Can the Hole–Board Test Predict a Rat’s Exploratory Behavior in a Free-Exploration Test?
Abstract
:Simple Summary
Abstract
1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Animals
2.2. Procedure
2.2.1. Phase I—The Hole–board Test
2.2.2. Phase II—The Free-Exploration Test
2.3. Data Processing and Statistical Analyses
3. Results
3.1. Time Spent in the Transporter
3.2. Time Spent in the Central Zone of the Chamber
3.3. Time Spent in the Unchanged Zone of the Chamber
3.4. Time Spent in the Changed Zone of the Chamber
3.5. Frequency of Moving between the Zones (Left/Right/Transporter) of the Chamber
3.6. Time Spent in Contact with the Tunnels in the Unchanged Zone of the Chamber
3.7. Frequency of Contact with the Tunnels in the Unchanged Zone of the Chamber
3.8. Time Spent in Contact with the Tunnels in the Changed Zone of the Chamber
3.9. Frequency of Contact with the Tunnels in the Changed Zone of the Chamber
3.10. Effect Size Analysis
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Hall, C.; Ballachey, E.L. A Study of the Rat’s Behavior in a Field. A Contribution to Method in Comparative Psychology. Univ. Calif. Publ. Psychol. 1932, 6, 1–12. [Google Scholar]
- Boissier, J.R.; Simon, P. The Exploration Reaction in the Mouse. Preliminary Note. Therapie 1962, 17, 1225–1232. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- File, S.E.; Wardill, A.G. Validity of Head-Dipping as a Measure of Exploration in a Modified Hole-Board. Psychopharmacologia 1975, 44, 53–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hughes, R.N. Neotic Preferences in Laboratory Rodents: Issues, Assessment and Substrates. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2007, 31, 441–464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown, G.R.; Nemes, C. The Exploratory Behaviour of Rats in the Hole-Board Apparatus: Is Head-Dipping a Valid Measure of Neophilia? Behav. Process. 2008, 78, 442–448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- van der Staay, F.J.; Gieling, E.T.; Pinzón, N.E.; Nordquist, R.E.; Ohl, F. The Appetitively Motivated “Cognitive” Holeboard: A Family of Complex Spatial Discrimination Tasks for Assessing Learning and Memory. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2012, 36, 379–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lundberg, S.; Högman, C.; Roman, E. Adolescent Exploratory Strategies and Behavioral Types in the Multivariate Concentric Square FieldTM Test. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2019, 13, 41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lundberg, S.; Nylander, I.; Roman, E. Behavioral Profiling in Early Adolescence and Early Adulthood of Male Wistar Rats After Short and Prolonged Maternal Separation. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2020, 14, 37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Meeking, M.M.; MacFabe, D.F.; Mepham, J.R.; Foley, K.A.; Tichenoff, L.J.; Boon, F.H.; Kavaliers, M.; Ossenkopp, K.-P. Propionic Acid Induced Behavioural Effects of Relevance to Autism Spectrum Disorder Evaluated in the Hole Board Test with Rats. Prog. Neuro Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 2020, 97, 109794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cowan, P.E. Neophobia and Neophilia: New-Object and New-Place Reactions of Three Rattus Species. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 1977, 91, 63–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Greenberg, R. The Role of Neophobia and Neophilia in the Development of Innovative Behaviour of Birds. In Animal Innovation; Reader, S.M., Laland, K.N., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2003; pp. 175–196. ISBN 978-0-19-852622-3. [Google Scholar]
- Kuczaj, S.A. Animal creativity and innovation. In APA Handbook of Comparative Psychology: Perception, Learning, and Cognition; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2017; pp. 627–641. [Google Scholar]
- Mettke-Hofmann, C.; Winkler, H.; Leisler, B. The Significance of Ecological Factors for Exploration and Neophobia in Parrots. Ethology 2002, 108, 249–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pisula, W.; Modlinska, K.; Chrzanowska, A. Behavioural Response to the Environmental Changes of Various Types in Lister-Hooded Male Rats. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 7111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pisula, W.; Modlinska, K. Protocol for Measuring Free (Low-Stress) Exploration in Rats. Bio Protoc. 2020, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Committee on Guidelines for the Use of Animals in Neuroscience and Behavioral Research. Guidelines for the Care and Use of Mammals in Neuroscience and Behavioral Research; National Academies Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2003; p. 10732. ISBN 978-0-309-08903-6. [Google Scholar]
- Pisula, W. The Roman High- and Low-Avoidance Rats Respond Differently to Novelty in a Familiarized Environment. Behav. Process. 2003, 63, 63–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pisula, W.; Siegel, J. Exploratory Behavior as a Function of Environmental Novelty and Complexity in Male and Female Rats. Psychol. Rep. 2005, 97, 631–638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tanas, L.; Pisula, W. Response to Novel Object in Wistar and Wild-Type (WWCPS) Rats. Behav. Process. 2011, 86, 279–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kelley, A.E. Chapter 19—Locomotor activity and exploration. In Techniques in the Behavioral and Neural Sciences; van Haaren, F., Ed.; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1993; Volume 10, pp. 499–518. ISBN 0921-0709. [Google Scholar]
- Labots, M.; Van Lith, H.A.; Ohl, F.; Arndt, S.S. The Modified Hole Board—Measuring Behavior, Cognition and Social Interaction in Mice and Rats. JoVE 2015, 52529. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Redolat, R.; Perez-Martinez, A.; Carrasco, M.; Mesa, P. Individual Differences in Novelty-Seeking and Behavioral Responses to Nicotine: A Review of Animal Studies. CDAR 2009, 2, 230–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zampachova, B.; Kaftanová, B.; Šimánková, H.; Landová, E.; Frynta, D. Consistent Individual Differences in Standard Exploration Tasks in the Black Rat (Rattus Rattus). J. Comp. Psychol. 2017, 131, 150–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ray, J.; Hansen, S. Temperament in the Rat: Sex Differences and Hormonal Influences on Harm Avoidance and Novelty Seeking. Behav. Neurosci. 2004, 118, 488–497. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ray, J.; Hansen, S. Temperamental Development in the Rat: The First Year. Dev. Psychobiol. 2005, 47, 136–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Márquez, C.; Nadal, R.; Armario, A. Influence of Reactivity to Novelty and Anxiety on Hypothalamic–Pituitary–Adrenal and Prolactin Responses to Two Different Novel Environments in Adult Male Rats. Behav. Brain Res. 2006, 168, 13–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- File, S.E.; Wardill, A.G. The Reliability of the Hole-Board Apparatus. Psychopharmacologia 1975, 44, 47–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Arenas, M.C.; Daza-Losada, M.; Vidal-Infer, A.; Aguilar, M.A.; Miñarro, J.; Rodríguez-Arias, M. Capacity of Novelty-Induced Locomotor Activity and the Hole-Board Test to Predict Sensitivity to the Conditioned Rewarding Effects of Cocaine. Physiol. Behav. 2014, 133, 152–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ohl, F.; Holsboer, F.; Landgraf, R. The Modified Hole Board as a Differential Screen for Behavior in Rodents. Behav. Res. MethodsInstrum. Comput. 2001, 33, 392–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mazzamuto, M.V.; Cremonesi, G.; Santicchia, F.; Preatoni, D.; Martinoli, A.; Wauters, L.A. Rodents in the Arena: A Critical Evaluation of Methods Measuring Personality Traits. Ethol. Ecol. Evol. 2019, 31, 38–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Descriptive Statistics | Female | Male |
---|---|---|
Valid cases | 40 | 40 |
Missing cases | 0 | 0 |
Mean | 63.125 | 65.850 |
Std. Deviation | 12.081 | 21.832 |
Minimum | 36.000 | 22.000 |
Maximum | 82.000 | 117.000 |
25th percentile | 55.750 | 50.500 |
50th percentile | 66.000 | 65.000 |
75th percentile | 72.000 | 80.250 |
Trial | Female | Male | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
HB_H | HB_L | HB_H | HB_L | |||||
Mean | Stdv | Mean | Stdv | Mean | Stdv | Mean | Stdv | |
Time spent in the transporter (seconds) | ||||||||
H | 53.489 | 16.865 | 78.091 | 21.987 | 57.950 | 18.938 | 71.255 | 30.743 |
T1 | 25.497 | 14.998 | 33.446 | 24.695 | 21.100 | 10.088 | 28.471 | 10.855 |
T2 | 27.500 | 11.119 | 39.975 | 25.770 | 38.097 | 13.819 | 38.350 | 25.722 |
T3 | 31.250 | 9.284 | 19.147 | 8.359 | 62.600 | 20.514 | 40.725 | 17.311 |
Time spent in the central zone of the chamber (seconds) | ||||||||
H | 87.382 | 17.671 | 129.922 | 18.244 | 103.405 | 28.115 | 131.752 | 31.083 |
T1 | 42.113 | 16.224 | 86.806 | 26.956 | 65.422 | 28.689 | 88.218 | 30.795 |
T2 | 44.400 | 22.570 | 92.755 | 31.475 | 62.176 | 17.438 | 90.301 | 35.716 |
T3 | 86.138 | 40.483 | 86.452 | 32.073 | 83.218 | 26.809 | 100.955 | 30.661 |
Time spent in the unchanged zone of the chamber (seconds) | ||||||||
H | 118.484 | 15.684 | 80.812 | 18.546 | 142.643 | 28.925 | 128.811 | 36.466 |
T1 | 69.950 | 29.519 | 52.200 | 22.475 | 77.350 | 20.072 | 77.278 | 36.888 |
T2 | 79.050 | 26.858 | 97.819 | 36.724 | 93.001 | 31.368 | 63.225 | 35.026 |
T3 | 80.276 | 27.460 | 107.424 | 38.002 | 78.147 | 29.488 | 81.350 | 32.115 |
Time spent in the changed zone of the chamber (seconds) | ||||||||
H | 158.247 | 28.266 | 127.658 | 21.996 | 113.634 | 22.628 | 85.126 | 17.594 |
T1 | 279.091 | 43.538 | 244.774 | 43.715 | 254.278 | 37.004 | 223.808 | 33.103 |
T2 | 268.749 | 26.178 | 186.350 | 33.690 | 223.852 | 40.256 | 226.049 | 63.430 |
T3 | 219.426 | 46.717 | 203.725 | 44.906 | 193.411 | 28.945 | 194.200 | 48.507 |
Frequency of moving between the zones (left/right/transporter) of the chamber | ||||||||
H | 16.600 | 4.537 | 18.200 | 2.654 | 19.100 | 2.648 | 18.500 | 4.238 |
T1 | 13.500 | 5.462 | 17.000 | 3.367 | 15.200 | 3.011 | 15.700 | 2.869 |
T2 | 11.800 | 4.341 | 16.200 | 3.490 | 17.000 | 3.266 | 15.400 | 2.836 |
T3 | 15.400 | 3.596 | 15.400 | 2.989 | 17.000 | 3.528 | 15.500 | 1.780 |
Time spent in contact with the tunnels in the unchanged zone of the chamber (seconds) | ||||||||
H | 81.598 | 12.940 | 47.508 | 14.835 | 98.234 | 23.204 | 80.869 | 39.429 |
T1 | 48.350 | 23.602 | 30.000 | 14.163 | 51.704 | 14.573 | 48.825 | 29.438 |
T2 | 61.747 | 22.725 | 74.880 | 37.181 | 67.179 | 25.447 | 39.950 | 32.483 |
T3 | 63.954 | 25.023 | 80.030 | 32.701 | 56.068 | 20.425 | 55.650 | 30.935 |
Frequency of contact with the tunnels in the unchanged zone of the chamber | ||||||||
H | 6.533 | 1.492 | 5.133 | 0.706 | 8.733 | 2.372 | 7.367 | 1.222 |
T1 | 4.500 | 1.958 | 4.700 | 1.636 | 6.300 | 1.567 | 5.400 | 1.776 |
T2 | 4.800 | 1.398 | 4.900 | 1.370 | 6.800 | 1.989 | 4.600 | 1.174 |
T3 | 4.700 | 1.337 | 5.200 | 0.632 | 6.100 | 2.079 | 5.400 | 1.174 |
Time spent in contact with the tunnels in the changed zone of the chamber (seconds) | ||||||||
H | 111.982 | 19.581 | 76.694 | 14.877 | 75.809 | 18.746 | 51.297 | 17.797 |
T1 | 248.350 | 43.400 | 200.213 | 40.112 | 220.723 | 40.276 | 175.604 | 35.720 |
T2 | 243.399 | 30.898 | 152.952 | 32.536 | 192.424 | 37.455 | 182.876 | 56.932 |
T3 | 195.926 | 46.201 | 166.971 | 44.976 | 164.299 | 23.887 | 157.450 | 48.469 |
Frequency of contact with the tunnels in the changed zone of the chamber | ||||||||
H | 7.700 | 1.836 | 7.500 | 1.672 | 7.100 | 1.277 | 6.333 | 1.812 |
T1 | 8.400 | 2.716 | 10.100 | 1.853 | 8.200 | 2.150 | 8.900 | 2.424 |
T2 | 6.900 | 1.370 | 7.600 | 0.966 | 9.600 | 2.171 | 8.900 | 2.726 |
T3 | 8.000 | 2.261 | 8.500 | 2.550 | 9.000 | 2.867 | 8.800 | 3.458 |
Variable | Effect of: | Eta2 |
---|---|---|
Time spent in contact with the tunnels in the changed zone of the chamber | trial | 0.757 |
Time spent in the changed zone of the chamber | trial | 0.726 |
Time spent in the unchanged zone of the chamber | trial | 0.433 |
Time spent in the central zone of the chamber | trial | 0.367 |
Time spent in contact with the tunnels in the unchanged zone of the chamber | trial | 0.303 |
Frequency of contact with the tunnels in the unchanged zone of the chamber | trial | 0.300 |
Frequency of contact with the tunnels in the unchanged zone of the chamber | sex | 0.292 |
Time spent in the unchanged zone of the chamber | trial by sex | 0.203 |
Time spent in contact with the tunnels in the unchanged zone of the chamber | trial by sex | 0.200 |
Frequency of moving between the zones (left/right/transporter) of the chamber | trial | 0.199 |
Time spent in the transporter | trial by sex | 0.180 |
Frequency of contact with the tunnels in the changed zone of the chamber | trial by sex | 0.130 |
Time spent in contact with the tunnels in the changed zone of the chamber | sex by group | 0.125 |
Time spent in the unchanged zone of the chamber | trial by sex by group | 0.120 |
Frequency of contact with the tunnels in the unchanged zone of the chamber | group | 0.116 |
Time spent in the unchanged zone of the chamber | trial by group | 0.114 |
Time spent in contact with the tunnels in the unchanged zone of the chamber | trial by group | 0.105 |
Time spent on contact with the tunnels in the unchanged zone of the chamber | sex by group | 0.104 |
Time spent in the changed zone of the chamber | trial by sex by group | 0.074 |
Effect Code | Description of Effect |
---|---|
Time spent in the transporter | |
Trial × Sex | The general pattern of the response was similar in both females and males. All subjects spent less time in the transporter in T1. Males, however, spent more time staying in the transporter in T3. |
Trial × Group | The general pattern of the response was similar in both HB groups. All subjects spent more time in the transporter in sessions T1 than T3. However, in subjects from the HB-low groups, this tendency was more pronounced. |
Time spent in the central zone of the chamber | |
Trial | All rats spent less time in the central zone in trial T1 compared to that spent in the habituation phase. |
Group | Subjects from the HB_high group spent less time in that zone than did subjects from the HB_low group across all experimental trials. |
Time spent in the unchanged zone of the chamber | |
Trial × Sex × Group | HB_high females and all males spent less time in the unchanged zone of the chamber in trial T1. However, no such effect was observed in females from the HB_low group. On the contrary—there was an increase in the amount of time spent in the unchanged zone between T2 and T3. |
Time spent in the changed zone of the chamber | |
Trial × Sex × Group | For all rats, there was an increase in the duration of staying in the changed zone of the chamber in trial T1. However, both females and males from the HB_high groups spent less time in the changed zone in trial T3 as compared to trial T1. The duration of staying in the chamber’s changed zone in HB_high individuals was generally longer than in HB_low individuals, which was most clearly manifested within the female subsample in trial T2. |
Frequency of moving between the zones (left/right/transporter) of the chamber | |
Trial | For all rats, there was a decrease in the frequency of moving between the zones in trial T1. |
Time spent in contact with the tunnels in the unchanged zone of the chamber | |
Trial × Sex × Group | HB_high male rats spent less time in contact with tunnels in the unchanged zone, but HB_low male rats and all females did not show this pattern. Females from the HB_low group spent more time in contact with the tunnels in this zone in trials T2 and T3 as compared to trial T1. |
Frequency of contact with the tunnels in the unchanged zone of the chamber | |
Trial | All rats interacted with the tunnels in the unchanged zone in trial T1 less frequently than they did in the habituation trials. |
Sex | Males interacted with the tunnels in the unchanged zone more frequently than females across all experimental trials. |
Group | HB_high subjects interacted with the tunnels in the unchanged zone more frequently than their HB_low counterparts across all experimental trials. |
Time spent in contact with the tunnels in the changed zone of the chamber | |
Trial | All individuals spent more time interacting with the tunnels in the changed zone in trial T1. |
Sex × Group | HB_high females spent more time interacting with the tunnels in the changed zone than all other counterparts. |
Frequency of contact with the tunnels in the changed zone of the chamber | |
Trial × Sex | All subjects responded to tunnel modification with an increase in the frequency of contact with the tunnels in trial T1. However, there was a decrease in the time spent by females on this activity in trial T2. |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Pisula, W.; Modlinska, K.; Goncikowska, K.; Chrzanowska, A. Can the Hole–Board Test Predict a Rat’s Exploratory Behavior in a Free-Exploration Test? Animals 2021, 11, 1068. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11041068
Pisula W, Modlinska K, Goncikowska K, Chrzanowska A. Can the Hole–Board Test Predict a Rat’s Exploratory Behavior in a Free-Exploration Test? Animals. 2021; 11(4):1068. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11041068
Chicago/Turabian StylePisula, Wojciech, Klaudia Modlinska, Katarzyna Goncikowska, and Anna Chrzanowska. 2021. "Can the Hole–Board Test Predict a Rat’s Exploratory Behavior in a Free-Exploration Test?" Animals 11, no. 4: 1068. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11041068
APA StylePisula, W., Modlinska, K., Goncikowska, K., & Chrzanowska, A. (2021). Can the Hole–Board Test Predict a Rat’s Exploratory Behavior in a Free-Exploration Test? Animals, 11(4), 1068. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11041068