Next Article in Journal
No Observed Adverse Effects on Health Were Detected in Adult Beagle Dogs When Fed a High-Calcium Diet for 40 Weeks
Previous Article in Journal
Oxytetracycline and Florfenicol Concentrations in Food-Additive Premixes Authorised for Broiler Chickens: Assessing Degree of Agreement with Manufacturers Labelling
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Basic Needs in Horses?—A Literature Review

1
Department of Equine Economics, Faculty of Agriculture, Economics and Management, Nuertingen-Geislingen University, Neckarsteige 6-10, 72622 Nürtingen, Germany
2
Zoology/Evolutionary Biology, University of Regensburg, Universitätsstraße 31, 93053 Regensburg, Germany
3
Department of Animal Welfare, Ethology, Animal Hygiene and Animal Husbandry, Ludwig Maximilian University Munich, Veterinarian Medicine, Veterinaerstr. 13/R, 80539 Munich, Germany
4
Centre for Social Learning & Cognitive Evolution, School of Psychology, University of St Andrews, St Andrews, Scotland KY16 9JPh, UK
5
Behavioural Physiology of Farm Animals, University of Hohenheim, Garbenstr. 17, 70599 Hohenheim, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Animals 2021, 11(6), 1798; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11061798
Submission received: 29 April 2021 / Revised: 11 June 2021 / Accepted: 14 June 2021 / Published: 16 June 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Processing of environmental stimuli and farm animal welfare)

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

All animals have requirements that are essential for their welfare, and when these basic needs are not met, the animal suffers. In horses, it is claimed that these needs include social contact, social companionship, free movement and access to roughage in the form of grass, hay and/or straw. To validate this claim, this review examines 38 studies that reported on horses’ responses when one or more of these factors are restricted. We categorised the type of responses investigated: (a) Stress (e.g., increased stress hormones), (b) Active (e.g., increased aggression), (c) Passive (e.g., depressive-like behaviour) and (d) Abnormal Behaviour (e.g., stereotypies), and analysed the frequencies with which the investigated responses were shown. Overall, the studies reported that horses did react to restrictions in the described basic needs, especially to combinations of restricted social contact, free movement and access to roughage. The observation of passive responses and the development of abnormal behaviour provided compelling evidence that horses were suffering under these restrictions, and existing abnormal behaviours indicated that they had suffered at some time in the past. We conclude that the literature supports the claim that social contact, free movement and access to roughage are basic needs in horses and need to be taken into consideration to ensure their mental and physical welfare in management and training.

Abstract

Every animal species has particular environmental requirements that are essential for its welfare, and when these so-called “basic needs” are not fulfilled, the animals suffer. The basic needs of horses have been claimed to be social contact, social companionship, free movement and access to roughage. To assess whether horses suffer when one or more of the four proposed basic needs are restricted, we examined several studies (n = 38) that reported behavioural and physiological reactions to these restrictions. We assigned the studies according to the four types of responses investigated: (a) Stress, (b) Active, (c) Passive, and (d) Abnormal Behaviour. Furthermore, the number of studies indicating that horses reacted to the restrictions were compared with the number of studies reporting no reaction. The limited number of studies available on single management restrictions did not allow conclusions to be drawn on the effect of each restriction separately, especially in the case of social companionship. However, when combinations of social contact, free movement and access to roughage were restricted, many of the horses had developed responses consistent with suffering. Passive Responses, indicating acute suffering, and Abnormal Behaviour, indicating suffering currently or at some time in the past, were especially clearly demonstrated. This provides further evidence of the usefulness of assessing behavioural parameters in combination with physiological measurements when evaluating horse welfare. This meta-analysis of the literature confirms that it is justified to claim that social contact, free movement and access to roughage are basic needs in horses.

1. Introduction

Every animal species has particular environmental requirements that are essential for its welfare [1,2,3] and these are described as basic needs. As a general assumption, it has been claimed that social contact, social companionship, free movement and access to roughage are horses’ basic needs [4,5,6,7,8,9,10]. Horses are said to need social contact because in a natural setting they live in large groups, with about 200–400 horses comprising a herd. These herds are divided into subgroups of harems (usually composed of one to five males, several females and their offspring) and bachelor bands (composed of males of different ages) [4,5,7,9,10]. Furthermore, horses are assumed to need social companionship because about one third of all horses form stable social bonds with members of their subgroup. Bonded animals mutually protect each other and their offspring, as well as protecting resources such as food, water and resting places [4,5,9]. In addition, it has been claimed that horses need free movement because under natural conditions, they cover between 3 to 30 km daily [4,6,7,8]. Finally, horses have been said to need access to roughage as, in nature, they feed on grass for 12 to 16 h per day [4,5,7,8].
However, it must be evaluated whether keeping horses under human management conditions that restrict their basic needs compromises welfare. Therefore, several studies have set out to assess whether horses suffer and, if so, which responses demonstrate suffering, when one or more of these needs are not fulfilled.
Many of these studies analysed whether horses reacted to such restrictions with metabolic [11,12,13,14], physiological [15,16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26], behavioural and/or cognitive [10,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32] signs of stress [11,12,13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25] that would indicate reduced welfare [27]. Horses may, for example, develop gastric ulcers caused by physiological stress and when access to roughage is restricted and gastric acidosis cannot be buffered by the feed and saliva amylase secreted during feeding [27]. Stress under restricted management conditions may also affect the animals’ emotional state and preferences for processing information in one or other brain hemisphere. [25,28,29,30,31,32]. Marr et al. [25], found that left shifts in horses’ motor and sensory laterality were useful behavioural indicators of changes in information processing in particular brain hemispheres when horses experienced stress from a change from group management to individual housing with initial training. Pioneering comparisons between hemispheric electroencephalogram (EEG) wave patterns in horses and those typical of emotional arousal in humans provide further evidence that horses respond negatively to restrictions in movement and social contact [26]. Furthermore, Löckener et al. [10] found that horses develop positive expectations towards their environment, i.e., a positive cognitive bias, when moved to group housing after experiencing management restriction.
Increased displays of certain behaviours in response to restrictions in basic needs have also been evaluated. Horses may show increased aggressive behaviour towards each other [18,22,33,34] and/or towards people [26,35], especially when their social relationships are disrupted [11,18,36,37,38]. Horses may show more interest in novel objects [38,39,40] and increased cooperativeness during training (i.e., trainability) when they are stabled in social groups rather than individually [13,21]. They may seek close proximity to their conspecifics more frequently when their social companionship is disrupted, as has been evaluated by applying nearest neighbour analysis [36]. Furthermore, horses may become more active when free movement, either individually or in social groups, is restricted [14,15,19,21,24,38,39], or when foals are weaned and separated from dams and social companions [40]. They may also show more hurried eating behaviour when roughage is limited [14]. On the other hand, horses may respond to restrictions in basic needs by reducing certain behaviour displays [11,15,18,19,20,21,22,24,26,35,38,39,40,41,42,43] and may even show depressive-type symptoms [22].
Behavioural disorders have also been assessed in horses [17,44], including stereotypies [8,26,45,46,47,48] and self-harming (i.e., redirected behaviour [8]). It has been debated whether abnormal behaviour is actually harmful or is rather a behavioural adaptation to a poor environment [47]. In this respect, most authors agree that stereotypies can be considered maladaptive behaviour indicating that horses are suffering under their housing or training conditions, and they give various reasons for this conclusion. Firstly, horses develop stereotypies in housing in which movement is reduced and there is little social contact. Secondly, free ranging, feral horses have never, to date, been observed showing stereotypic behaviour [7]. Thirdly, animals that displayed redirected behaviour and stereotypies also showed clinical signs of reduced welfare. These included: (a) self-inflicted skin lesions as a result of redirected behaviours, (b) gut damage, including lesions, ulceration and damaged mucosal tissue as a consequence of a wind-sucking and crib-biting [27,49] and (c) increased frequency of laminitis as a result of stereotypic movements such as weaving and box walking [27].
This literature review aims to provide an overview of studies that have evaluated the effects of changes in factors relating to horses’ environmental requirements (specifically social contact, social companionship, free movement, and access to roughage) on behavioural or physiological parameters (Table 1). We asked (a) whether a meta-analysis of the relevant literature supports the claim that social contact, social companionship, free movement and unlimited access to roughage are basic needs in horses, and (b) whether certain measurements can be considered reliable indicators for the analysis of animal welfare and basic need restrictions.

2. Materials and Methods

From August 2020 to February 2021, we searched the research platforms Research Gate, PubMed, Web of Science, Science Direct and Google Scholar for studies on social contact, social companionship, free movement and unlimited access to roughage in horses. We identified 38 studies (Table 1 and Table S1) on behavioural and physiological responses to management conditions in which one or more of the four proposed basic needs were restricted, and this will be the basis of this literature review. Information on the horses observed in the studies and the management conditions they lived under are given in the Supplementary Materials (Table S1). Of the studies we identified, 17 evaluated horses’ responses to the given management situation, and 21 studied horses’ responses to changes in management conditions. One study is cited, but not included in the analysis, as it describes the horses’ responses to particular management conditions but does not analyse responses to management restrictions.
As horse housing is a complex setting, isolating only one of the aspects is very difficult. Therefore, only a few studies have analysed changes in only one of the proposed basic needs, and most consider two or more needs simultaneously (see Table 1). These generally compared two or more horse groups under housing conditions that differed in one or more needs (see Table 1), but a few examined changes in housing conditions of one group.

2.1. Data Processing

Four steps were applied in the evaluation of the literature (Table 1 and Figure 1). Firstly, the literature was assigned to the proposed basic needs that were restricted. Secondly, the studies were categorized according to the type of response shown by the horse, and thirdly, we assessed whether changes in the behavioural and physiological reactions indicated that the restrictions were compromising the horses’ welfare. Finally, the frequencies of studies reporting responses were compared with the frequency of those reporting no responses to the restrictions.
The studies were grouped according to the proposed basic needs that were restricted (Table 1):
  • No basic need restricted, n = 1
  • Social Contact restricted, n = 4
  • Social Companionship restricted, n = 1
  • Free Movement restricted, n = 4
  • Access to Roughage restricted, n = 3
  • Social Contact and Access to Roughage restricted, n = 1
  • Free Movement and Social Contact restricted, n = 16
  • Free Movement and Access to Roughage restricted, n = 1
  • Free Movement, Social Contact and Access to Roughage restricted, n = 7
The studies were grouped according to evaluated behavioural and physiological measurements. The terms “shown” and “not shown” indicates whether the horses displayed any of the following types of responses to management conditions or not (Table 1, Figure 1):
a. Manuscripts evaluating ‘Stress Responses’, n = 16, methods of measuring ‘Stress Responses’, n = 21
i. shown, n = 14
ii. not shown, n = 7
Stress responses include increased metabolic rates and reduced body condition score [11], reduced growth rate [12], reduced eye temperature [13] and triiodothyronine and thyroxine excretion [14]. Physiological responses include changes in cardiovascular functions (heart rate [15,16,17,18,19], heart rate variability [19,20,21]), excretion of catecholamines [14], changes in stress hormone levels (blood cortisol [14,15,22], salivary cortisol [16,19,23], faecal glucocorticoid metabolites [12,13,17,20,24,25]), faecal immunoglobulin A [25], increased gastric acidosis [27] and changes in EEG wave patterns [26]. Behavioural parameters comprise changes in horses’ motor and sensory laterality [25,28,29,30,31,32] and changes in their positive or negative expectations towards their environment, i.e., in their cognitive bias [10].
b. Manuscripts evaluating ‘Active Responses’, n = 24, methods of measuring ‘Active Responses’, n = 33
i. shown, n = 22
ii. not shown, n = 11
Horses may show increased aggressive behaviour towards each other and/or towards people [11,18,22,26,33,34,35,36,37,38]. They may show more interest in novel objects [38,39,40] and increased cooperativeness during training (i.e., trainability) [13,21]. They may seek close proximity to their conspecifics more frequently, as observed by applying nearest neighbour analysis [36]. Furthermore, horses may become more active [14,15,19,21,24,38,39,40] and may also show more hurried eating behaviour [14].
c. Manuscripts evaluating ‘Passive Responses’, n = 17, methods of measuring ‘Passive Responses’, n = 23
i. shown, n = 15
ii. not shown, n = 8
Passive responses include reduced reactivity towards stimuli and human presence [15,22,26,40,41], reduced close contact with conspecifics [35] and reduced trainability [15,35,43]. Reduced activity [42] was evaluated by measuring the time spent lying down [11,21,39], moving [11,19], and the distance moved [20]. Some horses may show depressive-like behaviours [22].
d. Manuscripts evaluating ‘Abnormal Behaviour’, n = 14, methods of measuring ‘Abnormal Behaviour’, n = 17
i. shown, n = 14
ii. not shown, n = 3
Abnormal Behaviours include self-harming (i.e., redirected behaviour such as self-biting [8]), stereotypies such as crib-biting, wind-sucking, weaving and box-walking [14,41,48], as well as other behaviours such as, wood-chewing, bed-eating, manure-eating, rug-chewing or tearing, stable kicking, aggression towards humans and masturbation [8,44,55].

2.2. Data Analysis

The R-Project statistical software (R Development Core Team 2021, https://www.r-project.org/) was used for the statistical analysis and Excel for creating the figures when analysing the frequencies of studies on basic need restrictions. Some of the data were not normally distributed (Shapiro–Wilk Test). Therefore, Generalized Linear Models (GLMs) for multivariate testing with fixed factors were applied. For a general approach, we analysed the frequency of studies reporting reactions or no reaction under the particular restrictions of basic needs and the different responses shown. The GLM (formula = number of manuscripts evaluating response~response versus no response + response type, family = poisson (identity), data = Dataset) was used. We continued by applying a nested Generalized Linear Model (GLM) to analyse whether manuscripts reported differences for showing or not showing responses nested within the different types of response. Therefore, the GLM (formula = number of manuscripts. evaluating response~response versus no response % in % response type, family = poisson (identity), data = Dataset) was applied. The full statistical data are given in the Supplementary Materials (File S1). Binomial Tests were applied to compare the frequencies of showing or not showing certain types of responses under certain management restrictions. All tests were two-tailed, and the significance level was set at 0.05.

3. Results

Overall, a significant number of studies reported that horses did show responses to restrictions in the proposed basic needs (responses shown: n = 90, not shown: n = 29; GLM: n = 37, z = 4.08, p < 0.001; Table 1, Figure 1, File S1). However, reports on whether horses showed responses differed between the particular response types (GLM: n = 37, z = −2.57, p = 0.01; Figure 1, Table 1, File S1).
Of the studies on changes in behavioural and physiological measurements of ‘Stress Responses’, n = 13 studies reported that horses showed responses and n = 7 did not (Table 1, Figure 1). The studies did not clearly indicate stress responses for single restrictions, as when studies found several stress measurements changed, i.e., indicating stress, the same study or other studies also found other stress measurements remained unchanged, i.e., indicating no stress. When the reactions to all the different restrictions were compared, the number of reports showing stress responses correlated with the number of reports showing no response (GLM: n = 37, z = 2.49, p = 0.01; Figure 1, Table 1, File S1).
The same was true for the ‘Active Responses’, with n = 14 studies reporting that active responses were shown and n = 8 reporting they were not (Figure 1, Table 1). Again, while many parameters changed almost as many did not change when analysing a particular restriction and there was a general correlation between active responses and no active response over all the basic need restrictions (GLM: n = 37, z = 2.63; p = 0.008, Figure 1, Table 1, File S1).
‘Passive Responses’ were reported in n = 14 studies and no ‘Passive Responses’ for n = 8 studies (Figure 1, Table 1). ‘Passive Responses’ to particular restrictions indicated more clearly than ‘Stress Responses’ and ‘Active Responses’ that horses responded with changes in behaviour to certain restrictions. The literature revealed only a trend in correlation between the horses showing passive responses to those showing no passive response when comparing all the different restrictions in the basic needs (GLM: n = 37, z = 1.85, p = 0.06; Figure 1, Table 1, File S1).
Finally, a significant number of horses demonstrated the response ‘Abnormal Behaviour’ in response to any restriction in basic needs (‘Abnormal Behaviour’: n = 12, no ‘Abnormal Behaviour’: n =3, Binomial test: p = 0.02, Figure 1). Horses reacted more clearly by either showing abnormal behaviour or not showing abnormal behaviour to particular restrictions, as there was again only a trend for a correlation between reports of abnormal behaviour and reports of no abnormal behaviour across the studies on all the different restrictions examined (GLM: n = 37, z = 1.74, p = 0.08; Figure 1, Table 1, File S1).
The literature provides no clear conclusion as to whether horses generally show responses to restrictions in just one of the parameters of social contact, social companionship, free movement and access to roughage in isolation (Figure 1). For most of the comparisons the sample sizes were too small for applying statistical tests, the few that allowed statistical testing revealed no difference between measurements that show and those that do not show a response to the restrictions (Binomial tests: all p > 0.05; Figure 1). When social contact, free movement and access to roughage were mutually restricted, horses showed behavioural and/or physiological responses (Binomial test: p < 0.001; Figure 1).

4. Discussion

The aim of this study was to evaluate whether certain measurements can be considered reliable indicators for the analysis of animal welfare under basic need restrictions by analysing the results published in the literature. The interpretation of the studies calls for caution as positive reporting biases may result in more studies that prove a certain measurement to be effective than those reporting no effect [56]. However, the literature also contains several studies that actually report missing responses of horses to the measurements under observation. We therefore found the results of the meta analysis of these studies worth discussing.
Especially the development of abnormal behaviour and stress responses under the long-term stress of compromised environmental conditions has been considered a maladaptive strategy which does not provide the animals with options to deal with such conditions [8,57,58]. When animals develop these responses, they are said to be clearly suffering [58,59]. The present literature review provides evidence that this claim is justified in horses, especially in relation to abnormal behaviour. Horses develop significant levels of abnormal behaviour when social contact, social companionship, free movement and access to roughage are compromised. Therefore, it appears to be justified to use the display of abnormal behaviour as a behavioural animal welfare indicator when analysing the quality of horse housing and training, as has been established for animal welfare protocols [60].
However, it remains debatable whether a long-term display of abnormal behaviour provides strong evidence of a horse suffering under its present management conditions. Stereotypic behaviour may have developed under previous conditions and persist, even when management and training return to favourable conditions [45,46,57]. A recent development in EEG wave pattern analysis [26] offers promising new insights into this debate. Horses that had consistently displayed stereotypic behaviour for at least one year and lived under restricted management conditions showed EEG wave patterns comparable to those indicating negative emotional states in humans. However, caution should be exercised when evaluating welfare in horses displaying stereotypic behaviour as horses secrete dopamine when engaging in such behaviour and this elicits a positive emotional state in the animal (see for review: [61]).
Similarly, the measurement of behavioural and physiological stress parameters did not necessarily indicate that all horses experienced stress in compromised management conditions (Figure 1). Some studies found that horses did respond with behavioural or physiological stress parameters, and others did not (Table 1). This discrepancy may be due to difficulties in assessing long-term stress. When animals suffer stress for longer periods, such as when their environmental requirements are restricted for a long time, several physiological parameters, such as stress hormones and cardiovascular functions may return to base levels or below [13,25,62,63].
There are promising stress parameters that may allow long-term stress to be evaluated, such as immune cell suppression, changes in motor laterality [25,64], increased hemispheric laterality and EEG wave patterns analogous to those measured in humans with negative emotional arousal [26]. However, there is not yet sufficient evidence for these to be included in the present literature survey because they have only been studied in a few pioneering papers [25,26,64,65].
There may also be individual differences between horses in their stress resistance and the importance an individual attaches to any particular stressor [63]. In addition, previous experience with the restrictions in basic needs may be a factor. For example, horses that were born and raised in a stabled environment may be less stressed by restricted movement than horses raised at grass and then moved to a stabled environment [25]. Moreover, horses that had previously experienced individual housing did not display any significant differences in physiological stress responses between individual housing with semi-contact to conspecifics and group housing [13,24], whereas horses that were naïve to individual housing showed significant physiological stress responses when moved from group to individual housing [17,19,25]. A combination of several physiological and behavioural stress parameters may provide the strongest evidence for stress in horses as some studies found conflicting results when comparing a limited spectrum of physiological and behavioural data [63].
However, the proportion of horses showing passive responses supports the claim that many horses suffer long term stress under the investigated management restrictions [22,52,66]. Passive responses, such as reductions in activity, feeding, behaviour displays, contact to persons or other conspecifics and reactions to the environment indicate that horses withdraw from external stimuli and may show a depressive-like state [22,66]. These responses are maladaptive for animals such as horses, which are both flight animals that rely on fast responses to acute challenges and social animals that rely on fast responses to social challenges [9].
Interestingly, the analysis of ‘Active Responses’ did not provide a clear conclusion. Some studies reported that horses showed active responses when the animals were faced with restricted basic needs and others did not (Table 1 and Table S1). It may be difficult to clearly distinguish the level of activity that constitutes a stress response, as elevated aggression and movement may counteract mild stress [34,37].
As only very few studies succeeded in isolating the effects of restrictions in just one of the four proposed basic needs of social contact, social companionship, free movement and access to roughage, a clear statement on whether animals can generally cope with the particular restrictions remains elusive. However, there were sufficient studies on combined restrictions in social contact, free movement and access to roughage, and when the horses’ environments were restricted in these three conditions, they appeared to suffer, as ‘Abnormal Behaviour’ and ‘Passive Response’ had clearly developed. This is consistent with the finding that abnormal behaviour display may be caused by a variety factors (e.g., genetics, rearing conditions, housing and feeding [48]).

5. Conclusions

We conclude that under combined restrictions of social contact, social companionship, free movement and access to roughage horses display signs of suffering. The literature available on individual so called “basic needs”, does not allow us to isolate the effect of each of them, especially in the case of social companionship. However, the development of abnormal behaviour and passive coping strategies, can be considered signs of suffering, and these were displayed under separate restrictions in social contact, free movement and free access to roughage, as well as under combined restrictions of two or more of the proposed basic needs.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ani11061798/s1, File S1: Statistical Data. Complete Generalized Linear Models (GLMs), Table S1: Summary table of literature content. More detailed information on the restrictions, the horses, their management and their responses.

Author Contributions

Each author made substantial contributions to the conception or design of the work. Individual contributions were done for Conceptualization, K.K., L.E. and I.M.; Methodology, K.K., L.E. and I.M.; Software, K.K.; Validation, K.K., L.E. and I.M.; Formal Analysis, K.K.; Resources, K.K., L.E. and I.M.; Data Curation, K.K., L.E. and I.M.; Writing–Original Draft Prep-aration, K.K., L.E., K.F. and I.M.; Writing–Review & Editing, K.K., L.E., K.F. and I.M.; Visualization, K.K., L.E. and I.M.; Funding Acquisition, K.K. Each author has approved the submitted version (and versions substantially edited by journal staff that involves the author’s contribution to the study); and agrees to be personally accountable for the author’s own contributions and for ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work, even ones in which the author was not personally involved, are appropriately investigated, resolved, and documented in the literature. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The article processing charge was funded by the Baden-Württemberg Ministry of Science, Research and Culture and the Nuertingen-Geislingen University in the funding programme Open Access Publishing.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Ethical review and approval were waived for this study because it did not involve humans or animals.

Informed Consent Statement

No humans or animals were involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

All data are provided in the manuscript and the Supplementary Material.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

References

  1. Noë, R.; Hammerstein, P. Biological markets. Trends Ecol. Evol. 1995, 10, 336–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Pusey, A.E.; Packer, C. The Ecology of relationships. In Behavioural Ecology; Krebs, J.R., Davis, N.B., Eds.; Blackwell Scientific Publication Oxford: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2003; pp. 254–283. [Google Scholar]
  3. Korb, J.; Heinze, J. Ecology of Social Evolution; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2008; ISBN 9783540759560. [Google Scholar]
  4. Berger, J. Wild Horses of the Great Basin; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1986. [Google Scholar]
  5. Rubenstein, D.I. Ecology and sociality in horses and zebras. In Ecological Aspects of Social Evolution; Rubenstein, D.I., Wrangham, R.W., Eds.; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1986; pp. 282–302. [Google Scholar]
  6. Mills, D.; Clarke, A. Housing, Management and Welfare. In The Welfare of Horses; Waran, N., Ed.; Springer Publishing: New York, NY, USA, 2002; pp. 77–97. [Google Scholar]
  7. Goodwin, D. Horse behaviour: Evolution, domestication and feralisation. In The Welfare of Horses; Waran, N., Ed.; Springer Publishing: New York, NY, USA, 2002; pp. 1–18. [Google Scholar]
  8. Waters, A.J.; Nicol, C.; French, N.P. Factors influencing the development of stereotypic and redirected behaviours in young horses: Findings of a four year prospective epidemiological study. Equine Vet. J. 2010, 34, 572–579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  9. Krueger, K. Social Ecology of Horses. In Ecology of Social Evolution; Korb, J., Heinze, J., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2008; pp. 195–206. [Google Scholar]
  10. Löckener, S.; Reese, S.; Erhard, M.H.; Wöhr, A.-C. Pasturing in herds after housing in horseboxes induces a positive cognitive bias in horses. J. Vet. Behav. 2016, 11, 50–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Hoffmann, G.; Bentke, A.; Rose-Meierhöfer, S.; Berg, W.; Mazetti, P.; Hardarson, G.H. Influence of an active stable system on the behavior and body condition of Icelandic horses. Animal 2012, 6, 1684–1693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  12. Heleski, C.; Shelle, A.; Nielsen, B.; Zanella, A. Influence of housing on weanling horse behavior and subsequent welfare. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2002, 78, 291–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Yarnell, K.; Hall, C.; Royle, C.; Walker, S.L. Domesticated horses differ in their behavioural and physiological responses to isolated and group housing. Physiol. Behav. 2015, 143, 51–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Mal, M.E.; Friend, T.H.; Lay, D.C.; Vogelsang, S.G.; Jenkins, O.C. Behavioural responses of mares to short-term confinement and social isolation. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1991, 31, 13–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Rivera, E.; Benjamin, S.; Nielsen, B.; Shelle, J.; Zanella, A. Behavioral and physiological responses of horses to initial training: The comparison between pastured versus stalled horses. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2002, 78, 235–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Harewood, E.; McGowan, C. Behavioral and physiological responses to stabling in naive horses. J. Equine Vet. Sci. 2005, 25, 164–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Niederhöfer, S. Stressbelastung bei Pferden in Abhängigkeit des Haltungssystems. Ph.D. Thesis, Tierärztliche Hochschule Hannover, Hannover, Germany, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  18. Hartmann, E. Managing Horses in Groups to Improve Horse Welfare and Human Safety. Ph.D. Thesis, Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences, Uppsala, Sweden, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  19. Erber, R.; Wulf, M.; Aurich, J.; Rose-Meierhöfer, S.; Hoffmann, G.; Von Lewinski, M.; Möstl, E.; Aurich, C. Stress Response of Three-year-old Horse Mares to Changes in Husbandry System During Initial Equestrian Training. J. Equine Vet. Sci. 2013, 33, 1088–1094. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Hoffmann, G.; Bockisch, F.-J.; Kreimeier, P. Einfluss des Haltungssystems auf die Bewegungsaktivität und Stressbelastung bei Pferden in Auslaufhaltungssystemen [Influence of the husbandry system on the movement activity and stress exposure of horses in discharge husbandry systems]. Agric. For. Res. 2009, 2, 105–112. [Google Scholar]
  21. Werhahn, H.; Hessel, E.F.; Van den Weghe, H.F. Competition Horses Housed in Single Stalls (II): Effects of Free Exercise on the Behavior in the Stable, the Behavior during Training, and the Degree of Stress. J. Equine Vet. Sci. 2012, 32, 22–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Fureix, C.; Bourjade, M.; Henry, S.; Sankey, C.; Hausberger, M. Exploring aggression regulation in managed groups of horses Equus caballus. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2012, 138, 216–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Aurich, J.; Wulf, M.; Ille, N.; Erber, R.; Von Lewinski, M.; Palme, R.; Aurich, C. Effects of season, age, sex, and housing on salivary cortisol concentrations in horses. Domest. Anim. Endocrinol. 2015, 52, 11–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Wille, E. Vergleichende Untersuchungen von Pferden in Zeitweiliger Anbindehaltung, Einzelboxenhaltung und Gruppenhal-tung unter Besonderer Berücksichtigung des Liegeverhaltens; Tierärztliche Hochschule: Hannover, Germany, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  25. Marr, I.; Preisler, V.; Farmer, K.; Stefanski, V.; Krueger, K. Non-invasive stress evaluation in domestic horses (Equus caballus): Impact of housing conditions on sensory laterality and immunoglobulin A. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2020, 7, 191994. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Stomp, M.; D’Ingeo, S.; Henry, S.; Cousillas, H.; Hausberger, M. Brain activity reflects (chronic) welfare state: Evidence from individual electroencephalography profiles in an animal model. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2021, 236, 105271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Rowe, J.B.; Lees, M.J.; Pethick, D.W. Prevention of Acidosis and Laminitis Associated with Grain Feeding in Horses. J. Nutr. 1994, 124, 2742S–2744S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. LaRose, C.; Richard-Yris, M.-A.; Hausberger, M.; Rogers, L.J. Laterality of horses associated with emotionality in novel situations. Laterality 2006, 11, 355–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. De Boyer Des Roches, A.; Richard-Yris, M.-A.; Henry, S.; Ezzaouïa, M.; Hausberger, M. Laterality and emotions: Visual laterality in the domestic horse (Equus caballus) differs with objects’ emotional value. Physiol. Behav. 2008, 94, 487–490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Rogers, L.J. Relevance of brain and behavioural lateralization to animal welfare. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2010, 127, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Marr, I.; Farmer, K.; Krüger, K. Evidence for Right-Sided Horses Being More Optimistic than Left-Sided Horses. Animals 2018, 8, 219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  32. Giljov, A.; Karenina, K. Differential roles of the right and left brain hemispheres in the social interactions of a free-ranging ungulate. Behav. Process. 2019, 168, 103959. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Cooper, J.; McDonald, L.; Mills, D.S. The effect of increasing visual horizons on stereotypic weaving: Implications for the social housing of stabled horses. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2000, 69, 67–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Flauger, B.; Krueger, K. Aggression level and enclosure size in horses (Equus caballus). Pferdeheilkunde 2013, 29, 495–504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Søndergaard, E.; Ladewig, J. Group housing exerts appositive effect on the behaviour of young horses during training. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2004, 87, 105–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Christensen, J.W.; Ladewig, J.; Søndergaard, E.; Malmkvist, J. Effects of individual versus group stabling on social behaviour in domestic stallions. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2002, 75, 233–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Knubben, J.M.; Fürst, A.; Gygax, L.; Stauffacher, M. Bite and kick injuries in horses: Prevalence, risk factors and prevention. Equine Vet. J. 2008, 40, 219–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Werhahn, H.; Hessel, E.F.; Schulze, H.; Weghe, H.F.V.D. Temporary Turnout for Free Exercise in Groups: Effects on the Behavior of Competition Horses Housed in Single Stalls. J. Equine Vet. Sci. 2011, 31, 417–425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Chaplin, S.; Gretgrix, L. Effect of housing conditions on activity and lying behaviour of horses. Animal 2010, 4, 792–795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Nicol, C.; Badnell-Waters, A.; Bice, R.; Kelland, A.; Wilson, A.; Harris, P. The effects of diet and weaning method on the behaviour of young horses. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2005, 95, 205–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Visser, E.K.; Ellis, A.D.; Van Reenen, C.G. The effect of two different housing conditions on the welfare of young horses stabled for the first time. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008, 114, 521–533. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Jørgensen, G.H.M.; Liestøl, S.H.-O.; Bøe, K.E. Effects of enrichment items on activity and social interactions in domestic horses (Equus caballus). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2011, 129, 100–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Ruet, A.; LeMarchand, J.; Parias, C.; Mach, N.; Moisan, M.-P.; Foury, A.; Briant, C.; Lansade, L. Housing Horses in Individual Boxes Is a Challenge with Regard to Welfare. Animals 2019, 9, 621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Pell, S.M.; McGreevy, P. Prevalence of stereotypic and other problem behaviours in thoroughbred horses. Aust. Vet. J. 1999, 77, 678–679. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. McGreevy, P.; French, N.P.; Nicol, C.J. The prevalence of abnormal behaviours in dressage, eventing and endurance horses in relation to stabling. Vet. Rec. 1995, 137, 36–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. McGreevy, P.D.; Cripps, P.J.; French, N.P.; Green, L.E.; Nicol, C.J. Management factors associated with stereotypic and redi-rected behaviour in the thoroughbred horse. Equine Vet. J. 1995, 27, 86–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Cooper, J.J.; Mason, G.J. The identification of abnormal behaviour and behavioural problems in stabled horses and their rela-tionship to horse welfare: A comparative review. Equine Vet. J. 1998, 27, 5–9. [Google Scholar]
  48. Bachmann, I.; Audigé, L.; Stauffacher, M. Risk factors associated with behavioural disorders of crib-biting, weaving and box-walking in Swiss horses. Equine Vet. J. 2010, 35, 158–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Nicol, C. Equine learning: Progress and suggestions for future research. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2002, 78, 193–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Christensen, J.W.; Søndergaard, E.; Thodberg, K.; Halekoh, U. Effects of repeated regrouping on horse behaviour and injuries. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2011, 133, 199–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Brinkmann, L.; Gerken, M.; Riek, A. Effect of long-term feed restriction on the health status and welfare of a robust horse breed, the Shetland pony (Equus ferus caballus). Res. Vet. Sci. 2013, 94, 826–831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Brinkmann, L.; Gerken, M.; Hambly, C.; Speakman, J.R.; Riek, A. Saving energy during hard times: Energetic adaptations of Shetland pony mares. J. Exp. Biol. 2014, 217, 4320–4327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  53. Vitale, V.; Balocchi, R.; Varanini, M.; Sgorbini, M.; Macerata, A.; Sighieri, C.; Baragli, P. The effects of restriction of movement on the reliability of heart rate variability measurements in the horse (Equus caballus). J. Vet. Behav. 2013, 8, 400–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Lesimple, C.; Reverchon-Billot, L.; Galloux, P.; Stomp, M.; Boichot, L.; Coste, C.; Henry, S.; Hausberger, M. Free movement: A key for welfare improvement in sport horses? Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2020, 225, 104972. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Redbo, I.; Redbo-Torstensson, P.; Ödberg, F.O.; Hedendahl, A.; Holm, J. Factors affecting behavioural disturbances in race-horses. Anim. Sci. 1998, 66, 475–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Dwan, K.; Altman, D.G.; Arnaiz, J.A.; Bloom, J.; Chan, A.-W.; Cronin, E.; Decullier, E.; Easterbrook, P.J.; von Elm, E.; Gamble, C.; et al. Systematic Review of the Empirical Evidence of Study Publication Bias and Outcome Reporting Bias. PLoS ONE 2008, 3, e3081. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Nicol, C. Understanding equine stereotypies. Equine Vet. J. 2010, 31, 20–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Kupriyanov, R.; Zhdanov, R. The eustress concept: Problems and outlooks. World J. Med. Sci. 2014, 11, 179–185. [Google Scholar]
  59. Cooper, J.; Albentosa, M.J. Behavioural adaptation in the domestic horse: Potential role of apparently abnormal responses including stereotypic behaviour. Livest. Prod. Sci. 2005, 92, 177–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Costa, E.D.; Dai, F.; Lebelt, D.; Scholz, P.; Barbieri, S.; Canali, E.; Minero, M. Welfare assessment of horses: The AWIN approach. Anim. Welf. 2016, 25, 481–488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. McBride, S.D.; Parker, M.; Roberts, K.; Hemmings, A. Applied neurophysiology of the horse; implications for training, husbandry and welfare. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2017, 190, 90–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Pawluski, J.; Jego, P.; Henry, S.; Bruchet, A.; Palme, R.; Coste, C.; Hausberger, M. Low plasma cortisol and faecal cortisol metabolite measures as indicators of compromised welfare in domestic horses (Equus caballus). PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0182257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Von Borstel, U.K.; Visser, E.; Hall, C. Indicators of stress in equitation. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2017, 190, 43–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. May, A. Evaluierung von Stressparametern beim Pferd im Zusammenhang mit dem Klinikaufenthalt [Evaluation of Stress Parameters in the Hospitalized Horse]. Ph.D. Thesis, Ludwig Maximilians University (LMU), München, Germany, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  65. Rogers, L.J. A Matter of Degree: Strength of Brain Asymmetry and Behaviour. Symmetry 2017, 9, 57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  66. Fureix, C.; Jego, P.; Henry, S.; Lansade, L.; Hausberger, M. Towards an Ethological Animal Model of Depression? A Study on Horses. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e39280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Number of reports of responses (dark colours, i.e., black and blue) versus number of reports of no response (light colours: skin and yellow) to restrictions in proposed “basic needs” described for response types across all studies. Abnormal Behaviour responses were significantly shown across all studies (Binomial Test: p = 0.02). Responses were also shown when social contact, free movement and access to roughage were mutually restricted (Binomial Test: p < 0.001). More detailed information on the responses, the test conditions and the horses included in the studies are given in Table 1 and in the Supplementary Materials (Table S1).
Figure 1. Number of reports of responses (dark colours, i.e., black and blue) versus number of reports of no response (light colours: skin and yellow) to restrictions in proposed “basic needs” described for response types across all studies. Abnormal Behaviour responses were significantly shown across all studies (Binomial Test: p = 0.02). Responses were also shown when social contact, free movement and access to roughage were mutually restricted (Binomial Test: p < 0.001). More detailed information on the responses, the test conditions and the horses included in the studies are given in Table 1 and in the Supplementary Materials (Table S1).
Animals 11 01798 g001
Table 1. Literature on restrictions in “proposed basic needs” in horses. Background colours indicate studies on the restricted basic needs: grey = no restriction, light orange = social contact, light blue = social companionship, light yellow = movement, dark grey = feed, orange = social contact and feed, green = movement and social contact, yellow = movement, social contact and feed. The columns list the test condition or changes in test conditions, the response measurements and the horses’ responses that were observed. For detailed information on the horses and their management conditions please see Supplementary Materials (Table S1).
Table 1. Literature on restrictions in “proposed basic needs” in horses. Background colours indicate studies on the restricted basic needs: grey = no restriction, light orange = social contact, light blue = social companionship, light yellow = movement, dark grey = feed, orange = social contact and feed, green = movement and social contact, yellow = movement, social contact and feed. The columns list the test condition or changes in test conditions, the response measurements and the horses’ responses that were observed. For detailed information on the horses and their management conditions please see Supplementary Materials (Table S1).
ReferenceRestrictionCondition(s)
Observed
MeasurementsResponse(s)
Hoffmann et al. 2012 [11]No restrictionGroup housing-no other conditionsBody condition score
Behaviour: aggression
Behaviour: social hierarchy
Behaviour: synchronisation
Good
Low
Stable
Good
Christensen et al. 2002 [36]Social contactBehaviour of young horses that had either been raised in group housing or single boxes were compared with behaviour when the horses were put out to pasture with other horses of the same ageNearest neighbourHorses from group housing sought proximity to former stable mates
Behaviour: aggressionHigher in previously singly housed horses
Behaviour: agonistic encounters (action and retreat response)More subtle encounters in previously group housed horses
Behaviour: social groomingMore frequent in previously singly housed horses
Behaviour: playMore frequent in previously singly housed horses
Cooper et al. 2000 [33]Social contactComparison between different types of single box housing:
F: front top-half of the door open with a view of the stable courtyard FB: front half-door open and a similar half-door open at the back of the stable with a view to the surrounding fields
B: back open only
FS: front and one-side panel open with a view into the adjacent stable ALL 4: front, back and both sides open
Stereotypies: weavingMost common prior to feeding in the morning and prior to putting out to pasture in the afternoon. Less weaving in the FS and All4 designs than the F design
Stereotypies: repetitive noddingFB, B, FS and All4: less nodding than in the F treatment
Hartmann 2010 [18]Social contact5 min social isolation from group housing (individually or in pairs)Heart rate
Behaviour: towards humans
No change
No change
Nicol et al. 2005 [40]Social contactComparison between barn and paddock weaned foalsStressHigher levels of stress in barn weaned foals
Christensen et al. 2011 [50]Social companionshipComparison between housing in unstable (changing) groups and stable (constant) groupsBehaviour: agonisticMore in unstable groups
Behaviour: agonistic with contactMore in unstable groups
Behaviour: greetingMore in unstable groups
Behaviour: playMore variable in unstable groups
Behaviour: agonistic (further behaviours)No difference between housing groups
Behaviour: affiliative (further behaviours)No difference between housing groups
Chaplin and Gretgrix 2010 [39]MovementSame horses compared under Fully stabled (FS), Partially stabled (PS), Yard (Y), and Paddock (P) conditionsActivity: time spent active More active on release from FS and PS housing
Activity: time spent lying downNo change
Flauger and Krueger 2013 [34]MovementDifferent sizes of group paddockBehaviour: aggressiveDecreased with increasing size of group paddock
Behaviour: submissiveDecreased with increasing size of group paddock
Hoffmann et al. 2009 [20]MovementProvision of additional movement on pasture or in horse walker Stress: faecal glucocorticoidsDecreased after movement
Stress: heart rate variabilityDecrease of sympathetic activity after movement
Activity: movementIncreased
McGreevy et al. 1995a [45] MovementLength of time spent in single box housingStereotypic behaviourIncreased with time spent in individual boxes
Brinkmann et al. 2013 [51]FeedFeed restrictionBody condition scoreDecreased
Total bilirubinIncreased
NEFAIncreased
Total bilirubin and beta-hydroxyburyratHigher in males than in females
Thyroxine concentrationsNo change
Brinkmann et al. 2014 [52]FeedMetabolic adaptation to environmental conditions, comparing different quantities of feed and summer and winter temperaturesBody condition scoreReduced in winter
Body massReduced in winter
Resting heart rateReduced in winter
Metabolic rateReduced in winter
Nocturnal hypothermiaIncreased in winter
McGreevy et al. 1995b [46] FeedStabled without straw bedding and with less feed than 6.8 kg forage/day Abnormal behaviour Increased
Jørgensen et al. 2011 [42]Social contact &
feed
Single turnout on paddocks versus group turnout.
Feeding grass and roughage
Behaviour: passiveIncreased when turned out singly
Behaviour: passiveReduced when fed with roughage or grass
Behaviour: item explorationNo difference
Aurich et al. 2015 [23] Movement & social contactGroup versus individual housingStress: salivary cortisol No significant difference
Erber et al. 2013 [19]Movement & social contactTransfer from group housing to individual housing with initial ridingStress: salivary cortisolIncrease after transfer
Stress: heart rate Increase during transfer
Stress: heat rate variabilityDecrease after transfer and after riding (= increase of sympathetic activity)
Activity: locomotionDecrease after transfer
Stress: salivary cortisolNo change between housing conditions
Stress: heart rate No change between housing conditions
Fureix et al. 2012 [22]Movement & social contactHorses showing normal and horses showing withdrawn posture under conditions of no free movement and no free social contactStress: plasma cortisolLow after work-further decrease with increased withdrawn posture
Activity: body postureWithdrawn posture 1–4 times every 30 min
Activity: head, ear, eye movementReduced in withdrawn posture
Activity: response to tactile stimuliReduced in withdrawn posture
Activity: response to sudden approaching personReduced in withdrawn posture
Activity: response to novel objectsReduced in withdrawn posture
Harewood and McGowan 2005 [16]Movement & social contactGroup versus individual housingBehavioural scoresHigher in individual than in group housing
Stress: heart rateNo difference
Stress: salivary cortisolNo difference
Diurnal rhythm heart rate and salivary cortisolNo diurnal rhythm under either condition
Heleski et al. 2002 [12]Movement & social contactPaddock-kept weanlings versus stable housed weanlingsNearest neighbour Paddock weaned foals stayed near conspecifics for longer
Activity: grazingHigher in paddock weaned foals
BehavioursGreater variety in paddock weaned foals
Abnormal behaviour Greater in stable weaned foals
Stress: faecal glucocorticoid metabolitesNo difference
Löckener et al. 2016 [10]Movement & social contactLiving at pasture with social contact following single box housing Behaviour: positive cognitive biasEnhanced in horses on pasture with social contact
Niederhöfer 2009 [17]Movement & social contactComparison between group housing, single box without paddock, and single box with paddockStress: faecal glucocorticoid metabolitesLower in group housing
Stress: heart rate variabilityLower in group housing
Abnormal behaviourCircling in horses in single boxes without paddock
Pell and McGreevy 1999 [44]Movement & social contactStable housing compared to keeping at pasture Abnormal behaviourMore frequent in stabled horses
Rivera et al. 2002 [15]Movement & social contactStable housing versus keeping at pastureStress: heart rateLower in stabled horses
Illness: gastric acidosisMore frequent in stabled horses
Trainability: duration training procedureLonger in stabled horses
Trainability: duration habituation groundworkLonger in stabled horses
Trainability: head neck extension during trainingGreater in stabled horses
Behaviour: bucking and jumpingMore frequent in stabled horses
Stress: plasma cortisolNo difference
Trainability: between mount and dismountNo effect of housing conditions
Ruet et al. 2019 [43]Movement & social contactHousing with window opening towards the external environment and straw bedding compared with housing with no window and non-straw bedding, different forage: grain feeding ratios and meal frequenciesBehaviour: aggressionLower in housing with window and straw bedding
Stereotypies: oralHigher with grain feeding
Stereotypies: oralNumber of meals per day had no effect
Trainability: equitation and trainingNo difference
Sondergaard and Ladewig 2004 [35]Movement & social contactEffect of single versus group housing on trainingActivity: restlessness before trainingGreater in single housed horses
Behaviour: biting, kicking during trainingMore frequent in horses in single housing
Behaviour: defecation during trainingMore frequent in horses in single housing
TrainabilityHorses in group housing passed more training stages
Vitale et al. 2013 [53]Movement & social contactpaddock turnout versus individual box housing versus fixed in a stockStress: heart rate variabilityDecreased with reduced locomotion (= increased sympathetic activity)
Werhahn et al. 2011 [38]Movement & social contactNo turnout compared to turnoutBehaviours: standing alert, aggression, occupation with equipment, occupation with bedding, dozing, sternal recumbency and lateral recumbency More frequent in the horses with no turnout
Activity: walking, standing/watching More frequent in the horses with no turnout
Trainability: willingness to perform Enhanced in horses with turnout
Trainability: duration of training Shorter in horses with turnout
LocomotionNo difference
Werhahn et al. 2012 [21]Movement & social contactSingle box housing, individual turnout, group turnoutStress: heart rate variability measures SDNN, RMSSD and LF/HFHigher sympathetic activity when horses were stabled in single boxes
Behaviour: lying downLonger when horses had group turnout
Trainability: willingness to perform Slightly better when the horses had turnout
Behaviour: standing alert, dozing, eating, occupation No change
Locomotion No change
Wille 2010 [24]Movement & social contactOpen barn housing, individual box housing, tied up in stallsStress: faecal glucocorticoid metabolitesLower in open barn system
Behaviour: standingLonger when tied in stalls
Behaviour: lying on the chestLonger in open barn system or individual boxes
Behaviour: lying on the sideLonger in open barn system
LocomotionMore in open barn system
Behaviour: food consumptionNo difference
Yarnell et al. 2015 [13]Movement & social contactSingle housing with no contact (SHNC), group housing with full contact (GHFC), paired housing with full contact (PHFC)Stress: faecal glucocorticoid metabolitesHigher in SHNC
Stress: eye temperatureLower in GHFC
Behaviour: standingLower in GHFC
Behaviour: active and social negative behaviours Higher in GHFC and PHFC
Trainability: handlingMore difficult with SHNC horses
Lesimple et al. 2020 [54]Movement & feedChange from single box with no paddock to housing with turnout and ad lib hayBehaviour: vigilance, excitement and locomotion Decreased with turnout and ad lib hay
Behaviour: feeding with ears laid backDecreased with turnout and ad lib hay
StereotypiesDecreased with turnout and ad lib hay
Blood: oxytocinIncreased with turnout and ad lib hay
Blood: cell counts, serotoninNo change
Bachmann et al. 2003 [48]Movement, social contact & feedRestricting feed and daily pastureStereotypies: crib-biting, weaving and box-walkingIncreased
Mal et al. 1991 [14]Movement, social contact & feed Horses of different temperaments; housing in isolation (ISS), at pasture (P), in individual boxes with social contact (C)Behaviour: time eating grain, grain-eating bouts More in horses of medium and highly reactive temperaments in isolation (ISS)
Behaviour: forage-eating boutsLonger in calm horses at pasture
Activity: distance travelled, time spent trotting, number of trotting bouts, number of standing bouts, number of total activity bouts More in isolation horses
Activity: duration standingLess in isolation horses
Triiodothyronine Highest in isolation horses
Marr et al. 2020 [25]Movement, social contact & feedChange from group housing to individual housing, and initial trainingStress: faecal glucocorticoid metabolitesIncreased after change from group to individual housing after 24 h, 48 h, and 1 week. Increased after 24 h, 48 h, and 2 months of initial training
Behaviour: motor laterality Left shift for 1 week after change from group to individual housing, and after 2 months of initial training
Behaviour: sensory laterality Left shift 24 h after change from group to individual housing, and (not significantly) 24 h after initial training
Stress: Immunoglobulin ADecreased (not significantly) after change from group to individual housing, and 24 h after initial training
Redbo et al. 1998 [55]Movement, social contact &
Feed
Thoroughbreds compared with trottersBehaviour: wood-chewing No difference
StereotypiesMore in thoroughbreds
Stomp et al. 2021 [26]Movement, social contact & feedHemispheric activity in horses in individual housing compared with that in horses kept at pastureElectroencephalogram (EEG): bilateral predominance of theta wavesIncreased in pasture kept horses.
EEG: bilateral predominance of beta waves Increased in horses in individual housing
EEG: hemispheric laterality: bilateral and Left-Hemispheric theta activity Increased in pasture kept horses
EEG: hemispheric laterality: bilateral or Right-Hemispheric high production of gamma waves Increased in horses in individual housing
StereotypiesMore common in horses in individual housing
Behaviour: ear position while feedingMore common in horses in individual housing
Behaviour: human approach-tests Pasture kept horses more positive
Visser et al. 2008 [41]Movement, social contact &
feed
Housing in individual boxes versus housing in pairsStress: CRF challenge test-cortisol response and ACTH response Lower in individually housed horses
Behaviour: neighing, pawing, nibbling, snortingMore frequent in individually housed horses
StereotypiesMore frequent in individually housed horses
Activity: novel object testNo difference
Waters et al. 2002 [8]Movement, social contact &
feed
Weaning in a stable, a barn, on a paddock, and at grassAbnormal behaviourMore frequent after weaning in barns or stables
Stereotypic behaviour: wood chewingMore frequent after weaning in barns or stables
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Krueger, K.; Esch, L.; Farmer, K.; Marr, I. Basic Needs in Horses?—A Literature Review. Animals 2021, 11, 1798. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11061798

AMA Style

Krueger K, Esch L, Farmer K, Marr I. Basic Needs in Horses?—A Literature Review. Animals. 2021; 11(6):1798. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11061798

Chicago/Turabian Style

Krueger, Konstanze, Laureen Esch, Kate Farmer, and Isabell Marr. 2021. "Basic Needs in Horses?—A Literature Review" Animals 11, no. 6: 1798. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11061798

APA Style

Krueger, K., Esch, L., Farmer, K., & Marr, I. (2021). Basic Needs in Horses?—A Literature Review. Animals, 11(6), 1798. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11061798

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop