Next Article in Journal
A New Objective Function for the Recovery of Gielis Curves
Next Article in Special Issue
Mental Rotation Ability: Right or Left Hemisphere Competence? What We Can Learn from Callosotomized and Psychotic Patients
Previous Article in Journal
A Double Generally Weighted Moving Average Chart for Monitoring the COM-Poisson Processes
Previous Article in Special Issue
Keep a Left Profile, Baby! The Left-Cradling Bias Is Associated with a Preference for Left-Facing Profiles of Human Babies
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Hypnotizability-Related Asymmetries: A Review

by
Vilfredo De Pascalis
1 and
Enrica Laura Santarcangelo
2,*
1
Department of Psychology, University of Rome La Sapienza, 00185 Rome, Italy
2
Department of Translational Research and New Technologies in Medicine and Surgery, University of Pisa, 56127 Pisa, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Symmetry 2020, 12(6), 1015; https://doi.org/10.3390/sym12061015
Submission received: 24 March 2020 / Revised: 22 April 2020 / Accepted: 7 May 2020 / Published: 16 June 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Symmetry and Asymmetry in Brain Behavior and Perception)

Abstract

:
Hypnotizability is a dispositional trait reflecting the individual ability to modify perception, memory and behavior according to imaginative suggestions. It is measured by validated scales that classify the general population in high (highs), medium (mediums) and low (lows) hypnotizable persons, predicts the individual proneness to respond to suggestions, and is particularly popular in the field of the cognitive control of pain and anxiety. Different hypnotizability levels, however, have been associated with specific brain morpho-functional characteristics and with peculiarities in the cognitive, sensorimotor and cardiovascular domains also in the ordinary state of consciousness and in the absence of specific suggestions. The present scoping review was undertaken to summarize the asymmetries observed in the phenomenology and physiological correlates of hypnosis and hypnotizability as possible indices of related hemispheric prevalence. It presents the findings of 137 papers published between 1974 and 2019. In summary, in the ordinary state of consciousness, behavioral, neurophysiological and neuroimaging investigations have revealed hypnotizability related asymmetries mainly consisting of pre-eminent left hemisphere information processing/activation in highs, and no asymmetries or opposite directions of them in lows. The described asymmetries are discussed in relation to the current theories of hypnotizability and hypnosis.

1. Introduction

The story of medical hypnosis started long ago, in Switzerland, with Paracelsus and tells of various definitions and interpretations of this phenomenon, from mesmerism to psychoanalysis [1]. In the 20th century, two different approaches to it—clinical and experimental—were proposed. Clinical hypnosis, named also Ericksonian owing to the great work performed by Milton Erickson [2] in the clinical field, is based on psychoanalytic assumptions, considers hypnosis as a tool to access the unconscious mind and states that any person can be hypnotized [3]. Experimental hypnosis leaves any personality theory out of consideration. It conceptualizes the individual susceptibility to experience hypnosis as a measurable personality trait that has a high test-retest reliability [4,5] and has been found to be associated with a number of cognitive-emotional characteristics such as sustained attentional abilities, vividness of imagery, fantasy proneness, creativity and emotionality [6,7,8,9,10]. Hypnotizability predicts the proneness to modify perception, memory and behavior according to specific instructions named suggestions in both the awake and hypnotic state [11].
In recent decades, experimental findings related to the physiological mechanisms sustaining hypnotic phenomenology have made the edge between the clinical and experimental approach less clear-cut [12,13]. In fact, on one hand hypnotizability does predict the response to suggestions [11], and the effects of suggestions administered in the ordinary state of consciousness are not predicted, or are slightly different from those observed under hypnosis [13,14]. On the other hand, the theoretical assumption that only the persons scoring high on hypnotizability scales (highs) can accept suggestions has been challenged by studies reporting that also medium (mediums) and low hypnotizable individuals (lows) can respond to a few suggestions such as analgesia, although through different mechanisms [15,16].
Within the experimental reference frame, two major theoretical approaches have been proposed, namely neo-dissociative and socio-cognitive. The former assumes that hypnotic behavior, that is responding to suggestions and reporting involuntariness in action, depends on the hierarchical organization of cognitive activities which can be disrupted by hypnosis. This would allow lower cognitive modules to accept suggestions in the absence of conscious higher controls [15,16]. In contrast, the socio-cognitive approach states that the response to suggestions is a function of several individual and environmental factors allowing certain persons to behave according to suggestions independent from the induction of the hypnotic state. In brief, each individual responds through her/his specific cognitive-emotional characteristics and the experimental condition. In fact, it has been shown that targeting a relation as “hypnosis” and enhancing the expectation of hypnosis facilitates the response to suggestions more than actual hypnotic induction [12]. Experimental support has been provided to both theories, although recent research indicates the socio-cognitive reference frame as the most appropriate to account for most experimental findings.
Although the most popular aspect of hypnotizability is the proneness of the individuals with high hypnotizability to respond to suggestions, a renewed interest into this individual characteristic has come from the observation that it is much more than a cognitive disposition enabling individuals to enter the hypnotic state and accept suggestions, as it is associated with psychophysiological characteristics observable out of hypnosis and in the absence of suggestions. Indeed, different hypnotizability scores are associated with specific brain morpho-functional characteristics [17,18] and with sensorimotor, cardiovascular and cognitive-emotional correlates (Table 1), which makes hypnotizability relevant to the individual psycho-physiological identity and, thus, to the everyday life [19,20,21,22,23].
The aim of the present scoping review was to summarize the asymmetries observed in the phenomenology and the physiological correlates of hypnosis and hypnotizability as possible indices of related hemispheric prevalence. In fact, a debate about the pre-eminent role of the right or left hemisphere in hypnotizability-related psychophysiological features and in the shift from wakefulness to hypnosis has engaged hypnosis scholars for several decades. The review is based on all the research/review papers providing reliable statistical information derived from PsycINFO, PsycARTICLES and published in PubMed (years 1974–2019; key words: “hypnotic susceptibility and asymmetry”, “hypnotizability and asymmetry”, “hypnosis and asymmetry”, “hypnosis and laterality”, “ hypnotizability and laterality”). The review led to the selection of one hundred and thirty-seven studies. The results were reported considering separately individual differences in cognitive domains: fantasy proneness, cognitive and physiological flexibility, focused attention, divided attention, executive functions, memory, and visuospatial abilities. The main goals of this review were to (1) sum up the current state of the knowledge regarding the associations between hypnotizability and brain-related asymmetry patterns and (2) discuss the implications of this knowledge and summarize the most reliable view on hypnotizability-related physiological asymmetries. Then, we briefly report the most relevant behavioral findings. In the Section 3, we report the neurophysiological studies on the EEG hemispheric asymmetry of conventional alpha, theta, beta and gamma frequency bands and findings of nonlinear dynamical analyses of EEG as correlates of hypnotizability. In the Section 4 we describe the most relevant neuroimaging findings and in the Section 5 we summarize the described findings and indicate the questions deserving further investigation.
Since the nature of tasks influences the lateralization of brain activity, we predominantly limit our description of hypnotizability-related EEG asymmetries to resting conditions, in both the awake and hypnotic state. What can be said, in general, is that, during tasks, EEG hemispheric asymmetries occur more frequently in highs and are larger in highs than in lows. This has been observed, for instance, (i) for the recall of emotional events, which has been found to be associated, in highs, with an increase of 40-Hz EEG density in the right hemisphere during positive experience and with a density reduction in the left hemisphere during negative emotions [24]; (ii) for self-generated happy and sad emotions, during which highs showed significantly greater hemispheric asymmetries (right greater than left) in the parietal region in high theta, high alpha, and beta activity [25], (iii) for beta activity in eyes-open rest during both waking and hypnosis, which was larger in the highs’ left than right hemisphere [26].

2. Behavior Studies

Several cognitive characteristics of highs—greater creativity, fantasy proneness, absorption in mental images [27,28,29,30,31]—have been long considered the effect of strategies engaging the right hemisphere preferentially. In line with this hypothesis, a progressively reduced frequency of the F wave, which measures the motoneurons postsynaptic excitability [32,33], was observed in the right-hand abductor digiti minimi during long-lasting relaxation [34]. This finding, which was found to occur faster during hypnosis than during simple relaxation, was interpreted as being due to self-hypnosis. Thus, it would not contrast with the proposed inhibition of the left hemisphere during hypnotic induction [35,36].
A number of behavioral studies, indeed, have suggested a functional left rather than right directed hemispheric asymmetry in highs in the ordinary state of consciousness. In particular, the highs’ electrodermal responses are larger and haptic processing is faster for the left than for the right hand [35,37]; word fluency, a left prefrontal function [38], is greater in highs than in lows [39,40]; the highs’ processing of temporal judgments is faster in the left than in the right hemisphere, which was reversed by hypnotic induction [41] in a line bisection test [42]; the highs’ error is directed toward the right side at any distance of the line presentation [43], in contrast to the general population and to lows who exhibit left directed error for shorter distances and right directed error for larger distances [44,45]. In addition, the analysis of the highs’ written language indicates a larger use of abstract nouns, which is preeminently sustained by the left hemisphere activity [46], ideomotor responses are found more frequently on the left side, although not consistently reported [47,48] and during, suggestions of arms heaviness, the highs’ left arm lowers more than the right one [49] which could be accounted for by lower activation of the right hemisphere. Finally, only for highs, angry emotional valence is identified faster when presented to the right hemisphere [6]. To explain task-related asymmetry findings in term of hypnotic susceptibility [10], a greater overall cognitive and physiological flexibility can be advocated in highs, as they have more effective and flexible frontoparietal systems for both attention and inhibition of irrelevant stimuli [8,50,51,52,53].
In this research line, the inconsistency of the Right Hemisphere (RH)-activation hypothesis of hypnosis has been demonstrated by Kihlstrom and colleagues [54]. These authors assessed the hypnotizability of patients with either left- or right-hemisphere lesions and showed that brain-injured individuals can experience hypnosis and that their responses can be assessed using slightly modified versions of hypnotizability scales. Additionally, their results showed that unilateral damage of the right hemisphere following stroke does not impair hypnotic responding and that people with brain damage can be as hypnotically responsive as controls. Further, Dienes and Hutton [55], assuming that the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) is a key region involved in higher-order processes related to performing actions, used repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) under the hypothesis that a selective blocking of DLPFC by rTMS should make it harder for participants receiving a motor suggestion to be aware of the intention to perform the suggested action. If one is unaware of the intention, the arm will appear to lift by itself, producing the classic suggestion effect. In fact, the subjective rating of hypnotic suggestibility was higher after stimulation of the left-sided DLPFC. These findings support the theories postulating that diminished function in the frontal cortex is related to hypnotic response. Among these, the main is the cold control theory (see e.g., [56,57]), some forms of dissociated control theory (e.g., [58]) and left-frontal lobe inhibition neurophysiological approaches [36,39].

3. Neurophysiological Studies

3.1. EEG Alpha, Hypnotizability and the Right Hemisphere

A prevalent hypothesis on the hypnotizability-related asymmetries concerns differences in hemisphere functioning between resting-relaxation in waking and the so-called “neutral hypnosis” (i.e., resting hypnosis in the absence of targeted suggestions). This view suggests that the relative activation of the RH is characteristic of the high hypnotizable individuals during hypnosis (for reviews, see [59,60,61]). Bakan [62] was the first to suggest that hypnosis involves the RH activation owing to the association of hypnotic behavior with the creative, intuitive, nonanalytic, and holistic processing style assumed to reflect RH function. This hypothesis was proven as indefensible given that several general aspects of hypnosis, including the required attentional focus and the role of language in the establishment of hypnotic rapport, might be more appropriately seen as left hemisphere (LH) rather than RH brain functions [63,64], although the hypothesis of hypnosis as an RH task was seductive for many researchers (for reviews see [36,60,65]). Earlier research evidenced that the task-dependence of EEG alpha asymmetry, measured as the attenuation of the alpha rhythm in the activated hemisphere, reflects lateral specialization of the cerebral hemispheres. It was assumed that analytic tasks involving verbal or arithmetic manipulations yield relatively less alpha activity in the LH (LH tasks) than the musical and spatial (imagery, RH tasks) [66,67,68]. The ratio between left- and right-hemisphere alpha amplitude (LH / RH) was suggested as a reliable measure of task-related EEG asymmetry in a conducted follow-up study using essentially the same analytic and spatial tasks, but adding two so-called right-hemisphere tasks. These authors, using a slightly modified alpha laterality index ((RH – LH)/(RH + LH)) replicated their earlier findings, i.e., slightly more pronounced RH alpha was generated on analytic rather than on spatial tasks. Alpha during the hypnotic task resembled that of the spatial task and, contrary to expectations, alpha during the musical task resembled the alpha measured during the analytic (LH) tasks. These authors also found no alpha differences between high and low hypnotizable groups on any of the experimental tasks. In a subsequent study, MacLeod-Morgan and colleagues [69,70] followed the Morgan, McDonald and MacDonald [66] strategy. Subjects received all tasks under eyes-open and eyes-closed conditions, with three potential LH-tasks (random number generation, verbal categories, and mental arithmetic) and three potential RH tasks (spatial orientation, listening to music, and tonal memory). The authors found that the alpha laterality score computed for each of the LH and RH tasks did not reflect the predominance of right-hemisphere activity. Thus, the hypothesis that highly hypnotizable subjects would show greater laterality differences than lows on those tasks which favor their natural predisposition (i.e., RH tasks) was not confirmed. Interestingly, it was with the analytic or LH tasks that a significant relation was found between hypnotizability and lateralized alpha activity. These findings suggest that low hypnotizable people processed analytic information more asymmetrically than high hypnotizable people, and directly contradict the earlier findings of no laterality differences between high and low hypnotizable persons on analytic LH tasks. MacLeod-Morgan [71,72] failed to provide any cogent explanation for her findings. According to Perlini and Spanos [73], the findings regarding alpha rhythm may be difficult to interpret because MacLeod-Morgan’s sample was heterogeneous, as it included hypnosis-practitioners and naïve subjects, and their laterality index was questionable. In fact, it did not provide reliable, similar alpha laterality values on each of the three LH and RH tasks. Subsequently, De Pascalis and Palumbo [74] conducted an exact replication with 40 naïve-volunteers of the Macleod-Morgan and Lack study [72], but the findings obtained by these authors were the opposite to those reported in the referenced study. During the difficult tasks, in fact, highs and lows produced similar alpha laterality ratios, whereas in minimally demanding tasks highs showed a drop in negative alpha ratio reflecting greater relative LH activation (i.e., more relative left-hemisphere alpha attenuation). Further, lows had relatively higher RH activation during easy tasks. It is difficult to reconcile the disparate findings of these two studies. Rather, these discrepant findings suggest that there is no compelling evidence for hypnotizability differences in performance on reliable task-related hemispheric asymmetries. Additionally, De Pascalis, Silveri and Palumbo [75] investigated the relationship between alpha laterality and hypnotizability by employing covert mental activity tasks resembling natural thinking. Participants were required to perform covert verbal-numeric and imaginative-visual tasks, all under eyes-closed conditions. For imaginative-visual tasks (RH tasks) and a verbal task (LH task), there were no hypnotizability differences in alpha asymmetry; however, during a mental arithmetic task, lows produced a greater relative proportion of right-hemisphere alpha than highly hypnotizable individuals (i.e., LH activation). It is important to remark that this arithmetic task was the one considered by Macleod-Morgan and Lack [72] as attentionally demanding and hence one in which highs should lateralize more than lows. Rather, lows showed the shift in lateralization. In sum, findings from these studies suggest that the MacLeod-Morgan [70] and Macleod-Morgan and Lack [72] hypothesis of RH activation in highs did not hold to empirical testing. In addition, it is in contrast to the observation that the induction of hypnosis, necessary for the measure of hypnotizability, is obtained through verbal suggestion, and therefore requires the language-processing ability typically associated with the left hemisphere [54]. Therefore, the right-hemisphere hypothesis of hypnotizability and hypnosis has been abandoned.

3.2. Alpha Asymmetry, Hypnotizabiliy and Cognitive Flexibility

Although attempts to replicate MacLeod-Morgan and Lack’s findings have produced heterogeneous results; these author’s findings led to the hypothesis that highs had a flexible cognitive style which allowed them to shift easily between analytic (LH) and holistic (RH) style of processing according to task requirement. This flexibility is amplified by the induction of hypnosis [10]. In her seminal review, Crawford [10] examined anterior versus posterior alpha hemispheric asymmetry during eyes-closed resting-baseline, imagery and arithmetic conditions in high and low hypnotizable people. Low hypnotizable persons showed a greater relative proportion of right-hemisphere alpha in the anterior regions. Additionally, during each of these three conditions, highs showed a greater relative proportion of right-hemisphere alpha in the posterior regions than lows. Crawford suggested that the posterior right-hemisphere differences reflect a differential preference for imaginative processing of highs versus lows and that the anterior right-hemisphere differences reflect a more focused attention (relative to global attention) strategy preferred by lows in comparison to highs. This conclusion is rather strange since, in all previous reports, hypnotizability differences in alpha activity at posterior scalp sites were explained in terms of the greater capacity of highs for focused attention than lows (for a critical analysis see [73]). Later, Crawford and Gruzelier [65] pointed out that the choice of slow alpha (7–10 Hz) or fast alpha bands (10–13 Hz) across studies may have affected hypnotizability versus hemispheric asymmetry findings since research has shown that slow and high alpha frequency bands are functionally different [76,77]. The slow alpha activity is related to relative cognitive inactivity or low alertness [6,78] and the fast alpha activity to mental workload (see e.g., [79,80]) and memory performance [81]. More recently, Gruzelier [8] substantially abandoned any explicit reference to hemispheric asymmetry versus analytic and holistic tasks and proposed that highs are skilled for tasks involving either analytic or holistic processing, and for tasks involving either focused/sustained attention or disattention and that this ability is better evidenced when they are hypnotized. He suggested that, more than changes in laterality, hypnosis involves the inhibition of frontal-lobe functioning, particularly affecting the prefrontal cortex. Support and extension to this hypothesis were provided in a later study from this group showing that hypnosis reduces conflict-related activity in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) during the performance of a Stroop task [53].

3.3. EEG Theta

According to Crawford and Gruzelier [65], the most consistent relationship between EEG activity and hypnotizability exists in the 4–8 Hz theta band and more recently this hypothesis has been confirmed [60,82]. The evidence shows that highs tend to evidence more baseline theta activity than lows [26,83,84,85,86,87,88], although there are also inconsistent findings [89,90], suggesting that this relationship is not 100% consistent across all samples and all hypnotic procedures. Galbraith and collaborators [85] found the highest significant positive correlations between hypnotizability and EEG activity for the high-frequency theta band (5–7 Hz) in the eyes-open resting condition and was seen to reflect the ability of these subjects to focus attention on relevant stimuli and to shut off the irrelevant ones. However, research has devoted little attention to the relationship between theta hemispheric asymmetry and hypnotizability. Crawford, Clarke and Kitner-Triolo [25] reported that highs show a significantly greater activity in the right relative to the left hemisphere at parietal scalp region for the high theta, high alpha (11.5–13.45 Hz), and beta bands (16.5–25 Hz). Barabasz [91] reported that highs, but not lows generated more right-sided theta power just after a condition of restricted environmental stimulation. More recently, Lipari and coll. [92], using quantitative functional MRI and EEG neuroimaging changes (Low Resolution Brain Electromagnetic Tomography (sLORETA) method) in cortical activity during hypnosis induction and deep ‘‘pure hypnosis’’ in a hypnotic ‘‘virtuoso’’ subject, reported a reduced activity within a low theta and alpha band (4.5–10.5 Hz) reduced over left Brodmann areas BA17 and 19 for an eyes-closed resting in waking condition versus a late hypnotic induction stage comparison. Considering that in other studies conducted outside of the hypnotic context, high-theta has been associated with a continuous concentration of attention and selective attention [93,94,95,96], we think that this finding is in line with those reported by Galbraith and coll. [85] and by Sabourin and coll. [26], suggestive of greater absorptive attention in high hypnotizable people on hypnotic induction sustained by the occipital right hemisphere activation during neutral hypnosis. However, some findings show a tendency for all individuals to respond to hypnotic inductions and suggestions with increases in theta activity in both hemispheres [26,82,97,98,99]. Among these the most reliable findings were reported by Ray and collaborators [96,97]. These authors compared the EEG activity during an initial baseline period: baselines preceding and following a standard hypnotic induction and during the induction. In terms of theta activity, they found that highs, compared to the lows, had greater theta power in the more frontal areas of the cortex. Additionally, in the period preceding and following a standardized hypnotic induction, lows displayed an increase in theta activity, whereas highs displayed a decrease.
Considering that in these studies significant theta effects were also seen in low susceptibles from the pre- to post-hypnosis baseline, in agreement with Graffin, Ray and Lundy [97], we think that theta may also be related to some other factor such as relaxation and not hypnosis per se. More recently, Jamieson and Burgess [100] measured functional connectivity across conventional EEG bands during pre-hypnosis and hypnosis eyes-closed conditions. They found a relative increase in theta (4–7.9 Hz) coherence during hypnosis conditions in highs but not lows with a large proportion of significant links being focused on a central-parietal hub. Interestingly, this study seems to support the hypothesis of enhanced inter-hemispheric theta symmetry during hypnosis rather than the prevalence of hemispheric asymmetry in functional theta connectivity in high hypnotizable participants.
According to Jensen and collaborators [101], the EEG theta activity has yielded the more consistent findings between hypnotizability or hypnotic responding and theta band measures of structural connectivity, hemisphere asymmetry, and theta power. These findings are consistent with the possibility that, on the whole, they may reflect factors facilitating the response to hypnotic suggestions. This conclusion is derived from findings about hypnotizability/hypnosis and EEG oscillation measures showing that (i) highs display a rather robust trend of higher theta activity compared to lows and this effect has been proved to be the strongest one; (ii) with the exception of the theta band, studies have shown directions of changes different between highs and lows (i.e., increases in highs and decreases in lows, or vice-versa), and (iii) these changes with respect to basal conditions are greater in highs than in lows. This view is in line with previous findings disclosing differences between highs and lows in the brain areas (sLORETA) associated with theta and beta activity [102], and in subjects’ phenomenological experience of hypnosis and the associated frequency activity patterns [102].

3.4. EEG Beta and Gamma

Considering the hypothesized capacity of high hypnotizable people to focus attention out and within the hypnosis condition, it is surprising that little research has been devoted to the study of the relationship between EEG-beta asymmetry and hypnotizability in waking and hypnosis conditions. This may be due to the fact that both beta and gamma activities measured from the scalp are more prone to noise contamination from extracranial sources such as myogenic (muscle) activity and electrical background noise. The scalp EEG measures of faster wave frequencies are therefore less reliable than measures of slower wave frequencies. This may have produced less reliable findings of correlates of beta and gamma activity in EEG research, making tests of hypotheses related to these frequencies challenging and, thus, evoking less consideration for these fast-frequency oscillations.
De Pascalis and Imperiali [89] found no relationships between beta and hypnotizability at temporal and parietal derivations, bipolarly referenced to central vertex during eyes-open and eyes-closed waking rest. About 10 years earlier, Akpinar, Ulett and Itil [102,103] and Ulett and colleagues [103,104,105] showed that high hypnotizable subjects had greater beta EEG activity at the right occipital recording site during a resting non-hypnotic condition. Additionally, Sabourin and collaborators [26] found hemispheric asymmetry in the beta band (13–28 Hz) for high hypnotizable participants. In particular, highs, in the eyes-open rest condition but not in the eyes open condition, had substantially more mean beta power in the left hemisphere across frontal, central and occipital locations than did lows, while they did not differ significantly in the right hemisphere. In contrast, lows did not show significant differences in beta power between hemispheres. These findings are supportive of the hypothesis of higher focused arousal involving the left-hemisphere activation, expressed in beta hemispheric asymmetry across the entire scalp in high hypnotizable individuals. Recently, Fingelkurts and coll. [106], in a single case study of a virtuoso subject, found an enhanced left- rather than right-hemisphere activity at the frontal pole (Fp1versus Fp2 scalp sites) for the low-frequency beta (18.5–21 Hz) during hypnosis as compared to non-hypnosis and baseline conditions. A number of further studies have been based on the assumption that neural synchrony in humans reflects the phenomenal unity of consciousness [107] that implies a lower functional neural connectivity in highs as compared with lows to spontaneous phenomenological changes during hypnosis.
Following this perspective, White and colleagues [108] employed a virtual reality hypnotic induction and found that highs had decreased connectivity in the beta band (13–30 Hz) between medial frontal and left prefrontal sites, whereas lows exhibited the opposite pattern. Finally, Isotani and coll. [102] found that, before hypnotic induction, highs show higher dimensional complexity (higher arousal) than lows at baseline. Additionally, highs and lows were in different brain electric states, with beta-1 (12.5–18 Hz) and beta-2 (18.5–21 Hz) activities both more posterior and rightsided for highs than for lows.
More recently, Jamieson and Burgess [100] found a decrease in low-frequency (13–19.9 Hz) beta connectivity from a pre-hypnosis to hypnosis condition with a focus on a frontocentral and an occipital hub that was greater in high compared to low hypnotizable subjects. These authors interpreted their findings as indicating that the hypnotic induction elicited a qualitative change in the organization of specific control systems within the brain for high as compared to low susceptible participants and suggest this qualitative change as a potential indicator of the originally theorized hypnotic-state.
A high-frequency EEG activity centered at 40-Hz (36–44 Hz) has been found to co-vary with focused arousal, a functional component of the multi-dimensional attentional process [109,110,111,112,113,114]. Focused arousal, its brain circuitry, and direct electrical measurement procedure, and its relationship with an EEG activity within a 36–44 Hz frequency band, centering at 40-Hz has been described by Sheer [115,116]. This EEG activity is part of a currently labeled “gamma” band that is usually divided into two sub-bands, one narrow low gamma (30–80 Hz), including the classic labeled ‘40-Hz EEG’, and a broad high gamma band (80–200 Hz). Task-specific increased gamma activities have been observed in a number of recent studies, suggesting a functional role of gamma oscillations in humans (e.g., [117,118]). However, because of regional differences in the brain across tasks, and even inconsistencies in the frequency bands involved, the functional role of gamma oscillations is still not known [119]. Yet, gamma activity has been shown associated with brain activation and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) both in primary sensory-motor cortices and in the associative cortex, see review by [120].
Original studies linking hemispheric asymmetry and hypnotizability were conducted by De Pascalis and colleagues. In their first study, they evaluated the relationship between hypnotizability and hemispheric asymmetry during the recollection, in a waking state, of three positively and three negatively valenced personal life events [121]. These authors found a significantly lower time density of 40-Hz EEG production (second per minute of 40-Hz activity production) in highs than lows in a few waking conditions. In addition, highs, but not lows, exhibited increased density over both left and right hemispheres during two of three positive emotional tasks, while they showed a depressed activity over the left and an increased activity over the right hemisphere during negative emotional tasks. Thus, in comparison with lows, the highs showed greater absorptive ability in recollecting emotional events and emotional feelings and this ability was also paralleled by more consistent task-related 40-Hz EEG hemispheric shifts. The greater hemispheric specificity of high hypnotizable participants was later confirmed in hypnosis condition wherein highs accessed affects more easily than lows [24]. In another study [122] De Pascalis and colleagues examined the changes in 40-Hz EEG production in highs and lows during the hypnotic induction and during hypnotic susceptibility testing (12-items) of the Stanford Hypnotic susceptibility Scale (Form C). With respect to resting period, highs showed increased time density of 40-Hz EEG activity over both hemispheres in the early hypnotic induction, and showed decreased activity in the left and an increased activity in the right hemisphere in the late hypnotic induction. A reduction of 40-Hz EEG time density in both hemispheres characterized the lows. These differences in hemispheric asymmetry between hypnotizability groups were confirmed by a later study [123] examining the spectral amplitude changes across conventional EEG frequency bands and 40-Hz band (36–44 Hz) in high and low hypnotizable participants during the hypnotic induction and the hypnotic dream and age-regression tests of the Stanford Hypnotic Clinical Scale (SHCS). In eyes-open and -closed conditions, in waking and hypnosis, highs produced a greater 40-Hz EEG spectral amplitude than lows across frontal, central and posterior scalp sites. In the early and middle hypnotic induction highs displayed a greater amount of beta3 (20–36 Hz) than lows and this difference was even more pronounced in the left hemisphere. Highs, during the hypnotic dream, also displayed in the right hemisphere a greater 40-Hz EEG amplitude relative to the left hemisphere. This difference was even more evident for posterior recording sites. These results appear to be in agreement with Gruzelier’s [36] view of the hypnotic condition, where the inhibition of the initially dominant left hemisphere activity and the release of right-hemisphere processes characterize the hypnotic condition. Using an electrophysiological measure of phase synchrony, Terhune, Cardeña and Lindgren [124] examined frontal-parietal EEG functional connectivity in highs and lows in control and hypnosis conditions. Highs reliably displayed lower frontal-parietal alpha2 (10.5–12 Hz) synchronization in the left hemisphere during hypnosis than lows, but the two groups did not differ in the control conditions. The authors interpreted these results as suggesting that highs have a more labile left-sided frontal-parietal network than lows and are thereby more responsive to hypnotic inductions and related procedures. In another study Cardeña and coll. [125] focused on the neurophysiological correlates of hypnosis itself, i.e., in the absence of explicit suggestions, also referred to as ‘neutral’ hypnosis wherein participants usually report spontaneous alterations in various aspects of consciousness. These authors adopted a neuro-phenomenological approach consisting on analyzing participants’ (highs, mediums, lows) reports of perceived depth and spontaneous experience during rest following a hypnotic induction. Multiple rest periods of perceived hypnotic depth were analyzed and related to EEG frequency band power and global functional connectivity. Perceived depth increased substantially after the induction especially among highs and then mediums, but remained almost unchanged among lows. In the sample as a whole, depth correlated moderately to strongly with power and/or topographic heterogeneity in EEG power (measured using the Global Field Power (GFP) curve) for the fast EEG frequencies of beta2 (18.5–21.0 Hz), beta3 (21.5–30 Hz), and gamma (35–44 Hz). Although this study was designed assuming our brain as a single symmetrical rather than an asymmetrical entity, it provides a valid demonstration of how the experiential and overall-EEG data time-series analyses provide new data in terms of spontaneous temporal state fluctuations that occur in neutral hypnosis over time and across hypnotizability levels.
Using Low Resolution Brain Electromagnetic Tomography (sLORETA) method, Cardeña and colleagues [126] evaluated hypnosis depth reports and EEG activity in highs, mediums and lows during both voluntary and hypnotically induced left-arm lifting. In this study hypnotizability did not disclose a significant effect on EEG activity, but hypnotic depth correlated with left hemisphere increased anterior slow EEG and decreased central fast EEG activity. These authors also found that the hypnotic condition was associated with stronger activity in fast EEG frequencies (i.e., beta2: 18.5–21.0 Hz, beta3: 21.5–30 Hz, and gamma: 35–44 Hz) in left-anterior regions and slow EEG frequencies in left-centroparietal regions. On the whole, the hypnotic condition excitation was more anterior, the inhibition more posterior, and the opposite was found in the voluntary condition, and these anterior-posterior differences reached significance only among mediums and highs.
In sum, the above-mentioned findings support the importance of anterior-posterior differences in brain activity in hypnosis [97,127]. However, according to Jensen and colleagues [82], the relationship between gamma activity (as measured by scalp EEG) and hypnotizability/hypnosis is inconsistent across studies. These authors suggest that the relative unreliability of faster oscillations EEG activities measured from the scalp is due to contamination from the electric myogenic activity and background electrical noise. In addition, it has been postulated that when EEG theta oscillatory activity is present, it could inhibit recipient downstream neuron assemblies so that mostly phase-locked gamma frequencies occur [128]. In this way, slower frequencies such as hippocampal theta and/or theta from other structures such as basal ganglia might modulate the firing of faster cortical gamma frequencies [128,129,130,131]. Although some research in humans suggests that theta-gamma coupling might occur and be related to attention and memory functions, it is not entirely clear whether theta-gamma coupling always happens and whether both the phase and amplitude of gamma co-vary with the phase and amplitude of theta. If research will provide further support to the above-mentioned theta-gamma mechanism, this may also help explain the findings regarding the links between theta and gamma power, hemispheric gamma asymmetry and hypnotic responding (see review by [82]).

3.5. Nonlinear Dynamical Analysis of the EEG

Nonlinear dynamics or chaos has been applied to a variety of physiological processes, including EEG in hypnosis, but the implications and understanding of dimensionality are yet to be fully articulated. In contrast to the traditional techniques that decompose the component frequencies in the EEG and thus reflect one-dimensional information, nonlinear dynamic techniques, popularly known as “chaos,” suggest that a time series may be analyzed to reflect all other variables participating in the dynamics of the system. This approach can give important insight into the interpretation of EEG dynamics of a variety of cognitive-emotional processes including hypnosis [132,133].
Baghdadi and Nasrabadi analyzed the EEG during the hypnotic induction of the Waterloo-Stanford Group Scale (WSGS) to extract different features consisting mainly of fractal analysis and phase coherence and time-amplitude spectrum measures [134]. These different features were found to be a good index on the hypnotizability level. In particular, the feature based on the linear combination of the scaling exponent in theta band in channel C4 (right central site) was related to the hypnotizability level more than the other frequency bands. At the end of hypnotic induction scaling exponents decreased in all groups. In a later study, Yargholi and Nasrabadi used the recurrence quantification (RQ) analysis of the EEG recording data of the above-mentioned study obtained during the 12 suggestibility tests of the WSGS and showed that left hemisphere recordings were more efficient in distinguishing subjects with different hypnotic susceptibility than those of the right hemisphere [135].

4. Neuroimaging

Crawford and her colleagues [136] conducted a pioneering neuroimaging study measuring regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) with the 133-xenon inhalation method, while highs and lows, in waking and after hypnotic inductions, experienced ischemic pain to both arms under two conditions: attend to pain and suggested analgesia. They found that highs, but not lows, showed a significant increase in overall CBF, suggesting that hypnosis requires cognitive effort. Mainly, these authors observed during hypnotic analgesia a highly-significant bilateral CBF activation of the orbito-frontal cortex and CBF increase over the somatosensory in highs while low-hypnotizable persons showed decreases. Later studies using positron-emission tomography (PET) and statistical parametric mapping disclosed extensive distribution of activation amid both cerebral hemispheres and showed that right hemisphere injured patients exhibit normal hypnotic responsiveness [137,138]. Another original PET study by Szechtman and collaborators [139] employed PET to study positive auditory hallucinations under hypnosis in highs compared to lows. Activation in the right ACC i.e., Brodman’s area 32, was greater in hearing and hallucinating than in imagining or baseline in a group of highs under hypnosis when they heard an auditory stimulus and when they hallucinated hearing it, but not when they simply imagined hearing it. The same tasks did not yield this activation in the group of lows who were not able to hallucinate the auditory stimulus.
More recent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) findings disclosed asymmetries in grey matter volumes (GMV) and functional connectivity in highs. In particular, fMRI studies described larger GMV in the left superior and medial frontal gyri, middle occipital cortex, superior temporal gyrus and insula, right inferior parietal lobule [140], smaller GMV in the left superior temporal girus and posterior insula [141], in the right inferior temporal gyrus and in the left cerebellar lobules IV-VI [18]. An anteroposterior gradient was observed for the corpus callosum [142].
In resting conditions asymmetric activations were observed in the left inferior temporal gyrus, postcentral gyrus, left superior frontal and medial gyrus, precentral and inferior occipital gyrus [143]. In the comparison between hypnotic state and non-hypnotic rest, anterior-posterior asymmetries appear, as highs decrease brain activity in the anterior parts of the default mode circuit, whereas lows deactivate areas involved in alertness [144]. In addition, higher functional connectivity was observed in highs than in lows between the Dorsal Anterior Cingulate Cortex (DACC) and the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and anterior insula [145] as well as between the posterior cingulate cortex/precuneus and the left frontoparietal network including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), whereas lower functional connectivity was found between the right frontoparietal network and the right lateral thalamus/caudate [141].
Hypnotic induction modulates the described patterns of activation, although the different types of induction and the suggestions eventually administered beyond that of relaxation make the comparison between the reported findings very difficult. In the hypnotic condition [143] observed a reduced number of left activations with respect to wakefulness.
In this research line, De Pascalis and Scacchia (2016) conducted an ischemic pain study during waking and hypnosis conditions using LORETA analysis of the event-related potentials (ERPs) elicited by binaural auditory startle stimuli. This study aimed to identify cortical regions sensitive to temporal processing of pain reduction through placebo (analgesic cream) in waking and hypnosis on cold pain relief in the right-hand. Differences in cortical regional activity between waking and hypnosis placebo were found in an early processing stage (about 110 ms from auditory tone onset). That is, placebo in waking conditions showed an earlier inhibition in the right temporal lobe (or relative higher activation in the left temporal lobe), while the hypnotic placebo showed such an inhibition in both left and right frontal and ACC regions. Besides, in a late processing stage (about 175 ms from auditory tone onset) common to both waking placebo and hypnotic placebo analgesia was the enhanced activity in the ACC. These results in the whole were seen as indicating that placebo analgesia modulated pain-responsive areas known to reflect the ongoing pain experience.
Thus, generally speaking, we may assume that neuroimaging findings are in line with the results of EEG studies as they indicate major left activations and greater functional connectivity for the highs’ left hemisphere during wakefulness, but not in the hypnotic condition [54].
Cojan, Piguet and Vuilleumier [146] used the fMRI to investigate the recruitment of attentional networks during a modified flanker task in highs and lows in a normal waking state. They demonstrated that the right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) was more recruited in highs during selective attention conditions, suggesting better control of conflict in these individuals. In addition, this region was more connected with activity in ventromedial prefrontal cortex (PFC) and precuneus (which are involved in the default mode network, DMN) in highs, suggesting a tight dialogue between internally and externally driven processes that may permit higher flexibility in attention and underlie a greater ability of highs to dissociate. In contrast, low hypnotizable subjects recruited more parietal cortex and anterior cingulate regions during selective attention conditions (which are involved in the salience network), suggesting a better detection and implementation of conflict. This study demonstrated that the greater ability displayed by the highs in focalizing attention, while simultaneously engaging more introspective self-monitoring processes, may be a consequence of the enhanced modulation of connectivity between the selective attention and default-mode networks. Thus, this was one of the first neuroimaging studies to provide a shred of clear experimental evidence in support of Hilgard’s neo-dissociative theory accounting for hypnotic susceptibility and hypnotic phenomena [147] by suggesting a new neuroimaging evidence of the subjective experience of dissociation that characterizes the subjective experience during hypnosis. In a later study, [148] used fMRI to investigate activity and functional connectivity among the Default Mode Network (DMN), the executive control network (ECN), and the salience network (SN) during hypnosis for four conditions (rest, memory retrieval, and two different hypnosis experiences). In terms of brain asymmetry findings, these authors found a clear reduced connectivity between ECN (left DLPFC) and the DMN (i.e., PCC) in highs alone during hypnosis. These DMN regions are involved in self-referential processing and episodic memory [149], while the ECN is involved in cognitive control [150], and the two networks become anti- correlated during working memory tasks [151]. This finding is contrary to observations previously reported by McGeown and coll. [127]. Dissociation between ECN and DMN in response to hypnotic induction is likely to reflect the engagement in the hypnotic state and the associated detachment from internal mental processes such as mind-wandering and self-reflection. According to Jang and colleagues [148], their findings strongly support the hypothesis of hypnosis as a different state of consciousness, rather than a reduced level of arousal. Interestingly, this finding is related to the previous and more recent report by McGeown and coll. [140] wherein self-reported deeper levels of hypnosis were associated with decreased functional connectivity within the anterior DMN, also suggesting reduced awareness. Hypnotic depth ratings were positively correlated with GMV in the frontal cortex and the ACC, and hypnotic suggestibility was positively correlated with GMV in the left temporal-occipital cortex. Both the observations of greater GMV volume in the medial frontal cortex and ACC, and lower connectivity in the DMN during hypnosis were interpreted by these authors as findings indicating the individual proneness to experience a greater hypnotic depth. Significantly, these patterns of results suggest that hypnotic depth and hypnotic suggestibility should not be considered as synonyms.

5. Conclusions

In terms of the relationship between hemispheric asymmetry and hypnotic susceptibility, the earliest EEG studies did not provide consistent information in that, in the ordinary state of consciousness, few authors reported the prevalence of left-hemisphere activation (i.e., higher alpha activity in the right relative to the left hemisphere) in highs and this effect was not confirmed by other reports [98]. Consistently, De Pascalis [123] at the very beginning of hypnotic induction showed a greater amount of left-hemisphere beta3 amplitude (20–36 Hz) in highs than in lows, whereas other studies reported an anteroposterior gradient for theta [26,97] and gamma activity [152] as a function of hypnotizability. Additionally, review studies [54] have indicated no difference in hypnotizability between individuals with left versus right hemisphere lesions. Moreover, even though some above mentioned studies have reported a greater right hemisphere activity positively associated with hypnotizability, other studies have found a greater left hemisphere activity associated with higher hypnotizability [26] and other reports did not describe any difference between the right and left hemisphere activity [70,97]. Thus, the right-hemisphere activation hypothesis of hypnotizability and hypnosis was bound to fail. On the whole, research findings seem to be more consistent with a model of hypnosis hypothesizing, for highly hypnotizable individuals, more effective and flexible frontoparietal systems for both attention and inhibition rather than a general tendency to simply shift from left to right hemisphere processing with hypnosis [10]. In addition, from a dissociated control perspective, Woody and colleagues suggest that hypnotized subjects are similar to patients with lesions in the prefrontal cortex, whereas Gruzelier model views hypnosis as a consequence of the activation of anterior frontal-inhibitory processes involving the left hemisphere inhibition. Therefore, as suggested by Kihlstrom [60], we have three good reasons for thinking that investigations of the role of the frontal lobes and their functional relationship with the posterior regions in terms of individual differences on hypnotizability and hypnosis will be more productive than studies devoted to testing the sole right hemisphere activation hypothesis.
Brain morphofunctional asymmetries are present in highs in the ordinary state of consciousness and the functional features are modified by hypnotic induction. This is relevant to the definition of hypnosis as a mental state of reduced connectivity between left DLPFC and PCC [149] despite the absence of a discriminant index. Many questions, however, remain without an answer. For instance, is the left cerebellar reduced GMV sufficient to account for the highs’ postural, locomotor and visuomotor less strict control [19]. Are the asymmetric morphofunctional difference in limbic structures involved in the highs’ greater interoceptive sensibility and different interactions between interoception and emotion [42]. Is the functional connectivity between the precuneus and the left fronto-parietal [17] uniquely involved in the highs’ ability to easily change their state of consciousness and bodily condition? In addition, most of the reported studies did not enroll mediums, who represent the large majority of the population, and this reduces their impact by preventing a reliable interpretation of findings. Finally, the most interesting and fascinating aspect of hypnotic phenomenology is the highs’ greater proneness to respond to suggestions. In this respect, it is unlikely that, beyond attentional aspects [60,101], brain morphofunctional asymmetry may play a role. For instance, it has been shown for sensorimotor suggestions that highs display a largely distributed, bilateral mode of information processing for both the sensory and cognitive tasks [21,153], and suggestions displaying deeper relational aspects could be sustained by hormonal factors not easily referable to lateralized mechanisms [16].
The strength of the present review is its psychophysiological perspective, which has taken into account both behavioral and neurophysiological findings and the observation that, although a few experimental findings may support both neo-dissociative and socio-cognitive theories of hypnosis, the latter are the most appropriate to account for current experimental evidence. A weakness of the study consists of the heterogeneity in the methodology of the reported studies, which sometimes prevented a direct comparison between findings. In addition, not all the reported studies were conducted according to recent recommendations [153], thus disentangling the role of hypnotizability, hypnotic state and suggestions may have been difficult and may have limited the relevance of the reported findings.

Author Contributions

Both authors substantially contributed to the study. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Hammond, D.C. A Review of the History of Hypnosis Through the Late 19th Century. Am. J. Clin. Hypn. 2013, 56, 174–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Erickson, M.H. The Collected Papers of Milton; Erickson, M.H., Ed.; Irvington Publishers Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 1980; Volume 1–4. [Google Scholar]
  3. Haley, J. An Interactional Explanation of Hypnosis. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 2015, 63, 422–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Kumar, V.K.; Pekala, R.J.; Cummingsu, J. Trait factors, state effects, and hypnotizability. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 1996, 44, 232–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Piccione, C.; Hilgard, E.R.; Zimbardo, P.G. On the degree of stability of measured hypnotizability over a 25-year period. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1989, 56, 289–295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Crawford, H.J.; Harrison, D.W.; Kapelis, L. Visual Field Asymmetry in Facial Affect Perception: Moderating Effects of Hypnosis, Hypnotic Susceptibility Level, Absorption, and Sustained Attentional Abilities. Int. J. Neurosci. 1995, 82, 11–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Crowson, J.J.; Conroy, A.M.; Chester, T.D. Hypnotizability as Related to Visually Induced Affective Reactivity: A Brief Communication. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 1991, 39, 140–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Gruzelier, J.H. Redefining hypnosis: Theory, methods and integration. Contemp. Hypn. 2000, 17, 51–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Kirsch, I. Suggestibility or Hypnosis: What do our Scales Really Measure? Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 1997, 45, 212–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Crawford, H.J. Cognitive and Physiological Flexibility: Multiple Pathways to Hypnotic Responsiveness. In Suggestion and Suggestibility: Theory and Research; Gheorghiu, V.A.N.P., Eysenck, H.J., Rosenthal, R., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1989; pp. 155–167. [Google Scholar]
  11. Elkins, G.R.; Barabasz, A.F.; Council, J.R.; Spiegel, D. Advancing Research and Practice: The Revised APA Division 30 Definition of Hypnosis. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 2015, 63, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Wagstaff, G.F. Hypnosis and the Relationship between Trance, Suggestion, Expectancy and Depth: Some Semantic and Conceptual Issues. Am. J. Clin. Hypn. 2010, 53, 47–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Lynn, S.J.; Green, J.P. The Sociocognitive and Dissociation Theories of Hypnosis: Toward a Rapprochement. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 2011, 59, 277–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Derbyshire, S.W.G.; Whalley, M.G.; Oakley, D.A. Fibromyalgia pain and its modulation by hypnotic and non-hypnotic suggestion: An fMRI analysis. Eur. J. Pain 2009, 13, 542–550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Santarcangelo, E.L.; Consoli, S. Complex Role of Hypnotizability in the Cognitive Control of Pain. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 2272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Varga, K.; Kekecs, Z. Oxytocin and Cortisol in the Hypnotic Interaction1. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 2014, 62, 111–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Landry, M.; Lifshitz, M.; Raz, A. Brain correlates of hypnosis: A systematic review and meta-analytic exploration. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017, 81, 75–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Picerni, E.; Santarcangelo, E.L.; Laricchiuta, D.; Cutuli, D.; Petrosini, L.; Spalletta, G.; Piras, F. Cerebellar Structural Variations in Subjects with Different Hypnotizability. Cerebellum 2019, 18, 109–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Santarcangelo, E.L.; Scattina, E. Complementing the Latest APA Definition of Hypnosis: Sensory-Motor and Vascular Peculiarities Involved in Hypnotizability. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 2016, 64, 318–330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Santarcangelo, E.L.; Scattina, E. Responding to Sensorimotor Suggestions: From Endothelial Nitric Oxide to the Functional Equivalence Between Imagery and Perception. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 2019, 67, 394–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Ibáñez-Marcelo, E.; Campioni, L.; Manzoni, D.; Santarcangelo, E.L.; Petri, G. Spectral and topological analyses of the cortical representation of the head position: Does hypnotizability matter? Brain Behav. 2019, 9, e01277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Spina, V.; Chisari, C.; Santarcangelo, E.L. High Motor Cortex Excitability in Highly Hypnotizable Individuals: A Favourable Factor for Neuroplasticity? Neuroscience 2020, 430, 125–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Diolaiuti, F.; Huber, A.; Ciaramella, A.; Santarcangelo, E.L.; Sebastiani, L. Hypnotizability-related interoceptive awareness and inhibitory/activating emotional traits. Archives Italiennes de Biologie 2019, 157, 111–119. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  24. De Pascalis, V.; Marucci, F.S.; Penna, P.M. 40-Hz EEG asymmetry during recall of emotional events in waking and hypnosis: Differences between low and high hypnotizables. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 1989, 7, 85–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Crawford, H.J.; Clarke, S.W.; Kitner-Triolo, M. Self-generated happy and sad emotions in low and highly hypnotizable persons during waking and hypnosis: Laterality and regional EEG activity differences. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 1996, 24, 239–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Sabourin, M.E.; Cutcomb, S.D.; Crawford, H.J.; Pribram, K. EEG correlates of hypnotic susceptibility and hypnotic trance: Spectral analysis and coherence. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 1990, 10, 125–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Tellegen, A.; Atkinson, G. Openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences (“absorption”), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 1974, 83, 268–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Crawford, H.J. Hypnotic susceptibility as related to gestalt closure tasks. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1981, 40, 376–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Dasse, M.N.; Elkins, G.R.; Weaver, C.A. Correlates of the Multidimensional Construct of Hypnotizability: Paranormal Belief, Fantasy Proneness, Magical Ideation, and Dissociation. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 2015, 63, 274–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Silva, C.; Bridges, K.R.; Metzger, M. Personality, expectancy, and hypnotizability. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2005, 39, 131–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Lynn, S.J.; Rhue, J.W. The fantasy-prone person: Hypnosis, imagination, and creativity. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1986, 51, 404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Eccles, J.C. The central action of antidromic impulses in motor nerve fibres. Pflüger’s Archiv für die gesamte Physiologie des Menschen und der Tiere 1955, 260, 385–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Coombs, J.S.; Eccles, J.C.; Fatt, P. Excitatory synaptic action in motoneurones. J. Physiol. 1955, 130, 374–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Santarcangelo, E.L.; Busse, K.; Carli, G. Frequency of occurrence of the F wave in distal flexor muscles as a function of hypnotic susceptibility and hypnosis. Cogn. Brain Res. 2003, 16, 99–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Gruzelier, J.H. A working model of the neurophysiology of hypnosis: A review of evidence. Contemp. Hypn. 1998, 15, 3–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Gruzelier, J.H. Frontal functions, connectivity and neural efficiency underpinning hypnosis and hypnotic susceptibility. Contemp. Hypn. 2006, 23, 15–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Gruzelier, J.H.; Brow, T.; Perry, A.; Rhonder, J.; Thomas, M. Hypnotic susceptibility: A lateral predisposition and altered cerebral asymmetry under hypnosis. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 1984, 2, 131–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Thompson-Schill, S.L.; Swick, D.; Farah, M.J.; D’Esposito, M.; Kan, I.P.; Knight, R.T. Verb generation in patients with focal frontal lesions: A neuropsychological test of neuroimaging findings. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1998, 95, 15855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Gruzelier, J.H.; Warren, K. Neuropsychological evidence of reductions on left frontal tests with hypnosis. Psychol. Med. 1993, 23, 93–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Kallio, S.; Revonsuo, A.; Hämäläinen, H.; Markela, J.; Gruzelier, J. Anterior brain functions and hypnosis: A test of the frontal hypothesis. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 2001, 49, 95–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Naish, P.L.N. Hypnosis and hemispheric asymmetry. Conscious. Cogn. 2010, 19, 230–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Liepmann, H.; Kalmus, E. Ueber eine Augenmassstörung bei Hemianopikern. Berliner Klin Wochenschrift 1900, 38, 838–842. [Google Scholar]
  43. Diolaiuti, F.; Banfi, T.; Santarcangelo, E.L. Hypnotizability and the Peripersonal Space. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 2017, 65, 466–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Corbetta, M.; Shulman, G.L.; Miezin, F.M.; Petersen, S.E. Superior Parietal Cortex Activation During Spatial Attention Shifts and Visual Feature Conjunction. Science 1995, 270, 802. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Longo, M.R.; Lourenco, S.F. Bisecting the mental number line in near and far space. Brain Cogn. 2010, 72, 362–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  46. Huang, H.-W.; Lee, C.-L.; Federmeier, K.D. Imagine that! ERPs provide evidence for distinct hemispheric contributions to the processing of concrete and abstract concepts. NeuroImage 2010, 49, 1116–1123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Sackeim, H.A.; Greenberg, M.S.; Weiman, A.L.; Gur, R.C.; Hungerbuhler, J.P.; Geschwind, N. Hemispheric Asymmetry in the Expression of Positive and Negative Emotions: Neurologic Evidence. Arch. Neurol. 1982, 39, 210–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Nadon, R.; Laurence, J.-R.; Perry, C.W. The two disciplines of scientific hypnosis: A synergistic model. In Hypnosis and Imagination; Kunzendorf, R.G., Spanos, N.P., Wallace, B., Eds.; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA; Taylor & Francis Group: Abingdon, UK, 1991; pp. 78–91. [Google Scholar]
  49. Santarcangelo, E.L.; Cavallaro, E.; Mazzoleni, S.; Marano, E.; Ghelarducci, B.; Dario, P.; Micera, S.; Sebastiani, L. Kinematic strategies for lowering of upper limbs during suggestions of heaviness: A real-simulator design. Exp. Brain Res. 2005, 162, 35–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Woody, E.Z.; Bowers, K.S.; Lynn, S.; Rhue, J. A Frontal Assault on Dissociated Control. In Hypnosis: Theory, Research and Application; HMaK, I., Ed.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 1994. [Google Scholar]
  51. Woody, E.Z.; McConkey, K.M. What we don’t know about the Brain and Hypnosis, but need to: A View from the Buckhorn Inn. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 2003, 51, 309–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Woody, E.Z.; Szechtman, H. How Can Brain Activity and Hypnosis Inform Each Other? Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 2003, 51, 232–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Egner, T.; Jamieson, G.; Gruzelier, J. Hypnosis decouples cognitive control from conflict monitoring processes of the frontal lobe. NeuroImage 2005, 27, 969–978. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Kihlstrom, J.F.; Glisky, M.L.; McGovern, S.; Rapcsak, S.Z.; Mennemeier, M.S. Hypnosis in the right hemisphere. Cortex 2013, 49, 393–399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  55. Dienes, Z.; Hutton, S. Understanding hypnosis metacognitively: rTMS applied to left DLPFC increases hypnotic suggestibility. Cortex 2013, 49, 386–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Barnier, A.J.; Dienes, Z.; Mitchell, C.J. How Hypnosis Happens: New Cognitive Theories of Hypnotic Responding. In The Oxford Handbook of Hypnosis: Theory, Research, and Practice; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 141–177. [Google Scholar]
  57. Dienes, Z.; Beran, M.; Brandl, J.L.; Perner, J.; Proust, J. Is hypnotic responding the strategic relinquishment of metacognition. In Foundations of Metacognition; Beran, M.J., Brandl, J.L., Perner, J., Proust, J., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2012; pp. 267–278. [Google Scholar]
  58. Woody, E.Z.; Sadler, P. Dissociation Theories of Hypnosis. In The Oxford Handbook of Hypnosis: Theory, Research, and Practice; NMaB, A., Ed.; Oxford University Press Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 81–110. [Google Scholar]
  59. Barabasz, A.F.; Barabasz, M. Hypnosis and the Brain. In The Oxford Handbook of Hypnosis: Theory, Research, and Practice; NMaB, A.J., Ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2008; pp. 337–364. [Google Scholar]
  60. Kihlstrom, J.F. Neuro-hypnotism: Prospects for hypnosis and neuroscience. Cortex 2013, 49, 365–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  61. De Pascalis, V. Psychophysiological correlates of hypnosis and hypnotic susceptibility. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 1999, 47, 117–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Bakan, P. Hypnotizability, Laterality of Eye-Movements and Functional Brain Asymmetry. Percept. Motor Skills 1969, 28, 927–932. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Jasiukaitis, P.; Nouriani, B.; Hugdahl, K.; Spiegel, D. Relateralizing Hypnosis: Or, have we been Barking Up the Wrong Hemisphere? Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 1997, 45, 158–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Corballis, M.C. The Lopsided Ape: Evolution of the Generative Mind; Oxford University Press: Cary, NC, USA, 1993. [Google Scholar]
  65. Crawford, H.J.; Gruzelier, J.H. A Midstream View of the Neuropsychophysiology of Hypnosis: Recent Research and Future Directions. In Contemporary Hypnosis Research; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 1992; pp. 227–266. [Google Scholar]
  66. Morgan, A.H.; McDonald, P.J.; MacDonald, H. Differences in bilateral alpha activity as a function of experimental task, with a note on lateral eye movements and hypnotizability. Neuropsychologia 1971, 9, 459–469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Galin, D.; Ornstein, R. Lateral Specialization of Cognitive Mode: An EEG Study. Psychophysiology 1972, 9, 412–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Doyle, J.C.; Ornstein, R.; Galin, D. Lateral Specialization of Cognitive Mode: II. EEG Frequency Analysis. Psychophysiology 1974, 11, 567–578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Amochaev, A.; Salamy, A. Stability of EEG Laterality Effects. Psychophysiology 1979, 16, 242–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Morgan, A.H.; Macdonald, H.; Hilgard, E.R. EEG Alpha: Lateral Asymmetry Related to Task, and Hypnotizability. Psychophysiology 1974, 11, 275–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. MacLeod-Morgan, C. Hypnotic Susceptibility, EEG Theta and Alpha Waves, and Hemispheric Specificity. In Hypnosis 1979; Burrows, D.R.C., Dennerstein, L., Eds.; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1979. [Google Scholar]
  72. Macleod-Morgan, C.; Lack, L. Hemispheric Specificity: A Physiological Concomitant of Hypnotizability. Psychophysiology 1982, 19, 687–690. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Perlini, A.H.; Spanos, N.P. EEG Alpha Methodologies and Hypnotizability: A Critical Review. Psychophysiology 1991, 28, 511–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. De Pascalis, V.; Palumbo, G. EEG Alpha Asymmetry: Task Difficulty and Hypnotizability. Percept. Motor Skills 1986, 62, 139–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. De Pascalis, V.; Silveri, A.; Palumbo, G. EEG Asymmetry During Covert Mental Activity and its Relationship with Hypnotizability. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 1988, 36, 38–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Coppola, R.; Chassy, J. Subjects with low versus high frequency alpha rhythm reveal different topographic structure. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1986, 63, 41. [Google Scholar]
  77. Coppola, R. Issues in Topographic Analysis of EEG Activity. In Topographic Mapping of Brain Electrical Activity; Duffy, F.H., Boston, M.A., Eds.; Butterworth: Oxford, UK, 1986; pp. 339–346. [Google Scholar]
  78. Bösel, R. Slow alpha in the EEG power spectrum as an indicator for conceptual arousal. Zeitschrift fur Exp. und Angew. Psychol. Berl. 1992, 39, 372–395. [Google Scholar]
  79. Pfurtscheller, G.; Klimesch, W. Event-related desynchronization during motor behavior and visual information processing. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. Suppl. 1991, 42, 58–65. [Google Scholar]
  80. Sterman, M.B.; Mann, C.A.; Kaiser, D.A.; Suyenobu, B.Y. Multiband topographic EEG analysis of a simulated visuomotor aviation task. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 1994, 16, 49–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Klimesch, W. EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: A review and analysis. Brain Res. Rev. 1999, 29, 169–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Jensen, M.P.; Adachi, T.; Hakimian, S. Brain Oscillations, Hypnosis, and Hypnotizability. Am. J. Clin. Hypn. 2015, 57, 230–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Crawford, H.J. Cognitive and Psychophysiological Correlates of Hypnotic Responsiveness and Hypnosis. In Creative Mastery in Hypnosis and Hypnoanalysis: A Festschrift for Erika Fromm; FMLaBD, P., Ed.; Plenum Press: New York, NY, USA, 1990; pp. 47–54. [Google Scholar]
  84. Freeman, R.; Barabasz, A.; Barabasz, M.; Warner, D. Hypnosis and Distraction Differ in Their Effects on Cold Pressor Pain. Am. J. Clin. Hypn. 2000, 43, 137–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Galbraith, G.C.; London, P.; Leibovitz, M.P.; Cooper, L.M.; Hart, J.T. EEG and hypnotic susceptibility. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 1970, 72, 125–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Kirenskaya, A.V.; Novototsky-Vlasov, V.Y.; Zvonikov, V.M. Waking EEG Spectral Power and Coherence Differences Between High and Low Hypnotizable Subjects. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 2011, 59, 441–453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Montgomery, D.D.; Dwyer, K.V.; Kelly, S.M. Relationship between QEEG Relative Power and Hypnotic Susceptibility. Am. J. Clin. Hypn. 2000, 43, 71–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Tebecis, A.K.; Provins, K.A.; Farnbach, R.W.; Pentony, P. Hypnosis and the EEG: A quantitative investigation. J. Nerv. Mental Dis. 1975, 161, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  89. De Pascalis, V.; Imperiali, M.G. Personality, Hypnotic Susceptibility and EEG Responses: Preliminary Study. Percept. Motor Skills 1984, 59, 371–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. De Pascalis, V.; Perrone, M. EEG asymmetry and heart rate during experience of hypnotic analgesia in high and low hypnotizables. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 1996, 21, 163–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Barabasz, A.F. Effects of sensory deprivation on EEG theta and skin conductance. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 1991, 11, 9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Lipari, S.; Baglio, F.; Griffanti, L.; Mendozzi, L.; Garegnani, M.; Motta, A.; Cecconi, P.; Pugnetti, L. Altered and asymmetric default mode network activity in a “hypnotic virtuoso”: An fMRI and EEG study. Conscious. Cogn. 2012, 21, 393–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Başar-Eroglu, C.; Başar, E.; Demiralp, T.; Schürmann, M. P300-response: Possible psychophysiological correlates in delta and theta frequency channels. A review. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 1992, 13, 161–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Bruneau, N.; Roux, S.; Guérin, P.; Garreau, B.; Lelord, G. Auditory stimulus intensity responses and frontal midline theta rhythm. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1993, 86, 213–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Klimesch, W.; Schack, B.; Sauseng, P. The functional significance of theta and upper alpha oscillations. Exp. Psychol. 2005, 52, 99–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  96. Schacter, D.L. EEG theta waves and psychological phenomena: A review and analysis. Biol. Psychol. 1977, 5, 47–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Graffin, N.F.; Ray, W.J.; Lundy, R. EEG concomitants of hypnosis and hypnotic susceptibility. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 1995, 104, 123–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Ray, W.J. EEG Concomitants of Hypnotic Susceptibility. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 1997, 45, 301–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Williams, J.D.; Gruzelier, J.H. Differentiation of hypnosis and relaxation by analysis of narrow band theta and alpha frequencies. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 2001, 49, 185–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Jamieson, G.A.; Burgess, A.P. Hypnotic induction is followed by state-like changes in the organization of EEG functional connectivity in the theta and beta frequency bands in high-hypnotically susceptible individuals. Front. Human Neurosci. 2014, 8, 528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  101. Jensen, M.P.; Adachi, T.; Tomé-Pires, C.; Lee, J.; Osman, Z.J.; Miró, J. Mechanisms of Hypnosis: Toward the Development of a Biopsychosocial Model. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 2015, 63, 34–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  102. Isotani, T.; Lehmann, D.; Pascual-Marqui, R.D.; Kochi, K.; Wackermann, J.; Saito, N.; Yagyu, T.; Kinoshita, T.; Sasada, K. EEG Source Localization and Global Dimensional Complexity in High- and Low- Hypnotizable Subjects: A Pilot Study. Neuropsychobiology 2001, 44, 192–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Akpinar, S.; Ulett, G.A.; Itil, T.M. Hypnotizability predicted by digital computer-analyzed EEG pattern. Biol. Psychiatry 1971, 3, 387–392. [Google Scholar]
  104. Ulett, G.A.; Akpinar, S.; Itil, T.M. Hypnosis: Physiological, Pharmacological Reality. Am. J. Psychiatry 1972, 128, 799–805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  105. Ulett, G.A.; Akpinar, S.; Itil, T.M. Quantitative EEG analysis during hypnosis. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1972, 33, 361–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Fingelkurts, A.A.; Fingelkurts, A.A.; Kallio, S.; Revonsuo, A. Hypnosis induces a changed composition of brain oscillations in EEG: A case study. Contemp. Hypn. 2007, 24, 3–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Varela, F.; Lachaux, J.-P.; Rodriguez, E.; Martinerie, J. The brainweb: Phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2001, 2, 229–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  108. White, D.; Ciorciari, J.; Carbis, C.; Liley, D. EEG Correlates of Virtual Reality Hypnosis. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 2009, 57, 94–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  109. Başar, E.; Schürmann, M.; Başar-Eroglu, C.; Demiralp, T. Selectively distributed gamma band system of the brain. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2001, 39, 129–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Galambos, R.; Makeig, S.; Talmachoff, P.J. A 40-Hz auditory potential recorded from the human scalp. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1981, 78, 2643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  111. Sheer, D.E. Biofeedback Training of 40-Hz EEG and Behavior. In Behavior and Brain Electrical Activity; Burch, N., Boston, M.A., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1975. [Google Scholar]
  112. Ford, M.; Bird, B.L.; Newton, F.A.; Sheer, D. Maintenance and generalization of 40-Hz EEG biofeedback effects. Biofeedback Self-Regul. 1980, 5, 193–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. De Pascalis, V.; Ray, W.J. Effects of memory load on event-related patterns of 40-Hz EEG during cognitive and motor tasks. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 1998, 28, 301–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Oken, B.S.; Salinsky, M.C.; Elsas, S.M. Vigilance, alertness, or sustained attention: Physiological basis and measurement. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2006, 117, 1885–1901. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  115. Sheer, D.E. Focused Arousal, 40-Hz, E.E.G.; and Dysfunction. In Self-Regulation of the Brain and Behavior; Elbert, T., Lutzenberger, W., Birbaumer, N., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1984. [Google Scholar]
  116. Sheer, D.E. Sensory and Cognitive 40-Hz Event-Related Potentials: Behavioral Correlates, Brain Function, and Clinical Application. In Brain Dynamics; Başar, E., Ed.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1989; Volume 2. [Google Scholar]
  117. Tallon-Baudry, C.; Bertrand, O.; Hénaff, M.-A.; Isnard, J.; Fischer, C. Attention Modulates Gamma-band Oscillations Differently in the Human Lateral Occipital Cortex and Fusiform Gyrus. Cereb. Cortex 2004, 15, 654–662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  118. Hermes, D.; Miller, K.J.; Wandell, B.A.; Winawer, J. Stimulus Dependence of Gamma Oscillations in Human Visual Cortex. Cereb. Cortex 2014, 25, 2951–2959. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  119. Crone, N.E.; Korzeniewska, A.; Franaszczuk, P.J. Cortical gamma responses: Searching high and low. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2011, 79, 9–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  120. Ojemann, G.; Ramsey, N.; Ojemann, J. Relation between functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and single neuron, local field potential (LFP) and electrocorticography (ECoG) activity in human cortex. Front. Human Neurosci. 2013, 7, 34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  121. De Pascalis, V.; Marucci, F.S.; Penna, P.M.; Pessa, E. Hemispheric activity of 40 Hz EEG during recall of emotional events: Differences between low and high hypnotizables. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 1987, 5, 167–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. De Pascalis, V.; Penna, P.M. 40-Hz Eeg Activity During Hypnotic Induction and Hypnotic Testing. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 1990, 38, 125–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  123. De Pascalis, V. EEG spectral analysis during hypnotic induction, hypnotic dream and age regression. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 1993, 15, 153–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  124. Terhune, D.B.; Cardeña, E.; Lindgren, M. Differential frontal-parietal phase synchrony during hypnosis as a function of hypnotic suggestibility. Psychophysiology 2011, 48, 1444–1447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  125. Cardeña, E.; Jönsson, P.; Terhune, D.B.; Marcusson-Clavertz, D. The neurophenomenology of neutral hypnosis. Cortex 2013, 49, 375–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  126. Cardeña, E.; Lehmann, D.; Faber, P.L.; Jönsson, P.; Milz, P.; Pascual-Marqui, R.D.; Kochi, K. EEG sLORETA Functional Imaging During Hypnotic Arm Levitation and Voluntary Arm Lifting. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 2012, 60, 31–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  127. McGeown, W.J.; Mazzoni, G.; Venneri, A.; Kirsch, I. Hypnotic induction decreases anterior default mode activity. Conscious. Cogn. 2009, 18, 848–855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  128. Lisman John, E.; Jensen, O. The Theta-Gamma Neural Code. Neuron 2013, 77, 1002–1016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  129. Buzsáki, G. Rhythms of The Brain; Oxford University Press Inc.: NewYork, NY, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  130. Buzsaki, G. The Brain from Inside Out; Oxford University Press: NewYork, NY, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  131. Maris, E.; van Vugt, M.; Kahana, M. Spatially distributed patterns of oscillatory coupling between high-frequency amplitudes and low-frequency phases in human iEEG. NeuroImage 2011, 54, 836–850. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  132. Elbert, T.; Ray, W.J.; Kowalik, Z.J.; Skinner, J.E.; Graf, K.E.; Birbaumer, N. Chaos and physiology: Deterministic chaos in excitable cell assemblies. Physiol. Rev. 1994, 74, 1–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  133. Lutzenberger, W.; Elbert, T.; Birbaumer, N.; Ray, W.J.; Schupp, H. The scalp distribution of the fractal dimension of the EEG and its variation with mental tasks. Brain Topogr. 1992, 5, 27–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  134. Baghdadi, G.; Nasrabadi, A.M. Comparison of different EEG features in estimation of hypnosis susceptibility level. Comput. Biol. Med. 2012, 42, 590–597. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  135. Yargholi, E.; Nasrabadi, A.M. Chaos–chaos transition of left hemisphere EEGs during standard tasks of Waterloo-Stanford Group Scale of hypnotic susceptibility. J. Med. Eng. Technol. 2015, 39, 281–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  136. Crawford, H.J.; Gur, R.C.; Skolnick, B.; Gur, R.E.; Benson, D.M. Effects of hypnosis on regional cerebral blood flow during ischemic pain with and without suggested hypnotic analgesia. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 1993, 15, 181–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  137. Maquet, P.; Faymonville, M.E.; Degueldre, C.; Delfiore, G.; Franck, G.; Luxen, A.; Lamy, M. Functional neuroanatomy of hypnotic state. Biol. Psychiatry 1999, 45, 327–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  138. Rainville, P.; Hofbauer, R.K.; Paus, T.; Duncan, G.H.; Bushnell, M.C.; Price, D.D. Cerebral mechanisms of hypnotic induction and suggestion. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 1999, 11, 110–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  139. Szechtman, H.; Woody, E.; Bowers, K.S.; Nahmias, C. Where the imaginal appears real: A positron emission tomography study of auditory hallucinations. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1998, 95, 1956–1960. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  140. McGeown, W.J.; Mazzoni, G.; Vannucci, M.; Venneri, A. Structural and functional correlates of hypnotic depth and suggestibility. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 2015, 231, 151–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  141. Huber, A.; Lui, F.; Duzzi, D.; Pagnoni, G.; Porro, C.A. Structural and functional cerebral correlates of hypnotic suggestibility. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e93187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  142. Horton, J.E.; Crawford, H.J.; Harrington, G.; Downs, J.H., 3rd. Increased anterior corpus callosum size associated positively with hypnotizability and the ability to control pain. Brain 2004, 127, 1741–1747. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  143. Demertzi, A.; Soddu, A.; Faymonville, M.E.; Bahri, M.A.; Gosseries, O.; Vanhaudenhuyse, A.; Phillips, C.; Maquet, P.; Noirhomme, Q.; Luxen, A.; et al. Chapter 20—Hypnotic Modulation of Resting State Fmri Default Mode and Extrinsic Network Connectivity. In Progress in Brain Research; Van Someren, E.J.W., Van Der Werf, Y.D., Roelfsema, P.R., Mansvelder, H.D., Lopes Da Silva, F.H., Eds.; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2011; Volume 193, pp. 309–322. [Google Scholar]
  144. McGeown, W.J.; Venneri, A.; Kirsch, I.; Nocetti, L.; Roberts, K.; Foan, L.; Mazzoni, G. Suggested visual hallucination without hypnosis enhances activity in visual areas of the brain. Conscious. Cogn. 2012, 21, 100–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  145. Hoeft, F.; Gabrieli, J.D.E.; Whitfield-Gabrieli, S.; Haas, B.W.; Bammer, R.; Menon, V.; Spiegel, D. Functional Brain Basis of Hypnotizability. Arch. Gene. Psychiatry 2012, 69, 1064–1072. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  146. Cojan, Y.; Piguet, C.; Vuilleumier, P. What makes your brain suggestible? Hypnotizability is associated with differential brain activity during attention outside hypnosis. NeuroImage 2015, 117, 367–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  147. Hilgard, E.R. Divided Consciousness: Multiple Controls in Human Thought and Action; Wiley New York: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 1977. [Google Scholar]
  148. Jiang, H.; White, M.P.; Greicius, M.D.; Waelde, L.C.; Spiegel, D. Brain Activity and Functional Connectivity Associated with Hypnosis. Cereb. Cortex 2017, 27, 4083–4093. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  149. Greicius, M.D.; Menon, V. Default-Mode Activity during a Passive Sensory Task: Uncoupled from Deactivation but Impacting Activation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2004, 16, 1484–1492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  150. Seeley, W.W.; Menon, V.; Schatzberg, A.F.; Keller, J.; Glover, G.H.; Kenna, H.; Reiss, A.L.; Greicius, M.D. Dissociable Intrinsic Connectivity Networks for Salience Processing and Executive Control. J. Neurosci. 2007, 27, 2349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  151. Leech, R.; Kamourieh, S.; Beckmann, C.F.; Sharp, D.J. Fractionating the Default Mode Network: Distinct Contributions of the Ventral and Dorsal Posterior Cingulate Cortex to Cognitive Control. J. Neurosci. 2011, 31, 3217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  152. Ray, W.J.; Bjick, E. Psychophysiological measures of hypnosis and hypnotic susceptibility: Implications from 40 Hz activity. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 1997, 1, 61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  153. Jensen, M.P.; Jamieson, G.A.; Lutz, A.; Mazzoni, G.; McGeown, W.J.; Santarcangelo, E.L.; Demertzi, A.; De Pascalis, V.; Bányai, É.I.; Rominger, C.; et al. New directions in hypnosis research: Strategies for advancing the cognitive and clinical neuroscience of hypnosis. Neurosci. Conscious. 2017, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Table 1. Hypnotizability-related behavioral differences observed out of hypnosis.
Table 1. Hypnotizability-related behavioral differences observed out of hypnosis.
Highs with Respect to Lows
spinal reflexesH reflex habituation during long lasting relaxationSantarcangelo et al., 1989
F wave reduced frequency of occurrence (right hand)Santarcangelo et al., 2003
postural and locomotor controlless accurate during sensory alterationSantarcangelo et al., 2008;
Menzocchi et al., 2010a
Menzocchi et al., 2010b;
Solari et al., 2015
visuomotor controlless accurateMenzocchi et al., 2015
hapticsbetter performance in the visual recognition of
bimanually explored nonmeaninful objectsCastellani et al., 2011
better performance in the blindfolded reproduction
of haptically explored anglesMenzocchi et al., 2012
autonomic controlpreeminent parasympathetic control during
long-lasting relaxation
sympathetic control in standing position less engagedSantarcangelo et al., 2012
not reduced by stress and nociceptive stimulationSantarcangelo et al., 2007
flow-mediated artery dilationnot reduced by stress and nociceptive stimulationJambrik et al., 2004a
Jambrik et al., 2004b;
Jambrik et al., 2005a
Jambrik et al., 2005b
functional equivalence between
imagery and perceptionstrongerPapalia et al., 2014;
Santarcangelo, 2014;
Ibanez-Marcelo et al., 2019
cerebellar control of painparadoxical after anodal cerebellar stimulationBocci et al., 2017
motor cortex excitabilityhigher in basal and imagery conditionsSpina et al., 2020
μ1 polymorphismhigher frequency of the polimorphism less responsive
to opiatesPresciuttini et al., 2018
emotional reactivitygreater Facco et al., 2016
Kirenskaia et al., 2011
De Pascalis et al., 1989
interoceptive sensitivitygreater Diolaiuti et al. 2019

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

De Pascalis, V.; Santarcangelo, E.L. Hypnotizability-Related Asymmetries: A Review. Symmetry 2020, 12, 1015. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym12061015

AMA Style

De Pascalis V, Santarcangelo EL. Hypnotizability-Related Asymmetries: A Review. Symmetry. 2020; 12(6):1015. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym12061015

Chicago/Turabian Style

De Pascalis, Vilfredo, and Enrica Laura Santarcangelo. 2020. "Hypnotizability-Related Asymmetries: A Review" Symmetry 12, no. 6: 1015. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym12061015

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop