Next Article in Journal
Determination of Fatty Acids Profile in Original Brown Cows Dairy Products and Relationship with Alpine Pasture Farming System
Previous Article in Journal
Addition of a Mixture of Plant Extracts to Diets for Growing-Finishing Pigs on Growth Performance, Blood Metabolites, Carcass Traits, Organ Weight as a Percentage of Live Weight, Quality and Sensorial Analysis of Meat
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

What Is a Dingo? The Phenotypic Classification of Dingoes by Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander Residents in Northern Australia

by
Victoria J. Brookes
1,2,*,
Chris Degeling
3,
Lily M. van Eeden
4 and
Michael P. Ward
1
1
Sydney School of Veterinary Science, Faculty of Science, The University of Sydney, Camden 2570, Australia
2
School of Animal and Veterinary Sciences, Faculty of Science, Charles Sturt University, Wagga Wagga 2650, Australia
3
Australian Centre for Health Engagement, Evidence & Values, School of Health and Society, Faculty of the Arts, Social Sciences, and Humanities, University of Wollongong, Wollongong 2252, Australia
4
School of Life and Environmental Sciences, Faculty of Science, The University of Sydney, Camperdown 2006, Australia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Animals 2020, 10(7), 1230; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10071230
Submission received: 22 June 2020 / Revised: 7 July 2020 / Accepted: 15 July 2020 / Published: 20 July 2020
(This article belongs to the Section Ecology and Conservation)

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

Dingoes are an Australian icon with cultural, as well as ecological, value, yet defining a dingo is complicated by hybridisation with domestic dogs. Northern Australia is a high-risk zone for the arrival of rabies, a disease which affects dogs (including dingoes) and people. In a rabies outbreak, Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander peoples who live in this region would want dingoes protected. We visited the Northern Peninsula Area (NPA), Queensland, in 2018–2019 and surveyed Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander residents about how they define dingoes, using pictures from camera traps previously placed and operated in the area. We found that dingo definition was based on characteristics traditionally associated with the iconic dingo (medium to large-framed dogs, with a long nose, pointed ears, narrow abdomen, bushy tail, smooth tan coats, and white feet and tail tip) but hybrid features, such as curled tail or a lack of white points, were also acceptable features. Local definitions are important when designing and implementing management plans so that actions are supported by local communities, and our findings are a useful guide for identifying dingoes in the NPA so that, in the event of a rabies outbreak, locally valued dingoes could be identified and protected.

Abstract

Dingo classification and management is complicated by hybridisation with domestic dogs. Northern Australia is a relatively high-risk zone for a rabies incursion, and in the event of an incursion, Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander peoples who reside in this region would prioritise the protection of dingoes. Therefore, the classification of dingoes in this context is important. Twelve pictures of canids with features associated with both dingoes and domestic dogs from camera traps in the Northern Peninsula Area (NPA), northern Queensland, were shown to Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander rangers (n = 3), biosecurity officers (n = 2), environmental health workers (n = 2), and residents (n = 39) in the NPA. Nearly all pictures (10/12) were classified as dingo or domestic dog (none as hybrid) and two were inconclusive (no overall agreement). Dingoes were consistently identified as medium to large-framed dogs, with a long nose, pointed ears, narrow abdomen, a bushy or feathered tail, and smooth coats of a single base colour. Some hybrid features were acceptable, including sable coats, lack of white tail tip or feet, and curled tail. These findings are a preliminary guide for identifying canids in the NPA region for whom management might be controversial. Building on this approach via further consultation with residents is needed to inform rabies response policy. Our approach using locally acquired camera trap pictures could also be extended to other regions in which dingoes have value but their management is controversial.

1. Introduction

Understanding how people classify wild animals is an essential first step in determining the values which people assign to a wild animal, and is critical to planning and implementing conservation or management strategies [1]. Decision-making can become problematic in situations in which wild animals do not fit easily into Western classification systems—for example, in populations in which genetic material has introgressed from their domestic relatives. Management in such situations might require us to consider factors other than genetics when defining these populations in the wild—such as morphology, and their ecological and cultural roles—to determine how these animals are classified and, hence, assigned a value by human societies.
Management of the dingo in Australia is an example in which scientific classification causes controversy. Dingoes are variously referred to as Canis dingo, C. lupus dingo, and C. familiaris due to disagreements over their taxonomy and relationship with domestic dogs (C. familiaris), with whom they can interbreed [2,3,4]. Disagreement and ambiguity surrounding the dingo’s classification is reflected in policy and management in Australia, with both the protection and persecution of dingoes promoted or legislated [5,6]. In the context of livestock protection, dingoes are included with feral dogs and dingo–dog hybrids under what is typically termed, “wild dog management”, because distinguishing between these canid types based on morphological features is difficult [7]; consequently, management in this context generally does not seek to do so [8]. However, there are management contexts that require a more nuanced classification; a recent study in Northern Australia found that, in the event of an incursion of rabies, residents in remote communities would want dingoes protected [9]. As such, failure to understand which dogs are valued by these communities could erode trust in management authorities and prevent an effective response to a rabies outbreak. It is therefore important to understand how residents in remote communities define dingoes.
Northern Australia is considered a high-risk zone for a canine rabies incursion due to its abundant domestic dog populations in remote communities, dingo populations, its proximity to rabies-endemic Southeast Asia, and the recent spread of rabies in Indonesia [10,11,12]. A modelling study predicted that, should a rabies incursion occur, the probability of an epidemic in dingoes is 21%, most likely leading to endemicity without control interventions [13]. The nationally agreed policy for rabies control [14] states that if rabies is detected, susceptible populations need to be identified at the earliest possible time, including wild and free-roaming populations, so that appropriate, population-specific control strategies can be considered. In the case of wildlife, experts will be consulted to determine such strategies, which might include targeted vaccination. As stated in the current policy document [14], limited population reduction could also be considered on a case-by-case basis and could include both wild and feral animal populations.
In remote Indigenous communities in Northern Australia, most domestic dogs roam freely [15,16] and, therefore, can come into contact with dingoes. Hybridisation between dingoes and community-owned dogs in Northern Australia has been demonstrated [17]. The level of introgression in dingo populations is varied: it has been suggested to be widespread and increases in proximity to human habitation [4], but there are also regions in which it is still low [18]. Whilst there has been extensive research on the impact of dingoes and dingo–dog hybrids on ecosystems and livestock industries [19,20], and evidence that the Australian public values dingoes [21], little is known about how the Australian public perceives concepts such as hybridisation to identify and distinguish dingoes from other canids. Although morphometric measurement and phenotypic characteristics have been used to assess dingo purity [22], more recently, dingo definition has been based on genotype following analysis of DNA microsatellite loci [4]. These methods are not conclusive—for example, there is debate about relevant microsatellites and variation due to differing analyses, and phenotypic characteristics do not necessarily reflect the level of introgression. They also do not account for people’s contextual definitions of a dingo and their acceptability of hybridisation.
In this study, we explored how dingoes are defined by residents in remote Australian Indigenous communities in Northern Australia for the purposes of management to reduce disease risk, focusing on the Northern Peninsula Area (NPA) of Queensland. We specifically aimed to provide a preliminary characterisation of NPA residents’ definition of a dingo to inform Australian rabies incursion policy. We acknowledge parallels between the imposition of such classificatory systems and colonial practices of Indigenous assimilation [23,24]. However, our aim is that the findings of this study could provide foundations for in-depth community consultation to determine local preferences for dingo and dog management. More broadly, we demonstrate how using camera trap pictures could be used to increase the understanding and appreciation of the socio-cultural value that people assign to dingoes.

2. Materials and Methods

Three visits were made to the study region in 2018 and 2019. The study methods were approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee at the University of Sydney (Project No: 2018/451) and all methods were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations.

2.1. Study Region

The Northern Peninsula Area is a local government area in northwestern Cape York, Queensland (Figure 1). It covers a total land area of 105,207 hectares and incorporates the communities of Bamaga, Injinoo, New Mapoon, Seisia, and Umagico (total population 2796, of whom 87% are Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander people [25]). Vegetation coverage includes woodlands, grasslands, tropical rainforests, heathlands, wetlands, and river systems, of which over 99% is “remnant” vegetation (vegetation on land that has never been cleared by people) [26]. The NPA is adjacent to the Jardine National Park and the Great Barrier Reef which is a World Heritage listed site [27]. To the north lies the Torres Strait and Thursday Island, a principal administrative and transportation centre in the region.

2.2. Discussions with Stakeholder Groups

We initially collected background information to develop a questionnaire for community residents. We (VB, CD, MW) had existing relationships with relevant stakeholder groups in the study region (Land and Sea Rangers, biosecurity officers, and environmental health workers (EHWs)), developed over the previous 6 years through research focused on preparedness for a rabies incursion [9,28,29,30,31,32]. Members of these stakeholder groups who were community residents were selected purposively because they were considered likely to have a high level of knowledge about dogs and dingoes in the NPA due to their work roles. Group members were shown 12 pictures of dogs, all of unknown genotype, that demonstrated a range of phenotypes (observable characteristics of an individual; Table 1 and Figure 2), including those typically associated with the hybridisation of dingoes [22]. We discussed aspects of dingo phenotype, ecology, and behaviour that could be used to inform the questionnaire for community residents. Pictures of dogs and dingoes used in the study were selected from images captured by motion- and heat-activated cameras (Reconyx HC600 Hyperfire, http://www.reconyx.com.au/, accessed 22 February 2019) at sites within 5 km of NPA communities between May 2017 and May 2018 (Figure 1). This provided local context by demonstrating a range of phenotypes recently observed in the study region. Specific landmarks were excluded to reduce bias in the opinion of participants due to their experience with dingoes and dogs that were potentially recognisable based on location.

2.3. Questionnaire Implementation with Community Residents

Researchers developed a face-to-face questionnaire informed by discussions with the stakeholder group members (Section 2.1), in which participants who were community residents were asked to state whether each picture (n = 12) was a dingo, domestic dog, or hybrid. Participants in the questionnaire were not members of the stakeholder groups. Following consultations with the stakeholder group members, Picture 6b replaced Picture 6a (Table 1 and Figure 2) from the original set so that there was a balance of phenotypes and expected categories. Enrollment was by convenience, and participation was voluntary and by written, informed consent. A copy of the questionnaire is included in the Supplementary Materials. The proportions of classifications (dingo, domestic dog, or hybrid) for each picture were tabulated (including 95% confidence intervals) and plotted. Agreement over classification by community residents was estimated using Fleiss’ kappa statistic.

3. Results

3.1. Stakeholder Groups

Participants from stakeholder groups included Land and Sea Rangers (n = 3), biosecurity officers (n = 2), and EHWs (n = 2). All were residents who were Aboriginal or Torres Strait Islander people, male, and between 18–60 years old.
Picture 8 was the most readily categorised as a dingo by all stakeholder group participants, due to its tan-coloured coat, face shape, pointed ears, white feet and nose, and bushy tail, and was described as the archetype dingo totem associated with some NPA clans. Pictures 3, 5, and 9 were also unequivocally categorised as dingoes. Group members recognised the animal in Picture 3, with several commenting, “this one’s famous round here”. They stated that they see two or three dingoes with similar curly tails in the local area, and that curly-tailed dingoes live throughout the northern Cape York Peninsula and down the west coast. They thought that this feature was the result of breeding between wild dingoes and one or more domestic dogs in the 1990s. In contrast, group members stated that dingoes around Ussher Point (Jardine River National Park, eastern Cape York Peninsula) have tails “almost as straight as a normal dog”. The grey colouration of the dingo in Picture 5 was reportedly due to age—participants stated that the coat would have been tan when the dingo was younger. The red colouration of the animal in Picture 9 was also considered typical of dingoes in the NPA. Head, body, and tail shape were considered the typical defining features of the dingoes in Pictures 3, 5, 8 and 9.
Stakeholder group members used behaviour and apparent gait (as far as can be assessed in a still image) to categorise dingoes when phenotype was equivocal. Some said Picture 2 was a domestic dog due to its colour (black and tan) but when told that this dog was seen in the company of dingoes, they decided that he was most likely a hybrid but would be classified as a dingo in the NPA. Black-coated dingoes were considered relatively new in the region. Most group members were adamant that Picture 7 was a domestic dog but some were uncertain due to the way in which it appeared to be moving (fast, as if with a purpose). Location did not appear to be useful when classifying dingoes because both dingoes and unaccompanied (free-roaming) domestic dogs are seen in the bush.
Even though contextual features were removed from the pictures, some of the stakeholders drew on their knowledge of the local dingo and dog populations in explaining how to categorize some of the canids in the sample. For example, rangers told us that dingoes are “getting pretty brave” because they are seen more frequently along the roads around the communities than they were 2–3 years ago. They attribute this to an increase in waste food thrown from cars by residents. They also see dingoes feeding on turtle carcasses at the local refuse site, as well as horse carcasses or even foals whilst a mare is foaling. Despite this perceived increase in the frequency of dingo activity around communities, rangers also stated that dingoes are scared of domestic dogs, and all groups agreed that a dingo and a domestic dog are unlikely to be seen together.
All other pictures (1, 4, 6a, 10, 11, 12) were unequivocally categorised as domestic dogs due to floppy ears, patchy coat colour, or domestic breed appearance (Pictures 4 and 10 were recognised as a Kelpie and a Blue Heeler, respectively).

3.2. Community Residents

A total of 39 residents participated in, and completed, the questionnaire survey. All age groups were represented and more male (73%) than female residents were surveyed (Figure 3). Participants represented all community groups present: Bamaga (n = 13), Injinoo (8), New Mapoon (8), Seisia (4), and Umagico (4).
Overall agreement about picture classification between community residents was high (Fleiss’ κ = 0.62, p < 0.01, Figure 4). Participants used the term “house dog” rather than “domestic dog”, and the term “both” instead of “hybrid”.
Some pictures were unequivocally (Picture 3) or predominantly (Pictures 5, 6, 8, 9) classified as “dingo” by residents (Fleiss’ κ = 0.74 (p < 0.001) and the 95% CI of the proportion of residents who selected “dingo” did not overlap the 95% CIs of the proportions of those who selected “hybrid” or “domestic dog”, Table 2). Features that were consistent throughout these pictures included pointed ears, a long nose, medium size, light build with tucked-up abdomen, a brushy or feathered tail, and a single base coat colour (tan, red, or grey, with no colour patches). Features associated with hybridisation that were visible amongst these pictures included a curly tail, sable coat, and lack of white tail tip or white feet.
Some pictures were unequivocally (Pictures 1, 10, 11) or predominantly (Pictures 7, 12) classified as “domestic dog” by residents (Fleiss’ κ = 0.64 (p < 0.001) and the 95% CI of the proportion of residents who selected “domestic dog” did not overlap the 95% CIs of the proportions of those who selected “hybrid” or “dingo”; Figure 4). The dogs in these pictures all lacked some or all the features that were consistent in pictures identified as “dingo”. Instead, they included floppy ears, a short nose, broad cranium, larger frame and heavy build, thin tail, patchy coat colour, or obvious domestic breed type (Picture 10 was readily identified as the breed “Blue Heeler” by all residents).
Classifications of Pictures 2 and 4 were inconclusive. Although both dogs had features consistent with pictures classified overall as dingoes (consistent ear and head shape in both pictures, and body shape and tail in Picture 2), both dogs had bi-colour coats which are seen in the “Kelpie” breed of domestic dog.
Unlike the participants from the stakeholder groups, residents did not mention gait or behaviour when classifying dingoes and did not comment on the presence of the additional dog in Picture 6b. Instead, throughout all the pictures, they used ears, face, head, body, and tail shapes and coat colours to justify their choices.
Throughout all pictures, participants’ proportions of classifications as “dingo”, “hybrid”, and “domestic dog” were 0.42, 0.13, and 0.45, respectively.

4. Discussion

There are varied examples of how people in different Indigenous communities sometimes distinguish types of dogs, for example, as dingoes and domestic dogs, or “camp dogs” and “wild dogs” (reviewed in [6,33]), yet little has been reported about how these distinctions are made. In the current study, strong agreement about the types of dogs in pictures from camera traps demonstrated the characteristics that residents in the Northern Peninsula Area (NPA) of Australia consistently classified as belonging to dingoes, including their acceptance of some characteristics associated with hybridisation with domestic dogs. The residents’ opinions were consistent with those of the smaller group of stakeholders in animal and environmental health from the region (rangers, environmental health workers, and biosecurity officers).
It was apparent that respondents could base their definition of what a dingo is on phenotype and that this was generally sufficient to reach a majority opinion. Whilst we included one picture of a dog (Picture 8) in this survey that had the phenotypic characteristics that could be associated with a “pure” dingo [22,34], and members of one stakeholder group described this dog as the archetype dingo totem associated with some NPA clans, all respondents readily classified other dogs as dingoes, suggesting that NPA residents accept a broader range of canids into the “dingo” classification than scientists [22]. The constant morphological features of all pictures classified as dingoes were head and body shape and the size of the dog; all were pictures of medium- to large-framed dogs, with a long nose, pointed ears, tucked-up abdomen, and a bushy or feathered tail. Constants about coat were that all had smooth coats of a single base colour. Variable features included coat colour (tan, red, grey, and sable), white tips (not always present on all feet and tail), and tail shape (curly). These latter features are associated with hybridisation with domestic dogs [22].
Whilst this phenotypic approach to dingo classification in the NPA allowed for acceptance of some features of hybridisation, not all animals with the constant shape and size features associated with dingoes in this study were classified as dingoes. In these cases, other features associated with hybridization—including patchy coat colour, thin tail, and floppy ears—were present, and participants classified these as domestic dogs. Only two animals were inconclusively classified by residents, and disagreement appeared to be due to a coat colour that participants associated with a domestic breed (Kelpie) even though the body shape was compatible with a dingo. In this study, we did not investigate how the classification of dingoes changes with time and place, although discussion with stakeholder group members about the black and tan dog, as well as the dingo with the curly tail (with an accepted history of breeding with a domestic dog), suggested that that dingo status in the NPA is a fluid construct with acknowledged regional variation. Sightings of dingoes with black coats had only occurred recently in the NPA, and curly-tailed dingoes had appeared within living memory. Various coat colours, including black, have been documented historically in other parts of Australia [7], and also recently; for example, black and tan dingoes have been identified in the Tanami Desert [35]. In addition, Radford, et al. [36] found that dingoes in mountainous areas tended to be larger-framed than lowland dingoes. In the current study, behaviour and ecology played a role in classification by some stakeholder group members. It is possible that such factors might be influential in the acceptance of new dingo phenotypes in the NPA [37]; further investigation would be needed to understand how dingo classification evolves and varies between regions.
Residents rarely classified dogs in the pictures as hybrids and no dogs were majority classified as hybrids by residents. This might reflect the pictures selected for inclusion in the study, or it could reflect a reluctance of residents to use this term. The concept of hybridisation and its application in defining identity can be controversial beyond the sensitivities around managing an animal that has cultural value for these communities [33,38]. Arguments to maintain dingo “purity” mean that dingo–dog hybridisation is used as justification for destruction [39] (similar to how wolf–dog hybrids are culled in Europe [40]). However, this denies some wild canids the right to exist based on mixed ancestry that resulted from human actions; for example, the introduction of domestic dogs to Australia [6]. We acknowledge that, like most wildlife, dingoes have significance for Australian Indigenous cultures [2] that is largely overlooked in dingo management. As such, it is critical that decisions and implementation of the management of dingoes, especially where there is an attempt to distinguish between canids based on perceived ancestry, must be planned and conducted in consultation with Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander peoples.
Managing interactions between scientific and Indigenous knowledge systems and understanding how dingoes are defined by local residents would be important during a rabies response in Northern Australia [41]. An effective biosecurity response requires a balance between the control of disease spread and the potential impacts of the disease, and needs consideration of the resilience of the populations impacted by the disease and the response so that strategies are accepted and supported. Resilience depends on a range of inter-linked systems, including society, culture, the economy, and the environment. Whilst we generally focus on response strategies for the control of disease spread given the potential impacts of the disease, for example, in the context of rabies in Australia [31,42], preparedness often ignores resilience and how this can be supported. Rather than waiting for an incursion, we suggest that Australia’s nationally agreed policy for rabies control [14] should incorporate the findings of this study as a preliminary assessment of dingo classification for the NPA and that preparedness activities should continue to build on them so that relevant populations are defined at response onset. We note that these findings are preliminary. The number of pictures used was limited, the number of residents was relatively small, and important questions remain for local residents—such as who ultimately decides that this approach to dingo classification is acceptable for this region and how. Further refinement and discussion about their acceptability and incorporation into policy needs to be undertaken with residents in the study region.

5. Conclusions

The results of this study focus on a pragmatic approach to classifying dingoes from the perspective of local residents so that dingoes can be identified and managed in a way that supports residents’ socio-cultural values during a biosecurity response to rabies in northern Queensland. In this way, local knowledge systems can be understood, which might otherwise be difficult to access. Indigenous residents in northern Queensland demonstrated agreement in their classification of dingoes and, although this is specific to this region, our approach could be used in other regions to expand understanding of how different communities classify dingoes. The results are preliminary and we note that community consultation is essential to build on these results. Decisions that could have social and cultural impacts require community consultation, otherwise trust in management authorities is eroded.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/2076-2615/10/7/1230/s1: Questionnaire; questions in red were not used in the survey. Participants were asked questions and shown pictures, and surveyors completed the responses on the form.

Author Contributions

Conceptualisation, V.J.B., L.M.v.E. and M.P.W.; methodology, V.J.B., L.M.v.E. and M.P.W.; analysis, V.J.B., C.D. and M.P.W.; investigation, V.J.B., C.D. and M.P.W.; data curation, V.J.B.; writing—original draft preparation, V.J.B., L.M.v.E. and M.P.W.; writing—review and editing, V.J.B., C.D., L.M.v.E. and M.W.; project administration, M.P.W.; funding acquisition, M.P.W., V.J.B. and C.D. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the National Medical Research Council of Australia, grant number 1083079.

Acknowledgments

We thank the residents of the study communities for their participation, and Laurence Gunn for production of the composite figure of the pictures of dogs used in this survey.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Mace, G.M. The role of taxonomy in species conservation. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2004, 359, 711–719. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Smith, B.P.; Cairns, K.M.; Adams, J.W.; Newsome, T.M.; Fillios, M.; Déaux, E.C.; Parr, W.C.H.; Letnic, M.; van Eeden, L.M.; Appleby, R.G.; et al. Taxonomic status of the Australian dingo: The case for Canis dingo Meyer, 1793. Zootaxa 2019, 4564, 173–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Jackson, S.M.; Groves, C.P.; Fleming, P.J.S.; Aplin, K.P.; Eldridge, M.D.B.; Gonzalez, A.; Helgen, K.M. The wayward dog: Is the Australian native dog or dingo a distinct species? Zootaxa 2017, 4317, 201–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Stephens, D.; Wilton, A.N.; Fleming, P.J.S.; Berry, O. Death by sex in an Australian icon: A continent-wide survey reveals extensive hybridization between dingoes and domestic dogs. Mol. Ecol. 2015, 24, 5643–5656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Smith, B.; Appleby, R. Forging a new future for the Australian dingo. In The Dingo Debate: Origins, Behaviour and Conservation; Smith, B., Ed.; CSIRO Publishing: Clayton South, Australia, 2015; pp. 301–316. [Google Scholar]
  6. Van Eeden, L.M.; Dickman, C.R.; Newsome, T.M.; Crowther, M.S. What should we do with wild dogs?: Taxonomic tangles and the management of dingo-dog hybridisation. Aust. Zool. 2019, 40, 92–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Crowther, M.S.; Fillios, M.; Colman, N.; Letnic, M. An updated description of the Australian dingo (Canis dingo Meyer, 1793). J. Zool. 2014, 293, 192–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Australia, W. National Wild Dog Action Plan: Promoting and Supporting Community-Driven Action for Landscape-Scale Wild Dog Management; ACT: Barton, Australia, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  9. Degeling, C.; Brookes, V.; Lea, T.; Ward, M. Rabies response, One Health and more-than-human considerations in Indigenous communities in northern Australia. Soc. Sci. Med. 2018, 212, 60–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Hudson, E.G.; Brookes, V.J.; Ward, M.P. Assessing the Risk of a Canine Rabies Incursion in Northern Australia. Front. Vet. Sci. 2017, 4, 141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Sparkes, J.; Fleming, P.J.S.; Ballard, G.; Scott-Orr, H.; Durr, S.; Ward, M.P. Canine Rabies in Australia: A Review of Preparedness and Research Needs. Zoonoses Public Health 2015, 62, 237–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  12. ProMED. Rabies (06): Asia (Indonesia, Pakistan) Human, Dog, Control, Archive Number: 20190218.6322909. Available online: https://promedmail.org/promed-post/?id=6322909 (accessed on 19 April 2020).
  13. Johnstone-Robertson, S.P.; Fleming, P.J.S.; Ward, M.P.; Davis, S.A. Predicted Spatial Spread of Canine Rabies in Australia. PLoS Negl. Trop. Dis. 2017, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Animal Health Australia. Disease Strategy: Rabies (Version 3.0), Australian Veterinary Emergency Plan (AUSVETPLAN); Primary Industries Ministerial Council: Canberra, Australia, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  15. Maher, E.K.; Ward, M.P.; Brookes, V.J. Investigation of the temporal roaming behaviour of free-roaming domestic dogs in Indigenous communities in northern Australia to inform rabies incursion preparedness. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 14893. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Hudson, E.G.; Brookes, V.J.; Dürr, S.; Ward, M.P. Domestic dog roaming patterns in remote northern Australian indigenous communities and implications for disease modelling. Prev. Vet. Med. 2017, 146, 52–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Bombara, C.B.; Duerr, S.; Machovsky-Capuska, G.E.; Jones, P.W.; Ward, M.P. A preliminary study to estimate contact rates between free-roaming domestic dogs using novel miniature cameras. PLoS ONE 2017, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  18. Cairns, K.M.; Nesbitt, B.J.; Laffan, S.W.; Letnic, M.; Crowther, M.S. Geographic hot spots of dingo genetic ancestry in southeastern Australia despite hybridisation with domestic dogs. Conserv. Genet. 2020, 21, 77–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. McLeod, R. Cost of Pest Animals in NSW and Australia, 2013–2014; Report prepared for the NSW Natural Resources Commission; eSYS Development Pty Ltd: Sydney, Australia, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  20. Letnic, M.; Ritchie, E.G.; Dickman, C.R. Top predators as biodiversity regulators: The dingo Canis lupus dingo as a case study. Biol. Rev. 2012, 87, 390–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Van Eeden, L.M.; Newsome, T.M.; Crowther, M.S.; Dickman, C.R.; Bruskotter, J. Social identity shapes support for management of wildlife and pests. Biol. Conserv. 2019, 231, 167–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Elledge, A.E.; Allen, L.R.; Carlsson, B.-L.; Wilton, A.N.; Leung, L.K.-P. An evaluation of genetic analyses, skull morphology and visual appearance for assessing dingo purity: Implications for dingo conservation. Wildl. Res. 2008, 35, 812–820. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Dodson, M. The wentworth lecture the end in the beginning: Re (de) finding aboriginality. Aust. Aborig. Stud. 1994, 1, 2. [Google Scholar]
  24. Probyn-Rapsey, F. Dingoes and dog-whistling: A cultural politics of race and species in Australia. Anim. Stud. J. 2015, 4, 55–77. [Google Scholar]
  25. Australian Bureau of Statistics. 2016 Census QuickStats. Available online: https://quickstats.censusdata.abs.gov.au/census_services/getproduct/census/2016/quickstat/036 (accessed on 19 April 2020).
  26. Queensland Government. Remnant Regional Ecosystem Vegetation in Queensland. Available online: https://www.qld.gov.au/environment/plants-animals/plants/ecosystems/remnant-vegetation (accessed on 19 April 2020).
  27. United Nations Educational Scientific and Cultural Organisation, UNESCO. Great Barrier Reef. Available online: https://whc.unesco.org/en/list/154/ (accessed on 19 April 2020).
  28. Brookes, V.J.; Kennedy, E.; Dhagapan, P.; Ward, M.P. Qualitative Research to Design Sustainable Community-Based Surveillance for Rabies in Northern Australia and Papua New Guinea. Front. Vet. Sci. 2017, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  29. Hudson, E.G.; Brookes, V.J.; Ward, M.P. Demographic studies of owned dogs in the Northern Peninsula Area, Australia, to inform population and disease management strategies. Aust. Vet. J. 2018, 96, 487–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Dürr, S.; Ward, M.P. Roaming behaviour and home range estimation of domestic dogs in Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander communities in northern Australia using four different methods. Prev. Vet. Med. 2014, 117, 340–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Dürr, S.; Ward, M.P. Development of a Novel Rabies Simulation Model for Application in a Non-endemic Environment. PLoS Negl. Trop. Dis. 2015, 9, e0003876. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Hudson, E.G.; Dhand, N.; Dürr, S.; Ward, M.P. A Survey of Dog Owners in Remote Northern Australian Indigenous Communities to Inform Rabies Incursion Planning. PLoS Negl. Trop. Dis. 2016, 10, e0004649. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Carter, J.; Wardell-Johnson, A.; Archer-Lean, C. Butchulla perspectives on dingo displacement and agency at K’gari-Fraser Island, Australia. Geoforum 2017, 85, 197–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Newsome, A.; Corbett, L. The identity of the dingo III.* The incidence of dingoes, dogs and hybrids and their coat colours in remote and settled regions of Australia. Aust. J. Zool. 1985, 33, 363–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Newsome, T.M.; Stephens, D.; Ballard, G.-A.; Dickman, C.R.; Fleming, P.J. Genetic profile of dingoes (Canis lupus dingo) and free-roaming domestic dogs (C. l. familiaris) in the Tanami Desert, Australia. Wildl. Res. 2013, 40, 196–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Radford, C.G.; Letnic, M.; Fillios, M.; Crowther, M.S. An assessment of the taxonomic status of wild canids in south-eastern New South Wales: Phenotypic variation in dingoes. Aust. J. Zool. 2012, 60, 73–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Trigger, D.; Mulcock, J.; Gaynor, A.; Toussaint, Y. Ecological restoration, cultural preferences and the negotiation of ‘nativeness’ in Australia. Geoforum 2008, 39, 1273–1283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Simberloff, D. Confronting introduced species: A form of xenophobia? Biol. Invasions 2003, 5, 179–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Allen, B.L.; Allen, L.R.; Ballard, G.; Jackson, S.M.; Fleming, P.J. A roadmap to meaningful dingo conservation. Canid Biol. Conserv. 2017, 20, 45–56. [Google Scholar]
  40. Salvatori, V.; Donfrancesco, V.; Trouwborst, A.; Boitani, L.; Linnell, J.D.; Alvares, F.; Åkesson, M.; Balys, V.; Blanco, J.C.; Chiriac, S. European agreements for nature conservation need to explicitly address wolf-dog hybridisation. Biol. Conserv. 2020, 248, 108525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Robinson, C.J.; Wallington, T.J. Boundary work: Engaging knowledge systems in co-management of feral animals on Indigenous lands. Ecol. Soc. 2012, 17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Hudson, E.G.; Brookes, V.J.; Dürr, S.; Ward, M.P. Modelling targeted rabies vaccination strategies for a domestic dog population with heterogeneous roaming patterns. PLoS Negl. Trop. Dis. 2019, 13, e0007582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. The study area (Northern Peninsula Area) location and camera trap sites, in northwestern Cape York, Queensland, Australia.
Figure 1. The study area (Northern Peninsula Area) location and camera trap sites, in northwestern Cape York, Queensland, Australia.
Animals 10 01230 g001
Figure 2. Pictures used in a study of the phenotypic classification of dingoes by Indigenous residents in northern Queensland, Australia.
Figure 2. Pictures used in a study of the phenotypic classification of dingoes by Indigenous residents in northern Queensland, Australia.
Animals 10 01230 g002
Figure 3. Age and sex of participants in a survey to explore how Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander peoples define dingoes in the Northern Peninsula Area, Queensland.
Figure 3. Age and sex of participants in a survey to explore how Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander peoples define dingoes in the Northern Peninsula Area, Queensland.
Animals 10 01230 g003
Figure 4. Proportions and majority classifications of 12 pictures of free-roaming canids by Indigenous residents, in a study of definitions of dingoes and domestic dogs in the Northern Peninsula Area, Queensland.
Figure 4. Proportions and majority classifications of 12 pictures of free-roaming canids by Indigenous residents, in a study of definitions of dingoes and domestic dogs in the Northern Peninsula Area, Queensland.
Animals 10 01230 g004
Table 1. Characteristics of canids in pictures used in a survey to explore how Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islanders define dingoes in the Northern Peninsula Area, Queensland. The pictures are presented in Figure 2. Picture 6a was shown to participants from stakeholder groups, and Picture 6b was shown to community residents.
Table 1. Characteristics of canids in pictures used in a survey to explore how Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islanders define dingoes in the Northern Peninsula Area, Queensland. The pictures are presented in Figure 2. Picture 6a was shown to participants from stakeholder groups, and Picture 6b was shown to community residents.
PictureHeadEarsBody ConformationCoatTail ShapeWhite PointsDomestic Breed
1Short nose, broad craniumFloppyLarge frame, light buildRough, patchy white-brownThinNA
2Long nosePointedMedium frame, heavy buildBlack and tanBushyBack foot
3Long nosePointedMedium frame, light buildTanCurly and bushyFront foot
4Long nosePointedNABrown, kelpie markingsNANone apparentKelpie
5Long nosePointedMedium frame, light buildSable, grey to light tanBushyYes
6aLong noseUpright but not pointedLarge frame, heavy buildRedThinNose
6bLong nosePointedLarge frame, light buildRedThin with featheringAll feet
7Long nosePointedMedium frame, light buildPatchy white and brownThinNA
8Long nosePointedMedium frame, light buildTanBushyYes
9Long nosePointedMedium frame, light buildTan-redBushyNone apparent
10Long nosePointedSmall frame, heavy buildPatchy black and grey with tickingBushyNoneBlue cattle dog
11Short nose, broad craniumFloppyMedium frame, heavy buildPatchy brown and whiteThinNA
12Long noseUpright but not pointedLarge frame, heavy buildBlackThinNone apparent
Table 2. Proportion of respondents and 95% confidence intervals (“low”–“high”) that defined pictures as dingo, hybrid, or domestic dog in a survey of Indigenous residents in the Northern Peninsula Area, Queensland.
Table 2. Proportion of respondents and 95% confidence intervals (“low”–“high”) that defined pictures as dingo, hybrid, or domestic dog in a survey of Indigenous residents in the Northern Peninsula Area, Queensland.
DingoHybridDomestic
PictureProportionLowHighProportionLowHighProportionLowHighOverall Classification
1000.05000.05111Domestic
20.40.230.60.370.20.570.230.070.43Inconclusive
3111000.05000.05Dingo
40.20.030.390.30.130.490.50.330.69Inconclusive
50.930.910.070.030.17000.1Dingo
60.80.70.950.170.070.320.0300.19Dingo
70.0300.210.270.130.440.70.570.87Domestic
80.970.931000.060.0300.09Dingo
90.930.910.0300.130.0300.13Dingo
10000.05000.05111Domestic
11000.05000.05111Domestic
12000.10.070.030.170.930.91Domestic

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Brookes, V.J.; Degeling, C.; van Eeden, L.M.; Ward, M.P. What Is a Dingo? The Phenotypic Classification of Dingoes by Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander Residents in Northern Australia. Animals 2020, 10, 1230. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10071230

AMA Style

Brookes VJ, Degeling C, van Eeden LM, Ward MP. What Is a Dingo? The Phenotypic Classification of Dingoes by Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander Residents in Northern Australia. Animals. 2020; 10(7):1230. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10071230

Chicago/Turabian Style

Brookes, Victoria J., Chris Degeling, Lily M. van Eeden, and Michael P. Ward. 2020. "What Is a Dingo? The Phenotypic Classification of Dingoes by Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander Residents in Northern Australia" Animals 10, no. 7: 1230. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10071230

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop