Next Article in Journal
Energy Balance of Triathletes during an Ultra-Endurance Event
Previous Article in Journal
Piper and Vismia Species from Colombian Amazonia Differentially Affect Cell Proliferation of Hepatocarcinoma Cells
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Heart Rate Variability and Cognitive Function Following a Multi-Vitamin and Mineral Supplementation with Added Guarana (Paullinia cupana)

1
Laboratory of Human Motricity, Education, Sport and Health (EA 6309), University of Nice Sophia Antipolis, Nice 06205, France
2
Aix Marseille University, CNRS, LPC UMR 7290, FR 3C FR 3512, Marseille 13000, France
3
National Institute for Sport Expertise and Performance, Paris 75012, France
4
Creps Sud-Est, Aix en Provence 13080, France
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2015, 7(1), 196-208; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu7010196
Submission received: 31 October 2014 / Accepted: 16 December 2014 / Published: 31 December 2014

Abstract

:
The aim of this study was to assess cognitive performance and heart rate variability (HRV) following the ingestion of either a multi-vitamin-mineral preparation supplemented with 300 mg guarana (Ac); a caffeine supplement (C) or a placebo supplement (Pl). Fifty-six subjects took part in a randomized, double-blind crossover design, consisting of three experimental sessions ran on a different day. Cognitive performance was assessed using a go/no-go task and a simple reaction time (SRT) task. HRV was assessed in the time domain (RMSSD) and in the frequency domain (HF) and cognitive tasks were performed before ingestion, 15 min after ingestion and then every 15 min over the course of 3 h. Responses were faster (without change in accuracy) when the go/no-go task was performed between 30 and 90 min after ingestion of Ac (4.6% ± 0.8%, p < 0.05). No effect was observed on SRT task. A significant decrease in HRV was observed during the first hour under C and Pl, whereas HRV remained stable under Ac. The results suggest that the ingestion of a multi-vitamin-mineral with added guarana improves decision-making performance and is accompanied by a stable autonomic nervous system regulation during the first hour.

Graphical Abstract

1. Introduction

Guarana (Paullinia Cupana) is a climbing plant of the maple family. It is a rain forest vine that was domesticated in the Amazon for its fruits, which are rich in caffeine. Despite its increasing usage as a dietary constituents by athletes to support training or improve performance, guarana’s specific behavioral effects have received little attention until recently. Currently there has been a growing interest in the beneficial effects of guarana, which is supported by a number of experimental studies in humans and rodents. Guarana has been reported to delay physical and mental cancer-related fatigue [1,2], improve cognitive performance [3,4,5,6,7], have positive effects on mood [6,7] and facilitate weight loss [8]. Guarana administration in rats reduced total food intake, carcass fat content and decreased plasma lactate concentrations in trained rats [9]. Guarana’s may also be used as an antibacterial agent and has antioxidant properties [10].
The psychoactive properties of a single dose of guarana extract (75 mg) have been initially observed by Kennedy and colleagues in healthy adults [5]. Authors reported improvements in memory performance (i.e., long-term storage system or secondary memory) and in response speed (albeit accompanied with a decrease in accuracy, suggesting a speed-accuracy trade-off). Using different doses of guarana extract (i.e., 37.5 mg, 75 mg, 150 mg and 300 mg), Haskell and colleagues [6] confirmed an improvement of cognitive performance and highlighted changes in mood. Interestingly, there is an increase in self-rated “alertness” following the highest dose of guarana and an increase in self-rated “contentment” following all doses.
The psychoactive properties of guarana have often been attributed to the high concentration of caffeine contained in its seeds. However, the results of these latest studies [5,6] show that psychoactive effects can be observed using a 75 mg dose of guarana, which contains a level of caffeine considered too low (9 mg) to produce positive effects. These findings suggest that cognitive performance changes induced by guarana cannot be solely attributable to guarana’s caffeine content. Evidence suggests that other components, such as flavonoids [11] or other potentially psychoactive components, including saponins and tannins [12,13], may contribute to cognitive function changes under guarana. Scholey and Haskell [11] suggested that these psychoactive effects could be attributable to modulation of guarana’s caffeine content by other guarana components or by direct effects of noncaffeine constituents contained in guarana. To date, the relative contribution of caffeine and guarana is unclear and deserves further investigations to disentangle their psychoactive effects. The first aim of this study was to compare the effects of a multi-vitamin-mineral preparation supplemented with 300 mg guarana on cognitive function with an equivalent pure caffeine dose over the course of 3 h.
The efficacy of guarana supplementation associated with vitamins/minerals in terms of cognitive performance has been investigated more recently. Scholey and colleagues [14] were the first to bring to evidence that a multivitamin-mineral preparation containing guarana improved cognitive performance in humans and reduced mental fatigue associated with sustained mental effort. Following the administration of a single dose of a vitamin/mineral/guarana food supplement, an improvement in performance, both in terms of speed and accuracy, as well as an attenuation of subjective mental fatigue have been observed during a sustained cognitive task (i.e., Rapid Visual Information Processing Task, (RVIP) (During the RVIP task, subjects are requested to detect target sequences of digits and respond as quickly as possible by pressing a key response.)) [3]. More recently, Scholey and colleagues [7] evaluated the neurocognitive effects of multivitamin supplementations with and without guarana. The authors examined the neural substrates of guarana-induced effects using an fMRI protocol during a RVIP task and an Inspection Time (IT) activation task (During the IT task, subjects are requested to discriminate the longest of two lines displayed for a variable duration.). Results showed that both multivitamin treatments increased activation in areas involved in cognitive tasks, but intriguingly the activation was greater in the multivitamin with guarana condition.
Despite similar behavioral effects, it is not clear if the mechanisms of action responsible for the psychoactive effect of guarana on cognitive performance are similar with those described for caffeine [3]. The effect of caffeine on cognition is well documented; it has been shown that, caffeine alters attention and modifies the neural activity in cortical areas involved in the perceptual analysis of relevant stimulus information, e.g., [15]. However, the central nervous system does not seem to develop a great tolerance to caffeine and psychoactive effects can be accompanied by potential side effects, such as anxiety or nervousness [16], generated palpitations and arrhythmia. Because guarana contains caffeine, some similar potential side effects including psycho-physiological disturbances should be considered. Interactions between neural states and cognitive performance are a traditional topic of psychological research. Classical concepts from motivational psychology relate cognitive improvement with neural activation changes [17]. Within this framework, recent researchers have studied this relationship through the analysis of the autonomic nervous systems’ regulation assessed by heart rate variability (HRV) [18,19,20]. According to Porges’ [21] model, it could be predicted an association between higher resting levels of cardiac vagal tone and improved cognitive capacity [20]. Since HRV changes could represent activation [18] as well as anxiety [22], it could reflect, following ingestion, either positive or negative effects of guarana on cognition. To date, the effects of guarana on autonomic function remains unknown; the second aim of this study was to investigate potential side effects of guarana on the autonomic nervous systems’ regulation through the analysis of HRV over the course of 3 h.

2. Experimental Section

2.1. Participants

Fifty-six subjects (32 males and 24 females) volunteered to participate in the study after an Internet announcement of the research project on the university website. The mean age of the males was 27.7 years (range: 19–45 years, body mass (74.1 ± 5.7) kg, height (179 ± 3) cm) and of the females was 29.5 years (range: 18–42 years, body mass (65.6 ± 3.6) kg, height (166 ± 2) cm). All of the participants were caffeine consumers (range: 200–500 mg per day). Exclusion criteria comprised severe physical diseases, psychiatric disorders, chronic alcohol consumption, as well as the use of psychoactive drugs or medication affecting the cardiovascular system. The procedures followed were in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki 1975, revised Hong Kong 1989. The local ethics committee (Ile-de-France VII, Saint Germain en Laye, France) reviewed and approved the study before its initiation and all subjects gave their informed written consent before participation.

2.2. Procedure

This study used a randomized, double blind, experimental design. Subjects were required to report to the laboratory for testing on three separate sessions with at least 48 h between each. Each subject was his own control, thus this study included three paired treatments: a vitamin/mineral/guarana supplement (Ac, n = 56); a caffeine supplement (C, n = 56); and a placebo supplement (P, n = 56). The three conditions were prepared by an independent laboratory and presented to the subjects mixed with water in a standardized blind conditioning. The vitamin/mineral/guarana complex was a commercially available effervescent tablet, Isoxan Actiflash® (Menarini, NHS, Rungis, France). The constituents are shown in Table 1. Caffeine contains 100 mg of caffeine (Natrol®, Biovéa, Le Mans France), which corresponds to an equivalent dose to that contained in the vitamin/mineral/guarana tablet. All subjects were tested individually in a quiet room and the experimental session began after 15 min of rest. They were required to avoid any kind of exercise or consume any alcohol for 24 h prior to the experimental session. At the beginning of the session, subjects had to complete a self-administered questionnaire to assess overnight caffeine abstinence according to Hughes et al. [23] study. All assessments were done in the afternoon approximately at the same time (2–6 pm).
Cognitive tasks were performed before ingestion, 15 min after ingestion, and then every 15 min over the course of 3 h. Cognitive task duration was about 8 min. Before each cognitive task, an electrocardiogram (ECG) was recorded during 5 min using a Biopac 150 system (Biopac Systems Inc., Goleta, CA, USA). All measures before ingestion were used as a baseline value for each day.
Table 1. Contents of the vitamin/mineral tablets/guarana, Isoxan Actiflash® effervescent tablets.
Table 1. Contents of the vitamin/mineral tablets/guarana, Isoxan Actiflash® effervescent tablets.
ActiveUnitsPer DayEuropean RDA
MalesFemales
Vitamin C180 mg180 mg90 mg75 mg
Vitamin B10.705 mg0.705 mg1.2 mg1.1 mg
Vitamin B20.798 mg0.798 mg1.3 mg1.1 mg
Vitamin B39005 mg9005 mg16 mg14 mg
Vitamin B61003 mg1003 mg1.7 mg1.5 mg
Zinc7490 mg7490 mg11 mg8 mg
Guaraná ES300 mg300 mgNANA
Ginseng ES100 mg100 mgNANA
RDA (Recommended Dietary Allowances) are provided for males and females age ranging from 19 years to 70 years (European Food Safety Authority, 2010).

2.3. Cognitive Performance

Cognitive performances were assessed using two different tasks: a go/no-go task and a simple reaction time (SRT) task.

2.3.1. Go/No-Go Task

Stimuli were presented in white on a black background. Stimuli consisted of an H or an S. Participants were instructed to press a button if the stimulus was an H, and not to respond if the stimulus was an S. Both stimuli were equiprobable. Stimuli remained on the screen until a response was made or until 1200 ms had elapsed. There was an interval of 400–600 ms before the start of the next trial.

2.3.2. Simple Reaction Time (SRT) Task

The SRT task was measured from impulses recorded on a handgrip while the subject was seated. The subject was instructed to hold the handgrip in his/her preferred hand and to place his/her thumb on a button. SRT was calculated as the time required by the subject to remove his/her thumb from the button. This device was connected to a microcomputer with a sampling frequency of 120 Hz. The luminous stimuli appeared in the center of a screen and were separated by a variable foreperiod varying from 3–5 s Mean SRT and standard deviation were calculated for 40 trials and SRT under 160 ms was considered as an error (anticipated responses).

2.4. Heart Rate Measure

During the ECG assessment periods, participants were instructed to sit relaxed with hands on thighs without speaking or moving, and to breathe regularly. Two active electrodes (Ag/AgCl) were placed at the right mid-clavicle and the lowest left rib. Respiration was monitored with the same Biopac system using a piezoelectric thoracic belt. The QRS-signal waveform (R-R signal) was sampled at the resolution of 1 ms. The R-R intervals (i.e., the length of time between the R peaks of consecutive QRS complexes) were calculated and checked for artifacts. The root mean square difference of successive normal R-R intervals (RMSSD) was calculated for the 5 min of recorded data. A fixed linear resampling frequency of 1024 equally spaced points per 3 min period was used. Power density in the HF (0.15–0.50 Hz) band was calculated for every three-minute spectrum by integrating the spectral power density within the frequency band [18,19,20]. The Respiratory Rate (RR) measure enabled us to conduct analyses with RR controlled since the central frequency of the HF component has been shown to be highly correlated with strain gauge measures of respiration [24].

2.5. Statistics

Changes from baseline scores for all dependent variables were calculated in contrast with pre-dose performance. Each dependent variable was analyzed using a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA), including conditions (vitamin/mineral/guarana, Ac; caffeine, C; and placebo, Pl) × time (8 periods over the course of 3 h). The effect size was calculated using eta squared(eta2) values. Follow-up analyses were conducted using simple effect tests using Tukey’s procedure. For analyses involving time, if the sphericity assumption was violated, then Greenhouse-Geisser [25] conservative degrees-of-freedom adjustments were applied and critical p values were corrected [26]. The data was analyzed using the Statistica 7.1 program (StatSoft, Maisons-Alfort, France)

3. Results

3.1. Cognitive Performance

Cognitive performance values at baseline and among times are represented in Table 2. No significant difference was found between male and female responses in this study therefore data is presented for the whole population.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics for cognitive performance and Heart Rate variability as a function of experimental condition (vitamin/mineral/guarana, Ac; caffeine, C; and placebo, Pl) at baseline and every 15 min over the course of 3 h.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics for cognitive performance and Heart Rate variability as a function of experimental condition (vitamin/mineral/guarana, Ac; caffeine, C; and placebo, Pl) at baseline and every 15 min over the course of 3 h.
Go/No-GoCondition Time (min)
01530456090120150180
reaction time (ms)Acmean329329317 *314 *315 *325 *345347 *349 *
SD161324182519141720
Plmean330331334339345352 359 358 358
SD182532463325262113
Cmean327329329333335340350 355 356
SD171928322922201916
Go/no-go errors (%)Acmean10.60.80.71.30.50.71.20.5
SD0.70.70.41.50.30.60.40.80.8
Plmean10.60.00.90.60.50.40.80.6
SD0.50.01.00.30.30.40.40.30.5
Cmean00.60.40.80.90.51.31.00.6
SD0.60.30.70.90.30.50.40.50.7
Simple reaction time (ms)Acmean230224227256 *247 *251 *250 *242 *246 *
SD11917131813101214
Plmean228233242264 253 259 249 245 253
SD12182232231718159
Cmean 227227233258 248 253 248 242 248
SD121319222015141312
RMSSD (ms)Acmean 64.166.663.363.6 §60.6 *,§60.7 *,§57.3 *56.2 *54.5 *
SD8.910.512.74.34.15.85.77.47.8
Plmean 67.066.261.4 59.8 56.5 54.8 54.1 53.3 54.6
SD10.211.615.58.32.44.52.23.811.2
Cmean 65.266.064.361.4 58.2 57.4 55.4 54.5 54.3
SD9.511.014.06.33.35.18.95.69.4
HF (n.u.)Acmean 41.140.740.639.7 §38.7 *,§36.9 *35.5 *36.3 *35.6 *
SD2.84.55.88.15.98.42.15.63.7
Plmean41.640.536.6 35.6 35.5 35.4 35.8 36.1 35.8
SD9.83.23.76.33.23.65.14.14.8
Cmean41.440.539.937.5 36.6 36.0 35.9 36.5 36.0
SD6.23.84.77.14.56.03.63.95.1
* When a difference was significant with baseline for Ac condition; when a difference was significant with baseline for C condition; when a difference was significant with baseline for Pl condition; § when a difference was significant between Ac and C condition.

3.1.1. Go/No-Go Task

There was a significant main effect of time (p < 0.05; eta2 = 0.42) and a significant interaction between time and condition (p < 0.01; eta2 = 0.63) on mean reaction time (RT) during the go/no-go task (Figure 1). For Ac, faster RT was observed from the 30th minute to the 90th minute with a significant difference between Ac and C conditions (maximal difference between C and Ac at the 60th minute: 8.5% ± 0.7%, p < 0.05) (Figure 1). In all conditions a significant decrease in performance was observed at the end of the experiment (respectively, for Pl, C and Ac conditions: −8.1% ± 2.6% vs. −8.2% ± 1.9% vs. −5.9% ± 1.7%). This impairment became significant at the 90 min for Pl condition (−6.3% ± 1.8%, p < 0.05) ) and at the 120th minute for C condition (−6.5% ± 2.2%, p < 0.05), whereas it occurred only at the 150th minute for Ac condition (−5.4% ± 1.1%, p < 0.05). No significant effect was found on errors.
Figure 1. Change in reaction time performance (in percentage from baseline values) during the go/no-go task performed under vitamin/mineral/guarana (Ac), caffeine (C) and placebo (Pl) supplementation over the course of 3 h. * When a difference was significant with baseline for Ac condition; when a difference was significant with baseline for C condition; when a difference was significant with baseline for Pl condition; § when a difference was significant between Ac and C condition.
Figure 1. Change in reaction time performance (in percentage from baseline values) during the go/no-go task performed under vitamin/mineral/guarana (Ac), caffeine (C) and placebo (Pl) supplementation over the course of 3 h. * When a difference was significant with baseline for Ac condition; when a difference was significant with baseline for C condition; when a difference was significant with baseline for Pl condition; § when a difference was significant between Ac and C condition.
Nutrients 07 00196 g001

3.1.2. Simple Reaction Time (SRT) Task

A significant effect of the time spent on the task was observed on mean RT (p < 0.001 eta2 = 0.58) and no interaction was observed between time and condition. Reaction time lengthens with time on task in all conditions after 45th minute (respectively, for Ac, C and Pl at 45th minute: 11.45% ± 1.8% vs. 13.6% ± 2.5% vs. 15.8% ± 2.1%, p < 0.05). No further significant changes were observed. No significant effect of time or condition was observed on anticipated responses.

3.2. Heart Rate Variability (HRV)

Heart Rate variability values at baseline and among times are represented in Table 2. A significant interaction was observed between time and condition on both RMSSD and HF values (respectively, p = 0.01, eta2 = 0.68 and p = 0.01, eta2 = 0.63). In all conditions, a decrease in RMSSD was observed at the end of the experiment (respectively, for Pl, C and Ac conditions: −18.5% ± 3.8%; −16.8% ± 2.9% and −15.9% ± 2.9%). When compared with baseline, RMSSD values decreased at the 30th minute for Pl (−8.3% ± 2.4%, p < 0.05) and at the 45th minute for C (−5.9% ± 1.1%, p < 0.05), whereas it remained stable for Ac until the 60th minute (−5.1% ± 2.8%, ns). Furthermore, RMSSD values were significantly lower for C than for Ac between the 45th and the 90th minute (the maximal difference was observed at the 45th minute: 5.3% ± 0.5%, p < 0.05). A similar decrease was observed for HF values. After the 30th minute, the decline was maximal for both Pl (−12.2% ± 2.1%, p < 0.05) and C (−9.6% ± 1.8%, p < 0.05) and occurred later, at the 60th minute, for Ac condition (−5.8% ± 1.4%, p < 0.05). A significant difference was observed between C and Ac conditions between the 45th and the 60th minute (mean difference between C and Ac, respectively, at the 45th and the 60th minute: 6.1% ± 1.1% and 6.3% ± 0.9%, p < 0.05) (Figure 2).
Figure 2. Graphic representation of change from baseline scores (in %) in the High Frequency (HF) band over the course of 3 h. * When a difference was significant with baseline for Ac condition; when a difference was significant with baseline for C condition; when a difference was significant with baseline for Pl condition; § when a difference was significant between Ac and C condition.
Figure 2. Graphic representation of change from baseline scores (in %) in the High Frequency (HF) band over the course of 3 h. * When a difference was significant with baseline for Ac condition; when a difference was significant with baseline for C condition; when a difference was significant with baseline for Pl condition; § when a difference was significant between Ac and C condition.
Nutrients 07 00196 g002

4. Discussion

This study examined cognitive performance and autonomic function following the ingestion of either, a vitamin complex with guarana, a caffeine dose alone at the same concentration than in guarana, and placebo. The main findings of the study were that (1) decisional cognitive performance was significantly improved from 30 min to 90 min after the ingestion of the complex with guarana and this change differed from that observed after a single dose of caffeine; (2) after ingestion of the complex with guarana, stability in parasympathetic modulation was observed during the first hour of the experiment, whereas a decrease was observed for placebo and caffeine conditions; (3) In contrast with previous studies examining the effect of caffeine ingestion on cognition, no effect was observed on SRT.
The improvement in decisional cognitive performance observed during the go/no-go task following guarana ingestion is in agreement with previous results reported in studies comparing guarana and placebo ingestion [3,4,5,6,7]. Indeed, Kennedy et al. [5] have observed that the ingestion of 75 mg of guarana in healthy subjects leads to an improvement in memory performance. Thereafter, Haskell et al. [6] compared four different doses (37.5 mg, 75 mg, 150 mg and 300 mg) of a standardized guarana extract. Results indicated that the two lower doses produced greater beneficial effects on memory performance than the higher doses. One interesting result of this study is that 75 mg of guarana contains a level of caffeine (9 mg) that is generally considered too low to affect cognitive performance. The authors proposed that the positive effect of guarana is not only related to caffeine. Furthermore, Campos et al. [27] have compared guarana (25 and 50 mg/kg) and caffeine (10 and 20 mg/kg) effects on mouse behavior. These authors reported that both guarana and caffeine have a significant effect on behavior, but suggested that the mechanisms underlying antidepressant-like activity are different. The fact that our results indicate a differentiated effect between guarana and caffeine ingestion supports this hypothesis. In our study the improvement in cognitive performance during the go/no-go task is only observed following guarana ingestion. Guarana contains 4% to 8% caffeine, as well as other alkaloids, such as theophylline (500–750 ppm) and theobromine (300–500 ppm), tannins or saponins. We assume that the difference between the complex ingestion and caffeine could be related to the presence of other psychoactive substances including tannins and saponins that are known to influence cognitive performance [13]. Vitamins present in the complex could also have a positive effect on cognitive performance. In elderly people with neurological diseases several minerals and vitamins have been classically described to positively affect cognitive performance. For example, a significant effect has been observed in elderly people of vitamin B, e.g., [28], or vitamins C and E on cognition, e.g. [29]. In a recent review, Huskisson et al. [30] indicated that vitamins B and C also present in the complex used in our study seem most effective, however, these authors also suggested that the results in this area need to be validated in healthy subjects.
In our study no effect of the ingestion of a complex with guarana or caffeine alone has been observed on SRT. This result is different from those reported in previous works showing a significant improvement in TRS after ingestion of caffeine without increasing the number of anticipated responses [31,32]. However, in all studies doses of caffeine were greater than those used in this study and positive effects were observed for a dose of caffeine of 200 or 250 mg, respectively, in studies conducted by Lieberman, et al. [32] and Attwood et al. [31]. Therefore, the dose of caffeine in the complex with guarana seems too low to induce an improvement in performance during perceptual-motor tasks involving low uncertainty, such as SRT.
In this study after either complex with guarana, stability in parasympathetic modulation (RMSSD or HF) was observed during the first hour after ingestion, whereas a decrease was observed for placebo and caffeine alone. Furthermore these effects are concomitant with changes in decision-making performance.
Heart rate variability is a noninvasive measure of autonomic contributions to cardiac functioning. The time domain (RMSSD) and the frequency domain (HF) reflect the parasympathetic modulation of the autonomic system even if they are not always strongly correlated [18]. Therefore a decrease in RMSSD and/or HF indicates a more sympathetic-and less parasympathetic-related modulation. Within this framework previous works have drawn links between HRV decrease and anxiety or worry [22,33]. Therefore the stability in parasympathetic modulation observed in our study indirectly suggests that ingestion of guarana complex does not lead to an increase in anxiety in our healthy subjects immediately after the ingestion.
Many investigations from cognitive psycho-physiologists have previously studied the relationship between HRV and cognitive performance, but results remain contradictory according to the nature of the cognitive task and there are still many questions regarding the relationship between acute changes in HRV and cognitive performance, e.g., [18]. On the one hand, results recorded during task engagement suggest that a pattern of cardiovascular adjustments, including enhanced sympathetic and reduced vagal cardiovascular influences, may induce an adaptive state associated with improved cognitive functioning and faster RT [19,34]. On the other hand, when HRV is assessed independent of task engagement, like in our study, a decrease in HRV is associated with worse cognitive performance [35]. Recent models from the literature suggest a link between HRV and prefrontal cortical activity involved in executive functions, such as decision-making. For example, the Neurovisceral Integration Model proposes that all these processes of cognitive, affective and physiological regulation may be related for goal-directed behavior [36]. Within this framework, low HRV values or a decrease in HRV could be viewed as reflecting anxiety and worry, but it could also reflect mental load [37,38] and inefficient or ineffective cognitive function [21,37]. The existing literature tends to yield supportive evidence, indicating that HRV is positively associated with performance during response inhibition and working memory tasks [20,35,39,40]. Thus, in our study the decrease in HRV in Pl and C conditions during the first hour of the experiment could be related to an impairment in cognitive performance and an increase in mental load [36], whereas the stability in HRV in Ac during more than one hour seems to indicate the efficacy of such ingestion to maintain parasympathetic modulation and cognitive performance during cognitive tasks requiring executive processes.

5. Conclusions

This research supports previous findings concerning the psychoactive properties of guarana and provides evidence that a multi-vitamin–mineral ingestion with added guarana improves decision-making performance without any additional impairment of the autonomic nervous systems’ regulation or side effects on anxiety.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Laura Gray for English revisions. This study was sponsored by Menarini NHS (Nutrition-Hygiène-Santé) Medical Research Unit, Rungis, France. No funds were received for covering the costs to publish in open access.

Author Contributions

Jeanick Brisswalter and Karen Davranche conceived and designed the experiments; Laura Pomportes, Ioanna Brisswalter and Arnaud Hays performed the experiments; Laura Pomportes and Jeanick Brisswalter analyzed the data; Jeanick Brisswalter, Laura Pomportes and Karen Davranche wrote the paper.

Conflicts of Interests

The Authors declares no conflict of interest.

References

  1. De Oliveira Campos, M.P.; Riechelmann, R.; Martins, L.C.; Hassan, B.J.; Casa, F.B.; del Giglio, A. Guaraná (Paullinia cupana) improves fatigue in breast cancer patients undergoing systemic chemotherapy. J. Altern. Complement. Med. 2011, 17, 505–512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Campos, M.P.; Hassan, B.J.; Riechelmann, R.; del Giglio, A. Cancer-related fatigue: A review. Rev. Assoc. Med. Bras. 2011, 57, 211–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Kennedy, D.O.; Haskell, C.F.; Robertson, B.; Reay, J.; Brewster-Maund, C.; Luedemann, J.; Maggini, S.; Ruf, M.; Zangara, A.; Scholey, A.B. Improved cognitive performance and mental fatigue following a multi-vitamin and mineral supplement with added Guaraná (Paullinia cupana). Appetite 2008, 50, 506–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Galduroz, J.C.; Carlini, E.A. The effects of long-term administration of Guaraná on the cognition of normal, elderly volunteers. Sao Paulo Med. J. 1996, 114, 1073–1078. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Kennedy, D.O.; Haskell, C.F.; Wesnes, K.A.; Scholey, A.B. Improved cognitive performance in human volunteers following administration of Guaraná (Paullinia cupana) extract: Comparison and interaction with Panax ginseng. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2004, 79, 401–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Haskell, C.F.; Kennedy, D.O.; Wesnes, K.A.; Milne, A.L.; Scholey, A.B. A double-blind, placebo-controlled, multi-dose evaluation of the acute behavioural effects of Guaraná in humans. J. Psychopharmacol. 2007, 21, 65–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Scholey, A.; Bauer, I.; Neale, C.; Savage, K.; Camfield, D.; White, D.; Maggini, S.; Pipingas, A.; Stough, C.; Hughes, M. Acute effects of different multivitamin mineral preparations with and without Guaraná on mood, cognitive performance and functional brain activation. Nutrients 2013, 5, 3589–3604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Opala, T.; Rzymski, P.; Pischel, I.; Wilczak, M.; Wozniak, J. Efficacy of 12 weeks supplementation of a botanical extract-based weight loss formula on body weight, body composition and blood chemistry in healthy, overweight subjects—A randomised double-blind placebo-controlled clinical trial. Eur. J. Med. Res. 2006, 11, 343–350. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  9. Lima, W.P.; Carnevali, L.C.; Eder, R., Jr.; Costa Rosa, L.F.; Bacchi, E.M.; Seelaender, M.C. Lipid metabolism in trained rats: Effect of guarana (Paullinia cupana Mart.) supplementation. Clin. Nutr. 2005, 24, 1019–1028. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Yamaguti-Sasaki, E.; Ito, L.A.; Canteli, V.C.; Ushirobira, T.M.; Ueda-Nakamura, T.; Dias Filho, B.P.; Nakamura, C.V.; de Mello, J.C. Antioxidant capacity and in vitro prevention of dental plaque formation by extracts and condensed tannins of Paullinia cupana. Molecules 2007, 12, 1950–1963. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Scholey, A.; Haskell, C. Neurocognitive effects of guaraná plant extract. Drugs Fut. 2008, 33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Espinola, E.B.; Dias, R.F.; Mattei, R.; Carlini, E.A. Pharmacological activity of Guarana (Paullinia cupana Mart.) in laboratory animals. J. Ethnopharmacol. 1997, 55, 223–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Mattei, R.; Dias, R.F.; Espínola, E.B.; Carlini, E.A.; Barros, S.B. Guarana (Paullinia cupana): Toxic behavioral effects in laboratory animals and antioxidants activity in vitro. J. Ethnopharmacol. 1998, 60, 111–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Scholey, A.B.; Kennedy, D.O.; Zangara, A.; Robertson, B.C.; Reay, J.; Luedemann, J.; Maggini, S.; Ruf, M. A multivitamin–mineral preparation with guaraná positively effects cognitive performance and reduces mental fatigue during sustained mental demand. Appetite 2008, 50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Mednick, S.C.; Cai, D.J.; Kanady, J.; Drummond, S.P. Comparing the benefits of caffeine, naps and placebo on verbal, motor and perceptual memory. Behav. Brain Res. 2008, 193, 79–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Clauson, K.A.; Shields, K.M.; McQueen, C.E.; Persad, N. Safety issues associated with commercially available energy drinks. J. Am. Pharm. Assoc. (2003) 2008, 48, 55–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Brisswalter, J.; Collardeau, M.; Rene, A. Effects of acute physical exercise characteristics on cognitive performance. Sports Med. 2002, 32, 555–566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Luft, C.D.; Takase, E.; Darby, D. Heart rate variability and cognitive function: Effects of physical effort. Biol. Psychol. 2009, 82, 164–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Duschek, S.; Muckenthaler, M.; Werner, N.; del Paso, G.A. Relationships between features of autonomic cardiovascular control and cognitive performance. Biol. Psychol. 2009, 81, 110–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Hansen, A.L.; Johnsen, B.H.; Thayer, J.F. Vagal influence on working memory and attention. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2003, 48, 263–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Porges, S.W. Vagal tone: A physiologic marker of stress vulnerability. Pediatrics 1992, 90, 498–504. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  22. Miu, A.C.; Heilman, R.M.; Miclea, M. Reduced heart rate variability and vagal tone in anxiety: Trait versus state, and the effects of autogenic training. Auton. Neurosci. 2009, 145, 99–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Hughes, J.R.; Higgins, S.T.; Bickel, W.K.; Hunt, W.K.; Fenwick, J.W.; Gulliver, S.B.; Mireault, G.C. Caffeine self-administration, withdrawal, and adverse effects among coffee drinkers. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1991, 48, 611–617. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Thayer, J.F.; Sollers, J.J.; Ruiz-Padial, E.; Vila, J. Estimating respiratory frequency from autoregressive spectral analysis of heart period. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Mag. 2002, 21, 41–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Greenhouse, S.W.; Geisser, S. On methods in the analysis of profile data. Psychometrika 1959, 24, 95–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Winer, B.J.; Brown, D.R.; Michels, K.M. Statistical Principles in Experimental Design; McGraw-Hill: New York, NY, USA, 1991. [Google Scholar]
  27. Campos, A.R.; Barros, A.I.; Albuquerque, F.A.; Leal, L.K.A.M.; Rao, V.S. Acute effects of Guaraná (Paullinia cupana Mart.) on mouse behaviour in forced swimming and open field tests. Phytother. Res. 2005, 19, 441–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Durga, J.; van Boxtel, M.P.; Schouten, E.G.; Bots, M.L.; Kok, F.J.; Verhoef, P. Folate and the methylenetetrahydrofolate reductase 677C→T mutation correlate with cognitive performance. Neurobiol. Aging 2006, 27, 334–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Balk, E.M.; Raman, G.; Tatsioni, A.; Chung, M.; Lau, J.; Rosenberg, I.H. Vitamin B6, B12, and folic acid supplementation and cognitive function: A systematic review of randomized trials. Arch. Intern. Med. 2007, 167, 21–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Huskisson, E.; Maggini, S.; Ruf, M. The influence of micronutrients on cognitive function and performance. J. Int. Med. Res. 2007, 35, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Attwood, A.; Terry, P.; Higgs, S. Conditioned effects of caffeine on performance in humans. Physiol. Behav. 2010, 99, 286–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Lieberman, H.R.; Tharion, W.J.; Shukitt-Hale, B.; Speckman, K.L.; Tulley, R. Effects of caffeine, sleep loss, and stress on cognitive performance and mood during U.S. Navy SEAL training. Psychopharmacology 2002, 164, 250–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Friedman, B.H. An autonomic flexibility-neurovisceral integration model of anxiety and cardiac vagal tone. Biol. Psychol. 2007, 74, 185–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Hansen, A.L.; Johnsen, B.H.; Sollers, J.J.; Stenvik, K.; Thayer, J.F. Heart rate variability and its relation to prefrontal cognitive function: The effects of training and detraining. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 2004, 93, 263–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Elliot, A.J.; Payen, V.; Brisswalter, J.; Cury, F.; Thayer, J.F. A subtle threat cue, heart rate variability, and cognitive performance. Psychophysiology 2011, 48, 1340–1345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Thayer, J.F.; Lane, R.D. A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. J. Affect. Disord. 2000, 61, 201–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Jorna, P.G. Spectral analysis of heart rate and psychological state: A review of its validity as a workload index. Biol. Psychol. 1992, 34, 237–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Tattersall, A.J.; Hockey, G.R. Level of operator control and changes in heart rate variability during simulated flight maintenance. Hum. Factors 1995, 37, 682–698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Croizet, J.C.; Despres, G.; Gauzins, M.E.; Huguet, P.; Leyens, J.P.; Meot, A. Stereotype threat undermines intellectual performance by triggering a disruptive mental load. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2004, 30, 721–731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Hansen, A.L.; Johnsen, B.H.; Thayer, J.F. Relationship between heart rate variability and cognitive function during threat of shock. Anxiety Stress Coping 2009, 22, 77–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Pomportes, L.; Davranche, K.; Brisswalter, I.; Hays, A.; Brisswalter, J. Heart Rate Variability and Cognitive Function Following a Multi-Vitamin and Mineral Supplementation with Added Guarana (Paullinia cupana). Nutrients 2015, 7, 196-208. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu7010196

AMA Style

Pomportes L, Davranche K, Brisswalter I, Hays A, Brisswalter J. Heart Rate Variability and Cognitive Function Following a Multi-Vitamin and Mineral Supplementation with Added Guarana (Paullinia cupana). Nutrients. 2015; 7(1):196-208. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu7010196

Chicago/Turabian Style

Pomportes, Laura, Karen Davranche, Ioanna Brisswalter, Arnaud Hays, and Jeanick Brisswalter. 2015. "Heart Rate Variability and Cognitive Function Following a Multi-Vitamin and Mineral Supplementation with Added Guarana (Paullinia cupana)" Nutrients 7, no. 1: 196-208. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu7010196

APA Style

Pomportes, L., Davranche, K., Brisswalter, I., Hays, A., & Brisswalter, J. (2015). Heart Rate Variability and Cognitive Function Following a Multi-Vitamin and Mineral Supplementation with Added Guarana (Paullinia cupana). Nutrients, 7(1), 196-208. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu7010196

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop