Next Article in Journal
Impact of Relationship Breakdown, Including Abuse and Negotiation of Co-Parenting Arrangements, on Fathers’ Mental Health, Help-Seeking, and Coping
Previous Article in Journal
Mental Health, Resilience, and Well-Being Among Sexual Minority College Students: A Study Framed by the Minority Stress and Minority Resilience Models
Previous Article in Special Issue
Parent–Child Adaptive Responses for Digital Resilience
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Beyond Digital Literacy: Building Youth Digital Resilience Through Existing “Information Sensibility” Practices

1
Darwin College, University of Cambridge, Silver Street, Cambridge CB3 9EU, UK
2
Google (United States), 1600 Amphitheatre Parkway, Mountain View, CA 94043, USA
3
Gemic, 82 Nassau Street, #863, New York, NY 10004, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Soc. Sci. 2025, 14(4), 230; https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci14040230
Submission received: 31 January 2025 / Revised: 14 March 2025 / Accepted: 19 March 2025 / Published: 7 April 2025
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Promoting the Digital Resilience of Youth)

Abstract

:
Youth media consumption and disordered eating practices have historically been subjects of moral panics, often resulting in protective, deficit-based interventions like content removal. We argue for interventions which instead equip youth to evaluate and manage risks in their online environments, building upon their existing “information sensibility” practices. Drawing upon ethnographic research and intervention testing with 77 participants in the US and India, we analyze how youth (aged 13–26), including those with diverse political perspectives and those recovering from disordered eating (DE), engage with online news and health information. Participants generally algorithmically encountered (rather than searched for) information online, and their engagement was shaped more by social motivations—like belonging—than truth seeking. Participants interpreted online information collaboratively, relying on social cues and peer validation within their online communities. They demonstrated preference for personal testimonies and relatable sources, particularly those with similar social identities. We propose resilience-building interventions that build upon these youth online information practices by: (1) leveraging peer networks, promoting critical information engagement through collaborative learning and peer-to-peer support within online communities; (2) developing social media sensibility, equipping youth to critically evaluate information sources in situ; (3) providing pathways offline, connecting youth to desired in-person communities; and (4) encouraging probabilistic thinking.

1. Introduction

Youth media consumption and disordered eating (DE) practices have historically been subjects of moral panics (Gooldin 2008), often resulting in protective, deficit-based interventions like content removal and parental controls. We argue for interventions which instead equip youth to agentially evaluate and manage risks in their online environments, building upon their existing practices. This paper develops strength-based interventions for youth digital resilience by analyzing how youth (aged 13–26) engage with online information and testing interventions designed based on these findings. Strength-based interventions focus on how to leverage people’s existing abilities and practices to increase resilience against harm, rather than focusing on their deficits. Resilience-based approaches seek to maintain people’s autonomy and ability to self-regulate when facing harms, instead of paternalistically shielding them from encountering risk (Park et al. 2024).
Most research on youth digital literacy and DE follow medical approaches by centering analysis on individual encounters with “harmful” content, resulting in individual-oriented interventions which do not match the culturally mediated and socially oriented way that youth encounter, evaluate, and apply information. These interventions are often deficit-based, aimed at identifying, labeling, and removing “harmful” content through moderation to protect youth. We argue that a focus on content (1) fails to account for the contextually mediated nature of harm (that a piece of content can be helpful or neutral for one youth while being harmful to another) and (2) does not result in solutions which equip youth with the tools to evaluate content harms in situ.
Moving beyond traditional literacy-based paradigms, we argue for interventions based upon youth “information sensibility” practices—how youth develop a socially informed awareness of the value of information encountered online, relying on folk heuristics1 of credibility (Hassoun et al. 2023). Information sensibility differs from traditional information literacy, which often relies on individual linear models of engagement, the mastery of core competencies like fact-checking and lateral reading, and the assumption that accurate information is always the highest-quality or highest-value information (and that validating content accuracy is people’s primary information engagement goal) (Mackey and Jacobson 2011).
We instead engage anthropological analyses which frame digital literacy and DE practices as fundamentally relational (rather than individual). By explaining how dietary science has long been embedded in moral (often sexist and racist) frameworks, we argue for relational analyses of how youth make sense of eating-related health information online—particularly marginalized youth, who often doubt medical models of health and eating that have historically been discriminatory. Building on research explaining how DE practices are mediated by desire for social belonging, we analyze how youths’ desires to identify with emerging health-related online identities affect their information sensibility practices.
Information sensibility stresses the social nature of information processing for youth, who often encounter, rather than actively search for information, and sees social motivations as primary drivers in how they engage with and interpret it. Youth understanding and evaluation of online information is socially informed: heavily influenced by their social context, including their peers, family, and online communities. Youth interpret information together, as aspirational members of social groups, rather than as isolated individuals. Their informational and social needs are inseparably entangled. Youth assess information not just for its accuracy or facticity, but also for its value to their personal lives, relationships, and their ability to navigate their social environments. The social utility of information shapes its importance and purpose for them. This social significance of information is often as important as, or even more important than, its accuracy.
Our findings expand previous work on the social appeal of misinformation (Duffy et al. 2019; Geeng et al. 2020; O’Connor and Weatherall 2019) by detailing the social influences affecting youth information engagement (e.g., Herrero-Diz et al. 2020). We suggest that social support and belonging may be key solutions to the problem of youth misinformation susceptibility. Many participants’ core problem was not misjudging misinformation (solved via literacy-based competencies) but rather a desire for social belonging, which they sometimes solved by sharing misinformation. Interventions targeting youth susceptibility to misinformation, therefore, must go beyond classroom-based competency-building paradigms to support the underlying social and emotional needs driving youth misinformation sharing.

1.1. Research Questions

We conducted two ethnographic studies with 77 participants in the US and India, investigating their online information practices around news and health information. Study 1 asked the following research questions:
(RQ1) Seeking information—Why, where, and how do youth seek information online?
(RQ2) Assessing information—Under what circumstances do youth attempt to assess if information they engage with online is accurate, credible, and high-quality? How do they perform such assessments?
(RQ3) Susceptibility to misinformation—What factors affect the susceptibility of youth to misinformation?
From Study 1, we found:
  • Participant informational and social needs were inseparably entangled.
    (1)
    Participant information journeys rarely began with a truth-seeking search query.
    (2)
    Participants used information to orient themselves socially and define their emerging identities.
Study 2 included participants recovered from DE and asked the following additional eating- and health-related research questions:
(RQ4) Trusted health information—How do youth find trusted information on eating and health? What are their information sensibility practices?
(RQ5) Healthy or harmful behaviors—How do those patterns and encounters with online eating and health information lead to healthy or harmful behaviors?
(RQ6) Misinformation and authoritative information—What constitutes “misinformation” and authoritative health information in youth’s online contexts?
From Study 2, we found:
2.
Participants used similarity to themselves as a trust heuristic to determine whether an information source was trustworthy, asking if the content creator was “like-minded and like-bodied” (what we term n = 1 thinking).
3.
Participants crowdsourced credibility, using distributed trust heuristics to confirm with peers whether a source was trustworthy, instead of relying on more traditional markers of institutional or scientific authority.
4.
Participants sought to demonstrate online that they were living a holistically “healthy” life, in which performing the “right” body-based practices signified their “goodness” (and resulted in better access to social capital and material resources).
5.
Even when participants found useful online resources and communities, they believed that the “good life” was ultimately lived offline and wanted to decrease the time they spent online.
In both studies, we found that information encounter—whether for news or health information—was socially driven: participants generally encountered (rather than searched for) information online, and their engagement with it was shaped more by social motivations like belonging and identity formation than truth seeking. Further, social sensemaking shaped trust assessments: participants interpreted online information collaboratively, relying on social cues and peer validation within their online communities. They demonstrated preference for personal testimonies and relatable sources, particularly those perceived to share similar social identities (“like-minded, like-bodied”). These practices could increase potential digital-safety risk, particularly from following health guidance from such sources. These findings highlight limitations of content-focused interventions and emphasize the potential for strength-based approaches built upon the social embeddedness of online information practices.
Based upon these ethnographic research findings, we worked with designers and product managers at a technology company to create mockup interventions to test with participants in follow-up interviews. From the ethnographic findings and intervention-testing results, we propose resilience-building interventions that:
(1)
leverage peer networks, promoting critical information engagement through collaborative learning and peer-to-peer support within online communities;
(2)
develop social media sensibility, equipping youth with the skills to critically evaluate information sources in situ, situate creators’ motivations, consider potential harms from following their advice, and navigate social influences online;
6.
provide pathways offline, connecting youth to in-person communities;
7.
encourage probabilistic thinking, countering some of the digital safety risks increased by n = 1 thinking.
Next, we review existing literature to argue for a relational approach to building youth digital resilience, particularly with respect to eating and health information. We then describe our methodology, results, intervention testing, and discuss the implications of our findings for developing strength-based interventions for youth digital resilience.

1.2. Youth Information Literacy and Disordered Eating

While “media influence” has long been a concern in DE literature (Bordo 1993), the “always on” cadence and algorithmically powered delivery of online content give rise to new conditions shaping youth eating and health information engagement (Marwick and Boyd 2010; Buckingham 2008; Pater et al. 2016). Social media is a key site where youth learn how their bodies should visually reflect adherence to dietary and fitness cultures (Weinstein 2018; Marks et al. 2020). Wearable technologies and digital health tracking applications make health vitals, calories burned, and steps walked constantly referenceable (Eikey 2021; Schüll 2016). These technologies are premised on the assumption that “more information = better decision making” (Tregarthen et al. 2015), implying that “poor health” comes from literacy or data deficits rather than access inequities or discriminatory normative definitions of health (Neff and Nafus 2016).
Algorithmic recommendations present emergent challenges for youth engaging with eating and health content online (Griffiths et al. 2024; Costello et al. 2023). As Eaton describes (2016), social media platforms make decisions about what users see: “When I scrolled on the app, I made choices, but the algorithm made just as many, if not more”. Algorithms “work too well in catering to user preferences, bombarding users with the content they need to facilitate their self-harm” (Lai 2022). Lai (2022) argues:
For users with preexisting body image issues, seeking out one or two fitness or healthy recipe videos could fill their feeds with similar videos, and those who continue watching similar content could easily be led to content explicitly promoting eating disorders. Such regular exposure has the potential to trigger or worsen disordered eating behaviors.
Research on Internet use and DE frequently focus on pro-anorexia (“pro-ana”) content, portraying extreme fasting as a lifestyle choice (Logrieco et al. 2021). “Thinspo” and “thinspiration”, content that inspires people to be thin, has “a negative impact on viewers with regard to body image” (Yamamiya et al. 2005). Based on a survey of pro-ana Internet searches and websites, Greene and Brownstone (2021) find “search terms with references to thinspiration and thinspo are associated with the most harmful Website content”. Such content may induce a “toxic spiral” where content becomes intensified and more explicitly harmful (Austin 2021). Interventions tend to be content-focused (Dedrick et al. 2020): removing “harmful” content or replacing it with “healthy”, body-positive or body-neutral content, testing the effects on individuals’ mood and self-image in controlled, lab-based environments (Cohen et al. 2019).
Studies provide limited insight into how and when technology companies can (or should) moderate a user’s informational health ecosystem to prevent harm (Gillespie 2020; Lenhart and Owens 2021). Interventions which identify and remove harmful content alone have demonstrated limited evidence-based success, contributing to “creating isolated and polarized communities” and making search terms inaccessible for those seeking support—particularly at-risk youth (Zhang et al. 2024; Chancellor et al. 2016). Trigger warnings evidence little to no effect and can be countertherapeutic for those with trauma histories (Greenfield 2012; Jones et al. 2020). Skills-based information literacy approaches like fact-checking, widely taught in schools, show similar limitations (Mackey and Jacobson 2011; Breakstone et al. 2021; Gordon et al. 2021).
These content removal and literacy-based interventions may not be effective because conventional paradigms of information literacy do not fully capture what youth actually do online (Boyd 2017) or account for the contextually mediated nature of harm (Yeshua-Katz and Martins 2013). In their everyday lives, youth are motivated by more than what is “true”: they want to discover, learn, and share what is meaningful to them and their social communities (Hassoun et al. 2023). Youth also encounter information in situ and make sense of it with their peers (Eynon and Malmberg 2012; Evans et al. 2010; Feng et al. 2022), making lab-based studies measuring individual encounters inadequate simulations of their actual online practices. Further, most content is neither intrinsically harmful nor helpful—content harms differ based on an individual youth’s sociocultural context, user state, and mode of encounter (Warford et al. 2022).

1.3. Relationality of Disordered Eating Practices

Medical approaches to DE generally focus on individual pathologies, often including “culture” only as a risk factor or reductive proxy for ethnic categorization (Anderson-Fye et al. 2015). Studies of youth Internet use and DE largely follow this individual-focused medical model. We argue these approaches under-account for how youth use socioculturally mediated eating and health practices—including DE—to make meaning: “how the practices of body discipline and affective modulation shape a person’s sense of being in the world” (Lester 2021). We suggest that for youth, these meaning-making practices are not just individual: they are relational social practices performed with communities making sense of online information together.
Existential questions driving DE practices are likewise culturally specific and mediated (Pike and Borovoy 2004; Pike and Dunne 2015). Anthropological studies in India have long integrated questions of food and eating with purity, the body, and social distinction (caste) (Appadurai 1981; Khare 1992; Marriott 1989; Raheja 1988). Recent studies have analyzed everyday practices of eating, belonging, and identity-formation (Mines 2009; Rao 2009; Pandian 2009). Researchers in the US have begun analyzing how subjective experiences of anorexia and broader cultures of dietary restriction and moderation are shaped by religious idioms, symbols, and ideologies (Bell 1985; Cheney et al. 2018; Giles Banks 1996; Twigg 2011; Gressier 2021).
DE practices must also be historically situated, shaped as they are by the enduring influence of gender discrimination and colonialism. In eighteenth-century England, anorexia communicated a woman’s purity and delicateness (Bart and Scully 1979; Showalter 1985). The Victorian pathologizing of women’s DE co-produced modern dietary medicine (Brumberg 1988; Williams 2020): appetite, a feminine part of the body, needed to be combated by “masculine” self-control (Bartky 1990).
British dietary medicine, widely used in North America, also emerged partly from Britain’s colonial project in India. Early dietary medicine was invested in preserving white European bodies amidst growing wealth from colonization, overtly reinforcing ethnoracial discrimination (Dacome 2005; Albala 2005). It, for example, viewed Indian vegetarianism as a cultural weakness resulting in bodily and psychic impoverishment (Ghassem-Fachandi 2012; Philip 2004; Roy 2002, 2010; Zook 2000). Dieticians claimed that “hot” foods of the colonies would raise English “passions” to the detriment of their physical wellbeing (Arnold 1993; Stoler 2002). As appetite control became a British cultural expectation, dietary medicine idealized thin, white, female bodies as cultural containers of national virtue while casting black and brown bodies as dangerously insatiable (Hamel 2018; Ames 1691).
Indian indigenous medical, philosophical, and religious perspectives on food continued to flourish despite colonial rule (Alter 2005; Langford 2002). Engagement between Buddhist, Hindu, and Jain philosophies developed syncretic cultural ideals around ahimsa (nonviolence), eating, and moral practice. Ayurvedic conceptions of body and health have continuously informed local articulations of disease, even as they became blended with Western biomedical ideas (Das 2015; Ecks 2013; Solomon 2016). The dominant health regime remains medical pluralism, “the availability of different medical approaches, treatments, and institutions that people can use while pursuing health”, though socioeconomic factors like healthcare privatization, caste and religious discrimination, and financial inequality create treatment access disparities (Khalikova 2021; Banerjee 2009).
In the US, as anorexia and bulimia nervosa diagnoses gained public attention in the 1990s, researchers argued that media images promoted damaging body ideals, principally aimed at women (Wolf 1990). The belief that anorexia is an “upper class white woman’s illness” has led to medical and cultural ignorance about eating disorders among male-identified, non-binary, queer and trans individuals (Duggan and McCreary 2004; Feldman and Meyer 2007, 2009). Due to omission of men from studies and female-based diagnostic criteria, statistics on DE prevalence in men in most pre-2017 studies are gross underestimates (Wu et al. 2020). Yet in a sexist double-bind, because DE has been treated as a women’s health problem, treatment and research is chronically underfunded. DE in India is also under-researched, resulting in a lack of “culturally sensitive instruments for diagnosis” (Vaidyanathan et al. 2019). Our research focuses on these understudied populations while also attending to the unique harms this historical treatment of DE has had on women-identifying people.
Ultimately, DE practices are imbricated in historically shaped sociocultural systems of belief and practice. We must understand how youth evaluate and apply eating and health information within secular, syncretic, and cosmopolitan frameworks that incorporate alternative medicine, nutritional logics of “macros”, trending topics, popular beauty ideals, and religious beliefs (Bansal 2012; Kumar 2019; Lukose 2009; Nakassis 2016; Poonam 2018). Based upon this context, we analyze how medical pluralism continues to shape perceptions about what is “healthy” and “harmful” in India. We also demonstrate the lasting effects of these discriminatory dietary science histories in India and the US, showing how youth’s awareness of these historical inequities affects their trust in and evaluation of “authoritative” online health and eating information.

1.4. Health-Related Online Identities

Anthropological analyses deconstruct the causal explanation that young women uncritically consume (Western) media images of thin bodies, leading to DE behaviors (Bray 1996). They also demonstrate that DE practices are not simply Western imports stemming from fat-phobia and body image issues (Littlewood 1995; Ritenbaugh et al. 1996). These critiques urge focus on the relational motivations shaping DE practices. One such motivation is the social belonging that illness categories mediate (Eli and Warin 2018; Lavis 2015; Warin 2010).
Emerging health-related online identities and the sense of community belonging they make possible affect youths’ information sensibility and DE practices. Researchers have observed the multiplication of cultural identities around food, exercise, and wellness (Charmaraman et al. 2021; Jenzen 2017). From sub-cultures of carnivory to warring factions of vegan YouTubers, a growing number of eating-related identities are available to youth via online engagement (Bissell 2018; Hamblin 2018).
While research has shown how online communities become misinformation vectors (Hassoun et al. 2024), such communities might also provide support for youth digital resilience. During the pandemic, youth developed new ways to care for each other through online spaces—in part by sharing health and well-being information—amidst the increased digitization of social life (Unni and Weinstein 2021). Mental health discussions in online communities, like sharing coping stories, have demonstrated psychosocial benefits for youth (Yuan et al. 2023; Niu et al. 2024). We can learn from what youth are already doing in online communities dedicated to eating and health practices and identity-formation (Smith and Keel 2017).
In short, our research interrogates the prevailing wisdom that youth would be less susceptible to DE or political misinformation if they were more capable of decoding media messaging. Youth are not simply “fact-checking” when evaluating information online. They are weighing a host of social inputs derived from their social identities and communities when deciding whether a given piece of content or information is meaningful to them.
Next, we describe our materials and methods, detail the results of our ethnographic research and intervention testing, and discuss their applications for developing youth digital resilience.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Overview of Studies 1 & 2

We conducted two ethnographic studies. The first (documented in Hassoun et al. 2023) studied 35 internet users aged 13–24 from the US. Study 1 asked about youth information-seeking practices in general, with a secondary focus on news information. We recruited participants who reported mainstream (n = 19) and alternative (n = 16) political beliefs2. This was conducted between February and June 2022.
To examine whether these general findings held true in a specific, sensitive topic area and beyond the US, Study 2 asked if and how youth in the US and India apply the practices of “information sensibility” identified in Study 1 to information about eating and health. Half of Study 2 participants were recovering from disordered eating (DE) practices. This second study (briefly introduced in Xu et al. 2024) focused specifically on youth eating and health informational practices. For Study 2, we performed in-person ethnographic research, including walkalong interviews and participant-observation, with 42 youth aged 18–26 in the US and India between June and October 2022.
Ethnographic research helps uncover how youth interactively incorporate learned ideas about the news, eating, and health into their online informational practices. Our ethnographic approach responds to calls for sociotechnical (Marwick 2018) and sociocultural (Lloyd 2018) analyses of youth digital-safety risks to help researchers, governments, platforms, and youth develop better interventions (Brown and Duguid 2017).
The rest of this section presents detailed methods for Study 2; detailed methods for Study 1 can be found in Hassoun et al. 2023.

2.2. Study 2 Participants and Recruiting

We recruited participants from the US and India (Table 1 and Table 2). In the US, we selected participants through the platform respondent.io. We chose participants from the broader New York area. 25% of participants reported a household income (HHI) of less than USD 40,000, 30% earned between USD 40,000 and 60,000, 25% had an earned between USD 60,000 and 100,000, and 20% earned more than USD 100,000. In India, we used a local recruiter and included participants in Delhi and nearby peripheral areas. Incomes ranged from USD 7000 to 48,000. In both India and the US, all but one participant attended at least some college.
Because most youth who experience DE are not formally diagnosed (particularly in India, where DE is less pathologized and diagnosed than in the US), we recruited based on self-reported experiences with DE (rather than formal medical diagnoses).3 Ethical concerns also informed the decision to recruit participants in recovery, rather than those actively in distress or experiencing DE. Participants, however, largely did not see recovery as a linear or fully complete process. They were able to speak in detail about how their online experiences actively affected their eating and health practices.
We did not examine specific eating and health information or DE habits among 13–17 year olds for ethical reasons, deciding that such a study might expose participants to undue harm. This affects our ability to generalize about young adolescents across Study 1 and Study 2 and limits our eating and health observations to young adults aged 18–26. We did not observe sharp differences within 18–26 year olds in Study 2, but observed gradient effects, which we discuss later. Since Study 1 included early adolescents aged 13–17, the age, period, and cohort effects we observed are detailed in Hassoun et al. 2023.
Researchers in the US and India had extensive prior experience studying the eating and health cultures of their research context and were fluent in the languages spoken by participants. Field and interview guides were designed to account for cultural differences4, and the way researchers spoke about and contextualized DE in each site therefore differed. In both contexts, aspects of diet are (sometimes dangerously) politicized. For example, in India, eating meat, or even rumors of eating meat, have caused vigilante violence against Muslims and Dalits. Meat-eating in the US carried different symbolic meaning: masculinist discourses about meat-eating affected US participant experiences, sometimes to harmful effect.
We treated some “baseline” screening questions differently as a result. In India, we focused on eating behaviors while protecting participants’ safety by not probing for specific details about dietary choices (like eating meat). Based on existing research on food culture in India, we focused our recruitment on behaviors that could proxy for intensity of interest in the subject, for example, time spent thinking about food and engaging in exercise.
Additionally, public discourse about the body and health is articulated differently: in an Indian context, it is far more appropriate to talk explicitly and openly about weight, weight gain, weight loss, and bodily dissatisfaction in a way that might be inappropriate in a US context. While experiences with weight and health stigma overlapped significantly, participants often used different vocabulary, which we accounted for and analyzed (e.g., US youth spoke more about “energy” while Indian youth referred to “balance”). Categories of “eating disorders” or “disordered eating” are far less prevalent in India, and there can be significant stigma around discussing or experiencing mental health issues. In contrast, US participants regularly used medical terms like “bulimia”, “anorexia”, “depression”, and “disordered eating”. Researchers were sensitive to these differences.
Participants used many overlapping platforms, but one of the most commonly used image- and video-sharing platforms among participants in the US did not exist in India.

2.3. Study 2 Research Methods

Participants’ ability to self-report their own actions, design desires, and mental models, particularly in a controlled lab setting, is limited (Bernard 2017). These self-reports are shaped by social norms, generating gaps between user narration and practice that require qualitative analysis methods to explain.
As a result, in both studies we chose ethnographic methods that probe cultural values that shape human behavior, unearthing “that which is left unsaid” (Rosaldo 1993). We sought to understand youth’s information-seeking practices in situ, to understand the effects of family values and influences, social and economic conditions, and technology use in the context of everyday life. Study 1 included three phases of data collection, using dyadic interviews (Phase 1), digital diary exercises (Phase 2) and 1:1 interviews with an intervention testing component (Phase 3). It also included a data analysis and intervention design workshop between Phases 2 and 3. Details can be found in Hassoun et al. 2023.
Study 2 also employed a three-phase, mixed-methods approach with an intermediate data analysis and intervention design workshop:
Phase 1: Diary Exercise. Participants created visual representations of their conceptions of “healthy” and “unhealthy” with collages of images drawn from their social media environments.
Phase 2: In-Person Ethnographic Research. Researchers performed participant-observation and semi-structured interviews, conducting home and community visits and accompanying participants on a self-selected eating or health-related activity (e.g., a grocery shopping trip, jogging in the park).
Data Analysis and Intervention Design Workshop. Following ethnographic data collection, researchers performed preliminary data analysis to identify emergent participant needs. To contextualize these findings within existing platform moderation practices and design realistic potential youth digital resilience interventions, we conducted a design workshop with product managers and policy specialists specializing in health and youth digital-safety at a large technology company. We co-designed interventions to test with participants in Phase 3. Details about the design process can be found in the Supplementary Materials.
Phase 3: Online Follow-Up Interview and User Testing. Online follow-up interviews with participants were conducted to test and discuss potential interventions derived from the workshop, gather further qualitative data, and engage in member checking using collaborative reflection techniques (Urry et al. 2024).

2.4. Analysis Methods

Our analytic approach in Study 2 was reflexive, iterative, and inductive, following a grounded theory approach to qualitatively code data (Charmaz 2006; Mills et al. 2006; Saldaña 2021). Rather than testing participants for deficits or evaluating them against preset frameworks, we adopted an emic lens (Scarduzio 2017) to learn about what participants were doing with information online and for what purpose—from their own perspective and in their own words.
Two researchers coded each interview or field note during the data collection process. We performed consensus coding throughout, comparing codes assigned by researchers after each iteration and resolving divergence through discussion (Richards and Hemphill 2018). We used an open, descriptive coding method during our first coding pass. We then used a constant comparative method during subsequent focused analytical coding passes (Corbin and Strauss 2015), tabulating each discrete code (e.g., admitting mistakes is a sign of trustworthiness) against each participant whose interviews or diary submissions generated that code and comparing the underlying data.
Once data collection and preliminary coding were complete, we held several collaborative analysis sessions. We performed multiple theoretical coding passes, writing short memos to articulate overarching themes. We then iterated those interpretations through discussion. We referred regularly to the codebook to ensure that our analysis reflected the complete dataset and drew conclusions about how analyses applied to different populations within our dataset. We used the codebook to test data saturation and reflexively mitigate cognitive biases during data collection and analysis.

2.5. Limitations

Readers should consider several limitations of our study’s methodology when interpreting the findings. Qualitative research, while providing rich, deep insights into social phenomena, has inherent limitations. First, our small study sample, drawn from two geographically defined areas, cannot claim global representativeness. We studied a small number of experiences in-depth. We recruited to ensure that our sample did not skew toward a specific demographic and attempted to collect a diverse sample. To mitigate the over-representation of white, affluent, and female participants in prior studies, we prioritized diversity across gender and sexual identity (male, nonbinary, queer), race (non-white), geographic density (urban, peri-urban), income levels, political leaning, and eating and health behaviors. We propose that further explicit diversification across other characteristics and increased sample size could have expanded the study’s relevance.
Second, the study relied on self-reported data from participants through diary exercises and interviews. These data are subject to inherent limitations such as recall, observer, and social desirability biases. The use of both diary exercises and participant-observation was intended to mitigate these biases, enabling cross-referencing and in-depth probing into gaps between what participants said and what they did. Third, because we only studied those recovering from DE, our analysis cannot address the experiences of those actively experiencing DE. This gap could affect the generalizability of our analysis to all stages of the DE process. A study involving participants with active eating disorders would have changed our methodology and de-escalation procedures, and likely the research findings. It is unclear, based on existing research, whether participants actively experiencing DE would have given more or less specific insight into online harms or misinformation susceptibility (for example, due to the denial many in active DE experience), but our findings almost certainly would have differed in some way. It is possible that digital diaries would have been more illustrative of youth DE online ecosystems, while interviews would have been more fraught and potentially less reflective.
Finally, intervention design was conducted in partnership with a technology company and was therefore limited to testing interventions on the company’s platforms. We showed interventions on two types of surfaces where youth frequently seek and encounter information: a search engine and a video platform. This excluded other popular platforms such as image-sharing platforms, which many participants reported using.

2.6. Research Ethics

Our study went through a systematic internal review and approvals process to ensure that participants and parents of minor participants provided informed consent and were protected from undue risk. We followed human research ethics protocols to ensure participants fully understood the study and their ability to withdraw from it. When interviewing minors, we ensured there were always two adults present. A parent/legal guardian completed consent forms on behalf of their child and provided additional verbal consent. We also verbally explained the study process to both the minor and the parent, confirming they each understood the study. Each participant received USD 250–500 as a thank you for their 5–6 h of participation. This reflects best practices from our local research partners.
To design our research, we conducted a broad, interdisciplinary literature review, interviewed domain experts, and sought external review, following guidance on research with at-risk users (Bellini et al. 2024). We worked with a licensed psychologist specializing in DE to review prospective participants’ screener responses, to confirm that we excluded participants whose responses indicated risk for re-traumatization, and co-designed a de-escalation procedure. Participants were assigned pseudonyms during data collection (different from those used in this paper) and personally identifying information (PII) was removed from interview and diary data.
The results presented in the next section pertain to Study 2. To highlight differences between both studies, we first discuss the findings from Study 1.

3. Results

In this section we detail the findings from Study 2, which focused on youth eating and health information, explaining the challenges participants encountered and the information sensibility practices they used to manage these challenges. We indicate where and how they relate to Study 1 findings (Table 3), which analyzed youth information-seeking (and encountering) challenges and practices more broadly. In the last subsection, we discuss how content generally could not be classified into “healthy” and “harmful” categories—these were context- and participant-specific.

3.1. Challenges

While Study 1 participants felt a sense of information overload, Study 2 participants felt a sense of information overgeneralization or misrecognition (Table 4). While Study 1 participants also felt a general sense of misrecognition, the misrecognition that Study 2 participants felt was specific: that institutions, specifically medical institutions, did not recognize and serve their specific bodies. They sought information and trusted information they encountered based on how well they felt it recognized them.
Study 2 participants also used their engagement with algorithms to make sure that the information they encountered online was increasingly tailored to them, attempting to solve both information overload and misrecognition challenges. In alignment with Study 1—which found that participants tended to encounter, rather than search for, information—Study 2 participants wanted to actively search for tailored eating and health information less, by training their algorithms to deliver tailored content to them.
The fear of social error also took on a more specific form in Study 2—we term the specific version of this challenge individualistic conformity. Participants felt afraid of being judged for trying too hard to conform—to look “skinny” or like they were trying to cultivate an online persona—but knew they needed to do so to fit in and experience positive social feedback about their bodies and practices. They described it as feeling like a tightrope walk, where social media needed to be carefully cultivated to avoid social sanction in either direction.
Study 2 participants also felt an overarching need for meaningful connection, mapping on to the general motivation driving participant information sensibility practices in Study 1: the desire for social belonging. In the sections that follow, we describe the information sensibility practices that Study 2 participants performed in response to these challenges, connecting them to Study 1 information sensibility practices where relevant.

3.2. n = 1 Thinking

Study 1 found that youth value personal testimony over institutional authority, using personal relatability and authenticity as key trust heuristics. We termed this heuristic “surrogate thinking” (finding a trusted go-to source and relying on that source’s opinions) and “everyday experience” (privileging on-the-ground crowdsourced reporting, lived experience, and first-person narratives). Of the trust heuristics discovered in Study 1, Study 2 found youth used surrogate thinking and everyday experience most when evaluating eating and health information. Participants assessed social media content creator credibility based on how similarly and authentically creators felt to participants’ personal experience. Participants were largely not trying to become experts on nutrition or fitness, but experts on their own bodies. The wider generalizability of health information was not important: their goal was to find health advice that worked for at least one other person and could therefore potentially work for them.
We term this “n = 1 thinking”5: when youth assess health information based on personal testimony of like-minded, like-bodied people online rather than probabilistic measures. In part due to youth’s dwindling trust in health authorities and their comfort with online spaces, they sourced credible eating and health information via the “real” experiences of individuals online.
Participants gave concrete explanations for this evaluative process. They cited that both experts and non-experts have begun to acknowledge that universalized Western medicine lacks all the answers, particularly for nutrition and wellness. Further, many participants thought about eating and health through the lens of identity and were wary of how science had historically harmed and pathologized marginalized people like them. Many explained that health information is not universal—it is based on white, male bodies—and that therefore the advice of someone similar to them would be more likely to work than medical advice (e.g., that a young Black woman’s advice is more likely to work for me, as a young Black woman than NIH guidelines). With n = 1 thinking, participants who felt marginalized by generalized medical advice or mainstream images of fitness could access potential answers to their health goals. They spoke about “trusting their gut”, expressing that eating and health-related information had a unique relation to the individual body and that the consequences of “getting it wrong” felt higher than with other kinds of information.
When watching a video about someone else’s experience with a new diet, Lacey (23, US) said she did not worry if that person’s explanation did not sound plausible or stand up to scrutiny: “I might not believe the science behind your explanation but I believe your experience is legit. I accept that some things just don’t have an explanation”. Shivani (24, India) also expressed a need for authenticity, which content creators she watched signaled through relatability, that they are “just like you”. For Jewel (26, US), “Knowing to trust individual people is the important thing”: she finds beauty in the “moment of recognition that I know what the person is going through”. Amira (23, India) was drawn to Tahajjud prayer testimonials, specifically first-person accounts from individuals sharing anecdotes about what they prayed for and how the prayers manifested over the months of their prayer practice.
If a diet or workout did not work, it meant that it was not right for participants’ bodies, but not necessarily that the diet/workout itself was flawed. The faith in one person’s experience (n = 1) still held. After being diagnosed with a medical condition Amulya (23, India) “researched” on image- and video-sharing platforms to become an expert on what her diagnosis meant for her. She watched videos from a creator, hoping for weight loss tips she could follow to manage the condition. When the method did not work, she said: “I didn’t lose 10 kgs in 10 days … so it just didn’t work for me. But it could work for somebody else”. She needed to evaluate content “according to my body type and allergies”. Sascha (24, US) said: “everyone has different genetics, different bodies, and different lifestyles” and that if a content creator is “a teeny tiny influencer or a buffed-up gym bro, I’ll just keep scrolling”.
Brad (22, US) said that both he and his girlfriend would be deemed overweight in “doctorese” and pointed out that the BMI6 is not a reliable measure of health. For Brad, skepticism of the medical establishment went hand-in-hand with prioritizing personal experience: “I haven’t experienced health problems because of what I eat”, which was, for him, a far better indication of how healthy his relationship to food and eating was than his BMI.
Participants verified sources by checking if a creator was like-minded and like-bodied—if a creator met this criteria, their singular experience could be proof enough (n = 1). Study 2 participants asked the following questions when encountering health information online:
  • Does the creator think like me?
    • Do we have similar backgrounds?
    • Do we share the same politics?
    • Do we have similar gender, racial, and class identities?
  • Is their body like mine?
    • Do we have similar body types?
    • Are we at the same place in our health journeys (or could I arrive at theirs)?
    • Do we have similar symptoms?
    • Have we responded in similar ways to the same diet or fitness trends in the past?
Participants found sources of information online from like-minded, like-bodied communities that they lacked offline—especially people of color (POC), transgender (trans), and disabled participants. But online, participants could choose to be part of communities where they felt included. Korean-American Jinny (22, US) said that in the suburbs, the popular girls were always white and skinny. Online, Jinny followed creators who had bigger bodies and were healthy and happy at their size. She said: “No one trusts the medical industry. I did [fitness app] and I was not going out, no sugar, no pasta, and it was depressing”. Shivani (24, India) felt ostracized because of her weight: “fat is unacceptable in Indian society”. Shivani found a depression meetup group to talk to others like her. Jean (23, US) grew up in a conservative area where they did not know anyone else who was trans. Jean used image- and video-sharing platforms to learn about top surgery and body-neutral workouts. Taera (19, US), a woman of color, lived in a white community in an outer borough, and her parents body-shamed her. Online, Taera found like-minded fans of black cottagecore.7 Zara (26, US) felt exhausted navigating subways with a cane and did not know any differently abled people like her in real life, but found supportive communities, like differently abled hiking groups, online. Even if it was just one other person who experienced “success” in losing weight, our participants were willing to emulate them, in hopes that the advice would yield similar results for them.
For participants who felt under-represented in their offline communities, social media was a space where they could feel connected to and represented by people like them. Ariana (24, US) had saturated her feed with “healing Black girl content”. She shared that encountering Black representations of health via the algorithm, which she lacked in her mostly White community, felt healing and healthy. As Jean (23, US) said, “I have to wade through a lot of ’macho masc content’ to get to the type of trans bodies I relate to.” They used search terms like “queer workouts” and “trans strength training” to find the right fitness creatorss in preparation for their top surgery. They checked for similar mind and body to determine credibility. Lower-income participants in both India and the US found that video content teaching people how to be healthy without money was validating and useful.
Indian participants used video-sharing platforms for localized voices on health and eating—where the local became a useful proxy for like-mindedness and like-bodiedness. Participants noted positive differences between “global” esthetics, notions of status, performative photos, and aspirational symbols of class and “localized stories” representing local and niche identities, place-based vlogs, and everyday esthetics and local codes of “truthiness”. Some Indian participants felt that religious approaches to working on oneself—like those in the language of homeopathy, Ayurveda, and Unani—felt more trustworthy and relatable. When participants accessed localized results that felt credible and immediately helpful, they felt good. But when participants, like Arjun (23, India), could not find an answer, they dove back into social media and n = 1 thinking to find creator surrogate thinkers to tell them what to do (see Figure 1).
Personal relatability played a large role in trust. Shivani (24, India) said that “Finding relatable content always helps. Seeing people on [video-sharing platform] going through the same things made me feel less alone and more normal.” Sanjay (21, India) said that “I listen to [creator’s] interviews on [video-sharing platform] because somehow his experiences and my experiences are the same… I sort of thought that I have a link with his experiences, so he’s kind of my mentor.” Sangita (20, India) sought out exercise tips from “authentic” people: “If someone is suggesting something to you and it has worked for them, it gives a little bit more authenticity to it… I don’t want experts and their advanced workouts, you don’t know which one is authentic or if it will work for you.” For Sascha (24, US), if an creator says “this is what happened to me, this is how I changed … it gives you more background and I can see if and how they bettered themselves.” Krish (21, India) shared about the like-minded and like-bodied creators he followed: “if they can do it, I can do it too”.
This valorization of personal testimony exemplifies a larger shift participants expressed about social media. Instead of a social space to catch up with friends they knew offline, they used it as a personalized source of information fed by an algorithm that knew them intimately. They were generally comfortable sharing their own photo-sharing posts with researchers, but were hesitant about sharing their algorithm-suggested content feeds, because those feeds felt deeply personal. Many expressed caveats like “OK this is a weirdly specific interest but…”.
N = 1 thinking helped historically marginalized participants, but could also lead to misinformation susceptibility and testing out harmful remedies with the young person as the test subject. When using n = 1 thinking, nothing can be proven false or unlikely. Conversely, many participants did take a semi-scientific approach to n = 1 thinking, stringing together data points from a series of individuals who passed their similarity checks on social media to determine whether medical advice might work for them. The main risk with n = 1 thinking is that participants were not doing any follow-up research to what they heard online; rather, they immediately implemented the advice in their own lives. There was no friction to encourage them to vet the health information before copying someone online. Another risk is that n = 1 thinking encouraged “magic bullet” thinking (e.g., believing that they could get ’shredded’ in 2 weeks if they just followed the same diet and exercise program), potentially leading to harmful diet and exercise practices.

3.3. Distributed Trust

Participants relied on what we term “distributed trust” to crowdsource credibility, an information sensibility practice identified in Study 1, observable in the eating and health specific context of Study 2. Participants used this distributed trust heuristic by monitoring likes, comments, and reviews, particularly in interest-based communities, which felt pre-vetted and authentic. Shifting from institutional trust to distributed trust, participants called upon communities’ decentralized authority to determine validity, because approval of the crowd signified credibility. Participants’ already beloved online communities provided anonymous reassurance and an intimate sense of trust at scale.
Participants specifically used this distributed trust heuristic from communities online where they felt a sense of social belonging. Jinny (22, US) thought she had missed her chance to meet BTS8 member, Jungkook, when a karaoke bar claimed he made a surprise visit to a BTS fan event. To confirm this claim, she crowdsourced her online BTS community for consensus: “I looked at the comments and asked my other friends, and everyone thinks that venue was known for lying about this kind of stuff.” Jinny trusted the online BTS communities because “it’s only ARMYs [committed BTS fans] who reply”. She knew she had shared values and could read shared social codes in the space, increasing its trustworthiness. She repeatedly experienced and participated in the community’s social practice of working to decipher information—giving her a sense of shared responsibility for making sense of what goes on, rather than an individual sense of responsibility. Making false claims in the space was also risky, because it meant you could be removed or socially ostracized, increasing her trust in the crowd.
Jheel (23, India) saw the same messaging (about the type of diet and exercise dancers follow) repeated by many bhangra9 creators she followed: “I don’t feel like they’re saying anything wrong ‘cause I’m hearing it from every YouTuber”.
Participants also used distributed trust as a second check for verifying the credibility of a source—in tandem with n = 1 thinking—asking:
Do they think like me? [n = 1 thinking]
Is their body like mine? [n = 1 thinking]
Does this community of people like me trust it? [distributed trust]
For example, a popular creator’s diet videos (Figure 2) had more than 2.4 million views when he claimed drinking coffee and lemon accelerated weight loss. The creator showed his trim body in his normal-looking kitchen as evidence of the diet trend’s success (n = 1 thinking). He gained the faith of followers by appearing as a more familiar, local, and “relatable” source.
Participants showed how comment sections were used to debate the legitmacy of diet trends and were therefore useful for deciding whether to trust a creator (Figure 2). Commenters shared their doubts, personal successes, and even admiration of the creator’s body, creating an interactive forum to determine trustworthiness (Figure 3).
Amira (23, India) used comments to vet posts about healing prayer and therapeutic action, checking to see if commenters are Muslim like her and believe in the practices being recommended by the poster. She once reached out to a woman commenter on social media to see if she was real and would respond—which she did—confirming Amira’s belief in the content. Accessing shared experiences of mood and emotion at simultaneous points in the video with other commenters made watching this content deeply therapeutic for Amira.
Brad (22, US) also found a relatable surrogate thinker through his existing communities’ advice, whose popularity in those pre-vetted communities made the creator feel trustworthy (Figure 4). Suggestion algorithms then led him deeper into similarly fringe, and potentially dangerous, eating and health-related communities—which associated eating raw meat with a particular form of masculinity, related to sexist political ideologies (Muller et al. 2024). This led him to both follow their eating and health advice and start building a health-related political identity around these communiess’ principles.
Participants were skeptical of institutional trust and trust in the information literacy trust heuristics they learned in school. As Jewel (26, US) explained:
I was told not to use Wikipedia as a source. Then I was told to look up who is publishing the piece, and to ask myself about the author’s intentions and to critique the reading. But this was flawed because in my school, [US news outlet] was cited as a valid source but the [US news outlet] is neoliberal propaganda. Also, the way history was told was accurate in terms of events but not in a politically critical way … so now I am reframing my relationship with sources. I’m asking myself, ‘Who has authority?’ For example, “SuperSize Me”—it’s about the food we consume and how we should think about obesity as a moral issue. Obesity is a problem, but not how we are talking about it, as if underweight is the opposite of obesity. But this is a risk factor more than obesity. If you look at the J curve of health. You are at a higher risk post-surgery if you are too skinny vs. obese.
Jewel did not generally seek out health information, but nevertheless consumed a lot of it—she said that “I rely on the algorithm to direct it to me.” Her favorite content creator “can talk about how systemic oppression like ableism, fatphobia, transphobia, etc., impact the ways we exercise and the ways we diet”. Her negative experiences with doctors during her transition made her critical and skeptical of medical advice from doctors. For example, doctors told her that she would not experience wetness after bottom surgery10—but in her experience this was false, and she found many people online who also personally experienced wetness following surgery.
Using distributed trust, youth exercise less faith in single institutional authorities, and instead, rely on crowdsourced community knowledge.

3.4. The “Good Life”

Participants employed coded vocabulary to discuss and share DE practices, focused on the “good life” rather than “thinness” or other explicitly body-specific descriptors. Participants shared that it was taboo to have anorexia or to diet, but common to broadcast “lifestyle” achievements on social media to signal that they were living “the good life” to others. In both India and the US, this “goodness” was signaled through body-based eating and health practices, but not stated explicitly as such. Rather than signaling a cultural shift away from body-based moral judgments, the “good life” appeared to be a new way of framing the longstanding judgments and extreme expectations youth across generations have faced about their bodies and appearances.
Participants did not reference “anorexia” or “bulimia” when recounting their restriction/binge cycles. They talked about “energy”, “mental health”, “feeling good”—but their social media feeds reinforced the idea that certain bodies fit the esthetics of “the good life”. They felt pressured to be constantly performing this good life online, which many found harmful. Participants felt like performing that good life also meant that you were a “good person.” As Franny (24, US), articulated:
I thought to be a worthy person you have to have this body. Generally, I was doing a lot of restricting. I was a swimmer, and I would swim an hour and a half in the morning and afternoon. My mom wouldn’t allow me to eat afternoon snacks. This led to a misconception of how much food I needed or how to listen to your body.
Participants found that language about thinness being taboo did not actually remove the need to look thin, but instead added another layer of signification work to social media posts: you had to look “healthy”, but not look like you cared about looking thin. Language of holistic, “balanced” mental health and well-being replaced language about thinness.11 Ariana (24, US) said she was “just trying to be healthy all across the board”. Jivika (22, India) expressed: “You shouldn’t be too fat or too thin, I just want to maintain.” Johannes (25, US) said that “Being healthy means being conscious. If one is overweight, you have to take a look at how you feel mentally”.
For participants, having the “good life” required you to broadcast it. It was not enough to have it all—you had to also show that you had not given anything up to get there. In the US, taking care of one’s health was a value, and a fit body signified that one had the “right” type of life to maintain it. Chase (24, US) quit his strenuous job because he could not perform the right kind of life: “It didn’t leave me time to get to the gym or go hiking.” Now he does calisthenics and yoga “obsessively” and dreams about being a fitness creator. Chase described how he obsessed over looking fit with ease on social media, and also had to perform that he was not working too hard to achieve the perfect body and work-life balance.
Fit bodies were a marker of “balance” for Indian participants. In India, participants openly talked about achieving “balance” in diet and exercise. But looking like you were achieving balance took extreme work. Krish (21, India) said he worked on his body—“but not too much”—and his startup in tandem. Dhruv (24, India) believed health was about being motivated to slowly, slowly (“dhire dhire se body acha ban jayega”) and continuously work toward weight loss and fitness goals and slowly cut out “bad stuff” (“dhire dhire se khatam ho jayega”). He said that if you have the process and routine, you will see the results without being stressed (“tension mat lena”). Women participants expressed that they should not be too fat or too thin but, as Isha (23, India) put it, “curvy in a good way”.
As a result, US and Indian participants expressed that appearance indicated how one is living life. The body was grounds for judgments that affect what one was granted and denied. For participants, pursuing this “good life” image was not irrational—performing it successfully with the “right body” came with access to real social benefits. Brenda (24, US), for example, was moved to the front of their dance class when they lost weight. Mohammed (19, India) commented that “everyone is attracted to the steroid type of body”, that women he was interested in only seemed interested in men with that body type. After losing weight, Amira (23, India) noted that “all clothing suits me—girls should be a proper weight and look good.” Sangita (20, India) detailed how she was more socially accepted and received less criticismwhen she lost weight: she said you have to “keep yourself in a way that they can’t comment on”. Sangita, her mother, and her boyfriend all believed that Sangita would be happier if she could conform enough to social standards to keep people’s comments at bay.
Conversely Jinny (22, US) described how she “didn’t get into a club because I’m not a skinny white girl”. Taera (19, US) recounted comments on their posts: “All of that sabotage [fat shaming comments] would be there and I would swallow myself with hate.” Shivani (24, India) stopped going out altogether, because “People judge me for being fat. I’ve seen it in their eyes, when they ‘x-ray’ you—from people who don’t even know me”.
Chase (24, US) articulated that content removal would not change this: “It’s not the content that’s toxic. It’s the comments on people’s bodies”.
As Lester (2021) argues: “The motivation behind an eating disorder is ‘how do I exist as a good person’ for people who may feel like they aren’t valued as a full person.” Youth recovering from disordered eating were not alone in pursuing “goodness” in this way. Participants were all trying to figure out how to be and appear like a “good person” through eating and health. They were acutely aware of how their self-presentation on digital platforms invited outsiders to judge their lives and mediated access to benefits.

3.5. Getting Offline

Finally, participants found supportive communities online, but ultimately believed that the “good life” was lived offline. As discussed, for participants who felt underrepresented in their offline communities, online spaces felt more accessible and healthy because they saw their bodies represented—but those participants still did not want to live life online.
All participants expressed a gut instinct that time they spent on social media was not good for them—that they should be “out there” in the “real world”. Taera (19, US) expressed:
Staying on [video-sharing platform] is a hindrance to my life because of lack of sleep and the bad feeling of not listening to my intuition… I got addicted to it so easily. It’s so much fun learning hacks to your life, but that time could be spent different. I can feel my intuition that I need to get off it and I can’t. I will be on it until 4 or 5 in the morning.
BTS online fan communities fostered in real life (IRL) connections for Taera. She met “women of all races, sexual identities, body types, and walks of life. I could be different, and we are all worthy and special”.
Jean (23, US) said “[video-sharing platform] makes my brain feel like death. I have to delete it when I get too scrolly.” Nikhil (21, India) said “During the lockdown, I felt no energy and spent so much time on my phone late at night. I had a ‘self-realization’ that I needed to get out there and change my life”.
Broadcasting the “good life” on social media ultimately did not feel like the real good life. Participants sought offramps from online to “real life.” They valued and craved in-person interactions, especially after the pandemic. Priya (24, India) said (Figure 5):
I had a mild clinical depression but I can’t tell [my parents]. They would blame me. But I searched on [search engine] for meditation and found [program]. It’s a happiness program where you go in person and do meditation. I go with my dad.
Shivani (24, India) craved “real interaction” (Figure 6), expressing that:
Videos are a one-way community: you can just hear the other person and not talk to them. I found the [anonymized] app where you have a group call and have someone to talk to. I realized that people out there have similar problems, or problems bigger than mine.
Sanjay (21, India) used [search engine] to find local meditation groups and videos posted by those groups. When participants were able to translate healthy aspects of their online lives into meaningful connections offline, they felt better (see Figure 6).
These offline connections are ultimately what made participants feel a holistic sense of community and acceptance.

3.6. Beyond Healthy/Harmful Content Labels

We started Study 2 by asking how youth distinguished helpful from harmful content, asking them to make collages of both (Figure 7 and Figure 8).
But it became clear that delineating between universally helpful and harmful content was not their key goal, nor was it analytically useful. Deeper needs for personalization and belonging drove their eating and health behavior, and helpful/harmful distinctions varied based on personal experiences. The same content could be helpful or harmful depending on the context, and where the participant was in their journey.
As Franny (24, US) put it: “I was never not bingeing and restricting… But during the pandemic, I realized it wasn’t just periods of eating really healthy and falling off the wagon… I realized there were two cycles that continually happened. It was one big pattern of bad”.
Content unremarkable or helpful to one person could be triggering for someone else. Watching shopping “hauls” on video-sharing platforms during the pandemic reinforced the restriction/binge eating cycles that Franny had learned from her mother and grandmother. But video-sharing platforms later helped her find peer resources for cooking healthy on a budget, which did not trigger her.
Jean (23, US) explained that for them, recovery content, which many participants found incredibly helpful, was a pathway into DE practices: “This recovery stuff is more triggering. When I was 12, it helped me learn how people were engaging in [anorexic] behaviors”. Jean believes they know the difference on social media between recovery accounts that are going to be helpful and harmful after working through their contextually specific triggers in therapy.
Anil (22, India) was inspired by professional athletes’ bodies, content which other participants found “healthy”, but this led him down a path to an early heart attack (see Figure 9).
Participants wanted to train their algorithms to ensure that they only encountered “healthy” content in their social media and information ecosystems. They specifically wanted guidance on the right vocabulary to individualize their online searches and algorithms. Amira (23, India) struggled to cope with her “anger issues”—she found solace in prayer testimonials on video-sharing platforms, but lacked the vocabulary to connect her spirituality to mental health sources. Sammy (25, US) had an “eating disorder thing” that he could not name—but he knew what it was not. When he went online to find out more about his symptoms, he found that medical diagnostic content could be triggering: “When I searched it up those two ones, bulimia and anorexia, were in my results, and it was very intense. I can’t really find other ones”. Dhruv’s (24, India) social media was filled with content about “paper skin”, a term used to praise muscular, veiny bodies. He struggled to find more about where this term came from, why it was popular now, and understand whether it was achievable for him. Krish (21, India) wanted to try out a new fat-burner pill, but searched for videos to crowdsource reviews and found they were negative. It was helpful for him to know that he was “not the first person to think this way”.

4. Intervention Testing

Based on Study 1 findings and Study 2 preliminary analysis, we created interventions to test in follow-up online interviews with Study 2 participants (N = 42), using design mockups (Figure 10, Figure 11, Figure 12, Figure 13 and Figure 14). We coded 98 unmet needs articulated by participants and categorized them into five clusters, which served as the basis for intervention concepts.12 These five clusters were: discernment, diversification, social learning, self-protection, and personalization. Each intervention corresponds to one cluster (Table 5).
We detail aggregated participant reactions to the interventions, describing the two negatively received interventions (View As and Explorer Remix), the two mixed-reviewed interventions (Personal Safe Search and Search Together), and the one positively received intervention (Location-Based Search). We then summarize participant reactions to all interventions.

4.1. View As

The discernment cluster summarizes needs participants articulated around understanding whether creators and the information they shared were trustworthy. Needs included confirming whether creators were real or a character, understanding their “rabbit-holing” pathways, knowing whether content has been edited or not, and understanding whether content is “quality” or just popular.
Since Study 1 findings also indicated that participants wanted surrogate thinkers to help navigate information overload, we theorized that an intervention allowing participants to see their trusted creators’ online ecosystem might help participants with their discernment needs while supporting their preferred trust heuristics. The View As intervention (Figure 10) would allow users to see what their preferred creators saw on their social media feeds (no actual creator data were used in the design mockup). Aman (21, India) liked the idea: “it’s useful, helpful because I want to know what he [creator] is watching. I want to know more about them—what they’re into beyond what they show to the world—it’s like a background check”. But Brenda (24, US) thought it was “too invasive”. Most participants were not interested in knowing about the creator’s online life to this extent, but they did want to know whether the creator made money off of the content the creator posted.
Figure 10. View As intervention mockup. The user is able to see what creator Isabela Smith’s feed looks like (left) compared to the user’s own feed (right).
Figure 10. View As intervention mockup. The user is able to see what creator Isabela Smith’s feed looks like (left) compared to the user’s own feed (right).
Socsci 14 00230 g010
The failure of this intervention could be explained, in part, by the information sensibility practices discovered in Study 1—that is, that participants only wanted good enough information to make a decision about a creator, and “View As” led to information overload (more information than they wanted or felt they needed). Once a creator had become a surrogate thinker for them, they did not feel the need to learn more about the creator beyond what they followed the creator for in the first place (e.g., for specific types of news or health information).

4.2. Explorer Remix

The diversification cluster aggregates articulated participant desires for new informative content. Needs included hearing perspectives on health and eating beyond those of their family members, how to “reset” one’s algorithm on social media, immediately seeing other points of view from the video or content they were consuming (for some participants, beyond accepted medical science), content helping participants think critically about the eating ideas that they grew up with, and seeing creators with different body types.
In response, Explorer Remix (Figure 11) would allow participants to browse separately from their usual recommendations, seeing “outside” their algorithms. Participants were largely uninterested in seeing content outside of their usual interests. Jivika (22, India) said:
I don’t see why you’d want something outside of your feed. My brother is really crazy about cars, and likes watching videos about cars, but I’m not open to that knowledge. I’m not that much into cars.
Amira (23, India) echoed a similar sentiment:
There is no need to explore different types of content–there is no need to see everything. If you’re getting bored, you can use this to find something new, but I typically switch to a [Netflix] series.
Participants in both Study 1 and Study 2 valued trusted, interest- or identity-based groups (distributed trust) and creators (surrogate thinking), looking for markers of social (crowdsourcing credibility) or bodily (n = 1 thinking) similarity to evaluate information. Outside of their trusted, algorithmically moderated information ecosystems, these information sensibility practices are harder to implement. It therefore makes sense that stepping outside of these spaces via interventions like Explorer Remix would hold little interest or value to them, especially given how much care and effort they put into training their algorithms to enable them to encounter information specifically tailored to them.
Figure 11. Explorer Remix intervention mockup. The user can toggle on the “Explorer Remix” mode in their settings, seeing content different from that suggested on their main profile feed.
Figure 11. Explorer Remix intervention mockup. The user can toggle on the “Explorer Remix” mode in their settings, seeing content different from that suggested on their main profile feed.
Socsci 14 00230 g011

4.3. Search Together

The social learning cluster summarizes a set of needs around discussing health information with like-minded and like-bodied people, community-based ways of searching for and vetting online information, and developing deep and meaningful online and offline relations. The Search Together intervention sought to meet those needs.
Search Together would show results tailored to participants’ online communities, letting them see search terms and results that people in their communities searched for (Figure 12). Participants liked seeing these, but ultimately wanted more interpersonal interactivity. As Zara (26, US) put it:
“It would be cool if these search groups were also connected to something like a forum board where people could also post. This makes me feel like these are folks I don’t just want to search with, but what are they thinking in the realm of this topic.”
Figure 12. Search Together intervention mockup. The user can choose to see what search terms (e.g., “running support”) people in their selected communities (e.g., “Learning to run”) used.
Figure 12. Search Together intervention mockup. The user can choose to see what search terms (e.g., “running support”) people in their selected communities (e.g., “Learning to run”) used.
Socsci 14 00230 g012
Malia (26, US)—like many participants—described a desire to move beyond searching for information together to actually getting together. She wanted something that “takes you offline and into real life”. Aneet (21, India) said: “I would definitely use the community results because if there are people going through the same thing, and sharing their knowledge and results, then I can use that. Those people could advise me”.
Interestingly, participants’ desire for getting offline (Study 1) took precedence over their information sensibility practices of distributed trust (Study 2) and crowdsourcing credibility (Study 1). Seeking meaningful connection offline (Study 2) could be a solution for the challenge of information overload (Study 1), potentially more attractive to youth than online solutions.

4.4. Personal Safe Search

The self-protection cluster summarizes a set of needs around avoiding unhelpful online content. Needs included avoiding recommended content centered around body image, “What I ate in a day” and “this is what worked for me” videos and images, and labeling of food as “good” or “bad”. The specific information to avoid varied from participant to participant, requiring customization to individual needs, but the desire to avoid some kind of content seemed broadly shared. The Personal Safe Search intervention attempted to address this self-protection cluster.
Personal Safe Search would allow participants to filter out unwanted terms from their results (Figure 13). US participants liked this feature, but Indian participants were generally uninterested in blocking content. Ariana (24, US) said: “I love that you can search with intention, but more than just blocking out content, I like that you can hone it down, so this helps streamline that search.” Sanjay (21, India) wanted to use it for “healthy” ingredient searching: “I like this ‘exclude custom words’ option—if I was making a healthy smoothie, I’d exclude ‘protein powder’ to find protein shakes that use natural things.” Pooja (23, India) was uninterested in blocking, instead wanting something that allowed her to search more easily for what she wanted: “I would rather have an interface where I can get options not to block, but to specify the thing I’m looking for”.
Figure 13. Personal Safe Search intervention mockup. The user can choose to exclude terms from their search results.
Figure 13. Personal Safe Search intervention mockup. The user can choose to exclude terms from their search results.
Socsci 14 00230 g013
This desire for tailored content enhanced our understanding of information overload (Study 1) as a challenge. Information sensibility practices to reduce overload did not rely on reducing content in general—they relied on reducing general content. Participants did not want less information—they wanted information tailored to them.

4.5. Location-Based Search

The personalization cluster aggregates participant needs for customization of health and eating content to their local context. Participants wanted help finding and encountering information more socioculturally tailored to them, rather than general health and eating information, as well as help applying that advice in their everyday life.
Our corresponding intervention, Location-Based Search would allow users to filter search results specific to their region. In the mockup (Figure 14), we left the definition of “your region” open-ended, and asked participants what definition of “region” would be most useful (e.g., country, state, “within 5 miles/kilometers”). Participants liked the idea of searches based on region, but wanted to be able to adjust location on a sliding scale based on what they were searching for (rather than specifying what fixed scale would be useful in general). Sanjay (21, India) said: “Whatever I’m searching, I want results in my region—the content should be available in India. Things are different in India, so there’s no point in searching abroad first. I want to search regionally first, and then globally if I can’t find anything”.
Figure 14. Location-Based Search intervention mockup. The user can choose to show search results from their region.
Figure 14. Location-Based Search intervention mockup. The user can choose to show search results from their region.
Socsci 14 00230 g014
Maya (23, India) speaks Hindi mainly, and searches for information on her medical condition, yoga, and diet content. She wanted a Hindi language filter with a caveat: “I like this idea for finding recipes or products, especially in Hindi. But it can’t return formal (shudh) Hindi; it needs to return local or colloquial Hindi that would actually be useful.” Zara (26, US) was unsure about how she would use the intervention: “it would be tricky to know when to toggle that on”.
Dhruv (24, India) felt it was important to “improve your search power”, via using specific terms and targeted queries, even on social media. He felt that in order to avoid negative content, you have to “hack” your algorithms: “there’s a pattern to search—to learn how to find healthy content, and like, and subscribe, and that will tailor your [social media].” While Dhruv recognized the need for an Indian perspective when it comes to search, he did not initially understand that this was a new concept. In fact, he said he already uses this feature via “Search nearby results” and using “.in vs. .com.” He felt that Gen Z already has workarounds for finding some version of localized results, so this felt redundant and less exciting. However, he felt like the onus was on the user to learn these workarounds—such that a toggle would make this an easier process. Dhruv emphasized that there is a BIG difference in Western and Indian perspectives, for example, on turmeric, supplements, and protein.
Place-based content felt credible because another user from the same region was more likely to be similar to the participant—participants used Location-Based Search as a proxy for finding information from like-minded/like-bodied people (n = 1 thinking, Study 2).

4.6. Intervention Testing Summary

In summary (Table 6), the Location-Based Search intervention was the most positively received intervention. It gave participants a sense of control and agency over their search results and aligned with their existing practice of trusting information from people who are “like-minded and like-bodied”, as location was seen as a useful proxy for similarity. This intervention would also likely be the easiest to implement, though may be limited by both content creators’ and users’ location and privacy settings. Careful consideration and user control over the balance between localization and privacy is crucial to the successful application of an intervention like this.
The Search Together intervention received mixed feedback because, while participants liked the idea of interacting with their online communities, they ultimately wanted help to take those interactions offline. The Personal Safe Search intervention received mixed feedback because participants generally wanted more tailored information through filtering, not to block content (especially Indian participants). Providing basic content filters or better transparency and education about search engines and algorithmic recommendation systems could help meet participants’ personalization and self-protection needs. Peer- and AI-assisted learning techniques could also support these needs, guiding users in their interactions with online information ecosystems. Given the growing intergration of AI in tools like search, implementation seems possible (though potentially fraught, due to the propensity for both peers and AI to share inaccurate information).
The View As intervention was negatively received because participants felt it was too invasive and gave them irrelevant information. The Explorer Remix intervention was negatively received because users felt that content outside of their algorithms would be uninteresting and irrelevant. The View As intervention would be difficult to implement as it is uncertain whether content creators (particularly those invested in the use of misleading content) would consent to a View As setting. Explorer Remix would be easier to implement but, based on these findings, unlikely to be used—except potentially in the form of a “private browsing” session when participants would prefer not to have a particular session included in their algorithmic recommendations.

5. Discussion

Our findings across Study 1 and Study 2 indicated common challenges that youth face and practices that youth employ to navigate information online, which have significant implications for designing effective interventions to promote youth digital resilience. Both studies demonstrated that youth information seeking is deeply intertwined with social needs and identity formation. Participants rarely engaged in purely truth-seeking behavior; instead, they used information to orient themselves socially, define their emerging identities, and connect with like-minded peers. They also showed a strong preference for personal testimonies and relatable sources over traditional markers of authority, and they relied heavily on social cues and peer validation within their online communities to assess credibility. These shared findings point to a critical need to move beyond traditional, literacy-focused interventions and adopt strength-based approaches that leverage the social embeddedness of online information practices.
Literature on social media’s effects on youth, particularly those focused on DE, often assumes that content can be classified as “healthy” or “harmful” and effectively moderated (Ambwani et al. 2019). But our findings demonstrate that these distinctions do not exist as intrinsic qualities of content: they only make sense in contextual interactions between the user and the content. Much work on DE focuses on an individual pathology, evaluating an individual’s psychological state, family history, and reactions to therapeutic interventions, often in clinical or lab settings (Holland and Tiggemann 2016, 2017; Ioannidis et al. 2021; Ioannidis and Chamberlain 2021). To date, much intervention attention has focused on the moment of crisis, when a user is facing immediate harm. These interventions model the path to crisis as a funnel, asking how to intervene before users reach the crisis point at the end. This model imagines youth’s journeys as linear and their crisis points as homogenous. It also leads interventions toward solutions like content removals by platforms, suggesting that the best way to improve the digital resilience of youth is to grade pieces of content as inherently “harmful” or “helpful”, removing those that fall into the former category. Our findings, however, show that different youth had different journeys and relationships to content, mediated by the social contexts in which they encountered and shared information.

5.1. Summary of Design Recommendations

We found that meeting participants’ information needs meant understanding their underlying motivations for consuming and sharing content. Interventions should help youth assess whether content is helpful for them and provide them with resources. We propose supplementing content-grading interventions with interventions that support and re-channel that desire for a sense of purpose and belonging. This approach, we propose, helps support a wider range of youth (beyond those formally diagnosed with eating disorders) navigate an online information ecosystem saturated with eating and health information.
These common findings directly informed our design recommendations. First, recognizing the importance of peer networks and the prevalence of crowdsourcing credibility (Study 1) and distributed trust (Study 2), we recommend leveraging peer networks (6.1). This involves creating interventions that facilitate collaborative learning and peer-to-peer support within online communities.
Second, given the finding that participants often encountered information (Study 1) through algorithmic suggestion and tailoring (Study 2) rather than actively searching for it, we recommend developing social media sensibility (6.2). This means equipping youth with the skills to critically evaluate information sources in situ, understand creators’ motivations, and navigate social influences online. Implemented successfully, this sensibility could help them engage in their algorithmic tailoring work with more attention to the quality of sources they choose as surrogate thinkers (Study 1).
Third, acknowledging participants’ desire for meaningful offline connections and their belief that the “good life” is lived offline (Study 2), we recommend providing pathways offline (6.3). Interventions could connect youth to in-person communities and resources, such as local support groups or workshops, based on their online interests. Finally, recognizing the potential dangers of “n = 1 thinking” (Study 2), we recommend encouraging probabilistic thinking (6.4). This involves helping youth understand the limitations of personal testimonies citing everyday experience (Study 1) and the importance of considering broader evidence to enhance their good-enough reasoning (Study 1). All interventions would require careful consideration of digital privacy, safety, and security risks such as hackability, abuse, and infiltration. We discuss some of these risks.
These findings highlight limitations of solely literacy-focused interventions and emphasize the need for strength-based approaches built upon the social embeddedness of online information practices. We propose that interventions should not primarily focus on “cleaning up” the online ecosystem through content removal, but instead give high-quality information to youth in key moments when they are reaching for it and support them by helping them build the meaningful peer-to-peer relationships they crave.

5.2. Leveraging Peer Networks

Within social media, participants needed localized content and practical advice from communities they trusted. Participants also wanted communities for long-term learning and resources for putting that learning into practice. When participants accessed localized results that felt credible and immediately helpful, they felt more confident in their decision-making.
A version of the “Search Together” intervention described in Section 4.3 might help youth find supportive communities to synchronously discuss information with. When they were left to search alone, their chances of encountering potentially harmful misinformation seemed just as likely as on social media, so peer support mechanisms could help them use their existing information sensibility practices to avoid digital-safety harms. Location-based filters could also help younger users find eating and health information that feels more relevant to them.
Features that highlight community-sourced information—like ratings, reviews, and shared search histories—could help youth identify credible sources and connect with supportive peers. Our findings suggest that information should be community-sourced where possible, given youth’s reliance on crowdsourced credibility (Study 1) and distributed trust (Study 2). Disabled and minoritized participants wanted in-the-moment information to make their cities more physically accessible and guides to places hospitable for Black, Indigenous, and People of Color (BIPOC). Interventions could elevate community-based knowledge sources like @DisabledHikers, which provide up-to-date info about accessible trails.
Youth desire for identity- and interest-based communities could be supported through interventions strengthening peer-to-peer networks: researchers studying sexual minority youth found that they often join online communities to express themselves and feel less isolated (Charmaraman et al. 2021). These communities foster a sense of belonging and identity, and, when designed well, could provide peer-to-peer support, helping youth navigate mental health challenges. In doing so, these communities could also help protect youth from misinformation susceptibility, which is often increased by loneliness and other challenging emotional states (Hassoun et al. 2024).
Research indicates that users of online mental health communities are more engaged when posts are about social experiences and emotional expression (Liu and Kong 2021). Interventions could encourage users to share detailed content, express distinct emotions, and use images to enhance engagement and support.
However, there are potential risks involved in this recommendation. Over-reliance on peer networks could amplify harmful viewpoints and reinforce misinformation if communities are already entrenched in those views. The social benefits youth receive from such communities might make it hard for them to disengage and find more supportive communities.

5.3. Developing Social Media Sensibility

Participants needed a trusted way to evaluate new vocabulary they encountered on social media, especially when social media content used diagnostic words. Participants needed help in the moment directly after learning something new, where they were most receptive to new perspectives. They also needed ways to vet creators they found on social media. Useful interventions could help them successfully surface from social media algorithms, ask clarifying questions, and pause to reflect before diving back in.
Possible interventions could involve provenance tracking or a language tree of search queries which build on the vocabulary youth use on social media to better guide them to new terms and searches. Historically and globally contextualizing how a term became prominent and in which contexts could help young users understand emerging, confusing terms. Other potential interventions include tools that provide contextual information about content creators, such as their funding sources or potential biases, and prompts that encourage users to reflect on the information they encounter.
Study 1 found that participants liked having go-to sources (surrogate thinkers) that provided information they trusted. The like-minded, like-bodied checks undertaken by Study 2 participants helped them vet creators and determine whether the creators qualified as a go-to source. Rather than trying to push youth away from their go-to sources, helping them better evaluate content creators online could offer better and more impactful digital-safety support.
Social media sensibility interventions could help youth understand how algorithms work in situ, in the specific context of their social media environments: how content they see is promoted or recommended, the motivations and potential biases of content creators (Breakstone et al. 2021), and the effects of deceptive design elements on social media platforms (Costello et al. 2023; Montag et al. 2019). Interventions could also address the algorithmically mediated power of social influence online, like learning how “likes” affect perceptions of social norms (targeting their “crowdsourcing credibility” practices). Education should focus on how these dynamics and affordances could impact their everyday experience on social media platforms.
This recommendation has potential drawbacks. It still focuses on individual-level interactions, which may limit its efficacy. It could place an undue burden on individual users to engage actively with everything they encounter online, which is not feasible or sustainable—especially given the finding that youth employ good enough reasoning (Study 1) whenever possible, given the information overload they face.
Interventions in the literature that potentially counteract this risk address the surrogate thinking (Study 1) information sensibility practice. These include recruiting opinion leaders within online communities to promote positive health behaviors and counteract negative content, as peer influence can be a powerful tool for shaping online behavior (Valente and Pumpuang 2007). Using network analysis to identify go-to sources, “core pro-recovery users” that other users find supportive could be recruited or supported as behavior change agents (Wang et al. 2018).

5.4. Providing Supportive Pathways Offline

Participants valued resources that connected them to offline communities when they needed in-person resources and support. Participants wanted to connect with people offline and activate a sense of being seen and valued “IRL”. Providing youth with pathways offline using community-sourced information could meaningfully build their resilience to digital-safety harms. Developing connective online-offline interventions—like a digital community bulletin board that weights results toward local sources (e.g., in-person activities and events)—could help youth find and strengthen the offline relationships they sought based on the empowering connections with like-minded and like-bodied people they found online. This recommendation (along with the peer-to-peer network recommendation) is supported by literature on social-emotional learning, which focuses on helping youth make responsible decisions, increase their self- and social-awareness, build their relationship skills, and foster ability to manage their emotions and behaviors (Durlak et al. 2011).
These pathways could offer another source of support for their crowdsourcing credibility (Study 1) and distributed trust (Study 2) information sensibility practices, allowing youth to process information with in-person communities that meet their need for identity-based social belonging. Offline interest-based communities seemed less likely to form around extreme political viewpoints or misinformation sharing than online ones. Offline groups tended to be oriented around mental health discussions and kinetic activities (e.g., gardening, sports, hiking, knitting).
This recommendation risks excluding youth who lack access to offline resources or who prefer online interactions. More structured and interactive online communities, like synchronous online camps for younger youth, could augment this pitfall (Darvasi 2017; Ringland et al. 2016). This recommendation also has the same potential pitfalls as leveraging peer networks with regard to offline communities that are entrenched in harmful views and practices (e.g., supremacist groups, workout groups with pro-ana goals). It could also be abused, hacked or infiltrated, making it potentially difficult to responsibly implement.

5.5. Encouraging Probabilistic Thinking

As discussed, the problem with n = 1 thinking is that information cannot be proven false or unlikely. Encouraging probabilistic thinking (how likely something is to work, what percentage of people like them support it) may help to offset reliance on personal testimony. Youth need help putting n = 1 personal testimonials into broader context. Interventions could include sources that highlight probabilistic thinking (e.g., “86% of peer-reviewed studies conclude that there is not enough information to know if the Keto diet is effective or safe for anything other than epilepsy.”) or communities that vet claims (e.g., voting mechanisms in community spaces or underneath user-created content). Interventions could provide this probabilistic and voting information alongside personal anecdotes. Youth do think probabilistically—when they see n = 1 content on social media, they ask themselves, “What are the chances this would work for me?”—but more exposure to probabilistic thinking could help them make an educated guess without experimenting on themselves. One risk of this recommendation is that it could undermine the trust and connection that youth feel with relatable sources, which is a key aspect of their information sensibility.
The belief underpinning n = 1 thinking—that there is no universal right answer to most health questions—could also help protect youth from certain kinds of misinformation susceptibility if oriented more probabilistically. In Study 1, we found that the desire for certainty drove many participants toward misinformation’s definitive answers to difficult questions. The ability to accept ambiguity and a probabilistically likely, though not definite, answer helped participants not seek certainty in misinformation sources.
In Study 1, we found that participants engaged in “good-enough” reasoning—limiting effort to finding just enough information to fit their context-specific needs (e.g., data to support them in proving a family member wrong in an argument or obtaining background information on what their peers were discussing). Helping youth use this heuristic to support thorough decision making by making probabilistic information easily available could help offset n = 1 thinking. Because information overload—as discovered in Study 1—is such an issue for youth, such probabilistic information must be simple and easy for youth to access and parse.
This recommendation is especially crucial in the age of GenAI. GenAI tools—if used to generate highly personalized content that caters to individual biases and preferences—could make youth less likely to consider broader probabilistic data, particularly if such an algorithmically tailored GenAI tool becomes a surrogate thinker. Its authoritative answers and singular voice could undermine the impulse and ability to think probabilistically or consult other sources. Conversely, teaching youth to prompt GenAI tools for probabilistic evaluations could support this intervention. Given the reliance of youth on social cues, future research could explore the role of GenAI in youth information sensibility practices, as GenAI tools were not widespread during our field work.

5.6. Algorithmic Interventions

Finally, it is necessary to stress the importance of algorithmic suggestion and responsibility. While it is important to equip youth with strength-based, autonomy-preserving approaches to digital resilience, interventions should also ensure that social media platforms facilitate transparent and independent assessments of the impact of their algorithm design on youth (Costello et al. 2023). As Costello et al. (2023) argue, the focus should be on the design elements of these algorithms and the harms produced by them (e.g., infinite scrolling, social pressure and rewards, notification cadence, individualized content feeds), not just the content on the platform. While Costello et al. reached this conclusion from a legal-pragmatic evaluation, our ethnographic research produced the same recommendation: a focus on algorithmic mediation and its effect on information engagement—rather than content classification—from a youth-centered emic approach. Given the increasingly central role of GenAI algorithms in shaping online information ecosystems, future research and resulting frameworks should include algorithmically mediated engagement if they hope to positively impact youth digital safety and resilience.

5.7. Period, Age, and Cohort Effects

We hypothesize that youth engagement with information online is shaped by a combination of period, age, and cohort effects.13 The tendency to encounter information rather than actively search for it is likely a period effect experienced across age groups, but is more pronounced among younger generations, forming a generational gradient. Similarly, the desire for social belonging often outweighs the pursuit of accurate information for all ages, a period effect particularly strong in adolescents, indicating an age gradient. Adolescence is also marked by a heightened need for peer-based orientation and identity construction through information practices, as well as a stronger initial influence of family on beliefs, which shifts towards peer influence with age. The specific ways youth validate information, such as crowdsourcing credibility, are cohort effects enabled by social media affordances not available to prior generations. Furthermore, a greater reliance on everyday experience and n = 1 thinking over traditional expertise is a cohort effect potentially stemming from a decline in institutional trust during Gen Z’s formative years.

5.8. Future Work

We recommend researchers build on our findings by investigating strategies to improve upon interventions to encourage digital resilience, disseminate them to youth, and promote their adoption. It would also be valuable for future research to evaluate interventions in different geographies and digital contexts, including but not limited to the predominant forms of digital public squares that are centrally moderated (e.g., TikTok), community moderated (e.g., Reddit), or privately moderated (e.g., chat applications) (Goldberg et al. 2024).
There is also a timely need to study how large language models (LLMs) could better customize these interventions to tailor them to individual youth. For example, LLMs have demonstrated utility in personalizing educational messaging for youth—though not without serious limitations (Gill et al. 2024)—and could be used to surface probabilistic generated responses to youth queries for information to address n = 1 thinking. Conversely, the risks posed by youth interactions with LLMs should also be studied in relation to this paper’s findings.

6. Conclusions

This research underscores the limitations of solely literacy-focused or content removal interventions and highlights the necessity for strength-based approaches based upon the social dimensions of youth online information practices. Our findings suggest that digital resilience among youth can be fostered by: (1) leveraging peer networks to encourage collaborative learning and peer-to-peer support within online communities, (2) cultivating social media literacy skills to critically evaluate information sources and navigate social influences, (3) providing pathways offline, and (4) encouraging probabilistic thinking. We advocate for interventions that move beyond content removal, focusing instead on providing high-quality information to youth at critical junctures in their online journeys and supporting the development of meaningful peer-to-peer relationships. By addressing the social and contextual factors that shape online information engagement, we can empower youth to navigate their digital ecosystems with greater resilience and agency.

Supplementary Materials

The research ethics protocol (including ethics committee approval information), informed consent protocol and consent form language, recruitment screener, interview/field guide, diary study, de-escalation protocol, and guidance for the disordered eating expert reviewer can be found in the Open Science Framework at https://osf.io/ura8c (accessed on 14 March 2025).

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.H., B.G. and S.C.; methodology, A.H., I.B., T.C., B.G., L.M., R.S.P., B.S. and S.C.; validation, A.H., T.C., L.M., B.S. and S.C.; formal analysis, A.H., I.B., T.C., P.G.K., B.G., D.K., L.M., R.S.P., B.S. and S.C.; investigation, A.H., I.B., T.C., B.G., D.K., L.M., R.S.P., B.S. and S.C.; resources, A.H., B.G. and R.S.P.; data curation, A.H., T.C., L.M. and B.S.; writing—original draft, A.H. and S.C.; writing—review and editing, A.H., I.B., P.G.K., B.G., R.S.P. and S.C.; visualization, A.H., P.G.K. and L.M.; supervision, A.H., B.G., R.S.P. and S.C.; project administration, A.H., B.G., L.M., R.S.P., B.S. and S.C.; funding acquisition, A.H., B.G., R.S.P. and S.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was funded by Jigsaw (Google).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by Google Privacy, Security, Legal, Ethics and Data Protection Research Review Committee, 4130221. Date of approval: 4 March 2022.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The aggregated and pseudonymized data presented in this study are included in the article and Supplementary Materials. Supplementary Materials for this study are available in the Open Science Framework at https://osf.io/ura8c (accessed on 14 March 2025). De-aggregated and de-pseudonymized primary ethnographic data are not available due to privacy and ethical restrictions to protect participant identities. Further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

We are very grateful to the researchers who worked with us on earlier versions of this work, Rachel Xu and Vanessa Maturo. We would also like to thank Rebecca Lester, Krishnendu Ray, and several other experts and colleagues for sharing their knowledge and support as we planned and conducted this research. We thank Tara Matthews, Amanda Walker, and our reviewers for their valuable reflections on improving this manuscript. Finally, we extend deep gratitude to our participants for their time and for sharing their experiences with us.

Conflicts of Interest

Amelia Hassoun, Beth Goldberg, Ian Beacock, Patrick Gage Kelley, and Sunny Consolvo are employees of Google and/or Jigsaw, and their parent company, Alphabet. Devika Kumar, Behzad Sarmadi, Rebekah Su Park, Laura Murray, and Todd Carmody are employees of Gemic. The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Notes

1
Folk models are mental models that are shared among similar members of a culture, but are not necessarily accurate; the inaccuracies can lead to problems (Wash 2010; D’Andrade 2005).
2
For this work, alternative platforms were defined as congruent with the “alt-tech” ecosystem (Ebner 2019). These included in-house “safe haven” platforms developed by far-right extremists, ultra-libertarian platforms, and alternative social forums popularized due to the perception that alternative hosts provide less stringent content moderation than mainstream platforms. To classify participants as mainstream or alternative, we asked whether participants used 4chan, 8kun, Gab, Parler, or Rumble three or more times per week. We then refined classifications based on their answers to: “Do you use any apps, platforms, sites, or channels that your friends don’t? What are they? Do you have any opinions or views that you would describe as alternative or non-mainstream? Are you part of any online communities that you don’t tell anyone about? Do you have any political opinions that you don’t feel comfortable sharing with others?” Four were reclassified, twoin each direction.
3
Refer to the recruitment screener in the Supplementary Materials for detailed information on recruitment criteria and practices. Refer to the field guide in the Supplementary Materials for information on de-escalation procedure and design.
4
Refer to the interview and field guides in Supplementary Materials for specific differences.
5
The term “n = 1 thinking” is a direct quote from an expert interview conducted with Krishnendu Ray in 2022.
6
Body mass index (BMI) is a medical screening tool that estimates body fat by measuring height-to-weight ratio (weight in kilograms divided by the square of height in meters) (Cleveland Clinic 2024).
7
“Cottagecore” is a trend emphasizing rural esthetics, sustainability, and minimalist lifestyles. Black cottagecore pushes back on the white American colonial fantasy that some argue underpins cottagecore esthetics (Jackson 2022).
8
BTS is a South Korean boy band.
9
Bhangra is a traditional Punjabi folk dance that is widely practiced in India and the diaspora.
10
Gender-affirming surgery that can involve removal of the testicles, or removal of the testicles and penis and the creation of a vagina, labia and clitoris. Urinary problems like incontinence are potential side effects (Mayo Clinic 2024).
11
This study occurred before the widespread popularity and availability of Ozempic (a weight loss drug) and similar drugs in the US. Follow-up studies could investigate whether the popularity of such drugs has affected this analysis.
12
See the Supplementary Materials for a full list of unmet need codes and clusters.
13
Period effects are experienced by all age groups living at a particular moment in time. Some period effects are the result of a specific, discrete event; others may be caused by longer-term social, economic, or cultural conditions. Age effects are effects that people of a certain age experience, regardless of the time period they live in. Cohort effects are effects experienced by specific generations (e.g., Gen Z, Baby Boomers).

References

  1. Albala, Ken. 2005. Weight Loss in the Age of Reason. In Cultures of the Abdomen. Edited by Christopher E. Forth and Ana Carden-Coyne. New York: Palgrave Macmillan, pp. 169–84. [Google Scholar]
  2. Alter, Joseph. 2005. Asian Medicine and Globalization. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. [Google Scholar]
  3. Ambwani, Suman, Meghan Shippe, Ziting Gao, and Bryn Austin. 2019. Is #cleaneating a Healthy or Harmful Dietary Strategy? Perceptions of Clean Eating and Associations with Disordered Eating Among Young Adults. Journal of Eating Disorders 7: 17. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  4. Ames, Richard. 1691. The Female Fire-Ships. A Satyr Against Whoring. In English Poetry Full-Text Database. Cambridge: Chadwyck-Healey. [Google Scholar]
  5. Anderson-Fye, Eileen, Stephanie McClure, and Rachel Wilson. 2015. Cultural Similarities and Differences in Eating Disorders. In The Wiley Handbook of Eating Disorders, Assessment, Prevention, Treatment, Policy, and Future Directions. Edited by Linda Smolak and Michael P. Levine. New York: John Wiley & Sons, pp. 297–311. [Google Scholar]
  6. Appadurai, Arjun. 1981. Gastro-politics in Hindu South Asia. American Ethnologist 8: 494–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Arnold, David. 1993. Colonizing the Body: State Medicine and Epidemic Diseases in 19th-Century India. Berkeley: University of California Press. [Google Scholar]
  8. Austin, Bryn. 2021. How Social Media’s Toxic Content Sends Teens into ‘A Dangerous Spiral’. Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health—Featured News Stories. Available online: https://tinyurl.com/4wj2h7pn (accessed on 20 June 2022).
  9. Banerjee, Madhulika. 2009. Power, Knowledge, Medicine: Ayurvedic Pharmaceuticals at Home and in the World. New Delhi: Orient Blackswan. [Google Scholar]
  10. Bansal, Parul. 2012. Youth in Contemporary India: Images of Identity and Social Change. New York: Springer. [Google Scholar]
  11. Bart, Pauline, and Diana H. Scully. 1979. The Politics of Hysteria: The Case of the Wandering Womb. In Gender and Disordered Behavior: Sex Differences in Psychopathology. Edited by Edith S. Gomberg and Violet Franks. New York: Brunner/Mazel, pp. 354–80. [Google Scholar]
  12. Bartky, Sandra Lee. 1990. Femininity and Domination: Studies in the Phenomenology of Oppression. New York: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  13. Bell, Rudolph. 1985. Holy Anorexia. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  14. Bellini, Rosanna, Emily Tseng, Noel Warford, Alaa Daffalla, Tara Matthews, and Sunny Consolvo. 2024. SoK: Safer Digital-Safety Research Involving at-Risk Users. Paper presented at the 2024 IEEE Symposium on Security and Privacy (SP), San Francisco, CA, USA, May 19–23; pp. 635–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Bernard, H Russell. 2017. Research Methods in Anthropology: Qualitative and Quantitative Approaches. Lanham: Rowman & Littlefield. [Google Scholar]
  16. Bissell, Jordan. 2018. Vegan Youtube Stars are Held to Impossible Standards. The Atlantic. Available online: https://www.theatlantic.com/technology/archive/2018/05/vegan-youtube-stars-are-held-to-impossible-standards/560807/ (accessed on 15 September 2024).
  17. Bordo, Susan. 1993. Unbearable Weight: Feminism, Western Culture, and the Body. Berkeley: University of California Press. [Google Scholar]
  18. Boyd, Danah. 2017. Did media literacy backfire? Journal of Applied Youth Studies 1: 83–89. [Google Scholar]
  19. Bray, Abigail. 1996. The anorexic body: Reading disorders. Cultural Studies 10: 413–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Breakstone, Joel, Mark Smith, Sam Wineburg, Amie Rapaport, Jill Carle, Marshall Garland, and Anna Saavedra. 2021. Students’ Civic Online Reasoning: A National Portrait. Educational Researcher 50: 505–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Brown, John Seely, and Paul Duguid. 2017. The Social Life of Information: Updated, with a New Preface. Cambridge: Harvard Business Review Press. [Google Scholar]
  22. Brumberg, Joan Jacobs. 1988. Fasting Girls: The History of Anorexia Nervosa. New York: Vintage. [Google Scholar]
  23. Buckingham, David. 2008. Introducing Identity. In Youth, Identity, and Digital Media. Edited by David Buckingham. Cambridge: The MIT Press, pp. 1–24. [Google Scholar]
  24. Chancellor, Stevie, Tanushree Mitra, and Munmun De Choudhury. 2016. Recovery Amid Pro-Anorexia: Analysis of Recovery in Social Media. Paper presented at the 2016 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (CHI ’16), San Jose, CA, USA, May 7–12; pp. 2111–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Charmaraman, Linda, Rachel Hodes, and Amanda Richer. 2021. Young Sexual Minority Adolescent Experiences of Self-Expression and Isolation on Social Media: Cross-Sectional Survey Study. JMIR Ment Health 8: e2620. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Charmaz, Kathy. 2006. Constructing Grounded Theory: A Practical Guide Through Qualitative Analysis. London: Sage. [Google Scholar]
  27. Cheney, Ann, Sullivan Steve, and Grubbs Kathleen. 2018. The Morality of Disordered Eating and Recovery in Southern Italy. Medical Anthropology Quarterly 32: 443–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Cleveland Clinic. 2024. Body Mass Index (BMI). Available online: https://my.clevelandclinic.org/health/articles/9464-body-mass-index-bmi (accessed on 28 December 2024).
  29. Cohen, Rachel, Jasmine Fardouly, Toby Newton-John, and Amy Slater. 2019. #BoPo on Instagram: An Experimental Investigation of the Effects of Viewing Body Positive Content on Young Women’s Mood and Body Image. New Media & Society 21: 1546–64. [Google Scholar]
  30. Corbin, Juliet, and Anselm Strauss. 2015. Basics of Qualitative Research, Techniques and Procedures for Developing Grounded Theory, 4th ed. Thousand Oaks: Sage. [Google Scholar]
  31. Costello, Nancy, Rebecca Sutton, Madeline Jones, Mackenzie Almassian, Amanda Raffoul, Oluwadunni Ojumu, Meg Salvia, Monique Santoso, Jill R. Kavanaugh, and S. Bryn Austin. 2023. Algorithms, Addiction, And Adolescent Mental Health: An Interdisciplinary Study to Inform State-Level Policy Action to Protect Youth from the Dangers of Social Media. American Journal of Law & Medicine 49: 135–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Dacome, Linda. 2005. Useless and Pernicious Matter: Corpulence in Eighteenth-Century England. In Cultures of the Abdomen. Edited by Christopher E. Forth and Ana Carden-Coyne. London: Palgrave Macmillan, pp. 185–204. [Google Scholar]
  33. D’Andrade, Roy. 2005. Some methods for studying cultural cognitive structures. In Finding Culture in Talk. Berlin and Heidelberg: Springer, pp. 83–104. [Google Scholar]
  34. Darvasi, Paul. 2017. How Online Communities Lower Social Barriers for Kids Across the Spectrum. Connected Parenting. Available online: https://medium.com/connected-parenting/how-online-communities-lower-social-barriers-for-kids-across-the-spectrum-aac39a4e95f9 (accessed on 21 December 2024).
  35. Das, Veena. 2015. Affliction: Health, Disease, Poverty. New York: Fordham University Press. [Google Scholar]
  36. Dedrick, Ashley, Julie Williams Merten, Tammy Adams, Meghann Wheeler, Terrell Kassie, and Jessica L. King. 2020. A Content Analysis of Pinterest Belly Fat Loss Exercises: Unrealistic Expectations and Misinformation. American Journal of Health Education 51: 328–37. [Google Scholar]
  37. Duffy, Andrew, Edson Tandoc, and Rich Ling. 2019. Too Good to Be True, Too Good Not to Share: The Social Utility of Fake News. Information, Communication & Society 23: 1965–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Duggan, Scott, and Donald McCreary. 2004. Body Image, Eating Disorders, and the Drive for Muscularity in Gay and Heterosexual Men. Journal of Homosexuality 47: 45–58. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  39. Durlak, Joseph, Roger Weissberg, Allison Dymnicki, Rebecca Taylor, and Kriston Schellinger. 2011. The Impact of Enhancing Students’ Social and Emotional Learning: A Meta-Analysis of School-Based Universal Interventions. Child Development 82: 405–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Ebner, Julia. 2019. Counter-Creativity: Innovative Ways to Counter Far-Right Communication Tactics. In Post-Digital Cultures of the Far Right: Online Actions and Offline Consequences. Edited by Maik Fielitz and Nick Thurston. Bielefeld: Transcript. [Google Scholar]
  41. Ecks, Stefan. 2013. Eating Drugs: Psychopharmaceutical Pluralism in India. New York: New York University Press. [Google Scholar]
  42. Eikey, Elizabeth. 2021. Effects of Diet and Fitness Apps on Eating Disorder Behaviours: Qualitative Study. BJPsych Open 7: e176. [Google Scholar]
  43. Eli, Karin, and Megan Warin. 2018. Anthropological Perspectives on Eating Disorders: Deciphering Cultural Logics. Transcultural Psychiatry 55: 443–45. [Google Scholar]
  44. Evans, Brynn, Sanjay Kairam, and Peter Pirolli. 2010. Do Your Friends Make You Smarter?: An Analysis of Social Strategies in Online Information Seeking. Information Processing & Management 46: 679–92. [Google Scholar]
  45. Eynon, Rebecca, and Lars-Erik Malmberg. 2012. Understanding the Online Information-Seeking Behaviours of Young People: The Role of Networks of Support. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning 28: 514–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Feldman, Matthew, and Ilan Meyer. 2007. Eating Disorders in Diverse Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual Populations. The International Journal of Eating Disorders 40: 218–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Feldman, Matthew, and Ilan Meyer. 2009. Comorbidity and Age of Onset of Eating Disorders in Gay Men, Lesbians, and Bisexuals. Psychiatry Research 180: 126–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Feng, K. J. Kevin, Kevin Song, Kejing Li, Oishee Chakrabarti, and Marshini Chetty. 2022. Investigating How University Students in the United States Encounter and Deal with Misinformation in Private WhatsApp Chats During COVID-19. Paper presented at the Eighteenth Symposium on Usable Privacy and Security (SOUPS 2022), Boston, MA, USA, August 8–9; pp. 427–46. [Google Scholar]
  49. Geeng, Christine, Savanna Yee, and Franziska Roesner. 2020. Fake News on Facebook and Twitter: Investigating How People (Don’t) Investigate. Paper presented at the 2020 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (CHI ’20), New York, NY, USA, April 25–30; Melbourne: Association for Computing Machinery, pp. 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Ghassem-Fachandi, Parvis. 2012. Pogrom in Gujarat: Hindu Nationalism and Anti-Muslim Violence. Princeton: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
  51. Giles Banks, Caroline. 1996. ‘There Is No Fat in Heaven’: Religious Asceticism and the Meaning of Anorexia Nervosa. Ethos 24: 107–35. [Google Scholar]
  52. Gill, Sukhpal Singh, Minxian Xu, Panos Patros, Huaming Wu, Rupinder Kaur, Kamalpreet Kaur, Stephanie Fuller, Manmeet Singh, Priyansh Arora, and Parlikad Ajith Kumar. 2024. Transformative effects of ChatGPT on modern education: Emerging Era of AI Chatbots. Internet of Things and Cyber-Physical Systems 4: 19–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Gillespie, Tarleton. 2020. Content Moderation, AI, and the Question of Scale. Big Data & Society 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Goldberg, Beth, Diana Acosta-Navas, Michiel Bakker, Ian Beacock, Matt Botvinick, Prateek Buch, Renée DiResta, Nandika Donthi, Nathanael Fast, Ravi Iyer, and et al. 2024. AI and the Future of Digital Public Squares. arXiv arXiv:2412.09988. [Google Scholar]
  55. Gooldin, Sigal. 2008. Being Anorexic: Hunger, Subjectivity, and Embodied Morality. Medical Anthropology Quarterly 22: 274–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Gordon, Chloe S., Hannah K. Jarman, Rachel F. Rodgers, Siân A. McLean, Amy Slater, Matthew Fuller-Tyszkiewicz, and Susan J. Paxton. 2021. Outcomes of a Cluster Randomized Controlled Trial of the SoMe Social Media Literacy Program for Improving Body Image-Related Outcomes in Adolescent Boys and Girls. Nutrients 13: 3825. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Greene, Amanda, and Lisa Brownstone. 2021. ‘Just a Place to Keep Track of Myself’: Eating Disorders, Social Media, and the Quantified Self. Feminist Media Studies 23: 508–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Greenfield, Rebecca. 2012. To Ban or Not to Ban: How Do You Solve the Problem of Thinspo? The Atlantic. Available online: https://tinyurl.com/46z8u9rk (accessed on 20 June 2022).
  59. Gressier, Catie. 2021. Food as Faith: Suffering, Salvation and the Paleo Diet in Australia. Food, Culture, & Society 25: 670–82. [Google Scholar]
  60. Griffiths, Scott, Emily A. Harris, Grace Whitehead, Felicity Angelopoulos, Ben Stone, Wesley Grey, and Simon Dennis. 2024. Does TikTok Contribute to Eating Disorders? A Comparison of the TikTok Algorithms Belonging to Individuals with Eating Disorders Versus Healthy Controls. Body Image 51: 101807. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Hamblin, James. 2018. The Jordan Peterson All-Meat Diet. The Atlantic. Available online: https://www.theatlantic.com/health/archive/2018/08/the-peterson-family-meat-cleanse/567613/ (accessed on 20 June 2022).
  62. Hamel, Jessica Lynn. 2018. Women’s Food Refusal and Feminine Appetites in the Long British Eighteenth Century. Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Montreal, Montreal, QC, Canada. [Google Scholar]
  63. Hassoun, Amelia, Gabrielle Borenstein, Katy Osborn, Jacob McAuliffe, and Beth Goldberg. 2024. Sowing “Seeds of Doubt”: Cottage Industries of Election and Medical Misinformation in Brazil and the United States. New Media & Society. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Hassoun, Amelia, Ian Beacock, Sunny Consolvo, Beth Goldberg, Patrick Gage Kelley, and Daniel M. Russell. 2023. Practicing Information Sensibility: How Gen Z Engages with Online Information. In Proceedings of the 2023 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (CHI ’23) 662: 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Herrero-Diz, Paula, Jesús Conde-Jiménez, and Salvador Reyes de Cózar. 2020. Teens’ motivations to spread fake news on WhatsApp. Social Media + Society 6: 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Holland, Grace, and Marika Tiggemann. 2016. A Systematic Review of the Impact of the Use of Social Networking Sites on Body Image and Disordered Eating Outcomes. Body Image 17: 100–10. [Google Scholar]
  67. Holland, Grace, and Marika Tiggemann. 2017. ‘Strong Beats Skinny Every Time’: Disordered Eating and Compulsive Exercise in Women Who Post Fitspiration on Instagram. International Journal of Eating Disorders 50: 76–79. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  68. Ioannidis, Konstantinos, and Samuel R. Chamberlain. 2021. Digital Hazards for Feeding and Eating: What We Know and What We Don’t. Current Psychiatry Reports 23: 56. [Google Scholar]
  69. Ioannidis, Konstantinos, Charlotte Taylor, Leah Holt, Kate Brown, Christine Lochner, Naomi A. Fineberg, Ornella Corazza, Samuel R. Chamberlain, Andres Roman-Urrestarazu, and Katarzyna Czabanowska. 2021. Problematic Usage of the Internet and Eating Disorder and Related Psychopathology: A Multifaceted, Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 125: 569–81. [Google Scholar]
  70. Jackson, Jada. 2022. Black Women Are Reclaiming the Cottagecore Trend. The Zoe Report. Available online: https://www.thezoereport.com/fashion/black-women-cottagecore (accessed on 4 February 2022).
  71. Jenzen, Olu. 2017. Trans Youth and Social Media: Moving Between Counterpublics and the Wider Web. Gender, Place, and Culture: A Journal of Feminist Geography 24: 1626–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Jones, Payton, Benjamin Bellet, and Richard McNally. 2020. Helping or Harming? The Effect of Trigger Warnings on Individuals with Trauma Histories. Clinical Psychological Science 8: 905–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Khalikova, Venera. 2021. Medical pluralism. In The Open Encyclopedia of Anthropology. Edited by Felix Stein. Cambridge: Open Knowledge Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Khare, Ravindra S. 1992. The Eternal Food: Gastronomic Ideas and Experiences of Hindus and Buddhists. Albany: State University of New York Press. [Google Scholar]
  75. Kumar, Sanjay. 2019. Youth in India: Aspirations, Attitudes, Anxieties. New York: Routledge Chapman & Hall. [Google Scholar]
  76. Lai, Samantha. 2022. How Do We Solve Social Media’s Eating Disorder Problem? Brookings Institute. Available online: https://www.brookings.edu/articles/how-do-we-solve-social-medias-eating-disorder-problem/ (accessed on 2 May 2023).
  77. Langford, Jean. 2002. Fluent Bodies: Ayurvedic Remedies for Postcolonial Imbalance. Durham: Duke University Press. [Google Scholar]
  78. Lavis, Anna. 2015. Careful Starving: Reflections on (Not) Eating, Caring and Anorexia. In Careful Eating: Bodies, Food and Care. Edited by Emma-Jayne Abbots, Anna Lavis and Luci Attala. London: Routledge, pp. 91–108. [Google Scholar]
  79. Lenhart, Amanda, and Kelly Owens. 2021. The Unseen Teen: The Challenges of Building Healthy Tech for Young People. Data & Society. Available online: https://datasociety.net/library/the-unseen-teen/ (accessed on 20 June 2022).
  80. Lester, Rebecca. 2021. Famished: Eating Disorders and Failed Care in America. Berkeley: UC Press. [Google Scholar]
  81. Littlewood, Roland. 1995. Psychopathology and personal agency: Modernity, culture change and eating disorders in South Asian societies. British Journal of Medical Psychology 68: 45–63. [Google Scholar]
  82. Liu, Jingfang, and Jun Kong. 2021. Why Do Users of Online Mental Health Communities Get Likes and Reposts: A Combination of Text Mining and Empirical Analysis. Healthcare 9: 1133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Lloyd, Jenny. 2018. Abuse through sexual image sharing in schools: Response and responsibility. Gender and Education 32: 784–802. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Logrieco, Giuseppe, Maria R. Marchili, Marco Roversi, and Alberto Villani. 2021. The Paradox of Tik Tok Anti-Pro-Anorexia Videos: How Social Media Can Promote Non-Suicidal Self-Injury and Anorexia. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 18: 1041. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  85. Lukose, Ritty. 2009. Liberalization’s Children: Gender, Youth, and Consumer Citizenship in Globalizing India. Durham: Duke University Press. [Google Scholar]
  86. Mackey, Thomas, and Trudy Jacobson. 2011. Reframing information literacy as a metaliteracy. College & Research Libraries 72: 62–78. [Google Scholar]
  87. Marks, Rosie Jean, Alexander De Foe, and James Collett. 2020. The pursuit of wellness: Social media, body image and eating disorders. Children and Youth Services Review 119: 105659. [Google Scholar]
  88. Marriott, McKim. 1989. India Through Hindu Categories. Delhi: Sage Publications. [Google Scholar]
  89. Marwick, Alice. 2018. Why do people share fake news? A sociotechnical model of media effects. Georgetown Law Technology Review 2: 474–512. [Google Scholar]
  90. Marwick, Alice, and Danah Boyd. 2010. I tweet honestly, I tweet passionately: Twitter users, context collapse, and the imagined audience. New Media & Society 13: 114–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Mayo Clinic. 2024. Feminizing Surgery. Available online: https://www.mayoclinic.org/tests-procedures/feminizing-surgery/about/pac-20385102 (accessed on 28 December 2024).
  92. Mills, Jane, Ann Bonner, and Karen Francis. 2006. The Development of Constructivist Grounded Theory. International Journal of Qualitative Methods 5: 25–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Mines, Diane. 2009. Caste in India. Ann Arbor: Association for Asian Studies. [Google Scholar]
  94. Montag, Christian, Bernd Lachmann, Marc Herrlich, and Katharina Zweig. 2019. Addictive Features of Social Media/Messenger Platforms and Freemium Games against the Background of Psychological and Economic Theories. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 16: 2612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Muller, S Marek, David Rooney, and Cecilia Cerja. 2024. Long live the Liver King: Right-wing carnivorism and the digital dissemination of primal rhetoric. Frontiers in Communication 9: 133863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Nakassis, Constantine. 2016. Doing Style: Youth and Mass Mediation in South India. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  97. Neff, Gina, and Dawn Nafus. 2016. Self-Tracking. Cambridge: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
  98. Niu, Yanzhuo, B. Bradford Brown, Tingting Fan, and Yen Lee. 2024. Shelter in the online world? Benefits and risks associated with social media use for college students’ mental health in the early stage of COVID-19 pandemic. Translational Issues in Psychological Science 10: 216–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. O’Connor, Cailin, and James Owen Weatherall. 2019. The Misinformation Age: How False Beliefs Spread. London: Yale University Press. [Google Scholar]
  100. Pandian, Anand. 2009. Crooked Stalks: Cultivating Virtue in South India. Durham: Duke University Press. [Google Scholar]
  101. Park, Jinkyung Katie, Mamtaj Akter, Pamela Wisniewski, and Karla Badillo-Urquiola. 2024. It’s Still Complicated: From Privacy-Invasive Parental Control to Teen-Centric Solutions for Digital Resilience. IEEE Security & Privacy 22: 52–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Pater, Jessica A., Oliver L. Haimson, Nazanin Andalibi, and Elizabeth D. Mynatt. 2016. “Hunger Hurts but Starving Works”: Characterizing the Presentation of Eating Disorders Online. Paper presented at the 19th ACM Conference on Computer-Supported Cooperative Work & Social Computing (CSCW ’16), San Francisco, CA, USA, February 27–March 2; pp. 1185–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Philip, Kavita. 2004. Civilizing Natures: Race, Resources, and Modernity in Colonial South India. New Jersey: Rutgers University Press. [Google Scholar]
  104. Pike, Kathleen, and Amy Borovoy. 2004. The Rise of Eating Disorders in Japan: Issues of Culture and Limitations of the Model of “westernization”. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry 28: 493–531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  105. Pike, Kathleen, and Patricia Dunne. 2015. The Rise of Eating Disorders in Asia: A Review. Journal of Eating Disorders 3: 33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  106. Poonam, Snigdha. 2018. Dreamers: How Young Indians Are Changing the World. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  107. Raheja, Gloria. 1988. The Poison in the Gift: Ritual, Prestation, and the Dominant Caste in a North Indian Village. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  108. Rao, Anupama. 2009. The Caste Question: Dalits and the Politics of Modern India. Berkeley: University of California Press. [Google Scholar]
  109. Richards, Kevin Andrew, and Michael A. Hemphill. 2018. A practical guide to collaborative qualitative data analysis. Journal of Teaching in Physical Education 37: 225–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Ringland, Katherine, Christine T. Wolf, LouAnne E. Boyd, Mark Baldwin, and Gillian R. Hayes. 2016. Would You Be Mine: Appropriating Minecraft as an Assistive Technology for Youth with Autism. Paper presented at ASSETS ‘16: Proceedings of the 18th International ACM SIGACCESS Conference on Computers and Accessibility, Reno, NV, USA, October 23–26. [Google Scholar]
  111. Ritenbaugh, Cheryl, Catherine Shisslak, Nicolette Teufel, and Tina Leonard-Green. 1996. A cross-cultural review of eating disorders in regard to DSM-IV. In Culture and psychiatric diagnosis: A DSM-IV perspective. Edited by Juan E. Mezzich. Washington: American Psychiatric Press, pp. 171–86. [Google Scholar]
  112. Rosaldo, Renato. 1993. Culture & Truth: The Remaking of Social Analysis. Boston: Beacon Press. [Google Scholar]
  113. Roy, Parama. 2002. Meat–Eating, Masculinity, and Renunciation in India: A Gandhian Grammar of Diet. Gender & History 14: 62–91. [Google Scholar]
  114. Roy, Parama. 2010. Alimentary Tracts: Appetites, Aversions, and the Postcolonial. Durham: Duke University Press. [Google Scholar]
  115. Saldaña, Johnny. 2021. The Coding Manual for Qualitative Researchers, 4th ed. London: Sage. [Google Scholar]
  116. Scarduzio, Jennifer A. 2017. Emic approach to qualitative research. In The International Encyclopedia of Communication Research Methods, 1–2. Edited by Jörg Matthes. New York: John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
  117. Schüll, Natasha D. 2016. Data for life: Wearable technology and the Design of Self-Care. BioSocieties 11: 317–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Showalter, Elaine. 1985. The Female Malady: Women, Madness, and English Culture, 1830–1980. New York: Pantheon. [Google Scholar]
  119. Smith, Gregory T., and Pamela K. Keel. 2017. Contributions from Outstanding Young Eating Disorder Investigators: What We Are Learning, How We Are Learning It, and Where Might We Go Next? Journal of Abnormal Psychology 126: 607–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Solomon, Harris. 2016. Introduction. In Metabolic Living: Food, Fat and the Absorption of Illness in India. Durham: Duke University Press. [Google Scholar]
  121. Stoler, Laura Ann. 2002. Carnal Knowledge and Imperial Power: Race and the Intimate in Colonial Rule. Berkeley: University of California Press. [Google Scholar]
  122. Tregarthen, Jenna, James Lock, and Alison M Darcy. 2015. Development of a Smartphone Application for Eating Disorder Self-Monitoring. International Journal of Eating Disorders 48: 972–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  123. Twigg, Julia. 2011. Modern Asceticism and Contemporary Body Culture. In Beyond Pleasure: Cultures of Modern Asceticism. Edited by Evert Peeters, Leen Van Molle and Kaat Wils. New York: Berghahn Books, pp. 227–44. [Google Scholar]
  124. Unni, Zoya, and Emily Weinstein. 2021. Shelter in Place, Connect Online: Trending TikTok Content During the Early Days of the U.S. COVID-19 Pandemic. Journal of Adolescent Health 68: 863–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  125. Urry, Kristi, Anna Chur-Hansen, and Brett Scholz. 2024. From member checking to collaborative reflection: A novel way to use a familiar method for engaging participants in qualitative research. Qualitative Research in Psychology 21: 357–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  126. Vaidyanathan, Sivapriya, Pooja Patnaik Kuppili, and Vikas Menon. 2019. Eating Disorders: An Overview of Indian Research. Indian Journal of Psychological Medicine 41: 311–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  127. Valente, Thomas, and Patchareeya Pumpuang. 2007. Identifying Opinion Leaders to Promote Behavior Change. Health Education & Behavior 34: 881–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Wang, Tao, Markus Brede, Antonella Ianni, and Emmanouil Mentzakis. 2018. Social Interactions in Online Eating Disorder Communities: A Network Perspective. PLoS ONE 13: E0200800. [Google Scholar]
  129. Warford, Noel, Tara Matthews, Kaitlyn Yang, Omer Akgul, Sunny Consolvo, Patrick Gage Kelley, Nathan Malkin, Michelle L. Mazurek, Manya Sleeper, and Kurt Thomas. 2022. SoK: A Framework for Unifying at-Risk User Research. Paper presented at the Proceedings—43rd IEEE Symposium on Security and Privacy, SP 2022, San Francisco, CA, USA, May 22–26; Interlaken: Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers Inc., pp. 2344–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  130. Warin, Megan. 2010. Abject Relations: Everyday Worlds of Anorexia. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press. [Google Scholar]
  131. Wash, Rick. 2010. Folk models of home computer security. Paper presented at the Sixth Symposium on Usable Privacy and Security, Redmond, WA, USA, July 14–16; pp. 1–16. [Google Scholar]
  132. Weinstein, Emily. 2018. The Social Media See-Saw: Positive and Negative Influences on Adolescents’ Affective Well-Being. New Media & Society 20: 3597–623. [Google Scholar]
  133. Williams, Elizabeth. 2020. Appetite and Its Discontents: Science, Medicine, and the Urge to Eat, 1750–1950. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  134. Wolf, Naomi. 1990. The Beauty Myth. London: Chatto and Windus. [Google Scholar]
  135. Wu, Jiayuan, Jie Liu, Shasha Li, Huan Ma, and Yufeng Wang. 2020. Trends in the Prevalence and Disability-Adjusted Life Years of Eating Disorders from 1990 to 2017: Results from the Global Burden of Disease Study 2017. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences 29: e191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  136. Xu, Rachel, Nhu Le, Rebekah Park, and Laura Cecilia Murray. 2024. Like-minded, like-bodied: How users (18–26) trust online eating and health information. Paper presented at the Extended Abstracts of the CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (CHI EA ’24), Honolulu, HI, USA, May 11–16, Volume 227, pp. 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  137. Yamamiya, Yuko, Thomas F. Cash, Susan E. Melnyk, Heidi D. Posavac, and Steven S. Posavac. 2005. Women’s exposure to thin-and-beautiful media images: Body image effects of media-ideal internalization and impact-reduction interventions. Body Image 2: 74–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  138. Yeshua-Katz, Daphna, and Nicole Martins. 2013. Communicating Stigma: The Pro-Ana Paradox. Health Communication 28: 499–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  139. Yuan, Yunhao, Koustuv Saha, Barbara Keller, Erkki T Isometsä, and Talayeh Aledavood. 2023. Mental Health Coping Stories on Social Media: A Causal-Inference Study of Papageno Effect. Paper presented at the ACM Web Conference 2023, Austin, TX, USA, April 30–May 4; pp. 2677–85. [Google Scholar]
  140. Zhang, Cindy, Grayden Zaleski, Jaya N. Kailley, Katelyn A. Teng, Mahala English, Anna Riminchan, and Julie M. Robillard. 2024. Debate: Social Media Content Moderation May Do More Harm Than Good for Youth Mental Health. Child and Adolescent Mental Health 29: 104–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  141. Zook, Darren. 2000. Famine in the Landscape: Imagining Hunger in South Asian History, 1860–1990. In Agrarian Environments. Edited by Arun Agrawal and Kalyanakrishnan Sivaramakrishnan. Durham: Duke University Press. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Arjun’s (23, India) user journey.
Figure 1. Arjun’s (23, India) user journey.
Socsci 14 00230 g001
Figure 2. Video from diet creator, where he claimed that drinking coffee and lemon accelerates weight loss.
Figure 2. Video from diet creator, where he claimed that drinking coffee and lemon accelerates weight loss.
Socsci 14 00230 g002
Figure 3. Comments on creator’s video from Figure 2 are used as an interactive forum to determine trustworthiness.
Figure 3. Comments on creator’s video from Figure 2 are used as an interactive forum to determine trustworthiness.
Socsci 14 00230 g003
Figure 4. Brad (22, US)’s user journey.
Figure 4. Brad (22, US)’s user journey.
Socsci 14 00230 g004
Figure 5. Priya (24, Delhi)’s user journey.
Figure 5. Priya (24, Delhi)’s user journey.
Socsci 14 00230 g005
Figure 6. Shivani (24, India)’s user journey.
Figure 6. Shivani (24, India)’s user journey.
Socsci 14 00230 g006
Figure 7. Examples of content participants identified as helpful.
Figure 7. Examples of content participants identified as helpful.
Socsci 14 00230 g007
Figure 8. Examples of content participants identified as harmful.
Figure 8. Examples of content participants identified as harmful.
Socsci 14 00230 g008
Figure 9. Anil (22, India)’s user journey.
Figure 9. Anil (22, India)’s user journey.
Socsci 14 00230 g009
Table 1. Study 2 participant location.
Table 1. Study 2 participant location.
UrbanSuburbanSemi-RuralTotal
USA115521
India153321
Total268842
Table 2. Study 2 participant gender identity.
Table 2. Study 2 participant gender identity.
Male-IdentifyingFemale-IdentifyingTransNon BinaryTotal
USA5104221
India1011--21
Total15214242
Table 3. Study 1 findings.
Table 3. Study 1 findings.
ChallengesPracticesIn Study 2
Information Overload
Too many articles, facts, opinions and ads, often too wordy, too long, and too negative
Surrogate Thinking
Finding a trusted go-to source and relying on the source’s opinion
Yes
Misrecognition
Lack of ability to speak to their generation, seen in tone, lack of controls, vibes
Everyday Experience
Privileging on-the-ground crowdsourced reporting, lived experience, first-person narratives
Yes
Social Error
Fear of making mistakes, taking unpopular positions, or actions that lead to social harm
Crowdsourcing Credibility
Using comments, likes, and other peer discussions to evaluate content
Yes
Good Enough Reasoning
Limiting effort to finding just enough—a few facts, numbers, whatever is easy—and then moving on
No
Table 4. Study 2 findings.
Table 4. Study 2 findings.
ChallengesPracticesStudy 1 Equivalent
Misrecognition by Overgeneralization
Sense that institutions do not understand or serve their specific health needs
n = 1 thinking
Assessing health information based on personal testimony of like-bodied people online rather than probabilistic measures
Surrogate thinking, everyday experience
Distributed Trust
Trusting identity- or interest-based community opinion (sourced via likes and comments) rather than centralized institutional authority
Crowdsourcing credibility
Individualistic Conformity
Desire to conform to social norms, but needing to appear like conformity was not important
The “good life”
Signaling “holistic health” rather than thinness, but still requires performance of bodily conformity to hard-to-attain cultural norms
Response to fear of social error
Meaningful Connection
Craving deep connection with others and a sense of social belonging
Getting offline
Privileging online over offline sociality
Response to general desire for social belonging (over truth seeking)
Table 5. Tested Interventions and Corresponding Participant Needs and Practices.
Table 5. Tested Interventions and Corresponding Participant Needs and Practices.
InterventionNeeds ClusterPractices (Study 1 or 2)
View As
See through the eyes of trusted creators
DiscernmentSurrogate thinking (1)
Good enough reasoning (1)
Explorer Remix
Browse outside your algorithm
DiversificationSurrogate thinking (1)
Good enough reasoning (1)
Search Together
Search results tailored for your communities
Social learningCrowdsourcing credibility (1)
Distributed trust (2)
Getting offline (2)
Personal Safe Search
Filter out unwanted search terms
Self-protection
Location-Based Search
Filter search results specific to your region
PersonalizationCrowdsourcing credibility (1)
n = 1 thinking (2)
Table 6. Tested interventions and aggregated participant feedback. We list interventions with largely negative feedback with a negative sign (−) mixed-to-medium feedback with a plus/minus sign (+/−), and positive feedback with a plus sign (+).
Table 6. Tested interventions and aggregated participant feedback. We list interventions with largely negative feedback with a negative sign (−) mixed-to-medium feedback with a plus/minus sign (+/−), and positive feedback with a plus sign (+).
InterventionParticipant Feedback
View As
See the feeds of trusted creators
Didn’t care about knowing more about the creator than they were already exposed to(other than if the creator was monetized or not).
Explorer Remix
Browse outside your algorithm
Felt that content outside of their algorithms would be irrelevant to them.
Search Together +/−
Search results tailored for your communities
Liked interacting online, but ultimately wanted the intervention to help them meet offline.
Personal Safe Search +/−
Filter out select search terms
US users wanted to tailor their results against potential triggers. Indian users did not have the same discourse around triggering content. Participants largely encountered information. When searching, most wanted more good information, not less bad information; they wanted to use filters to help find better content.
Location-Based Search +
Filter search results specific to your region
Liked opting into searches based on region, but wanted to be able to adjust location on a sliding scale. Felt someone from the same region was more likely to be like-minded and like-bodied.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Hassoun, A.; Beacock, I.; Carmody, T.; Kelley, P.G.; Goldberg, B.; Kumar, D.; Murray, L.; Park, R.S.; Sarmadi, B.; Consolvo, S. Beyond Digital Literacy: Building Youth Digital Resilience Through Existing “Information Sensibility” Practices. Soc. Sci. 2025, 14, 230. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci14040230

AMA Style

Hassoun A, Beacock I, Carmody T, Kelley PG, Goldberg B, Kumar D, Murray L, Park RS, Sarmadi B, Consolvo S. Beyond Digital Literacy: Building Youth Digital Resilience Through Existing “Information Sensibility” Practices. Social Sciences. 2025; 14(4):230. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci14040230

Chicago/Turabian Style

Hassoun, Amelia, Ian Beacock, Todd Carmody, Patrick Gage Kelley, Beth Goldberg, Devika Kumar, Laura Murray, Rebekah Su Park, Behzad Sarmadi, and Sunny Consolvo. 2025. "Beyond Digital Literacy: Building Youth Digital Resilience Through Existing “Information Sensibility” Practices" Social Sciences 14, no. 4: 230. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci14040230

APA Style

Hassoun, A., Beacock, I., Carmody, T., Kelley, P. G., Goldberg, B., Kumar, D., Murray, L., Park, R. S., Sarmadi, B., & Consolvo, S. (2025). Beyond Digital Literacy: Building Youth Digital Resilience Through Existing “Information Sensibility” Practices. Social Sciences, 14(4), 230. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci14040230

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop