Next Article in Journal
Influences of Beta-Alanine and l-Histidine Supplementation on Growth Performance, Meat Quality, Carnosine Content, and mRNA Expression of Carnosine-Related Enzymes in Broilers
Next Article in Special Issue
Handlers’ Expectations and Perceived Compatibility regarding the Partnership with Their First Guide Dogs
Previous Article in Journal
Identification and Characterization of lncRNA and mRNA in Testes of Landrace and Hezuo Boars
Previous Article in Special Issue
Evidence for the Continued Occurrence of Chorioretinopathy in Working Sheep Dogs in New Zealand in 2010
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Give Them a Toy or Increase Time out of Kennel at Lawn Areas: What Is the Influence of These Interventions on Police Dogs’ Welfare?

by
Letícia Bicudo Nogueira
1,*,
Rupert Palme
2 and
Olívia Mendonça-Furtado
3,4
1
Faculdade de Medicina Veterinária e Zootecnia, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, SP 05508-270, Brazil
2
Unit of Physiology, Pathophysiology, and Experimental Endocrinology, Department of Biomedical Sciences, University of Veterinary Medicine Vienna, 1210 Vienna, Austria
3
Departamento de Psicologia Experimental, Instituto de Psicologia, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, SP 05508-030, Brazil
4
National Institute for the Atlantic Forest, Santa Teresa, ES 29650-000, Brazil
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Animals 2021, 11(8), 2264; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11082264
Submission received: 8 July 2021 / Accepted: 27 July 2021 / Published: 30 July 2021
(This article belongs to the Collection Health, Behaviour and Performance in Working Dog Teams)

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

Poor environments such as kennels can lead to compromised welfare, as they usually lack many kinds of stimuli. Working dogs are not only kept in kennels, but they are also often housed without any kind of environmental enrichment, allegedly due to motivational reasons, which is conducive to all kinds of behavioural issues. Thus, this study aimed to evaluate the impact of two interventions, a toy and going out to a lawn area, on a group of police dogs. Behavioural and physiological data were used to evaluate the animals’ responses to treatments. We identified a large variety of behavioural profiles amongst the animals that presented different levels of stereotypies, which is an abnormal behaviour pattern that can be associated with stress. A tendency to a reduction in these behaviours in high-stereotyping individuals was observed after dogs were able to go to the lawn area, indicating beneficial effects of this intervention. The physiological data indicated that the cumulative effect of interventions can also have a beneficial impact and that environmental enrichment plays an important role in kennelled police dogs’ welfare.

Abstract

This work was aimed at identifying the effectiveness of two interventions applied to a group of eight kennelled police dogs. Interventions consisted of access to a lawn area (350 m2) and “toy” (a 30 cm jute rag roll, hanging from the kennel ceiling), both available for 15 min a day, for four days in a row. We collected behavioural data and faecal samples for cortisol metabolites evaluation before, during and after interventions. Faecal cortisol metabolites levels were significantly reduced (Friedman, X2(3) = 12.450; p = 0.006) during the second round of intervention, regardless of the type of intervention, indicating that the interventions can have a beneficial cumulative effect. Regarding the frequency of stereotyped behaviour, cluster analysis identified two groups of individuals: (1) high-stereotyping individuals (n = 3) that had a tendency to reduce stereotyping behaviours in the lawn intervention when compared to toy intervention (Friedman, X2(3) = 2.530; p = 0.068), and (2) low-stereotyping animals (n = 5) that did not present significant behavioural changes during the experiment. The variety of behavioural and endocrine parameters evaluated highlights the need to account for the individual in behaviour and welfare research. Overall, our results suggest that even simple environmental enrichment can be an effective method to mitigate behaviour and physiological signs of stress.

1. Introduction

The existence of an association between the development of stereotyped behaviours and sub-optimal environments is widely recognized [1,2,3]. In kennelled domestic dogs, this link has been described by several authors, who interpreted repetitive behaviours as indicative of compromised welfare [4,5,6,7,8] and chronic stress [9,10,11,12,13,14,15,16]. Kennelled dogs may experience poor housing as this management can be associated with lack of control of the environment [17], unpredictability [12,18] and limited opportunities for social contact with humans [5,19] and/or other dogs [6] (for a definition and the importance of welfare for kennel dogs, see [13]). Thus, many efforts to investigate how to provide adequate stimulation in this situation have been made, and several studies have indicated that environmental enrichment is effective in minimizing kennelling stress [4,18,20,21,22,23,24].
Military/police working dogs are commonly housed in kennels (Belgium [8,25]; Brazil [26,27]; France [28]; The United States [29]; The United Kingdom [13,22]), and many of these dogs do not receive any kind of environmental enrichment, due to a widespread belief that providing some types of stimuli would decrease animals’ motivation to work because they are trained using toys as rewards [22]. However, current evidence indicates that environmental enrichment does not impair a dog’s working performance [22] and also has a stress reducing potential [8]. For an environmental enrichment program to be successful, it requires considering both the motivation of the subjects to interact with the provided stimuli [30] as well as how individuals can differ in the way they deal with environmental challenges [31]. Thus, it is important to investigate the best way of providing effective stimuli to target subjects.
Previous research on long-term kennelled dogs has focused on laboratory [5,21] and shelter dogs [20,32]. Toys, training routines and infrastructural environmental enrichment were demonstrated to be effective ways to enhance the welfare of those animals, by increasing exploration, reducing abnormal behaviours, promoting interaction with enrichment, increasing activity and lowering plasma cortisol levels. In working dogs, Lefebvre et al. [8] have compared two regimes of guided exercise practicing and training sessions based on a welfare perspective. They found reduced cortisol concentrations when the enrichment was more frequent. Nevertheless, further work is required to evaluate the effect of different kinds of interventions on the welfare of working dogs.
In an effort to contribute to the working dog literature, we evaluate the effectiveness of two different stimuli (access to a lawn area and “toy”) provided to a group of kennelled police dogs; we measured behavioural and physiological parameters before, during and after the interventions. Although we expected both interventions to be beneficial to the dog’s welfare, in response to the increase of stimuli, we hypothesized that the lawn intervention would be more efficient in reducing stress, leading to a reduction in repetitive behaviours and faecal cortisol metabolite levels because dogs would be able to reduce time spent inside kennels and have access to a great variety of stimuli, allowing better control of the environment.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Subjetcs

Subjects comprised eight police dogs from the city Guard of Santana do Parnaíba—SP, Brazil (Table 1). Most of the dogs had been born and raised in working dog training establishments, except subject No. 4, which came from a civilian house as a young adult.

2.2. Husbandry and Housing

Activities practiced by the dogs included scent detection and guard function. Dogs’ routine was based on a scale of 12 h of “work”, when they could be trained and take part in the city patrol, per 36 h of “rest”, when they were kept exclusively inside the kennels until their next workday. Each dog was designated to a guard that was both their handler and the caretaker, so the work scales of the animals coincided with the ones from their respective handlers. This division was the same used for experimental groups (see Table 1) to guarantee that animals from the same group were submitted to the same events during duty. There were no care assistants; all the management procedures were realized by the guards. Dogs were fed twice a day, at 11:00 and 23:00. Kennels were cleaned at 11:30.
Housing conditions consisted of individual 11.5 m2 sized kennels with an internal subdivision of 2.3 m2 as a resting area. Three walls and the floor were made of concrete and the front wall was made of wire mesh. Walls were 2.5 m high, surrounding the entire structure. Sixty percent of the structure was covered by a roof. Visual contact was possible only with the animals housed on the kennel box across the aisle.

2.3. Interventions

Animals received two kinds of interventions: access to a lawn area and a “toy”. Lawn intervention consisted of the subject being allowed to move freely in a 300 m2 lawn area, with natural grass, surrounded by a fence. There was no social interaction as subjects were individually released in the area. The “toy” intervention consisted of providing a jute rag roll (the same material used in the guard training sessions), 50 cm × 20 cm, tied from both sides by a rope and hanging from the kennel ceiling about 1.2 m above the ground. As the dogs were not used to be handled by unfamiliar persons, guards were responsible for taking the subjects to the lawn area and tying the “toys” in the kennels spaces; however, they were advised not to interact with the subjects at any moment. Both interventions had a fifteen-minute duration.

2.4. Experimental Design

Interventions were applied during the “rest” period of the dog’s schedule, between 12:00 and 12:30, just after the kennels were cleaned. As the experimental groups were predefined by the duty scale, we could only determine the order each group would receive the interventions. The whole experiment took 38 days from January to February 2018. It had three parts: pre-, during- and post-interventions, with eight days of behavioural and physiological (faecal cortisol metabolites) measurements during each part (Figure 1), always at the same time of the day (see items 2.5 and 2.6 for more information). The study followed a crossover experimental design. Animals had access to each intervention during a continuous period of four days, separated by an interval of five days (wash-out period).

2.5. Behaviour Assessment

To minimize human-induced perturbations [8,33], animals were filmed without the researcher’s presence. Records of 30 consecutive minutes were made for each dog between 12:30 and 14:30, a time during which, by routine, all dogs were inside their kennels. Filming happened on the same days as the faeces collection (see next section). On the first day, the order that individuals were filmed were drew. Throughout the experiment, the same order was used, but the last animal on the previous day was always the first on the next day. A total of 96 h of video recordings was taken, 12 h for each subject (4 pre, 4 during and 4 post interventions).
Video recordings were used for behavioural data collection by a one-minute interval instantaneous sampling method [34]. The first two minutes of each video were removed from analysis to minimize the interference of human presence when positioning the cameras [35]. Therefore, each dog had a total of 672 scans registered. Behaviours were classified according to the ethogram presented in Table 2. Analyses were based on two sets of variables. The first one consisted of the behavioural categories: “Readiness/Alert”, “Stereotyped” and “Rest/Relaxation” (Table 2). The second one consisted of the specific behaviours: “Pace”, “Circle”, “Spin”, “Anxious waiting” and “Stand” as variables (Table 2). L.B.N. was the only one to collect behavioural data from the videos.

2.6. Faecal Samples

Faecal samples were collected from 11:30 to 12:00, during the cleaning of the kennels, totalling 85 samples. The collection could also happen after filming, when the animal had not defecated before the time of faecal collection or had done it long before (noticeable too dry faeces), to guarantee the freshest samples possible. On rare occasions, when the animal did not defecate on that day, we could not collect a sample. The whole stool was collected in a plastic bag, manually homogenized by kneading the bag, and then a small amount was transferred to a 5 mL identified plastic tube. Tubes were immediately stored in a cooler with ice bags and transported to the Laboratory of Behavioural Endocrinology at the Institute of Psychology of The University of São Paulo, where they were kept in a freezer until extraction and analysis of faecal cortisol metabolites (FCMs).

2.7. FCM Extraction and Analysis

The extraction procedure used 80% methanol (0.5 g faeces plus 5 mL of 80% methanol) following the protocol described in Palme [36]. The supernatant was stored at −20 °C until assayed. Faecal hormone metabolites were measured in a 50 µL aliquot of the extract (diluted 1:10) with a cortisol enzyme immunoassay (EIA; for details, see Palme and Möstl [37]), validated for Canis familiaris by Schatz and Palme [38]. Both intra- and inter-assay coefficients of variation of pool samples were <12%. All samples were assayed in duplicate. Concentrations of FCMs are expressed as nanograms per gram of wet faecal matter.

2.8. Ethics and Data Collection Procedures

This study was approved by the Animal Research Ethics Committee of the Institute of Psychology from The University of São Paulo (CEUA/IPUSP nº 1396090518). All the procedures were conducted in accordance with the ethical guidelines laid down by the National Council for Control of Animal Experimentation (CONCEA) and with the current Brazilian laws on ethical standards.

2.9. Data Analysis

When analysing the data, we noticed that subjects performed stereotyped behaviours in very distinct frequencies (see Results). Therefore, we ran a Hierarchical Cluster Analysis with the Centroid Clustering method, by the Squared Euclidean distance, using the mean values, per individual, of all stereotyped behaviours performed in the pre-intervention period (see Figure 1). The clusters classified individuals regarding the number of stereotyped behaviours they presented.
To test if there were differences between treatments (pre-interventions, lawn area, toy and post-interventions) and periods (pre-interventions, first intervention, second intervention and post-interventions) for both behaviours (categories: “Readiness/Alert”, “Stereotyped” and “Rest/Relaxation” and specific behaviours: “Pace”, “Circle”, “Spin”, “Anxious waiting” and “Stand”) and FCM concentrations, we used Friedman’s test.
Spearman’s correlation test was used to verify the existence of a correlation between (1) FCM level and behaviour variables (using the same categories and specific behaviours used in the previous analysis) and (2) the number of different behaviours performed by the individual (behavioural repertory) and the percentage of stereotyped behaviours performed.
We used non-parametric tests because the small sample size of our study (n = 8) precluded verifying normality distribution of data in all experimental situations (all data checked employing Shapiro–Wilk tests). All analysis was made using SPSS (IBM SPSS Statistics 22). Statistical significance was determined at a p-value of less than 0.05. The effects of breed, sex or age have not been tested.

3. Results

Stereotyped behaviour accounted for 16.7% of all scans in our sample. All dogs exhibited some type of stereotyped behaviour, but rates varied considerably among individuals (from 0.2 to 60.0%).

3.1. Cluster Analysis of Stereotyped Behaviours

The cluster analysis recognized two patterns of behavioural responses (Figure 2): (1) subjects that displayed higher rates of stereotyped behaviours (n = 3, dotted line cluster), high-stereotyping individuals, and (2) subjects that displayed lower rates of stereotyped behaviours (n = 5, solid line cluster), low-stereotyping individuals. (See Figure A1).

3.2. Behavioural Analysis

3.2.1. Readiness/Alert Behaviours

Readiness/Alert behaviours did not differ significantly between the four experimental situations (pre-intervention, both interventions and post-intervention), neither regarding periods (Friedman, X2 (3) = 1.950; p > 0.05) nor regarding treatments (Friedman, X2 (3) = 2.250; p > 0.05). Even when considering only high-stereotyping individuals, there were no statistical differences (Friedman, X2 (3) = 1.000; p > 0.05).

3.2.2. Stereotyped Behaviours

Analysis, ran with the entire sample, did not present significant differences in stereotyped behaviours frequency, neither between periods (Friedman, X2 (3) = 4.897; p > 0.05), nor between treatments (Friedman, X2 (3) = 5.426; p > 0.05). The analysis made with only animals from high-stereotyping cluster showed a significant difference between treatments (Friedman, X2 (3) = 8.200; p = 0.042). However, when checking the pairwise comparison (Table 3) with Bonferroni correction, there was no p-value below 0.05, which suggests that the small sample of high-stereotyping individuals hinders any significant statistical result. Nevertheless, even with the Bonferroni correction, we observed a tendency that during the lawn treatment there was less engagement on stereotyped behaviours when compared to the toy intervention treatment (Friedman, X2 (3) = 2.530; p = 0.068) (Figure 3). Analysis by period found no statistical differences over time (Friedman, X2 (3) = 3.400; p > 0. 05).

3.2.3. Rest/Relaxation Behaviours

Rest/Relaxation behaviours did not differ significantly between the four experimental situations, neither in analysis by treatments (Friedman, X2 (3) = 4.050; p > 0.05) nor in analysis by period (Friedman, X2 (3) = 1.050; p > 0.05). When the test was performed only with the animals grouped in the high-stereotyping cluster, statistical differences were found between treatments (Friedman, X2 (3) = 8.200; p = 0.042). When checking the pairwise comparison with Bonferroni correction, again, there was no p-value below 0.05. Nevertheless, even with the adjusted significance, we could see a tendency that the percentage of rest/relaxation behaviours was higher in the period animals had access to the lawn area compared to the period when animals received the toy (Table 4). Analysis by period found no statistical differences over time (Friedman, X2 (3) = 3.400; p > 0.05).

3.2.4. Analysis of Specific Behaviours

Statistical analysis of specific stereotyped behaviours (Pace, Circle and Spin) by both treatment and period found no significant difference. The behaviours “Wall-bounce” and “Head twirl” could not be analysed due to low percentages of observations. The behaviours “Anxious waiting” and “Stand”, which were categorized as Readiness/Alert behaviour, were analysed as possible stress indicators, but showed no significant differences, neither by treatment nor by period.

3.3. Behavioural Repertory

The number of different behaviours performed by each animal was positively correlated (Spearman, ρ = 0.786; p < 0.05) with the percentage of stereotyped behaviours (Figure 4), and negatively correlated (Spearman, ρ = −0.810; p < 0.05) with the percentage of rest/relaxation behaviours performed by the same subject.

3.4. Correlations between Behaviour and FCM Levels

There were no correlations between behavioural categories (Readiness/Alert, r = 0.262; Stereotyped, r = 0.164; Rest/Relaxation, r = −0.235) or specific behaviours (r = −0.235; Pace, r = 0.370; Spin, r = 0.034; Circle, r = −0.308; Anxious Waiting, r = −0.053; Stand, r = 0.087) and FCM levels.

3.5. FCM Levels Analysis

FCM concentrations differed significantly between periods (Friedman, X2 (3) = 12.450; p = 0.006); the second period of interventions presented lower FCM concentrations than the pre-interventions period (Friedman, X2 (3) = 3.486; p = 0.003) (Figure 5). Graph with data on FCM throughout experimental periods by individual can be found on Supplementary Material (Figure S1). No statistical differences were found between treatments (Friedman, X2 (3) = 6.300; p > 0.05).

4. Discussion

Our subjects had higher (inter-subject mean = 16.7%) proportions of stereotyped behaviours on the behavioural budget than what has been reported in the literature for individually kennelled dogs (between <0.1% and 11% [4,39,40,41]). This could be explained by the fact that our subjects were working dogs with a very rigid management policy. Additionally, six out of our eight subjects were Malinois Shepherds, a breed bred to work as police dogs that shows a high correlation between performance in the field and stereotyped behaviour [42]. Furthermore, Malinois Shepherds were reported to have higher rates of stereotyped behaviours (4.8 to 10.6%, Lefebvre et al. [8], and 29.78 to 33.86%, Haverbeke et al. [25]) than other breeds.
It is noteworthy that, even in the absence of arousing stimuli, all our subjects presented some kind of stereotyped behaviour, which contrast sharply with available data for individually kennelled dogs performing stereotyped behaviours (1.3% to 15% [4,11,39,40]). Similar rates (93%) of subjects presenting stereotyped behaviour were described by Denham et al. [14], but only when their subjects were presented with arousing stimuli (e.g., care assistants walking through the kennel, the sound of clicking the clip on the end of a leash, sounds of feeding preparation, stranger standing outside kennel).
The cluster analysis pointed towards two different patterns of stereotype performance; three individuals were classified as high-stereotyping and five as low-stereotyping. Other studies on kennelled dogs also found individual differences in animals experiencing the same housing and management conditions [7,11,14,15,39,43,44,45,46,47,48,49]. Some authors propose that these variations are due to the existence of distinct personalities [50] or coping styles [51]. Stereotypies are rated as a proactive response to stress, whilst depression/inactivity is related to reactive’ subjects [31]. Further studies could test our subjects’ personalities in order to check this hypothesis.
High-stereotyping individuals showed a trend toward reducing stereotype levels when given the opportunity to use a lawn area (versus when allowed to play with a toy), which is compatible with the work of Lefebvre et al. [8], which found a reduction of stereotyped behaviour (“repetitive gait”) in working dogs that had access to regular outdoor physical activities.
Additionally, in the high-stereotyping cluster there was a trend to increase Rest/Relaxation behaviours during the lawn intervention (versus when allowed to play with a toy). When taken together, these results show that during the lawn intervention period, animals ceased to perform stereotyped behaviour and engaged in Rest/Relaxation behaviours, suggesting a successful improvement of their welfare state [2,52]. Some theories relate stereotypies origin in carnivores as thwart attempts to display behaviours they cannot perform (e.g., mate, roam, hunt, or interact with conspecifics) in the captive environment [53]. Aligned with those theories, we hypothesize that our subjects showed a reduction of stereotypies due to the given possibility to display highly motivational behaviours (e.g., roam, dig, sniff), even if only for a short period of time.
In the same line, we suppose that the lack of influence of the toy intervention on stereotyped behaviours was because it did not promote the performance of highly motivated behaviours [54,55]. Thus, although the animals were observed interacting intensively with the toy (e.g., chasing, jumping and biting), it did not promote time out of the kennel confinement, possibly the most relevant aspect of the lawn intervention.
However, our data not only indicate that stereotypies were not reduced during the toy intervention, but they also suggest that they had a slight increase. It is already recognized that situations that are positive to welfare can contribute to elicit or maintain stereotypies. This could occur because the stereotype no longer indicates a frustration from a non-executed behaviour or a need to use repetition as a “calming effect” but has turned into a “habit” [56]. An increase in stereotypies has also been reported in mink in response to providing them “playballs” [57] and in arctic foxes in response to an increase in cage size [58]. Thus, although stereotypies are an important sign to detect potential suffering associated with sub-optimal environments, they never should be taken as an exclusive indicator of welfare, and research is still increasing our knowledge on how they develop and differ between species and individuals [56]. When it comes to domestic dogs, considering that our subjects performed intense physical activity when interacting with the toy, it is also important to consider that additional exercise was already described to increase “active behaviours” [43,59], which could lead to a repetitive movement in restrained spaces. Furthermore, in the work of Protopopova et al. [48], exercising did not prevent the expression of abnormal in-kennel behaviours. Therefore, possible approaches to comprehend this effect in our subjects could be related to the arousal and excitement brought by interacting with the toy, not necessarily associated with a decrease in the welfare state.
Regarding the five low-stereotyping subjects, stereotyped behaviour was not a good proxy for stress response. Therefore, we would expect them to show alteration in other behavioural frequencies (e.g., decreased frequencies of Rest/Relaxation behaviours after the interventions). However, the environment that these dogs are maintained in is so sterile that an animal that did present stereotypies did not have anything else to do besides resting.
Regarding the analysis of specific behaviours, they did not present a correlation with any of the treatments/periods. Unfortunately, our small sample hinders any conclusion in this regard.
Our analysis shows that subjects that presented more stereotypies also had a wider behaviour repertory than subjects with fewer stereotyped behaviours. Although it sounds counter intuitive to present more repetitive behaviours and also a higher variability of behaviours, a closer analysis shows that high-stereotyping subjects were more active than low-stereotyping ones. This result also highlights that stereotypies are not always connected to compromise welfare [56] and that low-stereotyping subjects are not necessarily in a better welfare condition. Evidence in this regard is that we found a correlation between lower levels of stereotypies and less complex behaviour repertory. Several authors reported that, in kennel confined domestic dogs, a possible response to chronic stress was a reduction of the behavioural repertory [14,21,33,47]. Additionally, according to Ijichi et al. [31], subjects presenting less complex behavioural repertoires would be dealing with a reduced perception in the excitatory stimuli [60], reducing feedback in the pathways linked with motivation and appetite behaviours [31], leading to depressive like behaviours.
We found no correlations between behaviour (evaluated in the categories Readiness/Alert, Stereotyped, Rest/Relaxation and isolated: Pace, Spin, Circle, Anxious Waiting, Stand) and FCM levels, a non-invasive measure of adrenocortical activity [61]. This is not completely unexpected because different studies on dogs have also reported the absence of a relationship between cortisol levels and behaviours [44,62]. Our findings also corroborate studies that evaluated the relationship between personality traits and cortisol levels in domestic dogs [63] and confined domestic cats [64,65,66].
FCM levels were lower during the second period of interventions, but not associated with one specific type of intervention. Lefebvre et al. [8] found a reduction of cortisol levels in working dogs in response to the establishment of regular dog-handler interactions. Therefore, this result could be due to the increase of human contact with the animals throughout the experiment (put on the leash to walk the animal to the lawn area or enter the kennel to hang the toy and take it off). A non-mutually excluding explanation would be that the combination of both interventions was necessary to result in reduced cortisol levels. Additionally, new stimuli can have a “challenge” effect on dogs [9,25]. Therefore, the routine alteration due to the start of the experiment could have challenged the coping mechanism of the subjects, hampering the effects of the first period of intervention on FCM levels. In addition, it is important to consider that the lower levels of FCM seen only in the second period could also indicate that animals were less aroused by the interventions throughout time.

5. Conclusions

Considering that high-stereotyping individuals showed a tendency towards a reduction in stereotyped behaviours, and that the second round of interventions was related to a reduction in FCMs, we assume that these indexes were proxies of impaired welfare related to kennel confinement.
Fifteen minutes of access to the lawn area showed an impact on stereotyped behaviour levels on high-stereotyping dogs. Although this does not necessarily mean a reduction in stress levels, it implies an improvement in welfare once stereotyping behaviours can be deleterious. Additionally, FCM data indicate that the cumulative effect of interventions might benefit all subjects. Further research could evaluate if the length and the type of the interventions are crucial for their effect on the animals’ welfare.
The literature clearly points out that working dogs are extremely affected by the confinement they are subject to. Our research shows the positive effect that simple interventions can have on those animals’ welfare. Therefore, veterinarians and other staff responsible for these animals should consider applying regular interventions in order to maximize their welfare.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ani11082264/s1, Figure S1: FCM concentrations through experimental periods by individual.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, L.B.N. and O.M.-F.; data curation, L.B.N. and O.M.-F.; formal analysis, L.B.N. and O.M.-F.; funding acquisition, O.M.-F.; methodology, L.B.N. and O.M.-F.; project administration, O.M.-F.; resources, R.P. and O.M.-F.; supervision, O.M.-F.; validation, R.P.; writing—original draft, L.B.N.; writing—review and editing, R.P. and O.M.-F. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Coordination for the Improvement of Higher-Level Personnel (Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior—CAPES) and by the National Council for Scientific and Technological Development (Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico—Cnpq).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of Animal Research Ethics Committee of the Institute of Psychology from The University of São Paulo (CEUA/IPUSP nº 1396090518; approved on 3 July 2018).

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available in the present article and supplementary material and are shared with consent and in accordance with all authors.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the Municipal Guard of Santana do Parnaíba for authorizing the conduction of this research, the guard officers and the veterinary from the institution for all the support given.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Figure A1. Percentage of observations on stereotyped behaviour by individual (numbers indicated on Table 1) in our four experimental treatments (Pre: pre-interventions; Toy: toy intervention; Lawn: lawn intervention; Post: post-interventions).
Figure A1. Percentage of observations on stereotyped behaviour by individual (numbers indicated on Table 1) in our four experimental treatments (Pre: pre-interventions; Toy: toy intervention; Lawn: lawn intervention; Post: post-interventions).
Animals 11 02264 g0a1

References

  1. Mason, G.J. Stereotypies: A critical review. Anim. Behav. 1991, 41, 1015–1037. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. Broom, D.M.; Johnson, K.G. Approaching Questions of Stress and Welfare; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1993; pp. 1–7. [Google Scholar]
  3. Clubb, R.; Mason, G. Capitivity effects on wide-range carnivores. Nature 2003, 425, 473–474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Hetts, S.; Clark, J.D.; Calpin, J.P.; Arnold, C.E.; Mateo, J.M. Influence of housing conditions on beagle behaviour. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1992, 34, 137–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Hubrecht, R.C.; Serpell, J.; Poole, T.B. Correlates of pen size and housing conditions on the behaviour of kennelled dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1992, 34, 365–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Mertens, P.A.; Unshelm, J. Effects of group and individual housing on the behavior of kennelled dogs in animal shelters. Anthrozoös 1996, 9, 40–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Beerda, B.; Schilder, M.B.H.; van Hooff, J.A.; de Vries, H.W.; Mol, J.A. Chronic stress in dogs subjected to social and spatial restriction. I. Behavioral responses. Physiol. Behav. 1999, 66, 233–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Lefebvre, D.; Giffroy, J.-M.; Diederich, C. Cortisol and behavioral responses to enrichment in military working dogs. J. Ethol. 2008, 27, 255–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Beerda, B.; Schilder, M.B.H.; Bernadina, W.; Van Hooff, J.A.; de Vries, H.W.; Mol, J.A. Chronic stress in dogs subjected to social and spatial restriction. II. Hormonal and immunological responses. Physiol. Behav. 1999, 66, 243–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Beerda, B.; Schilder, M.B.H.; van Hooff, J.A.; Mol, J. Behavioural and hormonal indicators of enduring environmental stress in dogs. Anim. Welf. Potters Bar 2000, 9, 49–62. [Google Scholar]
  11. Stephen, J.M.; Ledger, R.A. An audit of behavioral indicators of poor welfare in kenneled dogs in the united kingdom. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2005, 8, 79–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Taylor, K.D.; Mills, D.S. The effect of the kennel environment on canine welfare: A critical review of experimental studies. Anim. Welf. 2007, 16, 435–447. [Google Scholar]
  13. Rooney, N.; Gaines, S.; Hiby, E. A practitioner’s guide to working dog welfare. J. Vet. Behav. 2009, 4, 127–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Denham, H.D.; Bradshaw, J.W.; Rooney, N.J. Repetitive behaviour in kennelled domestic dog: Stereotypical or not? Physiol. Behav. 2014, 128, 288–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Protopopova, A.; Mehrkam, R.L.; Boggess, M.M.; Wynne, L.D.C. In-kennel behavior predicts lenght of stay in shelter dogs. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e114319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  16. Polgár, Z.; Blackwell, E.J.; Rooney, N.J. Assessing the welfare of kennelled dogs—A review of animal-based measures. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2019, 213, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Hennessy, M.B.; Davis, H.N.; Williams, M.T.; Mellott, C.; Douglas, C.W.; Davis, H.N. Plasma cortisol levels of dogs at a county animal shelter. Physiol. Behav. 1997, 62, 485–490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Graham, L.; Wells, D.L.; Hepper, P.G. The influence of olfactory stimulation on the behaviour of dogs housed in a rescue shelter. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2005, 91, 143–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Coppola, C.L.; Grandin, T.; Enns, R.M. Human interaction and cortisol: Can human contact reduce stress for shelter dogs? Physiol. Behav. 2006, 87, 537–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Wells, D.L.; Hepper, P.G. A note on the influence of visual conspecific contact on the behaviour of sheltered dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1998, 60, 83–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Hubrecht, R.C. A comparison of social and environmental enrichment methods for laboratory housed dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1993, 37, 345–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Gaines, S.A.; Rooney, N.J.; Bradshaw, J.W. The effect of feeding enrichment upon reported working ability and behavior of kenneled working dogs. J. Forensic Sci. 2008, 53, 1400–1404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Schipper, L.L.; Vinke, C.M.; Schilder, M.B.; Spruijt, B.M. The effect of feeding enrichment toys on the behaviour of kennelled dogs (Canis familiaris). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008, 114, 182–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Kiddie, J.; Collins, L. Identifying environmental and management factors that may be associated with the quality of life of kennelled dogs (Canis familiaris). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2015, 167, 43–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Haverbeke, A.; Diederich, C.; Depiereux, E.; Giffroy, J. Cortisol and behavioral responses of working dogs to environmental challenges. Physiol. Behav. 2008, 93, 59–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Araujo, R.T.; Marcondes, C.B.; Bastos, L.C.; Sartor, D.C. Canine dirofilariasis in the region of Conceição Lagoon, Florianópolis, and in the Military Police kennel, São José, State of Santa Catarina, Brazil. Vet. Parasitol. 2003, 113, 239–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Louly, C.C.B.; Fonseca, I.N.; Oliveira, V.F.; Linhares, C.F.G.; Menezes, L.B.; Borges, L.M.F. Seasonal dynamics of Rhipicephalus sanguineus (Acari: Ixodidae) in dogs from a police unit in Goiânia, Goiás, Brazil. Ciência Rural 2007, 37, 464–469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Davoust, B.; Roqueplo, C.; Parzy, D.; Watier-Grillot, S.; Marié, J.-L. A twenty-year follow-up of canine leishmaniosis in three military kennels in southeastern France. Parasites Vectors 2013, 6, 323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Coppinger, R.; Zuccotti, J. Kennel enrichment: Exercise and socialization of dogs. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 1999, 2, 281–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Shepherdson, D.J.; Mellen, J.D.; Hutchins, M. Second Nature: Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals; Smithsonian Institute: Washington, DC, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  31. Ijichi, C.L.; Collins, L.; Elwood, R.W. Evidence for the role of personality in stereotypy predisposition. Anim. Behav. 2013, 85, 1145–1151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Wells, D.L. A review of environmental enrichment for kennelled dogs, Canis familiaris. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2004, 85, 307–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Hughes, H.C.; Campbell, S.; Kenney, C. The effects of cage size and pair housing on exercise of beagle dogs. Lab. Anim. Sci. 1989, 39, 302–305. [Google Scholar]
  34. Altmann, J. Observational study of behavior: Sampling methods. Behaviour 1974, 49, 227–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Lefebvre, D.; Depiereux, E.; Giffroy, J.-M.; Diederich, C. Research note: Installation of videorecording equipment had minimal effect on behavioral measurements in 14 kenneled military dogs. J. Vet. Behav. 2010, 5, 2–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Palme, R.; Touma, C.; Arias, N.; Dominchin, M.F.; Lepschy, M. Steroid extraction: Get the best out of faecal samples. Vet. Med. Austria 2013, 100, 238–246. [Google Scholar]
  37. Palme, R.; Möstl, E. Measurement of cortisol metabolites in faeces of sheep as a parameter of cortisol concentration in blood. Int. J. Mammal. Biol. 1997, 62, 192–197. [Google Scholar]
  38. Schatz, S.; Palme, R. Measurement of faecal cortisol metabolites in cats and dogs: A non-invasive method for evaluating adrenocortical function. Vet. Res. Commun. 2001, 25, 271–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Hiby, E.; Rooney, N.J.; Bradshaw, J.W. Behavioural and physiological responses of dogs entering re-homing kennels. Physiol. Behav. 2006, 89, 385–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Protopopova, A.; Gilmour, A.J.; Weiss, R.H.; Shen, J.Y.; Wynne, C.D.L. The effects of social training and other factors on adoption success of shelter dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2012, 142, 61–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Walker, J.K.; Waran, N.K.; Phillips, C.J. The effect of conspecific removal on the behaviour and physiology of pair-housed shelter dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2014, 158, 46–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Cao, X.; Irwin, D.; Liu, Y.-H.; Cheng, L.-G.; Wang, L.; Wang, G.-D.; Zhang, Y.-P. Balancing selection on CDH2 may be related to the behavioral features of the belgian malinois. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e110075. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Clark, J.D.; Rager, D.R.; Crowell-Davis, S.; Evans, D.L. Housing and exercise of dogs: Effects on behavior, immune function, and cortisol concentration. Lab. Anim. Sci. 1997, 47, 500–510. [Google Scholar]
  44. Hennessy, M.B.; Voith, V.L.; Mazzei, S.J.; Buttram, J.; Miller, D.D.; Linden, F. Behavior and cortisol levels of dogs in a public animal shelter, and an exploration of the ability of these measures to predict problem behavior after adoption. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2001, 73, 217–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Jones, S.; Dowling-Guyer, S.; Patronek, G.J.; Marder, A.R.; D’Arpino, S.S.; McCobb, E. Use of accelerometers to measure stress levels in shelter dogs. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2013, 17, 18–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Part, C.; Kiddie, J.; Hayes, W.; Mills, D.; Neville, R.; Morton, D.; Collins, L. Physiological, physical and behavioural changes in dogs (Canis familiaris) when kennelled: Testing the validity of stress parameters. Physiol. Behav. 2014, 133, 260–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Protopopova, A. Effects of sheltering on physiology, immune function, behavior, and the welfare of dogs. Physiol. Behav. 2016, 159, 95–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Protopopova, A.; Hauser, H.; Goldman, K.J.; Wynne, C.D. The effects of exercise and calm interactions on in-kennel behavior of shelter dogs. Behav. Process. 2018, 146, 54–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Titulaer, M.; Blackwell, E.J.; Mendl, M.; Casey, R.A. Cross sectional study comparing behavioural, cognitive and physiological indicators of welfare between short and long term kennelled domestic dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2013, 147, 149–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Gosling, S.D.; John, O.P. Personality dimensions in non-human animals. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 1999, 8, 69–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Wechsler, B. Coping and coping strategies: A behavioural view. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1995, 43, 123–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Dawkins, M.S. From an animal’s point of view: Motivation, fitness, and animal welfare. Behav. Brain Sci. 1990, 13, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Clubb, R.; Vickery, S. Locomotory Stereotypies in Carnivores: Does Pacing Stem from Hunting, Ranging or Frustrated Escape? CABI Publishing: Wallingford, Oxfordshire, UK, 2009; pp. 58–85. [Google Scholar]
  54. Broom, D. Assessing welfare and suffering. Behav. Process. 1991, 25, 117–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Duncan, I.J. Behavior and behavioral needs. Poult. Sci. 1998, 77, 1766–1772. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Mason, G.J.; Latham, N.R. Can’t stop, won’t stop: Is stereotypy a reliable animal welfare indicator? Anim. Welf. 2004, 13, 57–69. [Google Scholar]
  57. Jeppesen, L.L.; Falkenberg, H. Effects of play balls on peltbiting, behaviour and level of stress in ranch mink. Scientifur 1990, 14, 179–186. [Google Scholar]
  58. Korhonen, H.T.; Niemelä, P.; Jauhiainen, L. Effect of space and floor material on the behaviour of farmed blue foxes. Can. J. Anim. Sci. 2001, 81, 189–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Normando, S.; Contiero, B.; Marchesini, G.; Ricci, R. Effects of space allowance on the behaviour of long-term housed shelter dogs. Behav. Process. 2014, 103, 306–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Broom, D. The scientific assessment of animal welfare. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1988, 20, 5–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Palme, R. Non-invasive measurement of glucocorticoids: Advances and problems. Physiol. Behav. 2019, 199, 229–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Rooney, N.J.; Gaines, S.A.; Bradshaw, J.W. Behavioural and glucocorticoid responses of dogs (Canis familiaris) to kennelling: Investigating mitigation of stress by prior habituation. Physiol. Behav. 2007, 92, 847–854. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Carrier, L.O.; Cyr, A.; Anderson, R.E.; Walsh, C.J. Exploring the dog park: Relationships between social behaviours, personality and cortisol in companion dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2013, 146, 96–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Iki, T.; Ahrens, F.; Pasche, K.H.; Bartels, A.; Erhard, M.H. Relationships between scores of the feline temperament profile and behavioural and adrenocortical responses to a mild stressor in cats. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2011, 132, 71–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Ramos, D.; Reche-Junior, A.; Fragoso, P.; Palme, R.; Yanasse, N.; Gouvêa, V.; Beck, A.; Mills, D. Are cats (Felis catus) from multi-cat households more stressed? Evidence from assessment of fecal glucocorticoid metabolite analysis. Physiol. Behav. 2013, 122, 72–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Fukimoto, N.; Melo, D.; Palme, R.; Zanella, A.J.; Mendonça-Furtado, O. Are cats less stressed in homes than in shelters? A study of personality and faecal cortisol metabolites. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2020, 224, 104919. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Experimental design utilized. Each square represents a day.
Figure 1. Experimental design utilized. Each square represents a day.
Animals 11 02264 g001
Figure 2. Cluster dendrogram ran with the percentage of observations of all stereotyped behaviours performed on pre-interventions period. Numbers indicate subjects (Table 1), the dotted line cluster indicates high-stereotyping individuals and the solid line cluster indicates low-stereotyping individuals.
Figure 2. Cluster dendrogram ran with the percentage of observations of all stereotyped behaviours performed on pre-interventions period. Numbers indicate subjects (Table 1), the dotted line cluster indicates high-stereotyping individuals and the solid line cluster indicates low-stereotyping individuals.
Animals 11 02264 g002
Figure 3. Boxplot of percentage of stereotyped behaviour in our four experimental treatments (Pre: pre-interventions; Toy: toy intervention; Lawn: lawn intervention; Post: post-interventions). Analysis of only high-stereotyping individuals.
Figure 3. Boxplot of percentage of stereotyped behaviour in our four experimental treatments (Pre: pre-interventions; Toy: toy intervention; Lawn: lawn intervention; Post: post-interventions). Analysis of only high-stereotyping individuals.
Animals 11 02264 g003
Figure 4. Graph presenting the linear correlation coefficient between the percentage of stereotyped behaviours presented by each individual, and the extension of their respective behavioural repertories (number of different behaviours performed). Individual values are represented by the diamonds.
Figure 4. Graph presenting the linear correlation coefficient between the percentage of stereotyped behaviours presented by each individual, and the extension of their respective behavioural repertories (number of different behaviours performed). Individual values are represented by the diamonds.
Animals 11 02264 g004
Figure 5. Boxplot of faecal cortisol metabolite (FCM) concentrations of all individuals in the four experimental periods (Pre: pre-interventions; 1st: first intervention; 2nd: second intervention; Post: post-interventions). Asterisks indicate significant differences between the two periods: Pre and 2nd.
Figure 5. Boxplot of faecal cortisol metabolite (FCM) concentrations of all individuals in the four experimental periods (Pre: pre-interventions; 1st: first intervention; 2nd: second intervention; Post: post-interventions). Asterisks indicate significant differences between the two periods: Pre and 2nd.
Animals 11 02264 g005
Table 1. Subjects of the study identified by breed, sex, age when arrived at Guard kennel and at the beginning of the experiment and the guard duty scale which they belonged to. Animals from the same scales worked together and had the “rest” period at the same time.
Table 1. Subjects of the study identified by breed, sex, age when arrived at Guard kennel and at the beginning of the experiment and the guard duty scale which they belonged to. Animals from the same scales worked together and had the “rest” period at the same time.
SubjectBreed *SexAge When Arrived Guard KennelAge at Beginning of ExperimentDuty Scale
1MALF3 months4 years 7 months1
2GSM1 year 6 months1 year 8 months2
3MALF3 months4 years1
4ROM2 years 2 months2 years 7 months2
5MALM3 months2 years 7 months1
6MALM3 months4 years 7 months2
7MALM3 months4 years 3 months2
8MALM2 months3 years 2 months1
* MAL: Belgian Shepherd Malinois; GS: German Shepherd; RO: Rottweiler.
Table 2. Ethogram adapted from Hubrecht (1993) with behaviours and descriptions, organized into behavioural categories.
Table 2. Ethogram adapted from Hubrecht (1993) with behaviours and descriptions, organized into behavioural categories.
CategoryBehaviourDefinition
Readiness/AlertAnxious waitingIn front of the gate, eyes seeking, upright posture
StandStanding on four legs.
WalkAmbulatory gait.
StereotypedCircleCircular trajectory. At least three consecutive times.
PaceWalking back and forth along a boundary line, with no identified purpose (such as defecating, going to the water bowl, or reaching gate when listening to a noise).
SpinQuickly turning in a tight circle, pivoting about hind legs. At least three consecutive times.
Wall-bounceJumping at the wall and rebounding off it.
Head twirlCircular movement of the head, when the dog is at the end of a route, before changing direction.
Rest/
Relaxation
Rest with head upLying down with eyes open or closed, head up.
Rest with head downLying down with eyes open or closed, head down.
Not seenDog laid down on a wooden bed in the resting area.
OthersSitSitting on hind legs.
UrinateUrinating in a squatting position or with one leg cocked.
DefecateSquatting and defecating.
Sniff groundDirecting nose to the ground.
DigScratching the floor with the forepaws.
ClimbClimbing the kennel gate.
GnawGnawing gate bars or other non-nutritive material.
AutogroomLicking, pulling out body hair.
ScratchScratching body with one of the hind legs.
DrinkDrinking/with the mouth at the water bowl.
RunRunning gait.
Aggressive barkingLoud, rough vocalization.
Play with waterPlaying at the water bowl using the forelegs or nose.
LickLicking the floor or the walls.
Interaction with different objectsInteracting with novel objects at the kennel, such as insects or leaves.
Table 3. Pairwise comparison of Friedman test ran with animals from the high-stereotyping cluster for the frequency of stereotyped behaviours in the four experimental treatments. Adjusted significance (Adj. Sig.): significance after Bonferroni correction was applied.
Table 3. Pairwise comparison of Friedman test ran with animals from the high-stereotyping cluster for the frequency of stereotyped behaviours in the four experimental treatments. Adjusted significance (Adj. Sig.): significance after Bonferroni correction was applied.
Sample 1/Sample 2Test StatisticStd. ErrorStd. Test StatisticSig.Adj. Sig.
Lawn/Post1.0001.0540.9490.3431.000
Lawn/Pre2.3331.0542.2140.0270.161
Lawn/Toy2.6671.0542.5300.0110.068
Post/Pre1.3331.0541.2650.2061.000
Post/Toy1.6671.0541.5810.1140.683
Pre/Toy−0.3331.054−0.3160.7521.000
Table 4. Pairwise comparison of Friedman test ran with animals from high-stereotyping cluster for the frequency of rest/relaxation behaviours in the four experimental treatments. Adjusted significance (Adj. Sig.): significance after Bonferroni correction was applied.
Table 4. Pairwise comparison of Friedman test ran with animals from high-stereotyping cluster for the frequency of rest/relaxation behaviours in the four experimental treatments. Adjusted significance (Adj. Sig.): significance after Bonferroni correction was applied.
Sample 1/
Sample 2
Test StatisticStd. ErrorStd. Test StatisticSig.Adj. Sig.
Lawn/Post0.3331.0540.3160.7521.000
Lawn/Pre−1.6671.054−1.5810.1140.683
Lawn/Toy2.6671.0542.5300.0110.068
Post/Pre−1.3331.054−1.2650.2061.000
Post/Toy−2.3331.054−2.2140.0270.161
Pre/Toy1.0001.0540.9490.3431.000
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Nogueira, L.B.; Palme, R.; Mendonça-Furtado, O. Give Them a Toy or Increase Time out of Kennel at Lawn Areas: What Is the Influence of These Interventions on Police Dogs’ Welfare? Animals 2021, 11, 2264. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11082264

AMA Style

Nogueira LB, Palme R, Mendonça-Furtado O. Give Them a Toy or Increase Time out of Kennel at Lawn Areas: What Is the Influence of These Interventions on Police Dogs’ Welfare? Animals. 2021; 11(8):2264. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11082264

Chicago/Turabian Style

Nogueira, Letícia Bicudo, Rupert Palme, and Olívia Mendonça-Furtado. 2021. "Give Them a Toy or Increase Time out of Kennel at Lawn Areas: What Is the Influence of These Interventions on Police Dogs’ Welfare?" Animals 11, no. 8: 2264. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11082264

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop