1. Introduction
An increasing number of livestock are transported every year around the world, for both breeding and production of meat [
1,
2,
3]. Over the last thirty years transport by sea has become an increasingly common way to move sheep over long distances [
1,
2]. However, there is scientific and public concern about the welfare of the animals on board [
1,
4]. Space allowances are the most controversial aspect concerning the animal’s welfare, because providing sufficient space for normal behaviour has a high economic cost [
5,
6,
7,
8]. Under the current commercial conditions, space allowances are little more than the physical space occupied by the animals, which reduces the unit cost of transport [
7]. Transported sheep may not even receive recommended space allowances; in one study [
9] over 30% of 6578 sheep transported by road were at higher densities than the UK’s Farm Animal Welfare Council recommendations [
10]. Insufficient space limits their behavioural freedom, in particular opportunities to lie down, in both trucks [
11] and ships [
12]. Stock become frightened of falling and not being able to get up again if other animals have occupied their space. Balance control is therefore crucial at high stocking densities.
Space allowance interacts with many aspects of the animal’s environment [
6], especially their ability to keep their balance. In one study, sheep transported by road at a low space allowance (0.39–0.78 m
2/animal) had more losses of balance and slips than those at higher space allowances (1.04–1.56 m
2) [
13]. Sheep prefer to stand independently during the transport and not to touch each other. During sea transport, sheep have to address more varied movements (heave, sway, surge, yaw, pitch and roll) than during road transport, and the animal´s responses to these movements are poorly understood [
2]. Pitch, roll and heave are the most common motions, associated with loss of balance and probably motion sickness [
14]. Heave and roll increase sheeps’ heart rates, demonstrating greater arousal, than pitch [
15]. Their main strategy to maintain balance on a moving floor involves stepping with their forelimbs frontwards and backwards, especially when they are exposed to heave and roll motions [
16].
Sheep should be given sufficient space for postural adjustment during road transport [
8,
13], but little is known about the appropriate space and its association with the balance maintenance strategies during transport by sea. We previously compared the impact on sheep behaviour and heart rate of increasing Australian standard allowances for sheep in ships (0.26 m
2/sheep) by providing an extra 15% or 35% of space. Both levels of extra space increased lying, but only the biggest space allowance reduced aggression and increased heart rate variability, suggesting reduced stress [
17]. The biggest allowance also reduced pushing behaviour and stepping to correct balance, as long as the motion was regular. Given that the responses to extra space are dependent on how much extra space is given and the regularity of the movement, this research investigated sheep responses to an extra 50% of space, compared with the Australian standards, in simulated sea transport with regular and irregular motion.
4. Discussion
This study supports our previous finding [
17] that a low space allowance (0.26 m
2/head = ASEL standards) and exposing sheep to a sequence of motions (both Regular and Irregular) have an impact on the welfare of sheep in simulated ship transport. The sheep responses included increased agonistic behaviour (pushing), stepping behaviours, heart rate and LF/HF ratio in the Low space allowance, evidence of increased stress.
Compared with sheep in the high space allowance, sheep in the low space allowance showed more affiliative behaviour and doubled the time spent with their head above their partner (8.64 s/h v/s 3.75 s/h,
p = 0.02). This social attachment could be related to sheep attempting to stay close together under stress, as a gregarious species, protecting themselves as a pair when they have to face a stressful condition. It must be acknowledged that such affiliative behaviour is facilitated by closer proximity at the low space allowance, and further research could investigate the motivation for such behaviour. It has been observed previously [
15,
24] and suggests that having their head above their partner’s reduced the negative emotion or discomfort caused by lack of space.
Sheep also increased pushing in the Low space allowance (4.51 vs. 1.37 pushes/h in the High space allowance). This increase in pushing behaviour can be seen as a consequence of competition for space, particularly for postural adjustment during their attempts to maintain their balance. In pigs an increase in social interaction has been associated with stress [
25], with more social interactions at low space (2.0 m
2/head) compared with higher space allowances (2.4–4.8 m
2/head). Other research has shown an increase in negative social interactions, including pushing, in dairy ewes when less space was provided (1 m
2/ewe, compared with 2 and 3 m
2/ewe) [
26]. The increase in social interactions might also result from the closer physical proximity of neighbouring sheep in reduced space allowance, without any involvement of stress. However, the heart rate responses to low space (0.26 m
2/head), in particular the increase in HR rate and LF/HF ratio and the reduction in HF, support the hypothesis that the sheep were stressed as a result of lack of space.
Simulated sea motion appeared to exacerbate the responses, the pushing responses tended to be increased when sheep were exposed to motion (irregular and regular) compared with the control with no motion (I = 9.82, R = 8.04 and C = 5.30 n/h;
p = 0.07). Aggression was not affected by space allowance (
p = 0.20), in contrast to our previous research [
17] in which aggression increased at low (5.9 times/h) and medium (5.6 times/h) space allowances, compared with high space allowance (3.9 times/h). The results of the current study may be different due to the smaller group size of two instead of three. If the group size increases (using the same m
2/head), the amount of space to be shared among the animals (group space) also decreases, making it insufficient to allow the performance of certain interactive behaviours, such as aggression [
5]. It is also possible that the sheep had habituated to each other after the first trial of this sequence of two consecutive similar trials, having created a confirmed dominance order, making aggressive interaction unnecessary. Reuse of animals between experiments helps to reduce the number of animals exposed to stressful procedures.
Sheep spent more time standing against the crate support when they were exposed to low space allowance. Sheep prefer to travel independently and keep their balance by supporting their body against a vehicle [
13]. These results are consistent with our previous trial [
17], in which sheep increased their time standing against the crate when less space was provided (0.26 m
2/sheep), compared with medium (0.30 m
2/sheep) and high (0.35 m
2/sheep) space allowances. Increased stepping in the high space allowance reflects more opportunity to step to keep their balance without interfering with the personal space of the other animal. The increase in stepping in the regular movement suggests that prediction of the floor position occurred and that this facilitated stepping behaviour.
The decrease in rumination time produced by irregular motion compared with regular and no motion has been observed previously [
17]. A reduction in rumination behaviour has also been previously described in deer during transport by road compared with those remaining stationary [
27], and it is likely that reduced rumination reflects an increase in stress.
This study has limitations because it only addresses space allowance effects under simulated not actual travel, only in small groups of sheep compared to those transported commercially, and only for short periods of time compared to commercial transport. In addition, sheep were accustomed to the apparatus beforehand, whereas commercially-transported sheep are not accustomed to the ship and this could contribute to stress experienced on board. This was deliberate in our study to avoid any complications of neophobia on the effects of space allowance and motion. However, the very different conditions on board commercial transport vessels must be recognized. In commercial transport, the close proximity of an unknown conspecific may not motivate a sheep to put its head under or over its head, whereas our sheep’s familiarity with each other may facilitate that behaviour. Secondly, the vertical distance moved by sheep will depend on where they are on a ship (those in the metacentre move the least, those at the edges of the deck much more). When we rotated the platform, we used a proportion of 33% of the standard tolerances for actual ships, in terms of the angle through which they are required to tolerate when rotated during heavy seas. The study therefore attempts to identify a proof of concept for fundamental changes in behaviour, stress levels and heart rate variables. These should be investigated in commercial transport situations before any conclusions about the welfare of sheep on board are drawn.
Other limitations include that the observer was not blind to treatments, impossible in this study because of the different sizes of the pens. Furthermore, using just a single observer meant that we could not measure interobserver reliability. The best procedure is to have several observers all proven statistically to be coding behaviour in the same way, but labour availability precluded that in this study. We also chose not to have the same observer view video footage twice, which would have given us a measure of interobserver reliability; this was because of the large amount of footage that already needed to be observed. We also acknowledge that the models we constructed assume independence in behaviours. For many of behaviours that we measured this was not likely to be the case, e.g., stepping in different directions, lying and standing.