Next Article in Journal
Changes in Extreme Precipitation: A Case Study in the Middle and Lower Reaches of the Yangtze River in China
Previous Article in Journal
Quantifying Roughness Coefficient Uncertainty in Urban Flooding Simulations through a Simplified Methodology
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Should We Leave? Attitudes towards Relocation in Response to Sea Level Rise

1
Faculty of Architecture and Urban Planning, Key Laboratory of New Technology for Construction of Cities in Mountain Area, Chongqing University, Chongqing 400030, China
2
Department of Urban Studies and Planning, School of Architecture, Planning and Preservation, University of Maryland, College Park, MD 20742, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Water 2017, 9(12), 941; https://doi.org/10.3390/w9120941
Submission received: 9 October 2017 / Revised: 21 November 2017 / Accepted: 1 December 2017 / Published: 4 December 2017

Abstract

:
The participation of individuals contributes significantly to the success of sea level rise adaptation. This study therefore addresses what influences people’s likelihood of relocating away from low-lying areas in response to rising sea levels. The analysis was based on a survey conducted in the City of Panama Beach in Florida (USA). Survey items relate to people’s risk perception, hazard experience, threat appraisal, and coping appraisal, whose theoretical background is Protection Motivation Theory. Descriptive and correlation analysis was first performed to highlight critical factors which were then examined by a multinomial Logit model. Results show that sea level rise awareness is the major explanatory variable. Coping appraisal is qualitatively viewed as a strong predictor for action, while threat appraisal is statistically significant in driving relocation intention. These factors should be integrated in current risk communication regarding sea level rise.

1. Introduction

Recent incidences of huge fatalities due to cyclones and hurricanes have raised strong concern about coastal vulnerability to climate change related hazards. These stimulated a major debate on quantifying climate change impacts in coastal regions, and sea level rise has been identified as a primary impact [1]. Various sources have confirmed the historical trends of sea level variations and have projected as high as 2-m sea level rise by the end of this century [2,3,4]. Unfortunately, the overwhelming majority of coasts over the globe would be exposed to sea level rise in several decades, according to the fifth report published by [4]. However, coasts have become major poles attracting population migration and economic growth; as a result, millions of people will have to cope with the adverse consequences of sea level rise, such as erosion [5], flooding [6,7], and salt water intrusion [8,9].
In response to sea level rise, governments and communities have developed different adaptation programs. The European Union is now advocating projects related to integrated coastal zone management that is a concept to deal with sea level rise issues under an overarching framework [10]. The Netherlands is a pioneer in testing different adaptation strategies, and developing countries in Asia have attempted to seek alternative ways to minimize sea level rise consequences [11]. Distinct views, though, exist with regard to which strategy should be employed. “Optimists” have confidence in the effectiveness of coastal defense systems and promote the use of protective structures such as sea walls and dikes, thereby encouraging a phenomenal increase in economic activities [12]. However, “pessimists” doubt that coastal lowlands may survive sea level rise inundation and advocate such strategies as the realignment of shorelines and complete abandonment of vulnerable areas. While these two sides have held strong debates about the prioritization of adaptation strategies, the understanding of individual-level adaptation has been inadequate. The strategies can hardly be successful in isolation from the context in which households perceive climate change risks and self-efficacy regarding hazard mitigation [13]. Hence, there exists a shift from top-down analysis of adaptation measures to the cognitive and behavioral aspects of individuals in the face of risks. Nevertheless, there are only a few studies regarding people’s perception on sea level rise. Current research gaps are twofold. First, it remains unclear which factors drive residents’ voluntary relocation decisions in anticipation of disastrous flooding [14]. Second, little research has applied Protection Motivation Theory (PMT) into the analysis of sea level rise adaptation.
This article aims at bridging these gaps by (1) examining the relocation decisions of vulnerable households in the face of hypothetical inundation due to sea level rise and (2) identifying the linkages between risk perception, hazard experience, and intended mitigation behaviors. Its innovations are twofold. First, it illuminated whether the PMT social-psychological model, incorporated with the components of Social Amplification/attenuation of Risk Framework, is applicable to such invisible threats as sea level rise. Second, the authors explored those factors that encourage or deter residents’ intention to relocate, if their residences would be inundated. The findings can help to justify whether aggressive strategies are feasible in industrialized countries—from the perspective of local residents. Local authorities can use the information to facilitate conservations with the general public and deploy efficient strategies in susceptible coasts, not necessarily the relocation option.
The rest of this article is organized as follows. We commence the paper with a literature review on people’s cognitive and social variables associated with risks. Following the review, Section 3 presents several hypotheses regarding how residents view voluntary relocation because of sea level rise. Section 4 introduces the study area and a roadmap whereby we designed, administered, and analyzed structured surveys. Section 5 highlights crucial findings, and scholarly discussions are provided in Section 6. Concluding remarks are summarized in the last section.

2. The Linkages between Risk Experience, Perception, and Mitigation Behavior

Our literature review was guided by two questions. What are the classical theories and models that explain general risk perception and mitigation behaviors? What are the crucial factors that determine residents’ response to sea level rise?
A growing body of the literature focuses on individuals’ response towards flooding, storm surge, and other sea level rise related hazards, and the majority is centered on understanding the causal relationship between risk perception and mitigation actions. The UK, a country extremely vulnerable to flooding [15], has witnessed a proliferation of relevant studies. For instance, Harris [16] suggested that the impact of flooding experience on mitigating behaviors was mediated by the beliefs in defensive structures and insurance. In other words, people having been flooded would show greater anxiety about the adequacy of protective measures and less reliance on insurance. Similarly, Soetanto et al. [17] examined people’s perception on flooding resilience in four communities in Birmingham and London and asserted that the experience with flooding, age, and additional demographic attributes were significant variables affecting people’s social responsibility regarding hazard mitigation. Lo and Chan [18] further substantiated the role of risk perception in motivating individuals’ actions. Investigating a community in the UK, the authors stated that perceived severity of flooding drove actions for flooding management more effectively than risk information alone. Moreover, the experience with wet-weather-related events may be associated with raised concerns about climate change, according to a recent national survey conducted in the UK [19]. People residing in flooding-prone areas were more likely to form the belief of climate change than those having heat-wave discomfort experience only. The relationship was further justified by Demski et al. [20], who found additional significance of direct experience of extreme weather events and claimed that citizens who were affected by severe storms in the winters of 2013/2014 showed a higher level of vulnerability assessment and climate change awareness than the other individuals.
The above-mentioned studies largely imply a pattern between the risk perception and hazard mitigation, and such a pattern can be explained by the PMT. The PMT was first introduced as a psychological model [21] and was applied in the domain of health behaviors. For example, the model can interpret how a diabetes patient perceives long-term risks associated with unhealthy eating behaviors and whether such cognitive assessment may influence his/her diet habits accordingly. It was later justified by the scholars in behavioral studies concerning earthquake [22], wildfire [23], flooding [24], and other natural hazards [25], but the applications on slow-onset disasters are rare.
A thorough survey of the literature on all types of disasters goes beyond the scope of this paper, so we primarily focused on flooding, hurricane, and other sea level rise related hazards. One major contribution of the PMT is its illustration of the linkage between hazard perception and risk reduction behaviors. For example, Frondel et al. [26] and Ling et al. [27] both confirmed that flood experience was positively correlated with adaptation efforts, while Lawrence et al. [28] maintained that the witnessing of major floods among German residents failed to encourage people to take more active mitigation actions. Therefore, attitude or perception may not be a good behavior predictor [29]. Between risk perception and behaviors, specifically, the PMT adds two crucial components of the perceptual process: threat appraisal and coping appraisal [30]. The former includes the assessments of how severe a risk is, what the associated possibility is, and the degree to which people fear the occurrence of the risk. The latter, often termed as adaptation appraisal in the climate change literature, deals with people’s confidence and capacities of protective measures—and with the costs of such protection.
Adaptation appraisal normally occurs following threat appraisal, but their relationships may be bidirectional and dynamic [31]. Applying the PMT, Zheng and Dallimer [32] found that adaptation appraisal in response to climate change was a better predictor than risk perception. In line with this finding, Werg et al. [33] suggested that crucial cognition-related factors were confidence in official information channels, role model effects, perceived risk, and costs. Furthermore, Koerth et al. [34] stated that when faced with flooding risk, coastal residents relied more on coping appraisal to judge their adaptation capacities than threat appraisal. In addition to these variables, Werg et al. [33] found that traditional household characteristics—such as age, income, residence type—may be more important in predicting mitigating behaviors than psychological counterparts.
PMT is associated with perceptual process regarding natural hazards at the individual level, but it inadequately captures the impacts of social norm. Social Amplification/attenuation of Risk Framework (SARF) addresses such deficiency. The SARF assumes that people analyze information sources through “social and individual stations” [35]. Social stations mean that what homeowners learn from social circles influences their climate change adaptation decisions substantially [36]. This effect shows that the risk perception is socially formed and could affect how people position themselves in their social networks about the concerned climatic issues [14]. In other words, individual perception of natural risks is subject to social influence [37].
Lastly, the studies about the motivation of households’ adaptation to sea level rise are scarce. When we intend to take into account the full spectrum of factors from natural to psychological dimensions, understanding residents’ response to sea level rise becomes particularly difficult. People may simply use denial as a coping skill to respond to sea level rise [38]. This distant and invisible threat has gone largely unnoticed because “people are uncertain of the effectiveness of recommended actions” and “they are motivated to control their fear through denial, defensive avoidance, or reactance ([24], p. 1478). Additionally, some residents might be unable to understand the risk of what sea level rise means [38]. In some cases, sea level rise is merely one consideration among a variety of climatic and non-climatic risks [39]. For instance, Barnett et al. [40] investigated Lakes Entrance, Port Albert, and several other coastal communities in Australia and concluded that residents’ lived values were more related to natural beauty, relaxed lifestyle, and the proximity to water, while people generally lacked the awareness of sea level rise issues. However, to the knowledge of the author, little research has investigated people’s attitudes towards a specific sea level rise adaptation strategy at the individual level, which is a focal point of this study.

3. Hypotheses

The goals of this study are to investigate (1) whether hazard experiences and perception are directly associated with people’s intention to relocate away from vulnerable areas and (2) whether the threat and coping appraisal is feasible in examining people’s adaptation responses to sea level rise. Regarding disaster experience, the following hypothesis was formulated.
Hypothesis 1.
Hazard experience and perception can affect residents’ relocation decision in anticipation to sea level rise.
While we hypothesized such a linkage, the direction of effects still deserves investigation. Flood experience could increase people’s likelihood to take mitigation measures [26,41,42], there is also evidence that past experience does not contribute to a higher level of intention to take self-protective actions [28,43].
Concerning the appraisal process, the hypothesis was structured as follows:
Hypothesis 2.
Adaptation appraisal is statistically significant in determining households’ relocation decision.
Finally, we also wanted to identify which demographic factors contribute to people’s potential relocation preferences. The hypothesis was developed as:
Hypothesis 3.
Income, age, education background, the length of residence, and other household-level variables play a vital role in affecting residents’ intention to relocate.

4. Methods

4.1. Survey Development and Administration

To validate proposed hypotheses, face-to-face interviews guided by a structured questionnaire (in Supplementary Materials) were conducted in the City of Panama and Panama Beach in Bay County, Florida (USA) (Figure 1). Bay County lies in the northeastern part of Florida and is consistently threated by storm surge, hurricane, and other coastal hazards. Since 1877 it has been stricken by more than fifteen major hurricanes that resulted in huge economic loss and fatalities [44]. Increasing exposure to flooding, combined with recent land development, makes this region extremely vulnerable to sea level rise.
The questionnaire was designed to include the major components specified in the PMT and guided by the literature about sea level rise perception. It includes four major sections with the majority of closed questions being 5-point Likert Scale: sea level rise perception [45] and hazard experience [34], people’s intention to relocate in the face of permanent inundation due to sea level rise, threat appraisal and coping appraisal [34,46], adaptation preferences [47], and respondents’ demographic and housing information. An initial draft was produced and pre-tested by planning students and professors, and the text was revised according to the feedbacks from those participants. Five hundred questionnaires were administered in public beaches, city malls, and parks from March 28 to 30, 2014, in Panama City, and the respondents were randomly selected. Interviewees who were confirmed as local residents were asked to fill out a survey with the assistance of a trained investigator. We collected 226 effective questionnaires, and the return rate was 45%. The responses on the paper copies of the questionnaires were recorded in Qualtrics, an online survey instrument platform, and then exported to Statistical Package for Social Science for data cleaning and NLogit 5 for analysis.

4.2. Variables and Analytical Procedure

Table 1 presents all cognitive variables that were included in the questionnaire. At a first step, all variables underwent a Spearman correlation analysis with the dependent variable—the intention whereby a household would relocate following sea level rise inundation. The Spearman method was applied because some variables were not normally distributed, and the data were ranked before the analysis was conducted. The dependent variable was measured as an ordinal scale—i.e., not relocate, not sure, and will relocate. The correlation analysis offered an overview regarding the potential associations between factors and the relocation decisions of households. It also helped to exclude those factors with possible multi-collinearity issues. A multinomial Logit model (MNL) was then employed to identify significant variables that influence people’s decisions of relocation. Stepwise regression methods were used to exclude insignificant variables.
Two remarks should be noted here regarding variable selection. First, in representing coping/adaptation appraisal, we wanted to include two additional variables to enrich the existing model framework. The first stated that “inundation is inevitable”, and the second represents role model effects of neighbors and relatives. However, both correlation analysis and the Logit model indicated the insignificance of these variables. Second, as responsibility is a much debated topic in the climate change literature, the respondents were asked for opinions about how they think local governments should cope with the inundation due to sea level rise. This aspect may illustrate how households want to shift adaptation responsibilities to local authorities since people might consider climate change as a societal issue—rather than individual obligation.

5. Survey Results

5.1. Descriptive Statistics

The survey responses are representative of the demographic profile of the City of Panama. Specifically, out of all the respondents 50.9% with a confident interval of 44% and 58% at 0.05 significant level are male, while the male population of the city in 2012 was 49.1% [48]. The majority of the respondents are White with a percentage of 78% (72%, 83%, α = 0.05), which corresponds to the percentage of White population in the city in 2012 (70%). Other ethnical groups have some deviations from the real data, but the magnitude of fluctuation is acceptable (for example, approximately three percent of the respondents are Asian (1%, 5%, α = 0.05), which is comparable to the read share of 2.5% in 2012). There is an underrepresentation of people over 65 (5%). Such bias may be partly attributed to our sampling technique. Since the research was majorly conducted in public places, the elderly may be physically inconvenienced to go to these places.

5.2. Threat Appraisal and Coping Appraisal

It has been suggested that coping appraisal is a better predictor for mitigating behaviors than threat appraisal [32,34]. Figure 2 appears to show such a pattern since four out of the five items with the highest scores represent coping appraisal. In anticipation of invisible threats such as sea level rise, people have many other economic and livelihood priorities—apart from the relocation option. Surprisingly, the survey shows that the respondents rank safety issues in new communities as the most crucial factor (4.30) when they consider relocation choices. This is in line with Graham et al. [49] who concluded that people when faced with sea level rise adaptation options, attached a high value to the feeling of safety. Another important aspect is the value of current properties (4.08). The abandonment or resale of a high-value property is always a difficult decision. Additional critical factors relate to the medical (4.06) and educational (4.03) aspects of new communities, followed by job opportunities (4.03). However, adaptation strategies in existing communities are the least considered items. This may be due to a lack of public awareness of local adaptation efforts.

5.3. Additional Aspects Regarding Relocation, Action, and Responsibility

Not surprisingly, the respondents consider inland areas in the same city as the most ideal relocation destinations (Figure 3) and the more distant the destination is to current residences, the lower its preference score is.
One hundred and four respondents claim that family and social ties make them unwilling to relocate (Figure 4). A lack of economic stability is another deterrent for households to take actions, as reflected by 82 respondents agreeing with the statement “The relocation costs are too high” and 81 agreeing with the statement “It is difficult to find a job at another place”. It should be further noted that current adaptation strategies are minor reasons why people do not want to relocate. It may be due to the fact that the respondents do not “feel” the existence of protective structures when they are in fact protected in the case of hurricanes and floods.
Figure 5 shows which situations can motivate households’ relocation willingness. The statement “Find a job at another place” produces a majority of agreement (120). This is followed by the statement “You feel unsafe in the area” (109). Social influence is another important determinant, generating 63 responses of agreement. However, only seven respondents think that the absence of insurance plans would urge them to consider moving. These results further confirm that people place employment as a top priority.
In order to understand citizens’ expectation on governments and communities, we created the following statements to investigate respondents’ opinions on adaptation options (Figure 6). The mean values of the statements suggest that residents prefer hard protection structures (3.12) and beach nourishment (3.12) to the other alternatives. The respondents also highly agree that governments should raise funds to address sea level rise issues (3.12). These are three top rated governmental responsibilities and reflect that people still want to rely on coastal defense systems and public funds in the face of sea level rise.

5.4. Correlation Analysis

Table 2 and Table 3 give the correlation matrixes between risk perception, hazard experience, cognitive appraisal, and the intention to relocate. First, the results indicate that risk perception has uncertain relationships with relocation intention. While the respondents’ agreement with the fact of sea level rise (r = 0.235, p < 0.01) and its impacts on natural hazards (r = 0.297, p < 0.01) is positively associated with relocation willingness, the statement “sea level rise is a threat to our natural/built environments” has a negative effect. However, hazard experience largely has insignificant association with relocation intention. This is intuitively reasonable since previous studies have already drawn similar conclusions [28], and since people who have first-hand experience but survive disasters may have a lower level of willingness to take actions in the future [50]. Regarding coping and threat appraisal, the results are quite surprising. The majority of appraisal items turn out to be no association with relocation intention. Only the damage cost variable shows a moderately positive relationship (r = 0.189, p < 0.01) with relocation propensity. The insignificance of most variables may be partially due to a mixture of categorical and ordinal variables. Continuous data are absent in the model, which makes the correlation less obvious.

5.5. Multinomial Logit Model

Table 4 shows the regression results of a MNL between household’s relocation choices, cognitive variables, and social-demographical factors. The model’s log likelihood at convergence is −140.118. The log likelihood ratio test for the model is far beyond the corresponding critical Chi-squared value, suggesting that the included variables offer a good model fit. The table presents significant parameters for the choices of “Not sure” and “Will relocate”, with “Not move” being the base category.
Among the variables regarding risk perception and hazard experience, individuals believing that sea level rise is happening are more likely to relocate if their primary residences would be inundated due to change sea levels. Similarly, individuals with good knowledge of the effects of sea level rise on environmental hazards have a higher willingness to move. This may be related to the positive linkage between climate change awareness and mitigation behavior. People who admit that sea level rise threatens the natural environment are more inclined to stay if inundation would happen in the proximity of their homes. This is hard to interpret. A possible explanation is that they may think sea level rise is by large a natural process and therefore has no impacts on urban systems. People experiencing other types of hazards (wildfire, landslide, and so on) show a lower preference of relocation in anticipation of sea level rise flooding. However, the majority of hazard experience variables are statistically insignificant, which is in line with the correlation analysis.
While more than ten appraisal related variables were tested, only three are significant in the final specification. The insignificance of coping appraisal variables may be related to the fact that those variables are more relevant to such behaviors as migration and job needs, rather than the relocation response to sea level rise inundation. Moreover, two variables that are considered low priority in Section 5.2 appear to have substantial effects on people’s relocation choices, after we controlled the other variables. Specifically, people attaching a high value on existing adaptation strategies tend to reject the relocation decision. This may be because these individuals are confident that current seawalls, beach nourishment projects, and other countermeasures are sufficient to address sea level rise threat. Additionally, the negative coefficient of insurance suggests that individuals who consider flooding insurance as a very important factor prefer staying within hazard-prone regions. Lastly, as expected, individuals who fear the high damage cost due to sea level rise inundation would be more inclined to relocate.
In terms of social-demographic variables, adults aged between 18 and 45 are likely to stay in the events of sea level rise inundation, presumably because the population of this age group has stable income sources and maintains good social ties. It is therefore difficult for them to discard these life aspects and move to a new city. The influence of education variables suggests that people with college degrees or less are reluctant to move before the catastrophic inundation due to sea level rise. Additionally, the coefficients of the gender variable indicate that male individuals prefer leaving current residences only if inundation would happen. The only significant race variable is African American, and the magnitude of the coefficient is the largest among all the factors (4.300). It has been claimed that African American is a socially vulnerable group to natural hazards [51]. Therefore, that African Americans are more likely to relocate due to sea level rise is intuitively understandable because people with fewer resources and adaptive capacities are more willing to respond to natural threats [52].

6. Discussion

This paper contributes to a small body of the literature available regarding the effect of risk perception and cognitive appraisal of preparedness on the relocation intention of vulnerable households before sea level rise inundation. The authors identified some emerging patterns of how people respond to distant threats such as sea level rise and climate change.
Theoretically, well-educated adults would be likely to take individual-level adaptation strategies. As indicated by our results, however, people with college degrees do not want to relocate voluntarily. The root cause of such phenomenon may be a lack of the understanding of the risks. College graduates, often belonging to the high-income group, trust their own judgment and believe that sea walls and levees can protect them from floods, storm surges, and hurricanes [53]. Another worrying aspect of the false trust is that people think governments would provide total security for their properties, because urban developments are tolerated or even encouraged in hazard-prone regions [54]. Residents would expect that governments’ obligation is to undertake high-cost adaptation strategies, while they can thereby take precautionary measures of low costs and efforts [34].
Moreover, individuals have difficulties to visualize sea level rise and are therefore unaware of the risk. People hardly have direct experience of being flooded by sea level rise. Scientists always stress that sea level will increase dramatically, but such projections depend on a long time period up to several decades. Additionally, local residents envisage sea level rise by their time stories where past and present lived experience of places is connected with the future, while researchers are spatially isolated from these places [55]. The ways by which residents and scientists construct the images of climatic change are therefore incongruent. Furthermore, the invisibility of changing sea levels fails to convince people of its adverse consequences [56], and thus some people just deny the risk before thinking about any further countermeasures. How to raise the public’s awareness about sea level rise has been long discussed. Recent incidences of “King Tide” provide one possible solution. As a result of sea level rise, every year for several times the water overtopping of seawalls in Miami and the California coasts inundates major roads and forces local authorities to use pumps to drain the water. “King Tide” exposes people to higher water levels temporarily, and people with “King Tide” experience would have a stronger feeling toward sea level rise. Similar conclusions have been drawn by Zaalberg et al. [57].
People are emotionally attached to their familiar places [58] and reluctant to move to new environments. Ideally, future land use planning should leave room for inland development and develop economic strategies to attract migrants to less developed hinterlands. However, the implementation of such policies is difficult since people have additional economic considerations with high priorities [49] such as the high values of coastal properties. It appears that people are more concerned about livelihood stability than the mere exposure to natural hazards. Finally, the respondents did not want governments to purchase the land at risk. The implementation of this policy is financially difficult, and governments’ taking up coastward lands would be unfavored by residents—because governments may abandon scenic residential and commercial units in low-lying areas.
It should be emphasized that the consequences of sea level rise are diversified. Sea level rise affects coasts not only by inundating urban lands. It depletes sand volume along shorelines and drives wetlands to migrate inland [59]. However, human activities may lead to wetland loss and coastal squeeze, because seawalls cut off the migrating routes for wetlands and thereby result in widespread wetland drowning [6]. In addition, seawalls cannot entirely prevent sea water that may slip underneath the structures and invade fresh water systems [60]. Wetland loss, coastal squeeze, and salt water intrusion further expose urban areas to coastal hazards. However, many respondents we interviewed are unaware of these consequences. Thus, we cannot expect people having a low threat appraisal would take mitigation actions.
The communication of risk and adaptation should focus on increasing individuals’ confidence of their ability to relocate. Our results have shown that the most important factors related to relocation are job opportunities and the cost of actions. To secure enough job openings requires a coordinated work between the governments of hazard prone areas and potential relocation places. However, it is also challenging to let coastal governments abandon their labor forces. Thus, the empirical pillars of future research on this topic are needed to justify the feasibility of relocation. Regarding relocation costs, there has been ample evidence that governments can “use disasters as opportunities” to facilitate relocation [61]. For example, following Hurricane Katrina, affected households were offered a buyout program which allowed people to sell their properties at pre-storm market values if they opted to relocate—rather than rebuilding homes [62]. Such financial incentives have contributed to considerable resettlement of the displacees.
Social ties are another hindrance of relocation. Also, social circles are important in the decision making of individuals’ mitigating behavior, indicating that the campaign of adaptation strategies could focus on the community level. Educating the community as a whole is an essential ingredient in the recipe of raising public acceptance of the planned retreat option. Behavioral studies indicate that attitudes and behaviors are strongly influenced by subjective norms that are subjective to the belief of social groups [29,63]. A person may become more active to consider relocating in response to sea level rise, if his/her neighbors or community members expect he/her to do so. This requires a dialogue between governments and communities in a coordinated and communication-rich system.
However, several limitations of the current study must be acknowledged. First, empirical components are still lacking. Our results primarily rely on a stated preference survey. All situations are hypothetical, and therefore the findings deserve further investigation. It is recommended that an experimental survey is conducted in a concrete context—for example, “King Tide”—to explore people’s attitudes towards sea level rise. Second, an examination of the other adaptation strategies is needed in future research. This paper only investigated how people view planned retreat—the most aggressive adaptation option. Nonetheless, individuals’ opinions on seawalls, beach nourishment, and other forms of sea level rise adaptation are equally important. A comparison of the perceptions of different strategies is captivating and can contribute significantly to climate change resilience literature. Third, although this study applied the PMT, the measurements for coping and threat appraisal can be refined. Currently, none of the coping appraisal items is significant in explaining the relocation propensity of the respondents. One potential problem is the representation of coping appraisal in our model, and it may be corrected by a refinement and the re-specification of model structures. In addition, the variable representing social influence is insignificant in the model, and we fail to incorporate the SARF into the PMT. We stress the importance of social norms in shaping people’s beliefs in vulnerability and driving their propensity of protective measures, but this study merely considered the effects of neighbors’ action in the correlation analysis. Better representations of social norms should be constructed, and how the norms play a mediating role between risk perception and behavior could be further answered in the following-up research—either by multivariate regression or path dependency theories. Finally, the model specification may have bias since insignificant variables were included, and the application of some other specifications such as ordered Logit model could address such bias—given that adequate choice sets are available.

7. Conclusions

This paper addresses the overall question of what affects coastal residents’ intention to relocate in anticipation of sea level rise inundation. It relies on a household survey that was conducted in the City of Panama and Panama Beach, Florida (USA). The framework of the survey is based on the factors associated with the PMT in order to understand what motivates people’s adaptation to distant threats such as sea level rise. Three hypotheses are examined. First, sea level rise awareness is positively associated with people’s willingness to relocate their vulnerable houses. However, hazard experience appears to have an insignificant relationship with relocation decisions. Second, in the case of sea level rise, threat appraisal seems a better predictor than coping appraisal. In other words, there exist additionally unobserved factors related to coping appraisal that were not covered in this study, since relocation itself is a complex decision and involves the process of prioritizing a variety of non-climatic and climatic risks. Third, the characteristics of residents—education, gender, age, and race—influence the likelihood of implementing the planned retreat strategies in response to sea level rise. Unlike other acute and visible risks such as flooding and hurricane, sea level rise is a slow ongoing and invisible phenomenon. This means that our current communication campaigns should be refined in several aspects. The campaigns should illuminate the severe impacts of sea level rise on coastal hazards, focus on raising households’ confidence in adaptation measures, and inform people at the community level about the benefits of planned retreat.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at www.mdpi.com/2073-4441/9/12/941/s1.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Zhong-Ren Peng for his comments on the survey and research design. The authors also thank the following people for their assistance in collecting survey data: Yisi Liu, Mohamed Mansour Goma, and Chao Liu. This study was funded by the Florida Sea Grant, Grant No. R/GOM-RP-2, “A Parameterized Climate Change Projection Model for Hurricane Flooding, Wave Action, Economic Damages, and Population Dynamics”. It was also funded by the Chongqing Social Science Grant (No. 2017BS06), “An Assessment of Flooding Resilience in Mountainous Rural Areas, Southwest China”. The publication of this paper was supported by the start-up fund for young scholars from Chongqing University. Last but not least, two anonymous reviewers are greatly appreciated for their constructive comments that helped to improve an earlier version of the manuscript.

Author Contributions

Jie Song and Binbin Peng designed this study and were primary investigators for questionnaire distribution. Binbin Peng conducted data analysis and modeling building. The manuscript was prepared by Jie Song and later revised by Binbin Peng.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Liu, W.-C.; Liu, H.-M. Assessing the Impacts of Sea Level Rise on Salinity Intrusion and Transport Time Scales in a Tidal Estuary, Taiwan. Water 2014, 6, 324–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Jevrejeva, S.; Jackson, L.P.; Riva, R.E.; Grinsted, A.; Moore, J.C. Coastal sea level rise with warming above 2 °C. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2016, 113, 13342–13347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Kousky, C. Managing shoreline retreat: A US perspective. Clim. Chang. 2014, 124, 9–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. The Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change. Summary for Policymakers. In Climate Change 2013: The Physical Science Basis, Contribution of Working Group I to the Fifth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change; Stocker, T.F., Solomon, S., Qin, D., Manning, M., Chen, Z., Marquis, M., Averyt, K.B., Tignor, M., Mille, H.L., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK; New York, NY, USA, 2013; pp. 1–30. [Google Scholar]
  5. Per, B. The Bruun Rule of Erosion by Sea-Level Rise: A Discussion on Large-Scale Two- and Three-Dimensional Usages. J. Coast. Res. 1988, 4, 627–648. [Google Scholar]
  6. Song, J.; Fu, X.; Wang, R.; Peng, Z.-R.; Gu, Z. Does planned retreat matter? Investigating land use change under the impacts of flooding induced by sea level rise. Mitig. Adapt. Strateg. Glob. Chang. 2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Chinh, D.T.; Gain, A.K.; Dung, N.V.; Haase, D.; Kreibich, H. Multi-Variate Analyses of Flood Loss in Can Tho City, Mekong Delta. Water 2016, 8, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Werner, A.D.; Simmons, C.T. Impact of Sea-Level Rise on Sea Water Intrusion in Coastal Aquifers. Ground Water 2009, 47, 197–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Luoma, S.; Okkonen, J. Impacts of Future Climate Change and Baltic Sea Level Rise on Groundwater Recharge, Groundwater Levels, and Surface Leakage in the Hanko Aquifer in Southern Finland. Water 2014, 6, 3671–3700. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Pickaver, A.H.; Gilbert, C.; Breton, F. An indicator set to measure the progress in the implementation of integrated coastal zone management in Europe. Ocean Coast. Manag. 2004, 47, 449–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Smajgl, A.; Toan, T.Q.; Nhan, D.K.; Ward, J.; Trung, N.H.; Tri, L.Q.; Tri, V.P.D.; Vu, P.T. Responding to rising sea levels in the Mekong Delta. Nat. Clim. Chang. 2015, 5, 167–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Nicholls, R.J.; Marinova, N.; Lowe, J.A.; Brown, S.; Vellinga, P.; de Gusmão, D.; Hinkel, J.; Tol, R.S.J. Sea-level rise and its possible impacts given a ‘beyond 4 degrees C world’ in the twenty-first century. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. A Math. Phys. Eng. Sci. 2011, 369, 161–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Amos, E.; Akpan, U.; Ogunjobi, K. Households’ perception and livelihood vulnerability to climate change in a coastal area of Akwa Ibom State, Nigeria. Environ. Dev. Sustain. 2015, 17, 887–908. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Lo, A.Y. The role of social norms in climate adaptation: Mediating risk perception and flood insurance purchase. Glob. Environ. Chang. Hum. Policy Dimens. 2013, 23, 1249–1257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Boyer-Villemaire, U.; Bernatchez, Z.; Benavente, J.; Cooper, J.A.G. Quantifying community’s functional awareness of coastal changes and hazards from citizen perception analysis in Canada, UK and Spain. Ocean Coast. Manag. 2014, 93, 106–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Harries, T. The anticipated emotional consequences of adaptive behaviour-impacts on the take-up of household flood-protection measures. Environ. Plan. A 2012, 44, 649–668. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Soetanto, R.; Mullins, A.; Achour, N. The perceptions of social responsibility for community resilience to flooding: The impact of past experience, age, gender and ethnicity. Nat. Hazards 2017, 86, 1105–1126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Lo, A.Y.; Chan, F. Preparing for flooding in England and Wales: The role of risk perception and the social context in driving individual action. Nat. Hazards 2017, 88, 367–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Taylor, A.; de Bruin, W.B.; Dessai, S. Climate Change Beliefs and Perceptions of Weather-Related Changes in the United Kingdom. Risk Anal. 2014, 34, 1995–2004. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Demski, C.; Capstick, S.; Pidgeon, N.; Sposato, R.G.; Spence, A. Experience of extreme weather affects climate change mitigation and adaptation responses. Clim. Chang. 2017, 140, 149–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Rogers, R.W. Cognitive and psychological processes in fear appeals and attitude change: A revised theory of protection motivation. In Social Psychophysiology: A Sourcebook; Guilford: New York, NY, USA, 1983; pp. 153–176. [Google Scholar]
  22. De Man, A.; Simpson-Housley, P. Correlates of Responses to Two Potential Hazards. J. Soc. Psychol. 1988, 128, 385–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Kumagai, Y.; Carroll, M.S.; Cohn, P. Coping with Interface Wildfire as a Human Event: Lessons from the Disaster/Hazards Literature. J. For. 2004, 102, 28–32. [Google Scholar]
  24. Kievik, M.; Gutteling, J.M. Yes, we can: Motivate Dutch citizens to engage in self-protective behavior with regard to flood risks. Nat. Hazards 2011, 59, 1475–1490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Guion, D.T.; Scammon, D.L.; Borders, A.L. Weathering the Storm: A Social Marketing Perspective on Disaster Preparedness and Response with Lessons from Hurricane Katrina. J. Public Policy Mark. 2007, 26, 20–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Frondel, M.; Simora, M.; Sommer, S. Risk Perception of Climate Change: Empirical Evidence for Germany. Ecol. Econ. 2017, 137, 173–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Ling, F.H.; Tamura, M.; Yasuhara, K.; Ajima, K.; Van Trinh, C. Reducing flood risks in rural households: Survey of perception and adaptation in the Mekong delta. Clim. Chang. 2015, 132, 209–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Lawrence, J.; Quade, D.; Becker, J. Integrating the effects of flood experience on risk perception with responses to changing climate risk. Nat. Hazards 2014, 74, 1773–1794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Scolobig, A.; De Marchi, B.; Borga, M. The missing link between flood risk awareness and preparedness: Findings from case studies in an Alpine Region. Nat. Hazards 2012, 63, 499–520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Grothmann, T.; Reusswig, F. People at risk of flooding: Why some residents take precautionary action while others do not. Nat. Hazards 2006, 38, 101–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Begg, C.; Christian, K.; Maximilian, U.; Torsten, M. Interactions between citizen responsibilization, flood experience and household resilience: Insights from the 2013 flood in Germany. Int. J. Water Resour. Dev. 2017, 33, 591–608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Zheng, Y.; Dallimer, M. What motivates rural households to adapt to climate change? Clim. Dev. 2016, 8, 110–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Werg, J.; Grothmann, T.; Schmidt, P.; Werg, J.; Grothmann, T.; Schmidt, P. Assessing social capacity and vulnerability of private households to natural hazards—Integrating psychological and governance factors. Nat. Hazards Earth Syst. Sci. 2013, 13, 1613–1628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Koerth, J.; Vafeidis, A.T.; Hinkel, J.; Sterr, H. What motivates coastal households to adapt pro-actively to sea-level rise and increasing flood risk? Reg. Environ. Chang. 2013, 13, 897–909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Renn, O. The social amplification/attenuation of risk framework: Application to climate change. Clim. Chang. 2011, 2, 154–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Grothmann, T.; Patt, A. Adaptive capacity and human cognition: The process of individual adaptation to climate change. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2005, 15, 199–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Ruddell, D.; Harlan, S.L.; Grossman-Clarke, S.; Chowell, G. Scales of perception: Public awareness of regional and neighborhood climates. Clim. Chang. 2012, 111, 581–607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. McLennan, J.; Every, D.; Bearman, C.; Wright, L. On the concept of denial of natural hazard risk and its use in relation to householder wildfire safety in Australia. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 2017, 21, 176–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Elrick-Barr, C.; Glavovic, B.; Kay, R. A tale of two atoll nations: A comparison of risk, resilience, and adaptive response of Kiribati and the Maldives. In Climate Change and the Coast; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2015; pp. 313–336. [Google Scholar]
  40. Barnett, J.; Fincher, R.; Hurlimann, A.; Graham, S.; Mortreux, C. Equitable Local Outcomes in Adaptation to Sea-Level Rise: Final Project Report; The University of Melbourne: Melbourne, Australia, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  41. Shao, W.; Xian, S.; Keim, B.D.; Goidel, K.; Lin, N. Understanding perceptions of changing hurricane strength along the US Gulf coast. Int. J. Climatol. 2017, 37, 1716–1727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Bukvic, A.; Owen, G. Attitudes towards relocation following Hurricane Sandy: Should we stay or should we go? Disasters 2017, 41, 101–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Bubeck, P.; Botzen, W.J.; Aerts, J.C. A review of risk perceptions and other factors that influence flood mitigation behavior. Risk Anal. 2012, 32, 1481–1495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  44. Hurricane City. The History with Tropical Systems in Panama City, Florida. 2017. Available online: http://www.hurricanecity.com/city/panamacity.htm (accessed on 10 November 2017).
  45. Kettle, N.P.; Dow, K. Comparing Coastal Planner Expectations of Change to Climate Science Projections. J. Environ. Policy Plan. 2015, 17, 475–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Koerth, J.; Jones, N.; Vafeidis, A.T.; Dimitrakopoulos, P.G.; Melliou, A.; Chatzidimitriou, E.; Koukoulas, S. Household adaptation and intention to adapt to coastal flooding in the Axios—Loudias—Aliakmonas National Park, Greece. Ocean Coast. Manag. 2013, 82, 43–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Richard, J.T.K.; Robert, J.N.; Sachooda, R.; Michele, C.; James, A.; Earle, N.B. Technological Options for Adaptation to Climate Change in Coastal Zones. J. Coast. Res. 2001, 17, 531–543. [Google Scholar]
  48. U.S. Census Bureau. Total Population in Bay County. 2015. Available online: http://factfinder.census.gov/faces/tableservices/jsf/pages/productview.xhtml?pid=DEC_10_SF1_P1&prodType=table (accessed on 8 June 2015).
  49. Graham, S.; Barnett, J.; Fincher, R.; Hurlimann, A.; Mortreux, C. Local values for fairer adaptation to sea-level rise: A typology of residents and their lived values in Lakes Entrance, Australia. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2014, 29, 41–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Norris, F.H.; Smith, T.; Kaniasty, K. Revisiting the Experience-Behavior Hypothesis: The Effects of Hurricane Hugo on Hazard Preparedness and Other Self-Protective Acts. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1999, 21, 37–47. [Google Scholar]
  51. Cutter, S.L.; Boruff, B.J.; Shirley, W.L. Social vulnerability to environmental hazards. Soc. Sci. Q. 2003, 84, 242–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Tobin, G.A.; Whiteford, L.M.; Jones, E.C.; Murphy, A.D.; Garren, S.J.; Padros, C.V. The role of individual well-being in risk perception and evacuation for chronic vs. acute natural hazards in Mexico. Appl. Geogr. 2011, 31, 700–711. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Ludy, J.; Kondolf, G.M. Flood risk perception in lands “protected” by 100-year levees. Nat. Hazards 2012, 61, 829–842. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Cigler, B.A. The “Big Questions” of Katrina and the 2005 great flood of New Orleans. Public Adm. Rev. 2007, 67, 64–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Fincher, R.; Barnett, J.; Graham, S.; Hurlimann, A. Time stories: Making sense of futures in anticipation of sea-level rise. Geoforum 2014, 56, 201–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Burningham, K.; Fielding, J.; Thrush, D. ‘It’ll never happen to me’: Understanding public awareness of local flood risk. Disasters 2008, 32, 216–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Zaalberg, R.; Midden, C.; Meijnders, A.; McCalley, T. Prevention, Adaptation, and Threat Denial: Flooding Experiences in the Netherlands. Risk Anal. 2009, 29, 1759–1778. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Adger, W.N.; Barnett, J.; Brown, K.; Marshall, N.; O’Brien, K. Cultural dimensions of climate change impacts and adaptation. Nat. Clim. Chang. 2012, 3, 112–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Kirwan, M.L.; Megonigal, J.P. Tidal wetland stability in the face of human impacts and sea-level rise. Nature 2013, 504, 53–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Pulido-Leboeuf, P. Seawater intrusion and associated processes in a small coastal complex aquifer (Castell de Ferro, Spain). Appl. Geochem. 2004, 19, 1517–1527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Becker, S.L.; Reusser, D.E. Disasters as opportunities for social change: Using the multi-level perspective to consider the barriers to disaster-related transitions. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 2016, 18, 75–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Maly, E.; Kondo, T.; Banba, M. Experience from the United States: Post-Katrina and Sandy. In Land Use Management in Disaster Risk Reduction: Practice and Cases from a Global Perspective; Banba, M., Shaw, R., Eds.; Springer: Japan, Tokyo, 2017; pp. 79–106. [Google Scholar]
  63. Ajzen, I.; Fishbein, M. Understanding Attitudes and Predicting Social Behavior; Prentice-Hall: Upper Saddle River, NJ, USA, 1980. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. The cities where the survey study was conducted.
Figure 1. The cities where the survey study was conducted.
Water 09 00941 g001
Figure 2. Mean values of key factors associated with threat and coping appraisal that affect households’ decision to relocate. (5—very important; 1—not important at all).
Figure 2. Mean values of key factors associated with threat and coping appraisal that affect households’ decision to relocate. (5—very important; 1—not important at all).
Water 09 00941 g002
Figure 3. Mean values of residents’ responses toward the question: please rate the preference of the following places where you would move. (5—the most preferred; 1—the least preferred).
Figure 3. Mean values of residents’ responses toward the question: please rate the preference of the following places where you would move. (5—the most preferred; 1—the least preferred).
Water 09 00941 g003
Figure 4. A description of the denial of risk (response frequencies).
Figure 4. A description of the denial of risk (response frequencies).
Water 09 00941 g004
Figure 5. Motivation of relocation action (response frequencies).
Figure 5. Motivation of relocation action (response frequencies).
Water 09 00941 g005
Figure 6. Responses to the governmental responsibilities concerning sea level rise adaptation (mean values).
Figure 6. Responses to the governmental responsibilities concerning sea level rise adaptation (mean values).
Water 09 00941 g006
Table 1. An overview of perception and appraisal variables used in the analysis.
Table 1. An overview of perception and appraisal variables used in the analysis.
CategoryDescriptionCoding Scheme
Risk perceptionTo what degree do you agree with the fact that sea level rise is occurring?Likert Scale: 1–5
To what degree do you consider sea level rise as a threat to our future natural environment?5-point scale: 1 (not serious at all) to 5 (very serious)
To what degree do you consider sea level rise as a threat to our built environment?
Do you believe that sea level rise intensifies extreme weather events, e.g., floods, hurricanes and storm surges, etc.?Binary scale: 1 (yes), 0 (no)
Hazard experienceHave you experienced any damage due to the following extreme weather events?
Flooding
Extreme precipitation
Hurricane
Other disasters
Binary scale: 1 (yes), 0 (no)
Coping/adaptation appraisalTo what extent can each of the following factors affect your decision to move or not?
Job opportunities of new communities
The distance of new communities to current work places
The distance of new communities to current primary home residence
Infrastructure quality of new communities
School quality of new communities
Health care services of new communities
Social and family ties of new communities
Social activities within new communities
Safety issues like crime rates in new communities Relocation costs
5-point scale: 1 (not important at all) to 5 (very important)
Threat appraisalTo what extent can each of the following factors affect your decision to move or not?
Current adaptation strategies including seawalls, sand dunes, and marshes
Cost of damage to existing homes
Flooding insurance policies of existing communities
The value of your existing properties
5-point scale: 1 (not important at all) to 5 (very important)
Threat appraisalUnder what conditions would you consider moving?
Inundation would happen
Binary scale: 1 (yes), 0 (no)
Social InfluenceUnder what conditions would you consider moving?
Your family, relatives, or neighbors decide to move
Binary scale: 1 (yes), 0 (no)
The following variables were not included in the correlation and Logit modeling analysis.
Relocation destinationsPlease rate the preference of the following places where you would move?
Move inland but in the same city
Move out of the city but in the same county
Move out of the county but within the same state
Move into an adjacent state
Move into a non-contiguous state
Elevate existing house
Do nothing
5-point scale: 1 (least preferred) to 5 (most preferred)
Motivation of inaction and relocation, and the shift of responsibility
-
If you decide not to move, what are the major reasons?
-
Under what conditions would you consider moving? (except the social influence and inundation variable)
-
What should local communities and governments do to cope with inundation due to sea level rise from your perspective?
Measurements with binary scale: 1 (yes) or 0 (no)
Table 2. Spearman correlation between relocation decision, risk perception, and hazard experience.
Table 2. Spearman correlation between relocation decision, risk perception, and hazard experience.
12345678910
1 Relocation response-
2 Sea level rise fact0.235 ***-
3 Sea level rise and disaster0.297 ***0.149 **-
4 Sea level rise and natural environment−0.440 ***−0.208 ***−0.382 ***-
5 Sea level rise and built environment−0.368 ***−0.196 ***−0.320 ***0.745 ***-
6 Flood experience0.0550.1000.105−0.031−0.095-
7 Extreme precipitation experience0.0140.0560.0600.024−0.0330.072-
8 Hurricane experience−0.0100.045−0.1000.0950.0960.0210.049-
9 Storm Surge experience0.0770.1390.032−0.133−0.0790.150 **0.0160.183 **-
10 Other hazard experience−0.148 **−0.063−0.1190.0410.088−0.101−0.118−0.138−0.110-
Notes: *** significant at 99% level; ** significant at 95% level.
Table 3. Spearman correlation between relocation decision, threat appraisal, and coping appraisal.
Table 3. Spearman correlation between relocation decision, threat appraisal, and coping appraisal.
111121314151617181920212223242526
1 Relocation response-
11 Job chances in new community0.036-
12 Distance to current workplace0.0610.503 ***-
13 Distance to home0.1110.344 ***0.794 ***-
14 Facility0.1310.400 ***0.348 ***0.391 ***-
15 School0.0260.370 ***0.416 ***0.435 ***0.481 ***-
16 Medical service0.0750.307 ***0.343 ***0.343 ***0.489 ***0.544 ***-
17 Social ties0.0880.206 ***0.277 ***0.367 ***0.422 ***0.444 ***0.404 ***-
18 Social activities0.1230.282 ***0.263 ***0.285 ***0.382 ***0.354 ***0.393 ***0.512 ***-
19 Relocation cost0.1240.373 ***0.401 ***0.399 ***0.363 ***0.344 ***0.322 ***0.321 ***0.250 ***-
20 Safety of new community0.0350.310 ***0.350 ***0.337 ***0.438 ***0.461 ***0.529 ***0.322 ***0.236 ***0.471 ***-
21 Adaptation strategies of current community0.0750.1370.299 ***0.376 ***0.457 ***0.338 ***0.485 ***0.485 ***0.419 ***0.360 ***0.371 ***-
22 Damage cost0.189 ***0.305 ***0.315 ***0.337 ***0.537 ***0.359 ***0.510 ***0.504 ***0.319 ***0.557 ***0.456 ***0.583 ***-
23 Existing flood insurance0.0510.260 ***0.310 ***0.398 ***0.539 ***0.358 ***0.453 ***0.436 ***0.344 ***0.452 ***0.396 ***0.571 ***0.728 ***-
24 Current property value0.1190.295 ***0.307 ***0.328 ***0.456 ***0.345 ***0.452 ***0.358 ***0.243 ***0.448 ***0.424 ***0.358 ***0.544 ***0.562 ***-
25 Inundation is inevitable0.010−0.102−0.152 **−0.166 **−0.086−0.191 ***−0.010−0.0700.004−0.128−0.079−0.080−0.041−0.079−0.049-
26 Neighbor’s action0.0240.0140.0710.0760.1070.0920.1290.227 ***0.1260.1060.208 ***0.148 **0.0690.0290.030−0.024-
Notes: *** significant at 99% level; ** significant at 95% level.
Table 4. Household’s relocation choice model.
Table 4. Household’s relocation choice model.
VariablesNot SureWill Relocate
Parametert-StatisticParametert-Statistic
Constant1.7042.7861.0503.225
Risk perception and hazard experience
Sea level rise is occurring0.521 *0.2730.737 **0.295
Sea level rise and hazards0.6080.6251.516 **0.673
Sea lever rise and natural environment−0.756 *0.450−1.279 **0.491
Hazard experience (other types)−1.0811.009−3.681 **1.536
Threat and coping appraisal
Adaptation strategies of existing communities (sea walls and beach nourishment)−0.6790.424−0.789 *0.476
Flood insurance police of existing community0.0280.499−1.118 **0.555
Cost damage to existing homes0.5980.5871.684 **0.667
Household-level characteristics
Age between 18 and 45−1.511 **0.715−1.318 *0.783
High school graduate or less−1.846 *1.092−2.409 **1.141
College degree−1.2441.010−1.963 *1.074
Male0.0990.6161.166 *0.653
American African1.7311.8174.300 **1.973
Log likelihood
Convergence−140.118
Constant only−213.150
Number of observations196
Notes: ** significant at 95% level; * significant at 90% level.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Song, J.; Peng, B. Should We Leave? Attitudes towards Relocation in Response to Sea Level Rise. Water 2017, 9, 941. https://doi.org/10.3390/w9120941

AMA Style

Song J, Peng B. Should We Leave? Attitudes towards Relocation in Response to Sea Level Rise. Water. 2017; 9(12):941. https://doi.org/10.3390/w9120941

Chicago/Turabian Style

Song, Jie, and Binbin Peng. 2017. "Should We Leave? Attitudes towards Relocation in Response to Sea Level Rise" Water 9, no. 12: 941. https://doi.org/10.3390/w9120941

APA Style

Song, J., & Peng, B. (2017). Should We Leave? Attitudes towards Relocation in Response to Sea Level Rise. Water, 9(12), 941. https://doi.org/10.3390/w9120941

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop