2.1. Literature Review
Rally ‘round the flag’ effect points to an increasingly supportive public opinion toward politicians and governments at the outset of crisis. Early research on the impact of COVID-19 lockdowns on political support in 15 western European democracies shows that the support for the status quo decision makers, institutions, and regimes has grown, affirming citizens’ approval of strict confinement measures (
Bol et al. 2021).
Bækgaard et al. (
2020) have observed that the rally ‘round the flag’ effect in Denmark has spilled over to other democratic institutions, particularly, the judicial system and the public sector, and possibly to the parliament and the media.
Strömbäck (
2021) concluded that trust in media during crisis either fluctuated or increased.
Esaiasson et al. (
2020) show that in Sweden not only institutional but also interpersonal trust has increased despite the ongoing public contradictions on crisis management.
During the COVID-19 pandemic, differences in the rally ‘round the flag’ effect across countries have been attributed to a range of factors. Studying Austria and France,
Kritzinger et al. (
2021) demonstrate that the perceived threat to health has been the most important variable for augmented government support in Austria, whereas the perceived threat to health has not been essential in France—instead, the partisan alignment with the position or opposition parties determined the level of trust (
Kritzinger et al. 2021). In addition, in both countries, the perception on the appropriateness of the government measures has correlated positively with trust (
Kritzinger et al. 2021). Meanwhile,
Johansson et al. (
2021) illuminate that in Sweden the political ideology, alongside the perceived effects of the pandemic on the country, have become significantly more important at later phases of the pandemic, and it explains the declining support to the government in Sweden after the initially elevated levels of approval. The rally ‘round the flag’ effect has been associated with the levels of severity of the pandemic, claiming that in the countries hit harder by the pandemic, for example, the United States, Italy, and the United Kingdom, the rally effect has been weaker (
Cunningham 2020). On the contrary, exploring the case of the Netherlands,
Schraff (
2020) argues that it is emotionally determined collective anxiety, emerging because of the severity of the pandemic, i.e., the increase in the COVID-19 infections, which fosters the citizens’ support for political institutions. The study affirms that with the increasing numbers of COVID-19, the impact of social trust and economic evaluations on political trust decreases (
Schraff 2020).
In sum, studies show that such factors as perceived severity of the pandemic, perceived threat to health, perceived effects of the pandemic on the country, perceived appropriateness of the government measures, as well as collective anxiety and partisanship alignment have been important in driving political support in Western and Nordic democracies. However, less is known about the rally ‘round the flag’ effect in Eastern Europe, especially ethnically heterogenous, lower income societies with less established partisanship traditions such as Latvia.
2.3. Conceptual Framework and Research Questions
Adding these three factors—ethnic diversity, absence of established partisanship, and low income—to permanently low level of trust in democratic institutions in Latvia, our examination of the rally ‘round the flag’ effect adds new knowledge to the broader, global debate about the role of this effect during the COVID-19 pandemic. In the rally ‘round the flag’ effect studies, the focal point of analysis is trust, which is a key factor to mobilize the public support toward crisis mitigation action, proposed by the government. Studies confirm that a lockdown, imposed after the outbreak of the COVID-19 pandemic, increased the support to democratic institutions (
Bol et al. 2021;
Bækgaard et al. 2020). Besides curbing the pandemic, the rally effect provides benefits for politicians, for instance, enhancing their chances in elections (
Yam et al. 2020). Nevertheless, the rally effect lasts for a relatively short term (
Johansson et al. 2021), fostering scholarly interest in the causes of its rise and decline. We observe a similar effect also in Latvia during the first wave of COVID-19 pandemic from March to September 2020. The rally effect, we argue, partly explains the relatively lower infection rates at the beginning of the pandemic in Latvia. Although the term ‘trust’ has multiple meanings (
Luhmann 1979) and there is no single agreed definition (
Enli and Rosenberg 2018), in the context of government institutions, it means that people rely on the institution to do its duty, serve in the interest of the society, and not misuse its powers (
Pjesivac 2017). Trust is especially important in crisis situations such as the first wave of the COVID-19 pandemic, when the initial information is scarce, yet the situation demands an immediate response from the government and population. Regarding the rally ‘round the flag’ effect,
Bækgaard et al. (
2020) observe that heightened trust in government after the COVID-19 lockdown has been spilling over to other democratic institutions. Studies demonstrate that the pandemic’s course at the early stage was determined by the authorities’ ability to deliver trustworthy information (
Zarocostas 2020) to various audiences and by the society’s attitude toward this information (
Nielsen 2020). In agreement with these observations, for our examination of the rally ‘round the flag’ effect we focus on two major groups of democratic institutions as information sources: government and the media (including public and private broadcasters, printed press, and online news portals (ONPs)). In addition, we evaluate trust in international organisations such as World Health Organization (WHO), which was the main institution the governments followed in their communication strategies.
One’s peers, community opinion leaders, or other respected individuals tend to influence the attitudes toward government and media.
Esaiasson et al. (
2020) show that interpersonal trust has increased during the pandemic, thus helping to face the crisis. Thus, in addition to government and the media, we also included interpersonal communication because the informal networks of friends, relatives, and colleagues traditionally are notable sources of information in Latvia (
Kalniņa 2018;
Vasiļjeva 2016). We included also social networking sites (SNS) in our research as these sites have become a notable source of information for some individuals. The COVID-19 pandemic motivated people to increase their search for health information on the Internet, which, however, oftentimes caused more confusion and anxiety instead of helping (
Mohammadi et al. 2020;
Shetata 2020).
Finally, considering that the COVID-19 pandemic is a medical and public health emergency and healthcare professionals have had a visible place in public debates since the beginning of the pandemic, we also included this group in our research, although the medical field usually is not analysed alongside the democratic institutions.
Overall, we aimed to find out if trust or distrust in these four types of sources influence the perceived disease risk and contribute to the rally ‘round the flag’ effect.
In our research, trustworthy sources are those that the respondents believe to be deserving of their trust and confidence (
Ardèvol-Abreu et al. 2018;
Takahashi and Tandoc 2016;
Tsfati and Ariely 2013). The media are essential intermediaries in the process of agenda-setting by selecting the issues, which present to the public (
McCombs 2005;
McCombs and Shaw 1972), and transmitting information, still defined as ‘social glue’ (
Enli and Rosenberg 2018), yet the effect of the transmitted information will depend on the perception of the media trustworthiness. Personal factors determine an individual’s trust in the media (
Yamamoto et al. 2016). Individuals who generally do not trust other people and politicians are more likely to distrust the media (
Cappella 2002;
Cappella and Jamieson 1997). If the audience does not trust the media, then it is more likely to also distrust the information the media conveys (
Tsfati and Ariely 2013). Distrust in qualitative media can negatively affect the public’s opportunities to obtain diverse information (
Kohring and Matthes 2007) and lead to a situation where the audience may not receive vital information and instead relies on alternative sources such as SNS or friends (
van Dijck and Alinejad 2020), which, in a longer-term, may modify individuals’ perception of reality, as the cultivation theory suggests (
Gerbner et al. 2002), or provide alternative means to cope with the cognitive dissonance the pandemic may cause (
Festinger 1957). Notably, not only trust in government, but also trust in radio and television rose sharply in many countries during the COVID-19 pandemic (
Newman 2020). Furthermore, recent research has shown that higher attention to news media can be related to a higher degree of adherence to the preventive behaviour guidelines (
Faasse and Newby 2020;
Jiang et al. 2021).
During the first wave of the pandemic, healthcare professionals became highly visible information sources. Trust in sources about health issues determines the extent to which people will perceive the provided information as meaningful, credible, and relevant (
Rimer et al. 1999;
Alexander et al. 2011). Demographic, socio-economic, and lifestyle factors also influence the perception of sources on medicine and health in general (
Hesse et al. 2005) and COVID-19 in particular (
Jahangiry et al. 2020).
Plohl and Musil (
2020) suggest that trust in scientists in general is a pre-condition to curbing the spread of disinformation and conspiracy theories. However,
Eichengreen et al. (
2021) warn that the pandemic might eventually diminish trust in scientists and experts.
To measure the perceived disease risk, we focus on subjective self-assessment of a person’s vulnerability to health threats (
Brewer et al. 2004). Relying on the framework of health psychology, Health Belief Model (
Rosenstock 1974;
Becker et al. 1978) can be helpful for understanding the main determining factors of health behaviour during the COVID-19 pandemic. According to the model, any stimuli related to health create an assessment of perceived risk and the possible overcoming of this risk (
Sarwar et al. 2020). This model has been successfully employed in COVID-19 research (
Guillon and Kergall 2020;
Heydari et al. 2021;
Jiang et al. 2021;
Sperling 2020). Publicly available information helps individuals to understand their disease risk (
Lee et al. 2010), because perceived risk is not always related to actual risk (
van der Weijden et al. 2007). A failed communication can cause uncertainty about a person’s ability to evaluate their disease risk (
Orom et al. 2020). Government institutions, the media, and other people’s opinions are among those external factors that shape the risks alongside individual factors (genetics, age, experience, etc.) and culture (
Millstein and Halpern-Felsher 2002;
Siaki and Loescher 2011). Media literacy can also contribute to perceived risk of disease, as individuals’ ability to recognize disinformation may modify their perception of risk (
Rožukalne et al. 2021).
Relying on these conceptual approaches, we formulated the following research questions:
RQ1: What was the correlation between trust in information sources and such variables as perceived disease risk, gender, age, education, income, and language spoken in the family during the first wave of the COVID-19 pandemic in Latvia from March to September 2020?
RQ2: Does trust in government institutions spill over to other sources of information, most notably the media, the healthcare professionals, and interpersonal networks?
We expected to find a positive correlation between the perceived risk of disease and trust in information sources at the beginning of the pandemic, thus contributing to the rally ‘round the flag’ effect. Hypothetically, the rally effect should be similar across different sociodemographic and ethnolinguistic groups, given that the pandemic is universal in its character, and the imposed restrictions to curb the spread of the virus are the same for everyone. Yet we expected to see variations in the strength of the correlation across the measured variables.
2.4. Method and Data Collection
We collected the data for our study in a nationally representative sociological survey using an Internet panel (CAWI method) that took place in September 2020. The sample of research consisted of one group of respondents in September (n = 1005). Adult residents of Latvia aged 18 to 75 participated in the survey. The sample of quotas was used to form the sample of the survey; the data were weighted according to the data of the Population Register of the Office of Citizenship and Migration Affairs of the Ministry of the Interior of the Republic of Latvia on 27 January 2020. A random sampling method provided a sample of adequate representations of all socio-demographic and ethno-linguistic groups of Latvia. Females represent 53% of the sample, whereas males represent 47%. The most widely represented age groups in survey are 35–44 years and 45–55 years, 21% and 20% of respondents, respectively. Furthermore, 62% of the respondents mainly use Latvian as a language of communication in the family, 36% communicate in Russian, and 2% in another language.
The survey consisted of question blocks with closed questions. The respondents were asked to evaluate specific sources of information (friends and relatives, colleagues, healthcare professionals, Latvian government institutions, TV, radio, newspapers, ONPs, SNS, international organisations such as the World Health Organization (WHO)) on a four-point Likert scale of very trustworthy to very untrustworthy. To consider trust in sources in correlation with the perceived disease risk, we asked the respondents to evaluate their perceived risk on a five-point scale from Low and almost unreal to Very high and very real. Base for average value: respondents who gave a specific answer, i.e., the frequency of mentioning the answer I do not want to answer/hard to say was not taken into the account.
The SPSS programme was used for the initial processing of the data, their comparison, and grouping, using the functions of descriptive statistics and correlation. The survey data were weighted, and a correlation analysis was performed using Spearman’s rank correlation coefficient to determine statistically significant correlations between respondents’ risk perceptions and trust in various sources of information. The Kruskal-Wallis H test (also called the ’one-way ANOVA on ranks’) was used in the data analysis, focusing on data that could characterize differences between parts of population (criterions employed—gender, age, education, income, and language used in the family) and their trust in information sources. Comparing the results verified with mentioned tests, Kruskal-Wallis H test data were chosen for the further analysis.