly income
$3000 and under5318.3
$3001–$50007726.6
$5001 and above13646.9
Prefer not to say248.3
Perceived income hardship (at the end of the month)
More than enough money left14869.2
Some money left4521.0
Just enough money left125.6
Somewhat short of money83.7
Very short of money10.5
Caregiver work schedule
Full-time15472.0
Part-time6028.0
Average work hours214 34.5212.05
Caregiver marital status
Single125.6
Civil union10.5
Married19490.7
Co-habituating62.8
Divorced10.5
Caregiver age225 31.334.33
Caregiver race/ethnicity
American Indian/Alaska Native20.7
Asian217.4
Biracial72.5
Black155.3
White23483.0
Prefer not to say31.1
Child medical condition
Yes62.8
No20897.2
Household chaos294 26.697.25
ISCI score275 38.508.13
FI score294 29.375.82
EMI score270 38.708.59
Assorted snacks and processed foods342 11.922.65
Assorted vegetables, fruit, and fish342 14.503.38
Fruit juice and sweet items342 9.163.23
Assorted proteins342 5.291.81
Grains and nuts342 8.862.77
Assorted dairy and water342 10.651.80
Note. The Inhibitory Self-Control Index (ISCI) includes inhibit and emotional control subscales. The Flexibility Index (FI) includes shift and emotional control subscales. The Emergent Metacognition Index (EMI) includes working memory and plan/organize subscales.
Table 2. Dietary Intake: Principal Component Analysis.
Table 2. Dietary Intake: Principal Component Analysis.
DescriptionLoading
Assorted Snacks and Processed Foods
Savory snacks0.69
Refined carbohydrates0.69
Fried foods0.62
Processed meats0.54
Mixed foods0.54
Condiments0.51
Butter/Margarine0.49
Assorted Vegetables, Fruit, and Fish
Vegetables0.81
Starchy foods0.68
Legumes0.65
Fruit0.60
Fish/Seafood0.50
Fruit Juice and Sweet Items
100% Fruit juice0.76
Sweet beverages0.65
Sweet foods0.55
Assorted Proteins
Poultry0.68
Red meats0.67
Eggs0.57
Grains and Nuts
Grains0.69
Peanuts/Nuts0.67
Assorted Dairy and Water
Yogurt0.80
Dairy0.59
Water0.47
Note. A factor loading cutoff of 0.4 was used for analyses.
Table 3. Pairwise Bivariate Correlations Among all Model Variables.
Table 3. Pairwise Bivariate Correlations Among all Model Variables.
1234567
1Assorted snacks and processed foods----
2Assorted vegetables, fruit, and fish0.32 ***----
3Fruit juice and sweet items0.56 ***0.09----
4Household chaos0.17 **−0.12 *0.23 ***----
5ISCI subscale0.20 ***−0.100.17 **0.38 ***----
6FI subscale0.16 **−0.040.13 *0.25 ***0.86 ***----
7EMI subscale0.19 **−0.050.13 *0.38 ***0.82 ***0.67 ***----
Note. Statistically significant correlations are bolded (p ≤ 0.05 *, p ≤ 0.01 **, p ≤ 0.001 ***).
Table 4. Multiple Regression Analyzing Associations Between Assorted Snacks and Processed Foods, Household Chaos, and Inhibitory Self-Control Index (ISCI).
Table 4. Multiple Regression Analyzing Associations Between Assorted Snacks and Processed Foods, Household Chaos, and Inhibitory Self-Control Index (ISCI).
Model 1Model 2Model 3
B (SE)β (p)B (SE)β (p)B (SE)β (p)
Assorted snacks and processed foods0.64 (0.19)0.20 (0.001)0.40 (0.18)0.12 (0.03)0.40 (0.18)0.13 (0.24)
Household chaos 0.46 (0.06)0.41 (0.000)0.46 (0.06)0.41 (0.000)
Assorted snacks and processed foods × household chaos 0.03 (0.02)0.07 (0.20)
R20.040.200.20
ΔR20.040.190.20
ΔF11.0754.281.65
Note. Bolded values indicate statistically significant findings (p ≤ 0.05); N = 275; Assorted snacks and processed foods component includes Savory Snacks, Refined Carbohydrates, Fried Foods, Processed Meats, Mixed Foods, Condiments, and Butter/Margarine.
Table 5. Multiple Regression Analyzing Associations Between Assorted Snacks and Processed Foods, Household Chaos, and Emergent Metacognition Index (EMI).
Table 5. Multiple Regression Analyzing Associations Between Assorted Snacks and Processed Foods, Household Chaos, and Emergent Metacognition Index (EMI).
Model 1Model 2Model 3
B (SE)β (p)B (SE)β (p)B (SE)β (p)
Assorted snacks and processed foods0.65 (0.21)0.19 (0.002)0.43 (0.20)0.13 (0.03)0.43 (0.20)0.13 (0.03)
Household chaos 0.45 (0.07)0.37 (0.000)0.45 (0.07)0.37 (0.000)
Assorted snacks and processed foods × household chaos 0.02 (0.03)0.04 (0.52)
R20.040.170.17
ΔR20.030.160.16
ΔF9.7442.960.43
Note. Bolded values indicate statistically significant findings (p ≤ 0.05); N = 270; Assorted snacks and processed foods component includes Savory Snacks, Refined Carbohydrates, Fried Foods, Processed Meats, Mixed Foods, Condiments, and Butter/Margarine.
Table 6. Multiple Regression Analyzing Associations Between Assorted Snacks and Processed Foods, Household Chaos, and Flexibility Index (FI).
Table 6. Multiple Regression Analyzing Associations Between Assorted Snacks and Processed Foods, Household Chaos, and Flexibility Index (FI).
Model 1Model 2Model 3
B (SE)β (p)B (SE)β (p)B (SE)β (p)
Assorted snacks and processed foods0.36 (0.13)0.16 (0.01)0.24 (0.13)0.11 (0.06)0.24 (0.13)0.11 (0.06)
Household chaos 0.23 (0.05)0.28 (0.000)0.23 (0.05)0.29 (0.000)
Assorted snacks and processed foods × household chaos 0.02 (0.02)0.07 (0.25)
R20.020.100.11
ΔR20.020.100.10
ΔF7.2425.001.35
Note. Bolded values indicate statistically significant findings (p ≤ 0.05); N = 294; Assorted snacks and processed foods component includes Savory Snacks, Refined Carbohydrates, Fried Foods, Processed Meats, Mixed Foods, Condiments, and Butter/Margarine.
Table 7. Multiple Regression Analyzing Associations Between Fruit Juice and Sweet Items, Household Chaos, and Inhibitory Self-Control Index (ISCI).
Table 7. Multiple Regression Analyzing Associations Between Fruit Juice and Sweet Items, Household Chaos, and Inhibitory Self-Control Index (ISCI).
Model 1Model 2Model 3
B (SE)β (p)B (SE)β (p)B (SE)β (p)
Fruit juice and sweet items0.43 (0.16)0.17 (0.01)0.18 (0.15)0.07 (0.22)0.19 (0.15)0.07 (0.21)
Household chaos 0.47 (0.06)0.41 (0.000)0.47 (0.06)0.41 (0.000)
Fruit juice and sweet items × household chaos 0.01 (0.02)0.02 (0.72)
R20.030.190.19
ΔR20.020.180.18
ΔF7.6354.060.13
Note. Bolded values indicate statistically significant findings (p ≤ 0.05); N = 275; Fruit juice and sweet items component contains 100% Fruit Juice, Sweet Beverages, and Sweet Foods.
Table 8. Multiple Regression Analyzing Associations Between Fruit Juice and Sweet Items, Household Chaos, and Emergent Metacognition Index (EMI).
Table 8. Multiple Regression Analyzing Associations Between Fruit Juice and Sweet Items, Household Chaos, and Emergent Metacognition Index (EMI).
Model 1Model 2Model 3
B (SE)β (p)B (SE)β (p)B (SE)β (p)
Fruit juice and sweet items0.34 (0.16)0.13 (0.04)0.10 (0.15)0.04 (0.53)0.10 (0.16)0.04 (0.52)
Household chaos 0.46 (0.07)0.38 (0.000)0.46 (0.07)0.38 (0.000)
Fruit juice and sweet items × household chaos 0.004 (0.02)0.01 (0.87)
R20.020.160.16
ΔR20.010.150.15
ΔF4.5043.780.03
Note. Bolded values indicate statistically significant findings (p ≤ 0.05); N = 270; Fruit juice and sweet items component contains 100% Fruit Juice, Sweet Beverages, and Sweet Foods.
Table 9. Multiple Regression Analyzing Associations Between Fruit Juice and Sweet Items, Household Chaos, and Flexibility Index (FI).
Table 9. Multiple Regression Analyzing Associations Between Fruit Juice and Sweet Items, Household Chaos, and Flexibility Index (FI).
Model 1Model 2Model 3
B (SE)β (p)B (SE)β (p)B (SE)β (p)
Fruit juice and sweet items0.23 (0.11)0.13 (0.03)0.11 (0.11)0.06 (0.30)0.12 (0.11)0.07 (0.26)
Household chaos 0.23 (0.05)0.29 (0.000)0.23 (0.05)0.28 (0.000)
Fruit juice and sweet items × household chaos 0.02 (0.02)0.07 (0.24)
R20.020.090.10
ΔR20.010.090.09
ΔF4.7724.981.40
Note. Bolded values indicate statistically significant findings (p ≤ 0.05); N = 294; Fruit juice and sweet items component contains 100% Fruit Juice, Sweet Beverages, and Sweet Foods.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Iwinski, S.; Donovan, S.M.; Fiese, B.; Bost, K. The Impact of Household Chaos and Dietary Intake on Executive Function in Young Children. Nutrients 2021, 13, 4442. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13124442

AMA Style

Iwinski S, Donovan SM, Fiese B, Bost K. The Impact of Household Chaos and Dietary Intake on Executive Function in Young Children. Nutrients. 2021; 13(12):4442. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13124442

Chicago/Turabian Style

Iwinski, Samantha, Sharon M. Donovan, Barbara Fiese, and Kelly Bost. 2021. "The Impact of Household Chaos and Dietary Intake on Executive Function in Young Children" Nutrients 13, no. 12: 4442. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13124442

APA Style

Iwinski, S., Donovan, S. M., Fiese, B., & Bost, K. (2021). The Impact of Household Chaos and Dietary Intake on Executive Function in Young Children. Nutrients, 13(12), 4442. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13124442

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop