How Power Affects Moral Judgments: The Presence of Harm to Life Modifies the Association between Power and Moral Choices
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Power
3. Importance of the Link between Power and Moral Judgments
4. Presence of Harm in Dilemmas
5. How the Presence of Harm Modifies the Relationship between Power and Moral Thinking
6. Culture
7. The Present Research
7.1. Study 1
7.1.1. Participants and Design
7.1.2. Procedure
7.1.3. Results and Discussion
7.2. Study 2
7.2.1. Participants and Design
7.2.2. Procedure
7.2.3. Results and Discussion
7.3. Study 3
7.3.1. Method
7.3.2. Procedure
7.3.3. Results and Discussion
7.4. Study 4
7.4.1. Methods
7.4.2. Procedure
7.4.3. Results and Discussion
8. General Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Anderson, Cameron, and Adam D. Galinsky. 2006. Power, optimism, and risk-taking. European Journal of Social Psychology 36: 511–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Anderson, Cameron, and Jennifer L. Berdahl. 2002. The experience of power: Examining the effects of power on approach and inhibition tendencies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 83: 1362–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Anderson, Cameron, Oliver P. John, and Dacher Keltner. 2012. The personal sense of power. Journal of Personality 80: 313–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Anderson, Cameron, Sanjay Srivastava, Jennifer S. Beer, Sandra E. Spataro, and Jennifer A. Chatman. 2006. Knowing your place: Self-perceptions of status in social groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 91: 1094–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bandura, Albert. 1999. Social cognitive theory of personality. In Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research, 2nd ed. Edited by Lawrence A. Pervin and Oliver P. John. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 154–96. [Google Scholar]
- Bargh, John A., Paula Raymond, John B. Pryor, and Fritz Strack. 1995. Attractiveness of the underling: An automatic power sex association and its consequences for sexual harassment and aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 68: 768–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bartels, Daniel M. 2008. Principled moral sentiment and the flexibility of moral judgment and decision making. Cognition 108: 381–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Belk, Russell W. 1991. The ineluctable mysteries of posessions. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality 6: 17–55. [Google Scholar]
- Bentham, Jeremy. 1948. An Introduction to the Principles of Morals and Legislation. New York: Hafner. First published 1789. [Google Scholar]
- Bugental, Daphne Blunt, and Jeffrey Clayton Lewis. 1999. The paradoxical misuse of power by those who see themselves as powerless: How does it happen? Journal of Social Issues 55: 51–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bugental, Daphne Blunt, Jay Blue, and Michael Cruzcosa. 1989. Perceived control over caregiving outcomes: Implications for child abuse. Developmental Psychology 25: 532–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, Serena, Annette Y. Lee-Chai, and John A. Bargh. 2001. Relationship orientation as a moderator of the effects of social power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 80: 173–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cohen, Jacob. 1988. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Hilsdale: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
- Cushman, Fiery, and Joshua D. Greene. 2012. Finding faults: How moral dilemmas illuminate cognitive structure. Social Neuroscience 7: 269–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Darwall, Stephen L. 2003a. Consequentialism. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Darwall, Stephen L. 2003b. Deontology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Emerson, Richard M. 2019. Power-dependence relations. In Power in Modern Societies. Edited by Marvin E. Olsen, Martin N Marger and Valencia Fonseca. London: Routledge, pp. 48–58. [Google Scholar]
- Fast, Nathanael J., and Serena Chen. 2009. When the boss feels inadequate: Power, incompetence, and aggression. Psychological Science 20: 1406–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fiske, Susan T. 1993. Controlling other people: The impact of power on stereotyping. American Psychologist 48: 621–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fiske, Susan T., and Eric Dépret. 1996. Control, interdependence and power: Understanding social cognition in its social context. European Review of Social Psychology 7: 31–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fleischmann, Alexandra, Joris Lammers, Paul Conway, and Adam D. Galinsky. 2019. Paradoxical effects of power on moral thinking: Why power both increases and decreases deontological and utilitarian moral decisions. Social Psychological and Personality Science 10: 110–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Foot, Philippa. 1967. The problem of abortion and the doctrine of double effect. Oxford Review 5: 1–6. [Google Scholar]
- Friedman, Ori, and Hildy Ross. 2011. Twenty-one reasons to care about the psychological basis of ownership. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development 132: 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fu, Genyue, Fen Xu, Catherine Ann Cameron, Gail Heyman, and Kang Lee. 2007. Cross-cultural differences in children’s choices, categorizations, and evaluations of truths and lies. Developmental Psychology 43: 278–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Galinsky, Adam D., Deborah H. Gruenfeld, and Joe C. Magee. 2003. From power to action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 85: 453–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Galinsky, Adam D., Joe C. Magee, Deborah H. Gruenfeld, Jennifer A. Whitson, and Katie A. Liljenquist. 2008. Power reduces the press of the situation: Implications for creativity, conformity, and dissonance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 95: 1450–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gawronski, Bertram, and Skylar M. Brannon. 2020. Power and moral dilemma judgments: Distinct effects of memory recall versus social roles. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 86: 103908. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Graham, Jesse, Brian A. Nosek, Jonathan Haidt, Ravi Iyer, Spassena Koleva, and Peter H. Ditto. 2011. Mapping the moral domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 101: 366–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Graham, Jesse, Jonathan Haidt, and Brian A. Nosek. 2009. Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 96: 1029–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Greene, Joshua D. 2009. Dual-process morality and the personal/impersonal distinction: A reply to McGuire, Langdon, Coltheart, and Mackenzie. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 45: 581–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Greene, Joshua D., and Jonathan Haidt. 2002. How (and where) does moral judgment work? Trends in Cognitive Science 6: 517–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Greene, Joshua D., Fiery A. Cushman, Lisa E Stewart, Kelly Lowenberg, Leigh E. Nystrom, and Jonathan D. Cohen. 2009. Pushing moral buttons: The interaction between personal force and intention in moral judgment. Cognition 111: 364–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Greene, Joshua D., Leigh E. Nystrom, Andrew D. Engell, John M. Darley, and Jonathan D. Cohen. 2004. The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgment. Neuron 44: 389–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Greene, Joshua D., R. Brian Sommerville, Leigh E. Nystrom, John M. Darley, and Jonathan D. Cohen. 2001. An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science 293: 2105–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Greene, Joshua D., Sylvia A. Morelli, Kelly Lowenberg, Leigh E. Nystrom, and Jonathan D. Cohen. 2008. Cognitive load selectively interferes with utilitarian moral judgment. Cognition 107: 1144–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guinote, Ana. 2008. Power and affordances: When the situation has more power over powerful than powerless individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 95: 237–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guinote, Ana. 2017. How Power Affects People: Activating, Wanting, and Goal Seeking. Annual Review of Psychology 68: 353–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Guinote, Ana, and Adele Phillips. 2010. Power can increase stereotyping. Social Psychology 41: 3–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haidt, Jonathan. 2001. The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review 108: 814–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Haidt, Jonathan. 2003. The moral emotions. In Handbook of Affective Sciences. Edited by Richard J. Davidson, Klaus R. Sherer and H. Hill Goldsmith. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 852–70. [Google Scholar]
- Hauser, Marc, Fiery Cushman, Liane Young, R. Kang-Xing Jin, and John Mikhail. 2007. A dissociation between moral judgments and justifications. Mind & Language 22: 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heekeren, Hauke R., Isabell Wartenburger, Helge Schmidt, Kristin Prehn, Hans-Peter Schwintowski, and Arno Villringer. 2005. Influence of bodily harm on neural correlates of semantic and moral decision-making. Neuroimage 24: 887–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kahane, Guy. 2015. Sidetracked by trolleys: Why sacrificial moral dilemmas tell us little (or nothing) about utilitarian judgment. Social Neuroscience 10: 551–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kahane, Guy, Jim A. C. Everett, Brian D. Earp, Lucius Caviola, Nadira S. Faber, Molly J. Crockett, and Julian Savulescu. 2018. Beyond sacrificial harm: A two-dimensional model of utilitarian psychology. Psychological Review 125: 131–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kant, Immanuel. 1785. Foundations of the Metaphysics of Morals. New York: Macmillan. [Google Scholar]
- Keltner, Dacher, Deborah H. Gruenfeld, and Cameron Anderson. 2003. Power, approach, and inhibition. Psychological Review 110: 265–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kipnis, David. 1972. Does power corrupt? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 24: 33–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koenigs, Michael, Liane Young, Ralph Adolphs, Daniel Tranel, Fiery Cushman, Marc Hauser, and Antonio Damasio. 2007. Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judgments. Nature 446: 908–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lammers, Joris, and Diederik A. Stapel. 2009. How power influences moral thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 97: 279–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mann, Nikki H., and Kerry Kawakami. 2012. The long, steep path to equality: Progressing on egalitarian goals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 141: 187–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Millar, J. Charles, John Turri, and Ori Friedman. 2014. For the greater goods? Ownership rights and utilitarian moral judgment. Cognition 133: 79–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moore, Adam B., Brian A. Clark, and Michael J. Kane. 2008. Who shalt not kill? Individual differences in working memory capacity, executive control, and moral judgment. Psychological Science 19: 549–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ng, Sik Hung. 1980. The Social Psychology of Power. San Diego: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- O’Neill, Patricia, and Lewis Petrinovich. 1998. A preliminary cross-cultural study of moral intuitions. Evolution and Human Behavior 19: 349–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Overbeck, Jennifer R., and Bernadette Park. 2001. When power does not corrupt: Superior individuation processes among powerful perceivers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81: 549–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Overbeck, Jennifer R., and Bernadette Park. 2006. Powerful perceivers, powerless objects: Flexibility of powerholders’ social attention. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 99: 227–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Petrinovich, Lewis, Patricia O’Neill, and Matthew Jorgensen. 1993. An empirical study of moral intuitions: Toward an evolutionary ethics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 64: 467–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rochat, Philippe. 2011. Possession and morality in early development. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development 132: 23–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rossano, Federico, Hannes Rakoczy, and Michael Tomasello. 2011. Young children’s understanding of violations of property rights. Cognition 121: 219–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Royzman, Edward B., and Jonathan Baron. 2002. The preference for indirect harm. Social Justice Research 15: 165–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Russell, Bertrand. 1938. Power: A New Social Analysis. London: Routledge Classics. [Google Scholar]
- Schmid Mast, Marianne, Klaus Jonas, and Judith A. Hall. 2009. Give a person power and he or she will show interpersonal sensitivity: The phenomenon and its why and when. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 97: 835–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, Pamela K., and Wilhelm Hofmann. 2016. Power in everyday life. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences United States of America 113: 10043–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tetlock, Philip E., Orie V. Kristel, kantkant S. Beth Elson, Melanie C. Green, and Jennifer S. Lerner. 2000. The psychology of the unthinkable: Taboo trade-offs, forbidden base rates, and heretical counterfactuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 78: 853–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thomson, Judith Jarvis. 1976. Killing, letting die, and the trolley problem. The Monist 59: 204–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thomson, Judith Jarvis. 1985. The trolley problem. The Yale Law Journal 94: 1395–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thomson, Judith Jarvis. 1986. Rights, Restitution, and Risk: Essays in Moral Theory. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Valdesolo, Piercarlo, and David DeSteno. 2006. Manipulations of emotional context shape moral judgment. Psychological Science 17: 476–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Kleef, Gerben A., Christopher Oveis, Ilmo van der Lowe, Aleksandr LuoKogan, Jennifer Goetz, and Dacher Keltner. 2008. Power, distress, and compassion: Turning a blind eye to the suffering of others. Psychological Science 19: 1315–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Waldmann, Michael R., and Jörn H. Dieterich. 2007. Throwing a bomb on a person versus throwing a person on a bomb: Intervention myopia in moral intuitions. Psychological Science 18: 247–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wheatley, Thalia, and Jonathan Haidt. 2005. Hypnotic disgust makes moral judgments more severe. Psychological Science 16: 780–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Group | n | Female | Mage (SD) | Mwork-time (SD) |
---|---|---|---|---|
Government | ||||
High power | 40 | 21 | 44.28 (6.46) | 223.35 (110.07) |
Low power | 39 | 21 | 30.36 (6.48) | 65.10 (81.14) |
Total | 79 | 42 | 37.41 (9.51) | 145.23 (124.92) |
Companies | ||||
High power | 42 | 16 | 35.52 (6.69) | 94.52 (82.24) |
Low power | 75 | 39 | 30.45 (6.29) | 41.46 (39.24) |
Total | 117 | 55 | 32.27 (6.68) | 60.51 (63.45) |
Total | ||||
High power | 82 | 37 | 39.79 (7.88) | 157.36 (116.00) |
Low power | 114 | 60 | 30.42 (6.33) | 49.55 (57.87) |
Total | 196 | 97 | 34.34 (8.40) | 94.66 (101.85) |
High Power | Low Power | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Organization Type | n | M (SD) | 95% CI | n | M (SD) | 95% CI |
Government | 40 | 3.55 (2.22) | [2.22, 4.20] | 39 | 4.64 (2.59) | [3.91, 5.51] |
Companies | 42 | 3.19 (2.33) | [2.45, 3.95] | 75 | 3.47 (2.51) | [3.00, 4.22] |
Total | 82 | 3.37 (2.27) | [2.58, 3.83] | 114 | 3.87 (2.59) | [3.63, 4.68] |
Power Condition | Harm | No Harm | t | df | p | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
n | M (SD) 95% CI | n | M (SD) 95% CI | ||||
High power | 35 | 3.23 (2.39) [2.39, 4.07] | 35 | 4.09 (2.59) [3.25, 4.93] | −1.44 | 68 | 0.16 |
Control group | 36 | 3.58 (2.31) [2.79, 4.38] | 38 | 4.84 (2.47) [4.07, 5.62] | −2.26 | 72 | <0.05 |
Low power | 35 | 2.77 (2.40) [1.95, 3.59] | 35 | 5.40 (2.46) [4.58, 6.22] | −4.52 | 68 | <0.001 |
Harm | No Harm | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Power Condition | n | M (SD) | 95% CI | n | M (SD) | 95% CI |
High power | 35 | 5.11 (2.55) | [4.37, 5.86] | 40 | 5.60 (2.08) | [4.86, 6.34] |
Low power | 35 | 4.80 (2.40) | [4.11, 5.49] | 35 | 6.83 (1.72) | [6.09, 7.57] |
Total | 70 | 4.95 (2.46) | [4.45, 5.46] | 75 | 6.21 (1.99) | [5.69, 6.74] |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Zheng, M.; Guinote, A.; Luo, W. How Power Affects Moral Judgments: The Presence of Harm to Life Modifies the Association between Power and Moral Choices. Soc. Sci. 2024, 13, 256. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci13050256
Zheng M, Guinote A, Luo W. How Power Affects Moral Judgments: The Presence of Harm to Life Modifies the Association between Power and Moral Choices. Social Sciences. 2024; 13(5):256. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci13050256
Chicago/Turabian StyleZheng, Mufan, Ana Guinote, and Wei Luo. 2024. "How Power Affects Moral Judgments: The Presence of Harm to Life Modifies the Association between Power and Moral Choices" Social Sciences 13, no. 5: 256. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci13050256
APA StyleZheng, M., Guinote, A., & Luo, W. (2024). How Power Affects Moral Judgments: The Presence of Harm to Life Modifies the Association between Power and Moral Choices. Social Sciences, 13(5), 256. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci13050256