Next Article in Journal
Target-Site Mutations and Glutathione S-Transferases Are Associated with Acequinocyl and Pyridaben Resistance in the Two-Spotted Spider Mite Tetranychus urticae (Acari: Tetranychidae)
Next Article in Special Issue
Honey Bee Queen Replacement: An Analysis of Changes in the Preferences of Polish Beekeepers through Decades
Previous Article in Journal
Volatile Organic Compounds from Entomopathogenic and Nematophagous Fungi, Repel Banana Black Weevil (Cosmopolites sordidus)
Previous Article in Special Issue
Descriptive Analysis of the Varroa Non-Reproduction Trait in Honey Bee Colonies and Association with Other Traits Related to Varroa Resistance
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Individual-Level Comparisons of Honey Bee (Hymenoptera: Apoidea) Hygienic Behavior Towards Brood Infested with Varroa destructor (Parasitiformes: Varroidae) or Tropilaelaps mercedesae (Mesostigmata: Laelapidae)

1
Department of Breeding and Genetics, Bee Research Institute, 16540 Hohen Neuendorf, Germany
2
Natural History Museum of Tribhuvan University, Kimdol Manjushree Bazar, Kathmandu, Nepal
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Insects 2020, 11(8), 510; https://doi.org/10.3390/insects11080510
Submission received: 30 June 2020 / Revised: 29 July 2020 / Accepted: 4 August 2020 / Published: 7 August 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Honeybee Breeding)

Abstract

:

Simple summary

Parasitic mites are among the main causes of honeybee colony losses worldwide. Long-term selection has led to bees that are, in part, resistant to one specific mite, Varroa destructor. We investigated whether an important Varroa resistance trait, “hygienic behavior”, is also effective against Tropilaelaps mercedesae. T. mercedesae is another bee-parasitic mite of great economic importance and a lifecycle similar to that of V. destructor. “Hygienic behavior” means the ability of bees to recognize and destroy mite-infested bee brood, interrupting reproduction of the parasite. We also compared the expression of the behavior in two species of bees, one of which (Apis cerana) is thought to be resistant to T. mercedesae, while the other (Apis mellifera) is sensitive to it. We confirmed that both bees were able to express hygienic behavior also towards T. mercedesae. Moreover, we found that specialist bees destroying V. destructor-infested brood often also destroyed brood infested by T. mercedesae. This strongly suggests that hygienic behavior against the two mites is really the same trait, and selecting for V. destructor resistance likely also increases resistance to T. mercedesae. We also showed that A. cerana is more efficient at destroying T. mercedesae-infested brood, which may help to explain its resistance.

Abstract

The mites Varroa destructor Anderson and Trueman and Tropilaelaps mercedesae Anderson and Morgan are both serious threats to the Apis mellifera beekeeping industry. A trait frequently used in selection programs for V. destructor resistance is hygienic behavior, the selective removal of diseased/damaged brood. Here, we measured the level of association of the expression of hygienic behavior against both mites in A. mellifera, by observing whether the same individual bees would carry out the opening and removal of brood infested by the two parasites. The groups of bees showing these behaviors on cells artificially infested by either parasite showed a large overlap, making it appear likely that the two traits are at least closely coupled. Therefore, breeding for V. destructor resistance based on hygienic behavior could prepare A. mellifera populations for dealing with Tropilaelaps sp. mites, and vice versa. Using the same bioassay, we also compared the hygienic behavior of A. mellifera towards T. mercedesae to that of the Asiatic honey bee, Apis cerana. A. cerana workers eliminated a greater proportion of infested cells, which may in part explain the resistance of this bee to Tropilaelaps and the observation that Tropilaelaps reproduction on brood of this species is extremely rare.

1. Introduction

Parasitic mites and associated pathogens are among the most frequent causes of Apis mellifera (L.) colony losses in most parts of the world [1,2,3,4]. In Europe, North and Central America, and most of temperate Asia, Varroa destructor (Anderson and Trueman) is by far the mite species with the greatest economic impact. In those parts of Asia in which the introduced A. mellifera coexists with autochthonous giant honey bees (Apis dorsata, Apis breviligula, and/or Apis laboriosa), mites of the genus Tropilaelaps are often equally or even more damaging to the A. mellifera beekeeping industry [5,6].
The Asian giant honey bees are thought to be the original hosts of the genus Tropilaelaps [5]. Like V. destructor, all four Tropilaelaps species reproduce inside capped brood. Two species, Tropilaelaps clareae and Tropilaelaps mercedesae, are known to cause damage also to A. mellifera colonies, and at least T. mercedesae (Anderson and Morgan) is capable of transmitting Deformed Wing Virus as well as black queen cell virus [7,8]. Unlike V. destructor, adults of T. mercedesae also feed on early larval stages of A. mellifera [6]. It is not clear, however, whether mites of the genus Tropilaelaps are able to feed on adult bees, and they survive for only a few days if the host colony is broodless [5]. This is thought to be an important limiting factor of the spread of this genus of bee parasitic mites. However, the main condition for T. mercedesae survival, continuous brood rearing, is fulfilled in many parts of the distribution area of the western honey bee, and there are indications that the mite may also survive on rats [9]. Therefore, tropilaelapidosis is seen as a serious risk to A. mellifera beekeeping worldwide [10].
The best-studied Asian honey bee, Apis cerana (Fab.), appears to be resistant to both mites of the genus Tropilaelaps and V. destructor [11,12,13]. In the case of V. destructor, this resistance is probably due to a combination of behaviors of the adult bees, including grooming, “entombing” of infested drone pupae, and removal of infested worker brood, with the inability of the mite to reproduce on worker brood [11,14,15]. In the case of T. mercedesae, grooming behavior seems to play an important role [16]. Whether A. cerana removes T. mercedesae-infested brood has not yet been tested to our knowledge.
Beekeepers working with A. mellifera in regions where either T. mercedesae or T. clareae occur have to find ways to control these parasites, since untreated Tropilaelaps infestations regularly lead to colony collapse in this honey bee species. Although chemical treatments used to control V. destructor seem, for the most part, also effective against mites of the genus Tropilaelaps [17,18,19], the availability of resistant stocks of bees would represent a more sustainable solution. In the case of V. destructor, more than four decades of selective breeding, as well as natural selection of resistance traits in isolated populations, are finally beginning to show results [20,21,22,23,24]. Hygienic behavior, the detection and removal of damaged/diseased brood by worker bees, affects mite reproductive success [15] and is central to many resistance breeding programs. Khongphinitbunjong et al. [25] have studied T. mercedesae non-reproduction and infested brood removal by A. mellifera worker bees in Thailand, where bees of this species have been exposed to T. mercedesae for many years. They found removal rates of artificially infested brood of 53%, and high rates of non-reproduction on artificially and naturally infested A. mellifera brood. This seems to indicate that in A. mellifera, hygienic behavior could be a promising resistance trait not only with regard to varroosis, but also to tropilaelapidosis.
An important question now is whether the removal of T. mercedesae- and V. destructor-infested brood really constitutes the same trait. Given that the type of injuries produced by feeding of T. mercedesae on larvae and pupae is distinct from that produced by V. destructor [6,26], it may be hypothesized that different genetic specializations are required on behalf of the hygienic workers in order to sense them across the cell capping. Also, the role of hygienic behavior in A. cerana resistance to T. mercedesae remains to be elucidated.
In the present study, we aimed to:
(1)
Compare hygienic behavior of A. mellifera and A. cerana against T. mercedesae. For this, we first observed hygienic behavior separately in monospecific groups of bees. We then went on to observe mixed groups of bees on brood of either species. This was done with the objective of comparing their reaction to strictly identical removal stimuli.
(2)
Verify whether hygienic behavior against T. mercedesae and V. destructor are co-expressed in A. mellifera, by determining whether the same individual worker bees perform these behaviors.

2. Material and Methods

2.1. Bees and Mites

The experiments were performed in an experimental apiary in Kathmandu, Nepal. Colonies of A. mellifera used in the study included local hybrids as well as hives headed by Apis mellifera carnica queens imported from Germany. A. cerana was represented by colonies from three Nepalese regions, the Terai, the Kathmandu Valley, and the mountains around this valley. Bees from the different stocks within both species were mixed in the experiments, equalizing the numbers of bees from each stock as far as possible. All mites were obtained from local A. mellifera colonies, and were collected from newly-capped brood (prepupal or white-eyed stages). Great care was taken not to injure the brood while collecting the mites, and also in order to avoid contamination through leaking body fluids. The species affiliation of V. destructor and T. mercedesae was verified morphometrically by applying published identification keys [12,27].

2.2. Bioassay for Observing Hygienic Behavior

The bioassay used is described in detail in [28] and is depicted in Figure 1. The day before an experiment, combs of emerging brood were removed from the source colonies. An additional comb of freshly-capped brood was taken from a donor colony, unrelated to the bees used in the experiment, to be used for artificial infestation. Source colonies of these combs were treated with Amitraz, and were monitored for infestation by opening variable numbers of brood cells in order to avoid using brood with sizeable natural infestation. Between 20 and 35 mites were introduced into brood cells with a brush (one per cell) after carefully opening the cells with a razor blade. The caps were closed again, and their positions marked on transparency sheets. Between three and five cells were opened and closed without the introduction of a mite (“sham-manipulation”), and served to check whether brood removal was due to the presence of the mite or to manipulation of the cell. The comb was then placed inside a hive previously shown to present little brood hygiene, for repair of the manipulated cell caps. Groups of 1100–2100 newly-emerged (<24-h-old) worker bees were individually labelled with opalith plates of different shapes and orientation, using nail polish (Maybelline Express; L’Oréal, Paris, France) as glue. After 24 h, the manipulated comb was placed in a wire mesh cage with one glass side. The labelled bees were introduced together with a mated queen, and the cage was placed inside a strong colony of the same species as the labelled bees, with the glass pane facing an opening in the side wall of the hive. In cases where a mix of cerana and mellifera workers were used, the species of the fostering colony was alternated between repetitions of the experiment. Hygienic behavior was then observed under infrared illumination with the help of a suitable CCTV camera (PS/DX4-285GE; Kappa optronics, Gleichen, Germany). Observations were recorded continuously for 4–5 days (corresponding to days 2 to 5–6 post-infestation), and later viewed. From these videos, we determined the time of cell openings, as well as the identity of the bee initiating the cell opening, and of the first two bees continuing with this task. In order to limit the effort required to analyze the ~2400 h of video material, we only looked at those cells that had been emptied at the end of the experiment. Only the event that ultimately led to the removal of the pupa was observed. Potential earlier attempts at uncapping and events of recapping could not be included. The intensity of these behaviors was judged as either “low” (a bee nibbling briefly at a cell cap and then leaving), “medium” (a bee nibbling repeatedly with brief interruptions or for a period of up to 30 s), or “high” (sustained nibbling for >30 s).

2.3. Comparisons

The above-described assay was performed 19 times, with a total of 35,910 tagged worker bees:
(1)
Five times with A. cerana bees on A. cerana brood (designated below as “cbees_cbrood”).
(2)
Four times with A. mellifera bees on A. mellifera brood (“mbees_mbrood”).
(3)
Eight times with equal numbers of bees from both species and alternating origins of brood (“xbees_xbrood”).
(4)
One time with A. cerana bees on A. mellifera brood (“cbees_mbrood”).
One round of the experiment could not be used, because bees started to indiscriminately remove all brood, possibly due to overheating. V. destructor and T. mercedesae mites were used together in the same experiments at a ratio of approximately 1:2. In four of the experiments with A. cerana bees on A. cerana brood, only T. mercedesae mites could be used because no V. destructor mites were available.

2.4. Data Analysis

All statistical analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS software (IBM, Amonk, NY, USA).
We first verified, for each of the bees/brood combinations cbees_cbrood, mbees_mbrood, and xbees_xbrood, whether hygienic behavior was specific to infested cells, i.e., whether the bioassay really allowed us to observe mite-related responses. We did this by comparing removal ratios using the χ2 test. These tests were performed separately for T. mercedesae- and V. destructor-infested cells.
Next, we compared the removal dynamics of Tropilaelaps-infested brood among the three bee/brood combinations cbees_cbrood, mbees_mbrood, and xbees_xbrood. This was done using Kaplan–Meier analysis with pairwise comparisons (Mantel–Cox test). In the case of the experiments with mixed worker bees (xbees_xbrood), experiments with A. mellifera brood and with A. cerana brood were distinguished, in order to also visualize the role of the species of brood used.
We then went on to compare the efficiency and intensity of cell opening behavior towards T. mercedesae- and V. destructor-infested brood in A. mellifera using χ2 tests on the frequency distributions. Only data from those experiments in which bees and brood were of one and the same species were used for this analysis, because we could not exclude interactions between the factors “species of brood” and “species of workers”. In a separate analysis, we compared the intensity of opening events on T. mercedesae-infested brood from the mixed-species experiments (xbees_xbrood), contrasting cases in which workers had opened brood of their own or the other species.
Finally, we investigated whether a bee involved in the opening of cells infested by one of the two mite species had a greater-than-arbitrary probability of also being involved in the removal of the other. For this, we calculated the proportions of both T. mercedesae- and V. destructor-infested cell-openers in all bees in the experiments. We multiplied these two probabilities to obtain the likelihood of any bee to become an opener of both types of cells, under the hypothesis that the two traits occurred independently. This theoretical likelihood was then compared with the observed frequency of openers of both cell types using the χ2 test. This analysis was performed only for A. mellifera bees, because too few cases of A. cerana opening V. destructor-infested cells were available, due to the lack of V. destructor mites during experiments with this bee species.
The significance level of the type 1 error for all tests was 0.05.

3. Results

3.1. Specificity of Opening Behavior to Infested Cells

A. mellifera workers opened and removed V. destructor-infested brood significantly more frequently than sham-manipulated brood (45.5 vs. 8.8%; total n = 56; χ2 = 5.5; p = 0.017). The same was true for mixed-species groups of worker bees (xbees_xbrood; 43.6 vs. 18.2%; total n = 61; χ2 = 8.4; p = 0.004). In the cbees_cbrood experiments, insufficient numbers of cells were infested with V. destructor to carry out the test. Similar to V. destructor, the removal of T. mercedesae-infested larvae/pupae by A. mellifera workers was a specific response to infestation, rather than to the manipulation of the cells alone (37.6 vs. 8.8%; total n = 121; χ2 = 4.1; p = 0.043). This was also true for the xbees_xbrood experiments (41.3 vs. 18.2%; total n = 148; χ2 = 4.2; p = 0.039). The frequency of removal of T. mercedesae-infested brood by A. cerana (experiments cbees_cbrood), however, was not significantly different from that observed in sham-manipulated brood (37.6 vs. 20.0%; total n = 129; χ2 = 2.3; p = 0.13).

3.2. Comparison of Removal of T. mercedesae- and V. destructor-Infested Brood Cells by A. mellifera

Here, “survival” means that brood was not opened and removed by the worker bees. The graph is based on a total of 745 artificially infested cells. The period of 0–24 h post-infestation is not displayed, because combs were placed inside a non-hygienic hive for repair of manipulated cell caps by the bees. The time course of brood survival differed significantly between T. mercedesae- and V. destructor-infested cells (χ2 = 11.3; df = 1; p = 0.001).
The graphs are based on the following numbers of observations (= opening events; from left to right): 38; 49; 54; 22. In the case of cerana bees observed on cerana brood, no cells were infested with V. destructor mites. The intensity of opening behavior shown by A. mellifera bees was not different for T. mercedesae- and V. destructor-infested brood of this same species (left part of graph; χ2 = 3.9; p = 0.14). Different combinations of the factors “species of bees” and “species of brood” produced different intensities of opening behavior towards Tropilaelaps-infested brood (comparison of the four parts of the graph; χ2 = 14.9; p = 0.02); n.a.: not analyzed.
Figure 2 depicts the time course of infested brood removal of larvae/pupae for the two species of mites. Only A. mellifera bees observed on A. mellifera brood are included here. It shows that, although both types of infested brood are removed, V. destructor-infested cells are emptied slightly faster and to a greater proportion (log-rank test; χ2 = 11.3; df = 1; p = 0.001; total number of cells observed = 745). The intensity of removal behavior of A. mellifera towards T. mercedesae- and V. destructor-infested brood was not different, however (initiation of uncapping: χ2 = 3.9; total n = 49; p = 0.14; continuation of uncapping/removal: χ2 = 0.1; total n = 89; p = 0.98; data for initiation of uncapping shown in Figure 3). The chance of any A. mellifera worker bee to perform components of hygienic behavior on both T. mercedesae- and V. destructor-infested brood was far greater than would be expected if the two traits had occurred independently of each other (χ2 = 9.0; df = 1; total n = 1213; p = 0.003).

3.3. Comparison of Tropilaelaps Removal by A. mellifera, A. cerana, and Mixed-Species Groups of Worker Bees

Here, “survival” means that brood was not opened and removed by the worker bees. The graph is based on a total of 1228 artificially infested cells. The period of 0–24 h post-infestation is not displayed, because combs were placed inside a non-hygienic hive for repair of manipulated cell caps by the bees. The time course of brood survival differed significantly between treatments (χ2 = 26.8; p < 0.001).
Figure 4 compares the cumulated survival over time of Tropilaelaps-infested cells in the treatments mbees_mbrood, cbees_cbrood, and xbees_xbrood. It can be seen that A. cerana bees, when observed on their own brood and in the absence of A. mellifera workers, showed the fastest removal of infested brood, and also removed the greatest proportion of it. This difference was significant for the comparisons with mbees_mbrood, as well as with xbees_cbrood (χ2 = 20.7 and 7.3; p <0.001 and 0.007), while it was marginally non-significant in the case of xbees_mbrood (χ2 = 3.2; p = 0.075). The three sets of experiments in which mellifera workers were used, either alone or in combination with A. cerana, formed a homogeneous block (χ2 = 2.3–3.8; p = 0.127–0.053), indicating that the presence or absence of A. cerana workers did not influence hygienic behavior in these experiments. A. cerana and A. mellifera did not differ with regard to the median duration of uninterrupted cell opening events, although the range of durations was far wider in A. cerana (total n = 167; A. cerana: median = 104 s (7 to 5569); A. mellifera: median = 94 s (1 to 630)). The intensity of initiations of cell openings was generally quite similar in all treatments, but judged slightly lower in the case of xbees_cbrood than in the others (χ2 = 14.9; n = 169; p = 0.02; Figure 3). In contrast, the intensity of continuations of cell opening and brood removal was lowest in treatment mbees_mbrood (χ2 = 13.2; n = 262; p = 0.04; data not shown).

3.4. Hygienic Behavior in Mixed Groups of Worker Bees

Hygienic behavior in mixed groups of workers (experiments xbrood_xbees) was almost exclusively performed by A. mellifera bees, despite the fact that equal numbers of individuals from both species were present, and regardless of the species affiliation of the brood (173 observed actions of initiation or continuation by A. mellifera vs. only 4 by A. cerana). In order to clarify whether the absence of hygienic behavior by A. cerana on brood of A. mellifera was due to factors linked to the brood, we performed an additional experiment in which an A. cerana-only group of worker bees was placed on infested A. mellifera brood. In this experiment, 16 infested pupae out of 25 were removed.
The fact that, in each of the xbees_xbrood experiments, one half of the bees was confronted with brood from another species did not lead to indiscriminate removal of brood. This is illustrated by the removal of sham-manipulated cells, which was not greater in the “xbees_xbrood” experiments than in the mbees_mbrood or cbees_cbrood experiments (χ2 = 1.6; df = 2; total n = 86; p = 0.46).

4. Discussion

It has been reported several times that T. mercedesae, like V. destructor, can trigger hygienic behavior towards infested brood in Apis mellifera ([25,29], Ritter and Schneider-Ritter 1987; as cited in [15]). Our data confirm this finding. Khongphinitbunjong et al. [25] have also shown that T. mercedesae reproductive success in cells targeted by hygienic behavior is effectively reduced. However, while the earlier authors agree on the fact that cells singly infested by T. mercedesae females are removed faster than cells singly infested by V. destructor, our results point to the contrary. It may be speculated that differences between local strains of the two mite species are responsible. The two earlier studies were performed with locally-occurring mites in Thailand, while ours was performed in Nepal. According to Anderson et al. [30], at least two different haplotypes of T. mercedesae are present in Thailand, one of which is the “mainland Asia” type also found in both India and China, the two neighboring countries of Nepal. Samples of V. destructor from the Kathmandu region have been genotyped by Solignac et al. [31]. They presented an unusually high heterozygosity in microsatellite markers, and were classified as belonging to the rare “Japan” haplotype, based on the analysis of mtDNA. This haplotype is thought to show a lower virulence than the more common “Korean” haplotype [31]. Our bioassay involved the transfer of mites from early capped stages of donor brood to similar stages of recipient brood. As reproduction of both V. destructor and T. mercedesae started shortly after cell capping [26,32], the transfer might have interrupted reproduction, thereby influencing the onset of reproduction-related removal stimuli. From earlier experiments, we know that most transferred mites nevertheless manage to produce viable offspring. As a possible retardation of reproduction would concern both mites alike, it can be assumed to be of minor importance for the interpretation of the present results.
For more than 30 years now, big efforts have been, and are being, made to increase the prevalence of hygienic behavior (and related traits such as Varroa sensitive hygiene/suppressed mite reproduction) in populations of A. mellifera in many parts of the world [28,33,34,35,36,37,38,39]. If it can be shown that hygienic behavior against V. destructor and against T. mercedesae are really the same trait, or at least closely coupled, then this would mean that breeding for V. destructor resistance through selection on hygiene traits is also a preventive strategy in view of a possible introduction of T. mercedesae. It has been reported that lines of bees partially resistant to the latter (from a commercial strain used in Thailand) and bees partially resistant to V. destructor (ARS (Agricultural Research Service) “Russian bees”) are characterized by similarly high levels of hygienic behavior, as measured by the freeze-killed brood assay, making it likely that the genetic bases of V. destructor- and T. mercedesae-specific hygiene are at least linked [40]. Even in strongly hygienic colonies, recognition and removal of damaged brood is a behavioral specialization only expressed by a small minority of the worker bees [28]. Our data now show that V. destructor- and T. mercedesae-infested cells are often opened by the same specialized individuals, meaning that the two traits were coupled or identical. This strongly suggests a close association of the two traits.
It is known that A. cerana workers remove V. destructor-infested worker brood with far greater efficiency than A. mellifera workers [11,41]. We show here that this is also true for T. mercedesae-infested brood. This may seem surprising given that brood removal is thought to be triggered by stimuli linked to the reproduction of mites on the brood [42], and that T. mercedesae only very rarely reproduces on A. cerana pupae [30]. An explanation could be that hygienic behavior is the reason for Tropilaelaps non-reproduction. Another explanation may lay in the fact that mite infestations are often lethal to A. cerana pupae [43], and that dead pupae presumably emit stronger cues for removal. Viral replication, which has also been reported to increase the likelihood of mite detection inside brood [44], is triggered by feeding of the parasite on its host, and so may also be involved in T. mercedesae recognition by A. cerana. In every case, the stronger hygienic behavior of A. cerana towards T. mercedesae offers an additional explanation for the fact that A. cerana colonies, in general, appear to be more resistant to mites of the genus Tropilaelaps than colonies of the western honey bee [26]. The relative importance of hygienic behavior with regard to other resistance mechanisms, including other mechanisms suppressing mite reproduction, will be an important subject of future research.
Our experiments with mixed groups of A. cerana and A. mellifera workers were originally designed to be able to directly compare the ethology and temporal pattern of hygienic behavior of the two species side by side on the same brood comb, i.e., in response to strictly the same stimuli. This turned out not to be possible, because hygienic behavior was almost completely suppressed in A. cerana workers by the presence of A. mellifera bees. This suppression was apparently not due to an inability of A. cerana workers to sense infestation in A. mellifera brood, because it equally occurred in mixed groups of bees on A. cerana brood, and because in the absence of A. mellifera workers, infested A. mellifera pupae were removed at a high proportion. This result is interesting with regard to the question of task affiliation between worker bees. In mixed colonies of A. cerana and A. mellifera, both species participate in almost equal numbers in queen retinue behavior [45], whereas A. cerana workers tend to specialize in foraging [46]. Communication via the dance language is possible across the species barrier [47]. One hypothesis to explain the suppression of brood hygiene in A. cerana is that A. mellifera workers possess a lower detection threshold for the signals emitted by infested pupae. Hygienic behavior is at least in part triggered by chemical cues that are perceived through the wax capping [48,49]. A. cerana pupae have been shown to react more strongly to mite infestations [43]. Therefore, it seems possible that a moderate olfactory sensitivity of A. cerana workers would be sufficient to reliably sense them, whereas A. mellifera may have had to evolve a greater sensitivity, because the stimuli emitted by infested pupae are weaker.
T. mercedesae has recently been detected on A. mellifera in several areas outside of the distribution range of A. dorsata [31]. Our study confirms that one of the central defense mechanisms of the western honey bee against brood diseases, hygienic behavior, is also expressed vis-à-vis this new parasitic mite. Given that hygienic behavior against V. destructor and T. mercedesae appear to be closely coupled, breeding for behavioral resistance to varroosis likely can reduce the vulnerability of A. mellifera beekeeping also to T. mercedesae, and possibly also to other species of this genus. This is of particular importance, given the increasing trend of A. mellifera colonies to produce brood throughout the winter, even in “temperate” regions, brought about by climate change. At the same time, greater efforts will be needed to elucidate the biology of the four Tropilaelaps species, where great knowledge gaps need to be filled e.g., with regard to reproduction biology, dispersal, and host–pathogen interactions.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: I.G. and K.B.; data curation: J.W.; formal analysis: J.W. and K.B.; investigation: M.S. (Monica Shrestha), M.S. (Madhusudan Singh), and C.S.; methodology: M.S. (Madhusudan Singh) and C.S.; project administration: J.W. and I.G.; supervision: I.G. and M.S. (Madhusudan Singh); writing—original draft: M.S. (Monica Shrestha) and J.W.; writing—review and editing: K.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the European Union within the framework of the FP7-project SmartBees (Sustainable Management of Resilient Bee Populations; project number 613960), and materials used were funded by the European Fund for Regional Development (grant 80137041).

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to many helpers at the Natural History Museum, Tribhuvan University, who helped us to overcome earthquakes, an economic blockade, and hornet attacks of our apiary, and who assisted in marking bees and organizing colonies.

Conflicts of Interest

We hereby declare having no conflicts of interest that could have influenced the present study.

References

  1. Genersch, E.; Von Der Ohe, W.; Kaatz, H.; Schroeder, A.; Otten, C.; Büchler, R.; Berg, S.; Ritter, W.; Mühlen, W.; Gisder, S.; et al. The German bee monitoring project: A long term study to understand periodically high winter losses of honey bee colonies. Apidologie 2010, 41, 332–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. Le Conte, Y.; Ellis, M.; Ritter, W. Varroa mites and honey bee health: Can Varroa explain part of the colony losses? Apidologie 2010, 41, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  3. Neumann, P.; Carreck, N. Honey bee colony losses. J. Apic. Res. 2010, 49, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Van Der Zee, R.; Gray, A.; Pisa, L.; De Rijk, T. An Observational Study of Honey Bee Colony Winter Losses and Their Association with Varroa destructor, Neonicotinoids and Other Risk Factors. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0131611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Burgett, A.; Akratanakul, P.; Morse, R.A. Tropilaelaps clareae-A parasite of honey bees in Southeast Asia. Bee World 1983, 64, 25–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Phokasem, P.; De Guzman, L.I.; Khongphinitbunjong, K.; Frake, A.M.; Chantawannakul, P. Feeding by Tropilaelaps mercedesae on pre- and post-capped brood increases damage to Apis mellifera colonies. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Dainat, B.; Ken, T.; Berthoud, H.; Neumann, P. The ectoparasitic mite Tropilaelaps mercedesae (Acari, Laelapidae) as a vector of honeybee viruses. Insectes Sociaux 2008, 56, 40–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Khongphinitbunjong, K.; De Guzman, L.I.; Tarver, M.R.; Rinderer, T.E.; Chantawannakul, P. Interactions of Tropilaelaps mercedesae, honey bee viruses and immune response in Apis mellifera. J. Apic. Res. 2015, 54, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Delfinado, M.D.; Baker, E.W. Tropilaelaps, a new genus of mite from the Philippines (Laelaptidae (s. lat.): Acarina). Fieldiana Zool. 1961, 44, 53–56. [Google Scholar]
  10. Chantawannakul, P.; Ramsey, S.; Vanengelsdorp, D.; Khongphinitbunjong, K.; Phokasem, P. Tropilaelaps mite: An emerging threat to European honey bee. Curr. Opin. Insect Sci. 2018, 26, 69–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Peng, Y.-S.; Fang, Y.; Xu, S.; Ge, L. The resistance mechanism of the Asian honey bee, Apis cerana Fabr., to an ectoparasitic mite, Varroa jacobsoni Oudemans. J. Invertebr. Pathol. 1987, 49, 54–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Anderson, D.L.; Roberts, J.M.K. Standard methods for Tropilaelaps mites research. J. Apic. Res. 2013, 52, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Lin, Z.; Page, P.; Li, L.; Qin, Y.; Zhang, Y.; Hu, F.; Neumann, P.; Zheng, H.-Q.; Dietemann, V. Go East for Better Honey Bee Health: Apis cerana Is Faster at Hygienic Behavior than A. mellifera. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0162647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Boecking, O. Sealing up and non-removal of diseased and Varroa jacobsoni infested drone brood cells is part of the hygienic behaviour in Apis cerana. J. Apic. Res. 1999, 38, 159–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Boecking, O.; Spivak, M. Behavioral defenses of honey bees against Varroa jacobsoni Oud. Apidologie 1999, 30, 141–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Khongphinitbunjong, K.; De Guzman, L.I.; Burgett, M.D.; Rinderer, T.E.; Chantawannakul, P. Behavioral responses underpinning resistance and susceptibility of honey bees to Tropilaelaps mrecedesae. Apidologie 2012, 43, 590–599. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Woyke, J. Infestation of honey bee (Apis Mellifera) colonies by the parasitic mites Varroa jacobsoni and Tropilaelaps clareae in South Vietnam and results of chemical treatment. J. Apic. Res. 1987, 26, 64–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Wilde, J.; Woyke, J.; Neupane, K.R.; Wilde, M. Comparative evaluation tests of different methods to control Tropilaelaps clareae, a mite parasite in Nepal. In Proceedings of the 5th Asian Apicultural Association Conference 2000, Chiang Mai, Thailand, 19–25 March 2000; pp. 249–251. [Google Scholar]
  19. Pettis, J.S.; Rose, R.; Chaimanee, V. Chemical and cultural control of Tropilaelaps mercedesae mites in honeybee (Apis mellifera) colonies in Northern Thailand. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0188063. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Harbo, J.R.; Harris, J.W. Selecting honey bees for resistance to Varroa jacobsoni. Apidologie 1999, 30, 183–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Büchler, R. Design and success of a German breeding program for Varroa tolerance. Am. Bee J. 2000, 140, 662–665. [Google Scholar]
  22. Le Conte, Y.; De Vaublanc, G.; Crauser, D.; Jeanne, F.; Rousselle, J.-C.; Bécard, J.-M. Honey bee colonies that have survived Varroa destructor. Apidologie 2007, 38, 566–572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Harbo, J.R.; Harris, J.W. Responses to Varroa by honey bees with different levels of Varroa Sensitive Hygiene. J. Apic. Res. 2009, 48, 156–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Oddie, M.; Dahle, B.; Neumann, P. Norwegian honey bees surviving Varroa destructor mite infestations by means of natural selection. PeerJ 2017, 5, e3956. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Khongphinitbunjong, K.; De Guzman, L.I.; Buawangpong, N.; Rinderer, T.E.; Frake, A.M.; Chantawannakul, P. Observations on the removal of brood inoculated with Tropilaelaps mercedesae (Acari: Laelapidae) and the mite’s reproductive success in Apis mellifera colonies. Exp. Appl. Acarol. 2013, 62, 47–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. De Guzman, L.I.; Williams, G.R.; Khongphinitbunjong, K.; Chantawannakul, P. Ecology, Life History, and Management of Tropilaelaps Mites. J. Econ. Èntomol. 2017, 110, 319–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Dietemann, V.F.; Nazzi, S.J.; Martin, D.L.; Anderson, B.; Locke, D.S.; Delaplane, D.; Wauquiez, C.; Tannahill, E.; Frey, B.; Ziegelmann, P.; et al. Standard methods for Varroa Research. J. Apic. Res. 2013, 52, 1–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Bienefeld, K.; Zautke, F.; Gupta, P. A novel method for undisturbed long-term observation of honeybee (Apis mellifera) behavior–illustrated by hygienic behavior towards Varroa infestation. J. Apic. Res. 2015, 54, 541–547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Boecking, O. Removal behaviour of Apis mellifera colonies towards sealed brood cells infested with Varroa jacobsoni: Techniques, extent and efficacy. Apidologie 1992, 23, 371–373. [Google Scholar]
  30. Anderson, D.; Morgan, M.J. Genetic and morphological variation of bee-parasitic Tropilaelaps mites (Acari: Laelapidae): New and re-defined species. Exp. Appl. Acarol. 2007, 43, 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Solignac, M.; Cornuet, J.; Vautrin, D.; Le Conte, Y.; Anderson, D.; Evans, J.D.; Cros-Arteil, S.; Navajas, M. The invasive Korea and Japan types of Varroa destructor, ectoparasitic mites of the Western honeybee (Apis mellifera), are two partly isolated clones. Proc. R. Soc. B Boil. Sci. 2005, 272, 411–419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Rosenkranz, P.; Aumeier, P.; Ziegelmann, B. Biology and control of Varroa destructor. J. Invertebr. Pathol. 2010, 103, S96–S119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Spivak, M.; Reuter, G.S. Performance of hygienic honey bee colonies in a commercial apiary. Apidologie 1998, 29, 291–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Spivak, M.S.; Gilliam, M. Hygienic behavior of honey bees and its application for control of brood diseases and Varroa; part, II. Studies on hygienic behavior since the Rothenbuhler era. Bee World 1998, 79, 169–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Boecking, O.; Bienefeld, K.; Drescher, W. Heritability of the Varroa-specific hygienic behaviour in honey bees (Hymenoptera: Apidae). J. Anim. Breed. Genet. 2000, 117, 417–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Spivak, M.; Reuter, G.S. Varroa destructor infestation in untreated honey bee (Hymenoptera: Apidae) colonies selected for hygienic behavior. J. Econ. Èntomol. 2001, 94, 326–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Wilson-Rich, N.; Spivak, M.; Fefferman, N.H.; Starks, P.T. Genetic, individual, and group facilitation of disease resistance in insect societies. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 2009, 54, 405–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Guarna, M.M.; Melathopoulos, A.P.; Huxter, E.; Iovinella, I.; Parker, R.; Stoynov, N.; Tam, A.; Moon, K.-M.; Chan, Q.W.T.; Pelosi, P.; et al. A search for protein biomarkers links olfactory signal transduction to social immunity. BMC Genom. 2015, 16, 63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Spötter, A.; Gupta, P.; Mayer, M.; Reinsch, N.; Bienefeld, K. Genome-Wide Association Study of aVarroa-Specific Defense Behavior in Honeybees (Apis mellifera). J. Hered. 2016, 107, 220–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Kavinseksan, B.; Wongsiri, S.; Rinderer, T.E.; De Guzman, L.I. Comparison of the hygienic behavior of ARS Russian and commercial honey bees in Thailand. Am. Bee J. 2004, 144, 870–872. [Google Scholar]
  41. Rath, W.; Drescher, W. Response of Apis cerana Fabr towards brood infested with Varroa jacobsoni Oud and infestation rate of colonies in Thailand. Apidologie 1990, 21, 311–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Harbo, J.R.; Harris, J.W. Suppressed mite reproduction explained by the behaviour of adult bees. J. Apic. Res. 2005, 44, 21–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Page, P.; Lin, Z.; Buawangpong, N.; Zheng, H.-Q.; Hu, F.; Neumann, P.; Chantawannakul, P.; Dietemann, V. Social apoptosis in honey bee superorganisms. Sci. Rep. 2016, 6, 27210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  44. Schöning, C.; Gisder, S.; Geiselhardt, S.; Kretschmann, I.; Bienefeld, K.; Hilker, M.; Genersch, E.; Noren, S.R.; Redfern, J.V.; Edwards, E.F. Evidence for damage-dependent hygienic behaviour towards Varroa destructor-parasitised brood in the western honey bee, Apis mellifera. J. Exp. Biol. 2011, 215, 264–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  45. Yang, M.-X.; Tan, K.; Radloff, S.E.; Pirk, C.W.W.; Hepburn, H.R. Hetero-specific queen retinue behavior of worker bees in mixed-species colonies of Apis cerana and Apis mellifera. Apidologie 2010, 41, 54–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Zhang, C.; Pokhrel, S.; Wu, Z.; Miao, X.; Huang, Z.Y.; Yang, W. Longevity, food consumption, and foraging performance of Apis cerana and Apis mellifera in mixed colonies. Apidologie 2019, 50, 153–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Su, S.; Cai, F.; Si, A.; Zhang, S.W.; Tautz, J.; Chen, S. East Learns from West: Asiatic Honeybees Can Understand Dance Language of European Honeybees. PLoS ONE 2008, 3, e2365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Masterman, R.; Smith, B.H.; Spivak, M. Brood Odor Discrimination Abilities in Hygienic Honey Bees (Apis mellifera L.) Using Proboscis Extension Reflex Conditioning. J. Insect Behav. 2000, 13, 87–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Mondet, F.; Alaux, C.; Severac, D.; Rohmer, M.; Mercer, A.; Le Conte, Y. Antennae hold a key to Varroa-sensitive hygiene behaviour in honey bees. Sci. Rep. 2015, 5, 10454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Experimental setup for the observation of hygienic behavior.
Figure 1. Experimental setup for the observation of hygienic behavior.
Insects 11 00510 g001
Figure 2. Survival of Tropilaelaps mercedesae- or Varroa destructor-infested brood by Apis mellifera worker bees.
Figure 2. Survival of Tropilaelaps mercedesae- or Varroa destructor-infested brood by Apis mellifera worker bees.
Insects 11 00510 g002
Figure 3. Intensity of cell opening behavior towards V. destructor- and T. mercedesae-infested brood.
Figure 3. Intensity of cell opening behavior towards V. destructor- and T. mercedesae-infested brood.
Insects 11 00510 g003
Figure 4. Survival of T. mercedesae-infested Apis cerana and A. mellifera brood in the presence of workers of both species.
Figure 4. Survival of T. mercedesae-infested Apis cerana and A. mellifera brood in the presence of workers of both species.
Insects 11 00510 g004

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Shrestha, M.; Wegener, J.; Gautam, I.; Singh, M.; Schwekendiek, C.; Bienefeld, K. Individual-Level Comparisons of Honey Bee (Hymenoptera: Apoidea) Hygienic Behavior Towards Brood Infested with Varroa destructor (Parasitiformes: Varroidae) or Tropilaelaps mercedesae (Mesostigmata: Laelapidae). Insects 2020, 11, 510. https://doi.org/10.3390/insects11080510

AMA Style

Shrestha M, Wegener J, Gautam I, Singh M, Schwekendiek C, Bienefeld K. Individual-Level Comparisons of Honey Bee (Hymenoptera: Apoidea) Hygienic Behavior Towards Brood Infested with Varroa destructor (Parasitiformes: Varroidae) or Tropilaelaps mercedesae (Mesostigmata: Laelapidae). Insects. 2020; 11(8):510. https://doi.org/10.3390/insects11080510

Chicago/Turabian Style

Shrestha, Monica, Jakob Wegener, Ishan Gautam, Madhusudan Singh, Christoph Schwekendiek, and Kaspar Bienefeld. 2020. "Individual-Level Comparisons of Honey Bee (Hymenoptera: Apoidea) Hygienic Behavior Towards Brood Infested with Varroa destructor (Parasitiformes: Varroidae) or Tropilaelaps mercedesae (Mesostigmata: Laelapidae)" Insects 11, no. 8: 510. https://doi.org/10.3390/insects11080510

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop