Digital Shakespeare Is Neither Good Nor Bad, But Teaching Makes It So
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. The Dumbest (and Most Distracted) Generations?
Obviously, such deficiencies affect the individual student attempting to grapple with Shakespeare’s already difficult text but what may not be clear is how multitasking can also damage the literature classroom as a whole. We have known for more than a decade, for example, that heavy media multitaskers have a much more difficult time filtering out irrelevant information (Ophir et al. 2009). For those students reading a Shakespearean play or poem, the inability to identify pertinent characters and information will cripple their understanding. As important as it may be to understand what a “changeling” is in A Midsummer Night’s Dream, the student who does not recognize that the changeling boy is at the center of the disagreement between Titania and Oberon (or even that there is a disagreement) will fundamentally misunderstand an essential element of the plot. Some instructors of Shakespeare’s work struggle to make the text more intelligible to students of differing abilities, levels, and learning styles and most are familiar with social media’s detrimental effect on student comprehension and attention but many professors have given up the fight to reduce social media distractions in class. They allow phones and other personal technology into the classroom,4 fully understanding that some students will abuse the privilege but mistakenly believing that only that student’s learning experience will be diminished. In fact, one single student watching cat videos in a classroom can have a remarkably harmful impact on the students around her.The research is almost unanimous, which is very rare in social science and it says that people who chronically multitask show an enormous range of deficits. They’re basically terrible at all sorts of cognitive tasks, including multitasking. […] People who multitask all the time can’t filter out irrelevancy. They can’t manage a working memory. They’re chronically distracted. They initiate much larger parts of their brain that are irrelevant to the task at hand. And even—they’re even terrible at multitasking. When we ask them to multitask, they’re actually worse at it. So they’re pretty much mental wrecks.
3. Reading Digital Texts
4. Reading Complex Texts
Complex texts, such as those by Shakespeare, need different reading strategies than the more simplistic, information-driven texts that most students are accustomed to encountering. For that reason, I always begin my Shakespeare courses with a close reading exercise designed to help them acclimate to the demands of college-level reading and thinking.What psychologists and brain scientists tell us about interruptions is that they have a fairly profound effect on the way we think. It becomes much harder to sustain attention, to think about one thing for a long period of time and to think deeply when new stimuli are pouring at you all day long. I argue that the price we pay for being constantly inundated with information is a loss of our ability to be contemplative and to engage in the kind of deep thinking that requires you to concentrate on one thing.
Intuitively, one would think that an internet-enabled e-reader would be perfect for reading such texts. With built-in dictionaries, access to online encyclopedias, and searchable functionality, an e-reader seems ideal for dealing with the intricacy of something like Shakespeare. Unfortunately, the availability of such information in the e-reader actually makes it too easy to find. Students encounter an unfamiliar word or reference, click once or twice to find what it means, plug it into the immediate context, and move on, immediately forgetting the new information and never pausing to think deeply about it.Complex texts require a slower labor. Readers can’t proceed to the next paragraph without grasping the previous one, they can’t glide over unfamiliar words and phrases and they can’t forget what they read four pages earlier. They must double back, discern ambiguities, follow tricky transitions and keep a dictionary close at hand. Complex texts force readers to acquire the knack of slow linear reading.
The information in a Shakespearean play or poem is not like online news or scientific material or commodified data-points. It has the potential to connect students to a vast and fascinating world outside themselves but not if they are reading too quickly or too sloppily. If the information in Shakespeare’s text becomes analogous in the eyes of the students to that of a Wikipedia entry or a Yelp review or a Facebook feed, then they will swipe past myriad beautiful and revealing passages as they search for the next shiny tidbit of interest.Thus far we have suggested that the case for slow reading starts with questioning the nature of information. We’ve seen that the popular definition of information as a quantifiable piece of communication has resulted in an ongoing quest to increase the speed and lower the cost, of communicating. Information has become a commodity to be consumed in pursuit of some separate goal. The assumption that faster, more efficient information is better information has been largely unquestioned. However, this assumption can be challenged as we explore the ways in which information connects us to a world outside of ourselves and contributes to (or detracts from) our flourishing.
TLDR (“too long, didn’t read”) has become internet shorthand used by many digital readers to justify skipping over longer online comments or responses but these entries are usually just a fraction of the length of a Shakespearean text (often shorter than the average soliloquy) and almost certainly not as intellectually dense. How then do Shakespearean instructors train today’s digitally inundated students to work hard, read carefully, think deeply, and stay focused?I am worried about kids who are immersed in digital culture. They will get to college and they will have been Twittering so much that they won’t have the patience to read those really long cognitively convoluted and complex sentences. They may not have developed those rich networks which are required in order to read at a high level of sophistication. […] The effort is what we are going to lose. They are becoming not so much a lazy reader but an atrophied reader.
5. Digital Shakespeare
Funding
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Ackerman, Rakefet, and Morris Goldsmith. 2011. Metacognitive Regulation of Text Learning: On Screen Versus on Paper. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 17: 18–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ackerman, Rakefet, and Tirza Lauterman. 2012. Taking Reading Comprehension Exams on Screen or on Paper? A Metacognitive Analysis of Learning Texts under Time Pressure. Computers in Human Behavior 28: 1816–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ailles, Jennifer. 2014. ‘Is there an app for that?’: Mobile Shakespeare on the Phone and in the Cloud. In OuterSpeares: Shakespeare, Intermedia and the Limits of Adaptation. Edited by Daniel Fischlin. Toronto: University of Toronto Press, pp. 75–112. [Google Scholar]
- Baker, Mark C. 2001. The Atoms of Language: The Mind’s Hidden Rules of Grammar. New York: Basic Books. [Google Scholar]
- Barak, Miri, Alberta Lipson, and Steven Lerman. 2006. Wireless Laptops as Means for Promoting Active Learning in Large Lecture Halls. Journal of Research on Technology in Education 38: 245–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baron, Naomi S. 2015. Words Onscreen: The Fate of Reading in a Digital World. Oxford: Oxford UP. [Google Scholar]
- Baron, Naomi S. 2018. Do Students Lose Depth in Digital Reading? Paper or Tablet? Salon.com. August 13. Available online: https://www.salon.com/2018/08/12/do-students-lose-depth-in-digital-reading_partner/ (accessed on 4 April 2019).
- Bauerlein, Mark. 2008. The Dumbest Generation: How the Digital Age Stupefies Young Americans and Jeopardizes Our Future (Or, Don’t Trust Anyone Under 30). New York: Tarcher/Penguin. [Google Scholar]
- Bauerlein, Mark. 2011. Too Dumb for Complex Texts? Educational Leadership 68: 28–32. [Google Scholar]
- Beetham, Helen, and Rhona Sharpe. 2013. An Introduction to Rethinking Pedagogy. In Rethinking Pedagogy for a Digital Age: Designing for 21st Century Learning, 2nd ed. Edited by Helen Beetham and Rhona Sharpe. New York: Routledge, pp. 1–10. [Google Scholar]
- Biancarosa, Gina, and Gina G. Griffiths. 2012. Technology Tools to Support Reading in the Digital Age. The Future of Children 22: 139–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bloom, Gina. 2015. Videogame Shakespeare: Enskilling Audiences through Theater-Making Games. In Shakespeare Studies. Edited by James R. Siemon and Diana E. Henderson. Madison: Fairleigh Dickinson University Press, vol. 43, pp. 114–27. [Google Scholar]
- Bogost, Ian. 2012. Alien Phenomenology: Or, What It’s Like to Be a Thing. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. [Google Scholar]
- Brown, Emery N., and Marlene Behrmann. 2017. Controversy in Statistical Analysis of Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Data. PNAS (Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America) 114: E3368–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Buchweitz, Augusto. 2016. Language and Reading Development in the Brain Today: Neuromarkers and the Case for Prediction. Jornal de Pediatria 92 S1: S8–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Burt, Richard. 2003. Shakespeare, ‘Glo-cali-zation,’ Race and the Small Screens of Post-Popular Culture. In Shakespeare, the Movie II: Popularizing the Plays on Film, TV, Video and DVD. Edited by Richard Burt and Lynda E. Boose. London: Routledge, pp. 14–36. [Google Scholar]
- Carr, Nicholas. 2008. Is Google Making Us Stupid? What the Internet is Doing to our Brains. TheAtlantic.com. Available online: https://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/2008/07/is-google-making-us-stupid/306868/ (accessed on 4 April 2019).
- Carr, Nicholas. 2010. The Shallows: What the Internet is Doing to Our Brains. New York: Norton. [Google Scholar]
- Casey, Jim. 2017. HypeRomeo & Juliet: Postmodern Adaptation and Shakespeare. In Shakespeare/Not Shakespeare. Edited by Christy Desmet, Natalie Loper and Jim Casey. New York: Palgrave Macmillan, pp. 59–75. [Google Scholar]
- Casey, Jim. 2020. Romeo and Juliet: Cue Scripts Exercise. In How to Teach a Play: Essential Exercises for Popular Plays. Edited by Miriam Chirico and Kelly Younger. New York: Bloomsbury. [Google Scholar]
- Cuban, Larry, Heather Kirkpatrick, and Craig Peck. 2001. High Access and Low Use of Technology in High School Classrooms: Explaining an Apparent Paradox. American Educational Research Journal 38: 813–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cull, Barry W. 2011. Reading Revolutions: Online Digital Text and Implications for Reading in Academe. First Monday 16. Available online: https://firstmonday.org/ojs/index.php/fm/article/view/3340/2985 (accessed on 4 April 2019).
- Culler, Jonathan. 2010. The Closeness of Close Reading. ADE Bulletin 149: 20–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Curtis, Debra Moss, and Judith R. Karp. 2007. In a Case, On the Screen, Do They Remember What They’ve Seen? Critical Electronic Reading in the Law Classroom. Hamline Law Review 30: 247–84. [Google Scholar]
- Daniel, David B., and William Douglas Woody. 2013. E-textbooks at What Cost? Performance and Use of Electronic v. Print Texts. Computers & Education 62: 18–23. [Google Scholar]
- Desmet, Christy. 2017. Alien Shakespeares 2.0. Actes des Congrès de la Société Française Shakespeare 35 [Shakespeare après Shakespeare (Shakespeare after Shakespeare): Shakespeare à l’écran (Shakespeare on Screen)]. Available online: https://journals.openedition.org/shakespeare/3877 (accessed on 4 April 2019).
- Desmet, Christy, Natalie Loper, and Jim Casey, eds. 2017. Shakespeare/Not Shakespeare. New York: Palgrave Macmillan. [Google Scholar]
- Drouin, Michelle, Daren H. Kaiser, and Daniel A. Miller. 2012. Phantom Vibrations among Undergraduates: Prevalence and Associated Psychological Characteristics. Computers in Human Behavior 28: 1490–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eden, Sigal, and Yoram Eshet-Alkalai. 2013. The Effect of Format on Performance: Editing Text in Print versus Digital Formats. British Journal of Educational Technology 44: 846–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eklund, Anders, Thomas E. Nichols, and Hans Knutsson. 2016. Cluster Failure: Why fMRI Inferences for Spatial Extent have Inflated False-Positive Rates. PNAS (Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America) 113: 7900–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Felt, Laurel J., and Michael B. Robb. 2016. Technology Addiction: Concern, Controversy and Finding Balance [Executive Summary]. San Francisco: Common Sense Media. [Google Scholar]
- Flood, Alison. 2014. Readers Absorb Less on Kindles Than on Paper, Study Finds. The Guardian. August 19. Available online: https://www.theguardian.com/books/2014/aug/19/readers-absorb-less-kindles-paper-study-plot-ereader-digitisation (accessed on 4 April 2019).
- Gazzaley, Adam, and Larry D. Rosen. 2016. The Distracted Mind: Ancient Brains in a High-Tech World. Cambridge: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Gee, James Paul. 2004. Situated Language and Learning: A Critique of Traditional Schooling. New York: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Gee, James Paul. 2018. Affinity Spaces: How Young People Live and Learn Online and Out of School. Phi Delta Kappan 99: 8–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gray, Lucinda, Nina Thomas, and Laurie Lewis. 2010. Teachers’ Use of Educational Technology in U.S. Public Schools: 2009; NCES 2010-040. Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. Available online: https://nces.ed.gov/pubs2010/2010040.pdf (accessed on 4 April 2019).
- Gregoire, Carolyn. 2015. The Internet May Be Changing Your Brain in Ways You’ve Never Imagined. HuffingtonPost.com. October 9. Available online: https://www.huffingtonpost.com/entry/internet-changing-brain-nicholas-carr_us_5614037de4b0368a1a613e96?guccounter=1&guce_referrer_us=aHR0cHM6Ly93d3cuZ29vZ2xlLmNvLmpwLw&guce_referrer_cs=G6waPLZSU5CS8sb4Pr_4_A (accessed on 4 April 2019).
- Hart Research Associates. 2013. It Takes More Than a Major: Employer Priorities for College Learning and Student Success. Washington: Association of American Colleges and Universities and Hart Research Associates. [Google Scholar]
- Hayles, N. Katherine. 2012. How We Think: Digital Media and Contemporary Technogenesis. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- Hembrooke, Helene, and Geri Gay. 2003. The Laptop and the Lecture: The Effects of Multitasking in Learning Environments. Journal of Computing in Higher Education 15: 46–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Holl, Jennifer. 2017. YouShakespeare: Shakespearean Celebrity 2.0. In Shakespeare/Not Shakespeare. Edited by Christy Desmet, Natalie Loper and Jim Casey. New York: Palgrave Macmillan, pp. 203–19. [Google Scholar]
- Holmes, Jeffrey. 2009. Education Futures: University Grads Don’t Make the Grade. Melbourne: Woods Bagot. [Google Scholar]
- Jacobs, Claris, Elsie Bollinger, Minnie Grace, and Sally Bollinger [The Candle Wasters]. 2014. Nothing Much to Do. Webseries. YouTube. Available online: https://www.youtube.com/channel/UCFkxbSqhdwL8OjrTCwwELpQ (accessed on 4 April 2019).
- Jenkins, Henry, Katie Clinton, Ravi Purushotma, Alice J. Robison, and Margaret Weigel. 2006. Confronting the Challenges of Participatory Culture: Media Education for the 21st Century. MacArthur Foundation White Paper. Chicago: John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation. Available online: https://www.macfound.org/media/article_pdfs/JENKINS_WHITE_PAPER.PDF (accessed on 4 April 2019).
- Kaufman, Geoff, and Mary Flanagan. 2016. High-Low Split: Divergent Cognitive Construal Levels Triggered by Digital and Non-Digital Platforms. In CHI ’16: Proceedings of the 2016 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. New York: Association for Computing Machinery, pp. 2773–77. [Google Scholar]
- Kessler, Daniel, Mike Angstadt, and Chandra S. Sripada. 2017. Reevaluating ‘Cluster Failure’ in fMRI using Nonparametric Control of the False Discovery Rate. PNAS (Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America) 114: E3372–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kraushaar, James M., and David C. Novak. 2010. Examining the Effects of Student Multitasking with Laptops During the Lecture. Journal of Information Systems Education 21: 241–51. [Google Scholar]
- Lanier, Douglas. 2014. Shakespearean Rhizomatics: Adaptation, Ethics, Value. In Shakespeare and the Ethics of Appropriation. Edited by Alexa Huang and Elizabeth Rivlin. New York: Palgrave Macmillan, pp. 21–40. [Google Scholar]
- Lavagnino, John. 2014. Shakespeare in the Digital Humanities. In Shakespeare and the Digital World: Redefining Scholarship and Practice. Edited by Christie Carson and Peter Kirwan. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 14–23. [Google Scholar]
- Lehmann, Courtney. 2003. Out Damned Scot: Dislocating Macbeth in Transnational Film and Media Culture. In Shakespeare the Movie II: Popularizing the Plays on Film, TV, Video and DVD. Edited by Richart Burt and Lynda E. Boose. New York: Routledge, pp. 231–51. [Google Scholar]
- Liu, Alan. 2008. Re-Doing Literary Interpretation: A Pedagogy. Currents in Electronic Literacy. Available online: https://currents.dwrl.utexas.edu/Spring08/Liu.html (accessed on 4 April 2019).
- Mangen, Anne, Bente Walgermo, and Kolbjørn Brønnick. 2013. Reading Linear Texts on Paper Versus Computer Screen: Effects on Reading Comprehension. International Journal of Educational Research 58: 61–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Margolin, Sara J., Casey Driscoll, Michael J. Toland, and Jennifer Little Kegler. 2013. E-readers, Computer Screens or Paper: Does Reading Comprehension Change Across Media Platforms? Applied Cognitive Psychology 27: 512–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maxwell, Julie, and Kate Rumbold, eds. 2018. Shakespeare and Quotation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Middleton, Peter. 2005. Distant Reading: Performance, Readership and Consumption in Contemporary Poetry. Tuscaloosa: U of Alabama P. [Google Scholar]
- Nakamura, Kimihiro, Wen-Jui Kuo, Felipe Pegado, Laurent Cohen, Ovid J. L. Tzeng, and Stanislas Dehaene. 2012. Universal Brain Systems for Recognizing Word Shapes and Handwriting Gestures During Reading. PNAS (Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America) 109: 20762–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nass, Clifford. 2013. The Myth of Multitasking. Interviewed by Ira Flatow. Science Friday/Talk of the Nation. May 10. Available online: https://www.npr.org/2013/05/10/182861382/the-myth-of-multitasking (accessed on 4 April 2019).
- Nielsen, Jakob. 2006. F-Shaped Pattern for Reading Web Content. NNGroup.com [NN/g Nielsen Norman Group]. April 17. Available online: https://www.nngroup.com/articles/f-shaped-pattern-reading-web-content-discovered/ (accessed on 4 April 2019).
- O’Dair, Sharon. 2014. All’s Well that Ends Orwell. In Shakespeare and the Digital World: Redefining Scholarship and Practice. Edited by Christie Carson and Peter Kirwan. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 115–26. [Google Scholar]
- O’Neill, Stephen. 2014. Shakespeare and YouTube: New Media Forms of the Bard. London: Bloomsbury. [Google Scholar]
- O’Neill, Stephen. 2015. Shakespeare and Social Media. Literature Compass 12: 274–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ophir, Eyal, Clifford Nass, and Anthony D. Wagner. 2009. Cognitive Control in Media Multitaskers. PNAS [Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America] 106: 15583–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ostercamp, Matthew. 2014. A Case for Slow Reading. Theological Librarianship 7: 9–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pernice, Kara. 2017. F-Shaped Pattern of Reading on the Web: Misunderstood but Still Relevant (Even on Mobile). NNGroup.com [NN/g Nielsen Norman Group]. November 12. Available online: https://www.nngroup.com/articles/f-shaped-pattern-reading-web-content/ (accessed on 4 April 2019).
- Rosenwasser, David, and Jill Stephen. 2015. Writing Analytically, 7th ed. Stamford: Cengage. [Google Scholar]
- Rowe, Katherine. 2014. Living with Digital Incunables or a ‘Good-Enough’ Shakespeare Text. In Shakespeare and the Digital World: Redefining Scholarship and Practice. Edited by Christie Carson and Peter Kirwan. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 144–59. [Google Scholar]
- Sana, Faria, Tina Weston, and Nicholas J. Cepeda. 2013. Laptop Multitasking Hinders Classroom Learning for Both Users and Nearby Peers. Computers & Education 62: 24–31. [Google Scholar]
- Sanders, Laura. 2009. Trawling the Brain: New Findings Raise Questions about Reliability of fMRI as Gauge of Neural Activity. Science News 176: 16–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Santamaria, Michele R., and Kathryn M. Moncrief. 2016. Metacognition Meets Research-Based Learning in the Undergraduate Renaissance Drama Classroom. In Metaliteracy in Practice. Edited by Trudie E. Jacobson and Thomas P. Mackey. Chicago: Neal-Schuman [American Library Association], pp. 113–34. [Google Scholar]
- Schugar, Jordan T., Heather Schugar, and Christian Penny. 2011. A Nook or a Book? Comparing College Students’ Reading Comprehension Levels, Critical Reading and Study Skills. International Journal of Technology in Teaching and Learning 7: 174–92. [Google Scholar]
- Small, Gary, and Gigi Vorgan. 2008. iBrain: Surviving the Technological Alteration of the Modern Mind. New York: Harper. [Google Scholar]
- Sparrow, Betsy, Jenny Liu, and Daniel M. Wegner. 2011. Google Effects on Memory: Cognitive Consequences of Having Information at Our Fingertips. Science 333: 776–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Storm, Benjamin C., Sean M. Stone, and Aaron S. Benjamin. 2017. Using the Internet to Access Information Inflates Future Use of the Internet to Access Other Information. Memory 25: 717–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thompson, Ayanna. 2011. Passing Strange: Shakespeare, Race and Contemporary America. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Wallace, Kelly. 2015. Teens Spend a ‘Mind-Boggling’ 9 Hours a Day Using Media, Report Says. CNN.com. November 4. Available online: https://edition.cnn.com/2015/11/03/health/teens-tweens-media-screen-use-report/ (accessed on 4 April 2019).
- Walsh, Maureen, Jennifer Asha, and Nicole Sprainger. 2007. Reading Digital Texts. Australian Journal of Language and Literacy 30: 40–53. [Google Scholar]
- Ward, Adrian F., Kristen Duke, Ayelet Gneezy, and Maarten W. Bos. 2017. Brain Drain: The Mere Presence of One’s Own Smartphone Reduces Available Cognitive Capacity. Journal of the Association for Consumer Research 2: 140–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Watson, Jason M., and David L. Strayer. 2010. Supertaskers: Profiles in Extraordinary Multitasking Ability. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 17: 479–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wolf, Maryanne. 2008. Proust and the Squid: The Story and Science of the Reading Brain. New York: Harper Perennial. [Google Scholar]
- Wood, Eileen, Lucia Zivcakova, Petrice Gentile, Karin Archer, Domenica De Pasquale, and Amanda Nosko. 2012. Examining the Impact of Off-task Multitasking with Technology on Real-time Classroom Learning. Computers & Education 58: 365–74. [Google Scholar]
- Worthen, William B. 1997. Shakespeare and the Authority of Performance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Worthen, William B. 2014. Shakespeare Performance Studies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Young, Jackie. 2014. A Study of Print and Computer-Based Reading to Measure and Compare Rates of Comprehension and Retention. New Library World 115: 376–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
1 | Gina Biancarosa and Gina Griffiths note that the evidence regarding the effective training and support of e-reading technology use is “in short supply. Teachers most commonly report that what prepared them to make effective use of technology for instruction was not training but independent learning” (Biancarosa and Griffiths 2012, p. 150). For similar investigations, see Lucinda Gray, Nina Thomas, and Laurie Lewis’ U.S. Department of Education report Teachers’ Use of Educational Technology in U.S. Public Schools (Gray et al. 2010) and Larry Cuban, Heather Kirkpatrick, and Craig Peck’s “High Access and Low Use of Technology in High School Classrooms” (Cuban et al. 2001). |
2 | For a good summary of the history of the digital humanities in connection to Shakespeare, please see John Lavagnino’s “Shakespeare in the Digital Humanities” (Lavagnino 2014); as an incomplete introduction to recent studies in the field, see also (Ailles 2014; Bloom 2015; Desmet 2017; Holl 2017; O’Dair 2014; O’Neill 2014, 2015; Rowe 2014; Santamaria and Moncrief 2016). |
3 | It is important to note here that Bauerlein does not consider Millennials to be lacking in innate intelligence or potential; as he explained to some of my classes that he so generously agreed to Skype with, Bauerlein finds today’s students lacking in preparation (especially in history, civics, and literature), uninterested in “adult” matters, and continually distracted by social media. |
4 | Some teachers have attempted to co-opt mobile apps, social media, and videogames as learning resources in the service of the Shakespearean classroom; see Jennifer Ailles’ “Is there an app for that?” (Ailles 2014), Stephen O’Neill’s “Shakespeare and Social Media” (O’Neill 2015) and Gina Bloom’s “Videogame Shakespeare” (Bloom 2015). |
5 | See, for example, (Barak et al. 2006; Hembrooke and Gay 2003; Kraushaar and Novak 2010; Wood et al. 2012). |
6 | For more on Google’s damaging effects on brain function, please see Gary Small and Gigi Vorgan’s iBrain (Small and Vorgan 2008), Betsy Sparrow, Jenny Liu, and Daniel Wegner’s “Google Effects on Memory” (Sparrow et al. 2011) and Adam Gazzaley and Larry Rosen’s The Distracted Mind (Gazzaley and Rosen 2016). In “Cluster Failure: Why fMRI Inferences for Spatial Extent have Inflated False-Positive Rates,” Anders Eklund, Thomas Nichols and Hans Knutsson critique the use of fMRI (functional magnetic resonance imaging) data in studies such as these, claiming that flawed statistical methods in fMRI data evaluation invalidates the results (Eklund et al. 2016, [with their correction later that year in PNAS 113, no. 33 (August): E4929, removing some of the broader claims and high numbers of potential impact]). In turn, the accuracy of Eklund, Nichols, and Knutsson’s work has been challenged by Emery Brown and Marlene Behrmann’s “Controversy in Statistical Analysis of Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Data” (Brown and Behrmann 2017) and Daniel Kessler, Mike Angstadt, and Chandra Sripada’s “Reevaluating ‘Cluster Failure’ in fMRI using Nonparametric Control of the False Discovery Rate” (Kessler et al. 2017). Laura Sanders discusses the concerns of Eklund, Nichols, and Knutsson in “Trawling the Brain,” (Sanders 2009) and Katherine Hayles’ How We Think provides broader criticisms of the assumptions and methodologies associated with fMRI brain studies (Hayles 2012, pp. 67–68). |
7 | The assertion that the brain may be hardwired with specific neural pathways for language is supported by both neuroscience and linguistics; for more, see Augusto Buchweitz’s “Language and Reading Development in the Brain Today” (Buchweitz 2016), Mark Baker’s The Atoms of Language (Baker 2001) and Kimihiro Nakamura, Wen-Jui Kuo, Felipe Pegado, Laurent Cohen, Ovid J. L. Tzeng, and Stanislas Dehaene’s “Universal Brain Systems for Recognizing Word Shapes and Handwriting Gestures During Reading” (Nakamura et al. 2012). |
8 | Naomi Baron notes that “it has become common to let actual eBooks dominate the conversation about reading on digital screens. Of course, there is more involved: Facebook updates, movie reviews, blogs, scholarly articles” (Baron 2015, p. 6). Because this essay deals with reading as it relates to the classroom, I will not discuss the kinds of reading practices connected to leisure and social media activities. Students may be reading and writing more than ever before on platforms such as Twitter but I am interested in teaching deep reading and critical thinking and Twitter promotes the opposite of both. I do admit, however, that I have conflated at times the effects of reading on a computer with reading on an e-book. As research progresses, these two reading experiences should be separated but for now the transference of reading skills and strategies from online to e-book appear to be similar enough for us to talk about them together. |
9 | In contrast to these studies, Anne Mangen, Bente Walgermo, and Kolbjørn Brønnick also examined the differences in reading comprehension when reading in print versus digitally (Mangen et al. 2013) and found that “students who read texts in print scored significantly better on the reading comprehension test than students who read the texts digitally” (Flood 2014). |
10 | This is based on a 2013 survey conducted by Hart Research Associates for the Association of American Colleges and Universities in which 93% of employers stated that “a candidate’s demonstrated capacity to think critically, communicate clearly, and solve complex programs is more important than his or her undergraduate field of study” (Hart Research Associates 2013, p. 4). A 2009 study conducted by the Global Strategy Group for the architecture and consulting studio Woods Bagot returned similar results, with employers listing as their four most desired attributes problem-solving skills, the ability to work as a team, critical thinking, and written communication skills (Holmes 2009, p. 7). |
11 | For these students, I usually recommend using a summary to identify the characters and understand the basic plot and then reading along with an audio recording or following a taped performance in order to better understand what is being said. |
12 | I have stolen this phrasing of “10 on 1” rather than “1 on 10” from David Rosenwasser and Jill Stephen’s excellent textbook Writing Analytically (Rosenwasser and Stephen 2015, pp. 103–15). |
13 | Some might see Nothing Much To Do and the “Wrecking Ball” Hamlets as extensions of the long history of quoting Shakespeare outlined in Julie Maxwell and Kate Rumbold’s Shakespeare and Quotation (Maxwell and Rumbold 2018) but in most cases the text itself is not actually quoted but instead alluded to through other modernized language. |
14 | I should clarify that these suggestions are mainly for Shakespeare-as-literature courses, not for theatrical performance courses or other topics, such as a course on Shakespearean adaptations. For more information on how I might teach a Shakespearean play in a performance setting, please see my forthcoming “Romeo and Juliet: Cue Scripts Exercise” (Casey 2020); for more information on the various approaches to (and theoretical frames for) Shakespearean adaptation, please see the collection of essays, Shakespeare/Not Shakespeare, that I coedited with Christy Desmet and Natalie Loper (Desmet et al. 2017). |
15 | I always make these film screenings required. Colleagues tell me that they can never get their students to attend because they have so many other commitments. I tell my students that they can miss but they will have to watch the film on their own before we discuss it in class (and that they will be be quizzed for a grade on it) plus they must attend two additional university events approved by me to make up for missing—usually public readings by visiting authors or university theatre department productions. In the last five years, only one student has been unable to rearrange her schedule and attend. |
16 | For a discussion of the way Shakespearean adaptations and stage productions are themselves valid iterations of “Shakespeare” and not secondary to their print relatives, please see my “Introduction” with Christy Desmet and Natalie Loper in our collection Shakespeare/Not Shakespeare (Desmet et al. 2017), Douglas Lanier’s “Shakespearean Rhizomatics” (Lanier 2014), Richard Burt’s “Shakespeare, ‘Glo-cali-zation,’ Race and the Small Screens of Post-Popular Culture” (Burt 2003), and W. B. Worthen’s Shakespeare and the Authority of Performance (Worthen 1997) and Shakespeare Performance Studies (Worthen 2014, especially chapter 1 “Shakespeare Performance Studies”). For examples of the use of YouTube materials in the classroom, please see Ayanna Thompson’s chapter 7 “Archives: Classroom-Inspired Performance Videos on YouTube” in Passing Strange (Thompson 2011) and Stephen O’Neill’s chapter 5 “The Teaching and Learning Tube: Challenges and Affordances” in his Shakespeare and YouTube (O’Neill 2014). |
© 2019 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Casey, J. Digital Shakespeare Is Neither Good Nor Bad, But Teaching Makes It So. Humanities 2019, 8, 112. https://doi.org/10.3390/h8020112
Casey J. Digital Shakespeare Is Neither Good Nor Bad, But Teaching Makes It So. Humanities. 2019; 8(2):112. https://doi.org/10.3390/h8020112
Chicago/Turabian StyleCasey, Jim. 2019. "Digital Shakespeare Is Neither Good Nor Bad, But Teaching Makes It So" Humanities 8, no. 2: 112. https://doi.org/10.3390/h8020112