Next Article in Journal
Dietary Fiber Level Improve Growth Performance, Nutrient Digestibility, Immune and Intestinal Morphology of Broilers from Day 22 to 42
Previous Article in Journal
Business Intentions of Australian Veterinary Students—My Business or Yours? A Cluster Analysis
Previous Article in Special Issue
Infrared Thermography for Early Identification and Treatment of Shoulder Sores to Improve Sow and Piglet Welfare
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Performance and Welfare of Sows Exposed to Auditory Environmental Enrichment in Mixed or Collective Housing Systems

by
Janaina Palermo Mendes
1,
Fabiana Ribeiro Caldara
1,*,
Maria Fernanda de Castro Burbarelli
1,
Jean Kaique Valentim
1,
Daniela Ferreira de Brito Mandú
1,
Rodrigo Garófallo Garcia
1,
Ibiara Correia de Lima Almeida-Paz
2,
Agnes Markiy Odakura
1 and
Marconi Italo Lourenço da Silva
2
1
Animal Science Postgraduate Program, Agricultural Sciences, Federal University of Grande Dourados (UFGD), Dourados 79804-970, MS, Brazil
2
Department of Animal Production, School of Veterinary Medicine and Animal Science (FMVZ), São Paulo State University (UNESP), Botucatu 18610-034, SP, Brazil
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Animals 2023, 13(7), 1226; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13071226
Submission received: 19 February 2023 / Revised: 10 March 2023 / Accepted: 27 March 2023 / Published: 31 March 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Effects of Sows on Piglets: Nutrition, Behavior and Welfare)

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

Auditory environmental enrichment may be used to relieve animal stress and has been reported to have positive behavioral effects in pigs, considering it aims to improve restrictive living conditions associated with intensive production systems. This research aimed to assess the effects of auditory environmental enrichment on sows in mixed housing (caged until 35 days after insemination and then collective pens) or collective housing (caged until 72 h after insemination and then collective pens). The collective reproductive management did not demonstrate losses in the productive performance of swine, which may be an interesting management alternative as it reduces the time spent in individual facilities, improving the welfare of sows. These findings may be used to confront conservative protocols that indicate log periods in individual facilities to prevent gestational loses. The auditory environment enrichment also had positive influences on sows’ welfare, which was reflected in the performance of piglets; calm sows had heavier weaned litters, which may be considered another interesting finding for swine production.

Abstract

The research aimed to assess the effects of auditory environmental enrichment on sows in mixed housing (caged until 35 days after insemination and then collective pens) or collective housing (caged until 72 h after insemination and then collective pens). Reproductive performance, body surface and eye temperature (ET) were evaluated as sows’ welfare indicators. A sample of 56 sows between 2nd and 6th parity was submitted to the treatments from artificial insemination to weaning. The sows were assigned in a randomized block design with a 2 × 2 factorial scheme of treatments: mixed housing—control (MH-C), collective housing—control (CH-C), mixed housing—music (MH-M) and collective housing—music (CH-M). Auditory enrichment consisted of exposing sows daily throughout gestation and lactation to 6 h of classical music divided into 2 h periods. The ET of pregnant sows in collective housing was lower than that of sows in mixed housing (33.77 °C vs. 34.91 °C). Pregnant and lactating sows exposed to auditory environmental enrichment exhibited lower ET compared to those that had no access to the stimulus (pregnant 33.36 °C vs. 34.32 °C and lactating 34.21 °C vs. 34.83 °C). No housing type effect was found on the reproductive performance parameter; however, piglets from sows submitted to auditory environmental enrichment, regardless of the type of housing during gestation, were heavier at weaning (6.32 kg vs. 5.57 kg). Collective or mixed housing does not affect sows’ reproductive performance; perhaps, auditory environmental enrichment reduced stress in the gestation and lactation phases and provided greater piglet weight gain at weaning.

1. Introduction

The worldwide increase in demand for meat products and the increasingly competitive market has directed industries to reach greater productivity, higher meat quality [1,2] and lower costs. Therefore, facilities have become smaller, aiming to house the largest number of animals in the smallest space [3]. Sows have been housed in individual pens during gestation and lactation to optimize space and increase reproductive control [4]. However, this system directly impacts sows’ welfare, since in crates, movements are restricted and the expression of natural species behaviors are significantly restricted, which causes behavioral issues, such as stereotypies and decreased maternal ability, especially in nest building before farrowing [5,6,7].
With global pressure regarding the welfare of production animals, alternatives to those systems, particularly concerning gestation crates, have been gradually replacing the individual model. Currently, the most widely accepted management for the gestation phase allows pregnant sows to be housed in individual pens for up to 35 days after mating, after which they are transferred to collective pens [8]. The reason for the adoption of this practice is that the stress of regrouping unfamiliar sows with each other prior to embryo nidation, which occurs around 17 to 24 days after fertilization, may lead to higher embryonic mortality rates [9].
With the aim to improve the quality of life for pregnant sows, recently, group housing systems have been proposed, in which females are inseminated in individual crates and transferred to collective pens after a few days. However, making it viable requires ways of reducing the stress of regrouping, establishment of social hierarchy and competition for food [4,10]. In this context, environmental enrichment may be a fundamental tool in controlling stress, considering it aims to improve restrictive living conditions associated with intensive production systems and to provide an opportunity for the expression of typical behaviors of the species, thus helping manage undesirable and harmful behaviors, such as aggressiveness, tail biting, sham chewing, belly nosing, etc. [11].
Auditory environmental enrichment may be used to relieve animal stress and has been reported to have positive behavioral effects in pigs and other species [12,13]. In addition, it may also break the silence, transforming an auditory monotonous environment into a more pleasant and productive one, which positively affects the cognitive development of animals [14]. For pregnant sows, Silva et al., [15] concluded that classical music promoted greater relaxation and reduction of agonistic behaviors and stereotypies, in addition to modulating physiological parameters that are indicators of stress, such as reduced respiratory rate. However, few reports were found relating the effects of auditory environmental enrichment on production parameters, as well as the evaluation of its benefits for other categories, such as lactating sows and their litters. Thus, the effects of environmental sound enrichment on aspects of welfare and productivity in swine production need to be better elucidated scientifically.
In the face of this, the present research was carried out aiming to assess the effect of auditory environmental enrichment on the performance and welfare of sows in mixed or collective housing during the gestation phase.

2. Material and Methods

All procedures of this study were approved by the Ethics Committee on the Use of Animals (CEUA) of the Federal University of Grande Dourados—UFGD, Dourados, Mato Grosso do Sul State, Brazil, under protocol n° 02/2020

2.1. Site Description

The experiment was conducted between February and June 2020 in a commercial piglet production unit hosting 3,500 sows in Ivinhema, Mato Grosso do Sul, Brazil. The site is located at 22°21′45″ S, 53°52′49″ W, and 406 m altitude. The climate in the region, according to the Köppen classification, is Aw, i.e., tropical climate with dry winters, with an average of 1200 to 1800 mm of annual rainfall and a mean annual temperature of 25 °C, possibly reaching highs up to 40 °C in spring and lows of 10 °C in winter.
During the experimental period, the ambient temperature was measured using thermo-hygrometers (model Novo Test TH802A®, NovotestBR, Vinhedo, SP, Brazil), positioned in the center of the pens and cages. Ambient temperature over the experimental period varied between 13.2 °C and 34.9 °C in the morning and 22.8 °C to 36.0 °C in the afternoon.

2.2. Animals, Experimental Design and Treatments

For the trial, 56 DanBred sows between the 2nd and 6th parturition were selected after weaning. Immediately after weaning, they were transferred to two identical gestation barns, 15 m from each other. The crates had 0.64 × 2.20 m and a compacted floor. All facilities were separated by enough physical space to ensure acoustic isolation between treatments, ensuring the absence of treatment bias. All pens and crates had the same environmental condition, and all management was the same in every treatment (except for the music).
There were 28 sows in each barn, according to the group of treatment (music or control), housed in individual cages. The experimental period began at the moment the sows were artificially inseminated and lasted until the end of the subsequent lactation phase, totaling 135 days (114 days of gestation and 21 days of lactation).
Sows of the collective housing treatment were transferred to group pens 72 h after artificial insemination (AI), while those in the mixed management group remained in individual cages until 35 days after insemination, and then they were transferred to collective pens. The stocking rate in the collective pens was 2.25 m2 per sow. The sows were assigned in a 2 × 2 factorial randomized block design, according to parturition order, to the following treatments (Table 1).
Approximately seven days prior to the expected delivery date, all sows were transferred to two identical farrowing rooms, in the respective barn, and housed in conventional farrowing crates with fully slatted floors and creep. The sows were fed with commercial corn–soybean meal-based diets, formulated to meet the nutritional requirements of each phase [16] (gestation AMEn 3240 kcal/kg, crude protein 14.95%, crude fiber 1.86% and 0.72% lysine; lactation AMEn 3510 kcal/kg, crude protein 19.92%, crude fiber 2.16% and 1.25% lysine). Feed was provided twice a day in the gestation phase and three times a day in the lactation phase. All animals (sows and piglets) had ad libitum water access with nipple drinkers.

2.3. Auditory Stimuli

Sows in treatments with auditory enrichment (MH-M and CH-M) were exposed to classical music every day, which was randomly chosen. Auditory enrichment was a 2 h playlist with Bach violin concertos with movements ranging from Largo (44 to 48 BPM) to Presto (180 to 200 BPM) tempos. The playlist was played three times a day, with 2 h intervals between each sequence; thus, the animals were exposed to a total of 6 h of music from 9 a.m. to 7 p.m.
The sound stimuli started 72 h after artificial insemination and lasted throughout the gestation and lactation phase of the sows. The auditory stimulus was reproduced via an amplified speaker (Frahm® CM 600 BT, Frahm, Rio do Sul—SC, Brazil) with 200 W RMS power and USB input positioned in the shed so that all animals received a sound stimulus in which the intensity remained between 60 and 75 dB, measured with a decibel meter (AKSO® AK823, Akso, São Leopoldo-RS, Brazil).

2.4. Body Surface and Eye Temperature

The surface and eye temperature of the sows were measured using an infrared thermography camera (FLIR thermal camera® Teledyne Flir, Wilsonville—United States) throughout the experimental period once a week during gestation and twice a week during lactation. All measurements were taken on the same animals in the same phase throughout the experimental period. Images were captured at a distance of 1.0 m (perpendicular to the sow), at 7:30 a.m. and 11:30 a.m., prior to music reproduction, and at 10:30 a.m. and 2:30 p.m., during music reproduction.
Images were assessed using the specific software of the equipment (FLIR Report Studio® Wilsonville—United States), where the reading of the color spectrum was converted into surface temperature. The coefficient of emissivity employed was 0.96 both for body surface and eye temperatures. The mean surface temperature and standard deviation of the body area were calculated using the temperature from 30 evenly distributed points selected to represent the overall body surface of the animals (Figure 1).

2.5. Reproductive Indices

The following reproductive indices of the sows were assessed: delivery duration (from the birth of the first piglet to placental expulsion), interval between births, number of live births, stillbirths, mummified piglets and individual piglet weight at birth and at weaning (21 days of age). Following the standard procedures of the farm for pig management, approximately 24 h after birth, litters were standardized in order to balance them in terms of weight and number of piglets per sow. This procedure was carried out between sows of the same treatment. After weaning the piglets, the weaning-estrus interval was evaluated, considering the number of days that elapsed between the weaning date and the 1st artificial insemination.

2.6. Statistical Analysis

The variables delivery duration, interval between births, live births, stillbirths, mummified piglets, piglet weight at birth and weaning, and surface and ocular temperatures (before and after the music playing) were evaluated for normality of the residues according to the Shapiro–Wilk test and for homogenicity of the variances according to Levene’s test.
Analysis of variance was performed using the PROC MIXED of the software [17], and data were analyzed as a randomized complete block design with block as a random effect. As there were sows with different parturition orders, this character was added to the mathematical model as a co-variable for data correction. The values on the surface and eye temperature were influenced by the ambient temperature at the time of measurement; thus, the ambient temperature was added to the mathematical model as a co-variable for data correction. The effects of interactions between the type of housing and auditory environmental enrichment were evaluated; when the interactions were significant, means were compared with Tukey’s test. When only principal effects were observed, means were compared using the F-test. All analyses used a 5% probability significance.

3. Results

3.1. Body Surface Temperature (ST) and Eye Temperature (ET) of Pregnant Sows

There was no interaction between the type of housing after insemination and environmental enrichment for ST or ET either before or during music reproduction.
However, a main effect of the type of housing and environmental enrichment on ET was observed in both periods evaluated (before exposure to music and during exposure to music). The ET of sows in collective housing was lower than those in the mixed housing system in both periods evaluated. Regarding auditory environmental enrichment, sows submitted to music stimuli had lower ET than those with no access to the auditory stimulus in both periods evaluated (Table 2).
No effect of housing was found on ST before or during music reproduction. Sows with auditory environmental enrichment exhibited higher ST compared to those in the control groups regardless of the type of housing assessed before music reproduction. However, during the reproduction of the auditory stimulus, no difference was seen among the groups (Table 2).

3.2. Body Surface Temperature (ST) and Eye Temperature (ET) of Lactating Sows

Before music reproduction, the ST of sows in the treatments with auditory environmental enrichment was lower than that in the control groups during the lactation period (Table 3).
An interaction was found between the type of housing and environmental enrichment only for ST during music reproduction. Sows from the collective housing (CH-C and CH-M) did not differ from each other; however, for sows from the mixed housing system (MH-C and MH-M), the surface temperature was higher for those submitted to the musical stimulus (MH-M). Comparing sows receiving auditory stimuli or not between housing systems (MH vs. CH), there was no difference in both comparisons (Table 3).
For eye temperature, there was only a main effect for the musical stimulus, sows exposed to music were lower than those with no access to the auditory stimulus during lactation both before and during music reproduction (Table 3).

3.3. Reproductive Indices

No interaction was observed between housing systems and environmental enrichment, nor any main effect of type of housing for any of the performance parameters assessed. No effect of auditory environmental enrichment was found on the weaning-to-estrus interval, delivery duration, number of live births, stillbirths, mummified piglets or weight at birth. However, piglets from sows submitted to auditory enrichment, regardless of the type of housing during gestation, were heavier at weaning than those in the control groups (Table 4).

4. Discussion

Body Surface and Eye Temperature

During the gestation phase, the housing system after insemination had no effect on the sows surface temperature. However, sows in the CH groups exhibited lower eye temperature. In homeothermic animals, stress is related to an increase in body temperature and alteration in peripheric blood circulation [18,19,20]. Faraji and Metz [21] observed an increase in body surface temperature in rats submitted to space deprivation or contained. Foster and Ijichi [22] observed that cats living alone, with no others of the same species in the house, had higher eye temperatures than cats housed in groups. When an animal is stressed, the hypothalamus-hypophysis-adrenal axis is activated and produces heat due to an increase in catecholamine and cortisol concentrations, mediating physiological and behavioral alterations [20,23]. The central nervous system is primarily responsible for regulating body temperature; thus, brain temperature may be recognized as the core system temperature. Considering eye proximity with the brain, eye temperature is considered a good indicator of core temperature [24]. Blood flow in the eye is closely related to sympathetic activity [25]; stress responses may be detected using eye temperature variations. The present results indicated that the greater sensitivity of eye temperature in relation to skin temperature was an indicator of stress. Sows from mixed housing systems showed an increase in eye temperature independent of environmental enrichment; perhaps, skin temperature had no pronounced effects. Pigs are sensitive to high environmental temperatures, which may be explained by the keratinized sweat glands, in this sense, heat may strongly affect sows’ physiology [26]. However, in our study, the effect of environmental temperature was considered in the data analysis in an attempt to isolate its effect, allowing a possible more accurate assessment of the effect of stress on surface temperature, even so, no effect of treatments on surface temperature was observed, which reinforces the possibility of this parameter being less affected than the core temperature resulting from stress.
In the present study sows exposed to auditory enrichment exhibited lower eye temperature than those in the control. These findings were similar to Yáñez-Pizaña et al., [27] with piglets kept in environments enriched with ropes, bottles, flavorings and other toys and exhibited lower values of eye temperature compared to those kept in pens with no stimulus. A decrease in eye temperature may be related to relaxation provided by music, thus reducing ambiental stress [28]. Musical stimulus may lead to physical, mental and social alterations in individuals by activating brain regions responsible for cognitive and motor changes [29,30,31], particularly in the auditory association areas. Higher concentrations of neurotrophins in brain tissue have been associated with improved cognitive functions [30] and greater resilience against stress in studies that successfully used environmental enrichment [29].
No effect of gestation housing type on surface and eye temperatures of sows was observed during lactation, these findings may be related to the crates housing system during lactation, the same system for all the treatments. The effects of auditory enrichment on eye temperature of lactating sows were similar to those observed during gestation, suggesting music is a good tool for alleviating stress during the confinement period and also in individual farrowing crates.

5. Reproductive Indices

Regardless of the use of auditory environmental enrichment, no effects of type of housing during gestation were observed on sow reproductive indices, which indicates that the collective housing system may be considered a viable alternative to reduce the time of individual confinement without compromising economic and performance results. Gentilini et al. and Bampi et al., [32,33] found no differences in birth rates between the two housing systems, crates or collective pens. Mixed housing, in which sows are kept for about 28 to 35 days in individual crates after artificial insemination, mainly aims to avoid embryo losses, which is influenced by stress due to disputes to establish a social hierarchy or for food [9]; thus, fights related to social hierarchy may be observed in the first days of regrouping. Embryo implantation occurs between the 17th and 24th days after fertilization; thus, regrouping sows during that phase may increase embryonic mortality and, consequently, reduced fertilization rate or litter size [34]
According to Varley [35], regrouping should be done either before or after the embryo implantation period. If sows were regrouped 72 h after artificial insemination, the risk of embryonic resorption is reduced. This condition may explain the lack of difference in reproductive indices between types of housing.
However, it is important to highlight that fights between pregnant sows in collective housing do not occur only to establish social hierarchy and may last for longer periods, mainly in situations where sows were fed with a restricted diet at about 40 to 60% of their voluntary intake capacity, which results in a low degree of satiety and constant seeking for feed [36]. These stressful situations must also be under control in order to avoid embryonic losses.
There were no influences of auditory enrichment on most of the reproductive indices, except for piglet weight at weaning, which was higher for those from sows exposed to classical music during gestation and lactation. Fetal development is impacted by environmental factors. Thus, physical and psychological stressors in pregnant individuals impact lower offspring weight at birth, an increase in complications risks during delivery, greater incidence of abnormalities and neonatal mortality rates [37]. It was expected that exposing pregnant sows to music would be able to reduce stress and consequently increase the number of live births, weight at birth and decrease in embryonic mortality, which was not observed.
Auditory enrichment may help the development of innate behaviors, which benefits animal welfare and performance. The positive effects of music on piglets’ weaning weight may be related to the welfare of sows and piglets; stress reduces maternal care [38]. Lactating females in better conditions of welfare present reduced stress and stereotyped behaviors, resulting in a higher frequency of suckling. These facts may explain the better results of piglet weaning weight [38].
Additionally, piglets exposed to music had a lower expression of agonistic behaviors and an increased expression of playful behaviors [12,13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39]. The expression of abnormal behaviors, such as stereotypies and excessive aggressiveness, indicate unfavorable welfare conditions [40], which are related to facility issues and improper animal management, resulting in a decrease in productive performance. To emphasize the magnitude of these findings, considering the average difference in weaning weight observed in this study (0.746 kg), and estimating values of 2.4 births/sow/year, with an average of 13 weaned piglets per birth, auditory enrichment would be able to provide approximately 23,275 kg more of weaned piglets for every 1000 sows in the batch.

6. Conclusions

Collective or mixed housing systems do not affect sows’ reproductive performance, regardless of the use of auditory environmental enrichment. Exposing pregnant and lactating sows to classical music reduced their eye temperature, which suggests a decrease in stress inherent to these productive phases. Stress reduction resulted in higher piglet weight at weaning.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: J.P.M., F.R.C. and I.C.d.L.A.-P.; methodology, F.R.C., R.G.G. and I.C.d.L.A.-P.; formal analysis, M.F.d.C.B.; investigation, J.P.M., J.K.V., D.F.d.B.M., A.M.O. and M.I.L.d.S.; resources, F.R.C.; data curation, M.F.d.C.B.; writing—original draft preparation, J.P.M., F.R.C. and M.F.d.C.B.; writing—review and editing, J.P.M., F.R.C. and M.F.d.C.B.; supervision, F.R.C., R.G.G. and I.C.d.L.A.-P.; project administration, F.R.C.; funding acquisition, F.R.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by CAPES—Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior, Brazil with author’s scholarship.

Institutional Review Board Statement

All procedures of this study were approved by the Ethics Committee on the Use of Animals (CEUA) of the Federal University of Grande Dourados—UFGD, Dourados, Mato Grosso do Sul State, Brazil, under protocol n° 02/2020 on 16 November 2020.

Informed Consent Statement

“Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.” The company responsible for the animals, as well as the professionals involved in this research, consented to its realization and a partnership contract was signed between the university and the company.

Data Availability Statement

The data that support this study will be shared upon reasonable request to the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by CAPES—Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior, Brazil.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Sardi, L.; Gastaldo, A.; Borciani, M.; Bertolini, A.; Musi, V.; Martelli, G.; Cavallini, D.; Rubini, G.; Nannoni, E. Identification of Possible Pre-Slaughter Indicators to Predict Stress and Meat Quality: A Study on Heavy Pigs. Animals 2020, 10, 945. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Sardi, L.; Gastaldo, A.; Borciani, M.; Bertolini, A.; Musi, V.; Garavaldi, A.; Martelli, G.; Cavallini, D.; Nannoni, E. Pre-Slaughter Sources of Fresh Meat Quality Variation: The Case of Heavy Pigs Intended for Protected Designation of Origin Products. Animals 2020, 10, 2386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Nannoni, E.; Buonaiuto, G.; Martelli, G.; Lizzi, G.; Trevisani, G.; Garavini, G.; Sardi, L. Influence of Increased Freedom of Movement on Welfare and Egg Laying Pattern of Hens Kept in Aviaries. Animals 2022, 12, 2307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Koketsu, Y.; Lida, R. Sow housing associated with reproductive performance inbreeding herds. Mol. Repr. Develop. 2017, 84, 979–986. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Oliviero, C.; Heinonen, M.; Valros, A.; Hälli, O.; Peltoniemi, O.A.T. Effect of the environment on the physiology of the sow during late pregnancy, farrowing and early lactation. Anim. Repr. Sci. 2008, 105, 365–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Singh, C.; Verdon, M.; Cronin, G.M.; Hemsworth, P.H. The behaviour and welfare of sows and piglets in farrowing crates or lactation pens. Animal 2017, 11, 1210–1221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Loftus, L.; Bell, G.; Padmore, E.; Atkinson, S.; Henworth, A.; Hoyle, M. The effect of two different farrowing systems on sow behaviour, and piglet behaviour, mortality and growth. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2020, 232, 105102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. MAPA–Ministério Da Agricultura Pecuária E Abastecimento. Instrução normativa no. 113, de 16 de dezembro de 2020. Estabelece as boas práticas de manejo e bem-estar nas granjas de suínos de criação comercial. Diário Oficial da União, 16 December 2020; p. 5. [Google Scholar]
  9. Alvarenga, A.L.N.; Chiarini-Garcia, H.; Cardeal, P.C.; Moreira, L.P.; Foxcrot, G.R.; Fontes, D.O.; Almeida, F.C.R.L. Intra-uterine growth retardation affects birth weight and post natal development in pigs, impairing muscle accretion, duodenal mucosa morphology and carcass traits. Reprod. Fert. Develop. 2013, 25, 387–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Ludtke, C.; Calvo, A.V.; Bueno, A.D. Perspectivas para o Bem-Estar Animal na Suinocultura. In Produção de Suínos–Teoria e Prática, 1st ed.; Associação Brasileira dos Criadores de Suínos: Brasília, Brazil, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  11. Van de Weerd, H.; Ison, S. Providing Effective Environmental Enrichment to Pigs: How Far Have We Come? Animal 2019, 9, 254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  12. Jonge, F.H.; Boleij, H.; Baars, A.M.; Dudink, S.; Spruijt, B.M. Musica during play-time: Using context conditioning as a tool to improve welfare in piglet. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008, 15, 138–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Alworth, L.C.; Buerkle, S.C. The effects of music on animal physiology, behavior and welfare Lab. Anim. 2013, 4, 54–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Cruz, J.G.P.; Magro, D.D.; Cruz, J.N. Efeitos da música clássica como elemento de enriquecimento ambiental em Mus musculus em cativeiro (Rodentia Muridae). Biotemas 2011, 23, 191–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Silva, F.R.; Miranda, K.O.D.S.; Piedade, S.M.D.S.; Salgado, D.D.A. Effect of auditory enrichment (music) in pregnant sows welfare. Engen. Agric. 2017, 37, 215–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Rostagno, H.S.; Albino, L.F.T.; Donzele, J.L.; Gomes, P.C.; de Oliveira, R.F.; Lopes, D.C.; Euclides, R. Tabelas Brasileiras para Suínos e Aves; Rostagno, H.S., Ed.; Universidade Federal de Viçosa: Viçosa, Brasil, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  17. Statistical Analisys System (SAS). User’s Guide; SAS: Cary, NC, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  18. McManus, C.; Tanure, C.B.; Peripolli, V.; Seixas, L.; Fischer, V.; Gabbi, A.M.; Costa, J.B.G., Jr. Infrared thermography in animal production: An overview. Comp. Electron. Agric. 2016, 123, 10–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Unruh, E.M.; Theurer, M.E.; White, B.J.; Larson, R.L.; Drouillard, J.S.; Schrag, N. Evaluation of infrared thermography as a diagnostic tool to predict heat stress events in feedlot cattle. Am. J. Vet. Res. 2017, 78, 171–777. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Herborn, K.A.; Jerem, P.; Nager, R.G.; McKeegan, D.E.; McCafferty, D.J. Surface temperature elevated by chronic and intermittent stress. Physiol. Behav. 2018, 191, 47–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Faraji, J.; Metz, G.A. Infrared Thermography Reveals sex-specific responses to stress in mice. Front. Behav. Neurosc. 2020, 14, 79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Foster, S.; Ijichi, I. The association between infrared termal imagery of core eye temperatura, personality, age and housin in cats. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2017, 189, 79–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Soroko, M.; Howell, K.; Zwyrzykowska, A.; Dudek, K.; Zielińska, P.; Kupczyński, R. Maximum eye temperature in the assessment of training in race horses: Correlations with salivar cortisol concentration, rectal temperature and heart rate. J. Equi. Vet. Sci. 2016, 45, 39–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Johnson, S.R.; Rao, S.; Hussey, S.B.; Morley, P.S.; Traub-Dargatz, J.L. Thermographic eye temperature as an index to body temperature in ponies. Equi. Vet. Sci. 2011, 31, 63–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Stewart, M.; Stafford, K.J.; Dowling, S.K.; Schaefer, A.L.; Webster, J.R. Eye temperature and heart rate variability of calves disbudded with or without local anaesthetic. Physiol. Behav. 2008, 93, 789–797. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Justino, E.; Nääs, I.D.A.; Carvalho, T.M.; Neves, D.P.; Salgado, D.D.A. The impact of evaporative cooling on the thermoregulation and sensible heat loss of sows during farrowing. Engen. Agric. 2014, 34, 1050–1061. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Yáñez-Pizaña, A.; Mota-Rojas, D.; Ramírez-Necoechea, R.; Castillo-Rivera, M.; Roldán-Santiago, P.; Mora-Medina, P.; González-Lozano, M. Application of infrared thermography to assess the effect of different types of environmental enrichment on the ocular, auricular pavilion and nose area temperatures of weaned piglets. Comp. Electron. Agric. 2019, 156, 33–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Krout, R.E. The effects of single-session music therapy interventions on the observed and self-reported levels of pain control, physical comfort, and relaxation of hospice patients. Am. J. Hosp. Palliat. Care. 2001, 18, 383–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Mosaferi, B.; Babri, S.; Mohaddes, G.; Khamnei, S.; Mesgari, M. Post-weaning environmental enrichment improves BDNF response of adult male rats. Int. J. Develop. Neurosci. 2015, 46, 108–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Novkovic, T.; Mittmann, T.; Manahan-Vaughan, D. BDNF contributes to the facilitation of hippocampal synaptic plasticity and learning enabled by environmental enrichment. Hippocampus 2015, 25, 145010. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Luz, C.S.M.; Farias, L.A.; Pimenta, J.L.L.A.; Santos, J.D.S.; Sousa, R.A.; Machado, L.P.; Sousa Júnior, S.C. Physiological Parameters of Pigs Raised with and without Environmental Enrichment. J. Agric. Sci. 2017, 4, 176–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Gentilini, F.; Dallanora, D.; Peixoto, C.; Bernardi, M.; Wentz, I.; Bortolozzo, F. Produtividade de leitoas alojadas em gaiolas ou baias coletivas durante a gestação. Arch. Vet. Sci. 2003, 8, 9–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Bampi, D.; Borstnez, K.K.; Dias, C.P.; Costa, O.A.D.; Moreira, F.; Peripolli, V.; Bianchi, I. Evaluation of reproductive and animal welfare parameters of swine females of different genetic lines submitted to different reproductive management and housing systems during pregnancy. Arq. Bras. Med. Vet. Zoot. 2020, 72, 1675–1682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Gonyou, H.W.; Rioja-Lang, F. Re-grouping and timing of re-grouping. Sci. Ethology 2014, 1, 6. [Google Scholar]
  35. Varley, M. Stress and reproduction. Pig News Inf. 1991, 12, 567–571. [Google Scholar]
  36. Bergeron, R.; Meunier-Salaun, C.; Robert, S. The welfare of pregnant and lactating sows. Welfare of Pigs from Birth to Slaughter; Wageningen Academic Publishers: Wageningen, The Netherlands, 2008; pp. 65–95. [Google Scholar]
  37. Kim, C.H.; Lee, S.C.; Shin, J.W.; Chung, K.J.; Lee, S.H.; Shin, M.S.; Kim, K.H. Exposure to music and noise during pregnancy influences neurogenesis and thickness in motor and somatosensory cortex of rat pups. Int. Neurourol. J. 2013, 17, 107–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  38. Ringgenberg, N.; Bergeron, R.; Meunier-Salaün, M.C.; Devillers, N. Impact of social stress during gestation and environmental enrichment during lactation on the maternal behavior of sows. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2012, 136, 126–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Cruz, J.N.; Lima, D.D.; Dal Magrom, D.D.; Cruz, J.G.P. Anxiolytic effect of mozart music over short and long photoperiods as part of environmental enrichment in captive Rattus norvegicus (Rodentia: Muridae). Scand. J. Lab. Anim Sci. 2015, 41, 17–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Broom, D.M.; Molento, C.F.M. Bem-estar animal: Conceito e questões relacionadas–Revisão. Arch. Vet. Sci. 2004, 9, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Thermographic image of the body of a sow with 30 points highlighted to calculate mean body surface temperature (left). Thermographic image of the eye of a sow with one highlighted point (right) during gestation.
Figure 1. Thermographic image of the body of a sow with 30 points highlighted to calculate mean body surface temperature (left). Thermographic image of the eye of a sow with one highlighted point (right) during gestation.
Animals 13 01226 g001
Table 1. Treatments used in the research.
Table 1. Treatments used in the research.
DescriptionMixed HousingCollective Housing
MH-CMH-MCH-CCH-M
Duration of housing in cages after AI35 days35 days72 h72 h
Duration of housing in collective pens72 days72 days104 days104 days
Auditory environmental enrichmentNOYESNOYES
MH-C = mixed housing with no music; MH-M = mixed housing with music; CH-C = collective housing with no music; CH-M = collective housing with music.
Table 2. Mean body surface temperature and eye temperature of pregnant sows in mixed housing or collective housing before and during exposure to auditory environmental enrichment.
Table 2. Mean body surface temperature and eye temperature of pregnant sows in mixed housing or collective housing before and during exposure to auditory environmental enrichment.
Variable (%)MusicHousingMeanSEMp-Value
MHCHHOUMUSHOU * MUS
ST (°C)—BMYES34.7534.6934.68 A0.0270.14780.01840.3379
NO34.6334.5234.58 B
AVG34.6634.6034.63
ST (°C)—DMYES34.4834.3434.410.0260.50650.84460.0571
NO34.3634.4534.41
AVG34.4234.4034.41
ET (°C)—BMYES33.2333.1633.19 B0.0370.0153<0.00010.1341
NO33.9333.6633.79 A
AVG33.58 a33.41 b33.52
ET (°C)—DMYES33.4133.3133.36 B0.0350.0193<0.00010.4437
NO34.4134.2234.32 A
AVG33.91 a33.77 b33.89
* Means followed by different capital letters in the same column and different small letters on the same row are statistically different according to Tukey’s test (p < 0.05). SEM = Standard error of the mean. HOU = Type of housing after AI. MUS = Music; ST = Mean body surface temperature; ET = eye temperature; MH = mixed housing; CH = collective housing; BM = before exposure to auditory environmental enrichment; DM = during exposure to auditory environmental enrichment.
Table 3. Mean body surface temperature and eye temperature of lactating sows in mixed housing or collective housing before and during exposure to auditory environmental enrichment.
Table 3. Mean body surface temperature and eye temperature of lactating sows in mixed housing or collective housing before and during exposure to auditory environmental enrichment.
Variable (%)MusicHousingMeanSEMp-Value
MHCHHOUMUSHOU * MUS
ST (°C)—BMYES34.9734.8434.90 B0.0460.51970.03610.2285
NO35.0535.0935.07 A
AVG35.0134.9634.990
ST (°C)—DMYES34.9 Aa34.65 Aa34.770.0480.44070.24020.0211
NO34.61 Ba34.74 Aa34.68
AVG34.7534.6934.72
ET (°C)—BMYES34.4734.3734.42 B0.0380.27610.02260.9231
NO34.6434.5634.6 A
AVG34.5534.4734.52
ET (°C)—DMYES34.2334.2034.21 B0.0310.8351<0.00010.6812
NO34.8334.8434.83 A
AVG34.5334.5234.55
* Means followed by different capital letters in the same column and different small letters on the same row are statistically different according to Tukey’s test (p < 0.05). SEM = Standard error of the mean. HOU = Type of housing after AI. MUS = Music; ST = Mean body surface temperature; ET = eye temperature; MH = mixed housing; CH = collective housing; BM = before exposure to auditory environmental enrichment; DM = during exposure to auditory environmental enrichment.
Table 4. Reproductive indices of sows from mixed (MH) or collective (CH) housing exposed or not to auditory environmental enrichment.
Table 4. Reproductive indices of sows from mixed (MH) or collective (CH) housing exposed or not to auditory environmental enrichment.
VariableMusicHousingMeanSEMp-Value
MHCHHOUMUSHOU * MUS
Weaning-to-estrus interval (days)YES5.314.504.90.3070.58840.0770.4335
NO3.733.883.81
AVG4.544.184.35
Birth Weight (kg)YES1.3791.3281.3530.0110.30160.2040.1486
NO1.3131.3231.318
AVG1.3461.3251.335
Weaning Weight (kg)YES6.2946.3466.320 A0.0470.2778<0.00010.6021
NO5.5005.6485.574 B
AVG5.8975.9975.927
Livebirths (n)YES18.16616.07117.1190.4840.08470.350.6798
NO16.85715.56216.209
AVG17.51115.81716.571
Stillbirths (n)YES0.6660.4280.5380.097---
NO0.8570.5000.666
AVG0.7690.4660.607
Mummified (n)YES0.5830.3570.4610.104---
NO0.2850.6250.466
AVG0.4230.5000.464
Delivery duration (min)YES27621224412.4670.18490.7310.2374
NO237234235
AVG257223239
* Means followed by different capital letters in the same column statistically differed according to Tukey’s test (p < 0.05). SEM = Standard error of the mean. HOU = Type of housing after AI. MUS = Music. - Due to the low occurrence of mummified and stillbirths, it was not possible to statistically evaluate the data. Only observed averages are presented.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Palermo Mendes, J.; Ribeiro Caldara, F.; de Castro Burbarelli, M.F.; Valentim, J.K.; Ferreira de Brito Mandú, D.; Garófallo Garcia, R.; Correia de Lima Almeida-Paz, I.; Markiy Odakura, A.; Lourenço da Silva, M.I. Performance and Welfare of Sows Exposed to Auditory Environmental Enrichment in Mixed or Collective Housing Systems. Animals 2023, 13, 1226. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13071226

AMA Style

Palermo Mendes J, Ribeiro Caldara F, de Castro Burbarelli MF, Valentim JK, Ferreira de Brito Mandú D, Garófallo Garcia R, Correia de Lima Almeida-Paz I, Markiy Odakura A, Lourenço da Silva MI. Performance and Welfare of Sows Exposed to Auditory Environmental Enrichment in Mixed or Collective Housing Systems. Animals. 2023; 13(7):1226. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13071226

Chicago/Turabian Style

Palermo Mendes, Janaina, Fabiana Ribeiro Caldara, Maria Fernanda de Castro Burbarelli, Jean Kaique Valentim, Daniela Ferreira de Brito Mandú, Rodrigo Garófallo Garcia, Ibiara Correia de Lima Almeida-Paz, Agnes Markiy Odakura, and Marconi Italo Lourenço da Silva. 2023. "Performance and Welfare of Sows Exposed to Auditory Environmental Enrichment in Mixed or Collective Housing Systems" Animals 13, no. 7: 1226. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13071226

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop