Next Article in Journal
Development and Validation of the Teacher Career-Related Support Self-Efficacy (TCSSE) Questionnaire
Previous Article in Journal
Gender Differences in Emotional Valence and Social Media Content Engagement Behaviors in Pandemic Diaries: An Analysis Based on Microblog Texts
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Prevalence of Mild, Moderate, and Severe Nomophobia Symptoms: A Systematic Review, Meta-Analysis, and Meta-Regression

1
Ministry of Health, Manama 410, Bahrain
2
Department of Psychiatry, College of Medicine and Medical Sciences, Arabian Gulf University, Manama 323, Bahrain
3
High Institute of Sport and Physical Education of Sfax, University of Sfax, Sfax 3000, Tunisia
4
Research Laboratory—Education, Motricity, Sport and Health, EM2S, LR19JS01, University of Sfax, Sfax 3000, Tunisia
5
SIESTA Research Group, School of Allied Health, Human Services and Sport, La Trobe University, Melbourne 3086, Australia
6
Ministry of Health, Sulaibkhat, Jamal Abdel Nasser Street, Kuwait 13001, Kuwait
7
Somnogen Canada Inc., College Street, Toronto, ON M5S 1A8, Canada
8
Saveetha Medical College and Hospitals, Saveetha Institute of Medical and Technical Sciences, Saveetha University, Chennai 602105, Tamil Nadu, India
9
Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON M5S 1A8, Canada
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2023, 13(1), 35; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13010035
Submission received: 12 November 2022 / Revised: 22 December 2022 / Accepted: 26 December 2022 / Published: 30 December 2022

Abstract

:
NOMOPHOBIA, or NO MObile PHone Phobia, refers to a psychological condition in which people fear being disconnected from their mobile phones. The purpose of this review was to establish the prevalence of nomophobia symptoms in youth and young adults according to severity, country, culture, population, measurement tool, and year of data collection. An electronic search of fourteen databases, two digital preservation services, and three content aggregator services was conducted from the inception of each database until 15 September 2021. A total of 52 studies involving 47,399 participants from 20 countries were included in the analyses. The prevalence of nomophobia was defined as the proportion of individuals scoring at or above established cut-offs on validated measures. Based on a random-effects meta-analysis, approximately 20% of individuals showed mild symptoms of nomophobia, 50% showed moderate symptoms, and 20% showed severe symptoms. Our results showed that university students from non-Western cultures are the most likely to suffer severe symptoms. In the year 2021, the prevalence rate of nomophobia increased. The instrument that was best able to detect nomophobia was the nomophobia questionnaire. Most individuals who own mobile phones experience mild or moderate symptoms of nomophobia. Severe symptoms deserve attention from clinicians and research scientists. A valid method of identifying individuals with a severe addiction to their mobile phones will help with timely and effective therapeutic management.

1. Introduction

‘Nomophobia’ stands for “no-mobile-phone phobia”, an acronym first coined by the authors of a UK Postal Office study in 2008 [1]. The study found that, out of a sample of about 2000 adults who owned mobile phones, >50% experienced symptoms of anxiety when unable to access their phones [1]. These are the characteristic symptoms of this condition [2,3] and they appear regardless of the reasons for access failure (losing or misplacing one’s phone, loss of battery life, or finding oneself in an area with no network coverage) [2]. In spite of its name, this condition does not appear to be a phobia, but rather a behavioral addiction [2].
Research into the determinants of nomophobia is ongoing [3] but the most common unanswered research question, thus far, remains accurate prevalence, i.e., the establishment of specific numbers of individuals with the condition in any given population over a specific time period [4,5].
The Nomophobia Questionnaire (NMP-Q), developed by researchers at Iowa State University, is the most commonly used tool in prevalence studies [4,5]. The NMP-Q uses a Likert-like scale and, therefore, can quantify the severity of nomophobia [4,5]. Four main dimensions and/or causes are involved in nomophobia: (1) fear or nervousness associated with not being able to communicate with others; (2) fear of not connecting with others; (3) fear of not having immediate access to information; and (4) fear of giving up the comfort provided by mobile devices [4,5].
Nomophobia is connected with feelings of loneliness, low self-esteem, and unhappiness, particularly among young people [2,3,4]. The development of a significant reliance on mobile technology that produces continual diversions also impacts other elements of life. School, work, and general productivity are negatively influenced [1,4]. Furthermore, such reliance contributes to interpersonal distance and isolation, impacting relationships and interactions [3,5].
There are several reasons why a systematic review and meta-analysis of the prevalence rate of nomophobia is needed [6]. The first is to increase the power and precision of point prevalence estimates to understand the magnitude of the problem at any point in time [6]. The second reason is to identify the prevalence differences between subpopulations, for instance, between university students and age-matched non-students, to determine the distribution of preventive and interventive resources [6]. Perhaps the most important scientific reason is to attempt to resolve conflicting results among past studies [6]. For example, estimates for nomophobia symptoms (of various severity) have ranged between 25% [7] and 100% [8]. This may depend on the measuring tool, the targeted demographic, or cultural differences in the importance placed on mobile phone communication. It may also depend on public health policies relative to behavioral addictions.
Our team recognized the need for prevalence accuracy and conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis at the end of 2020 [2]. We analyzed 20 papers, involving about 12,500 participants from ten countries. The prevalence of mild, moderate, and severe nomophobia was about 25%, 50%, and 20%, respectively [2].
Many questions were left unanswered in the previous work. Is this condition becoming more prevalent? Does it occur with undue frequency in specific local communities? Does its incidence correlate with a suspected cause? Are there variations among countries, cultures (Western vs. Non-Western), demographics, measurement tools, and time periods? Can COVID-19 be held responsible for fluctuations? This possibility arises because several public health measures implemented by different governments in response to COVID-19 have increased the use of communication technology [9].
For these reasons, we decided to conduct an updated review using the PICO framework (Population, Intervention, Comparator, Outcomes) [10]; Population: individuals over the age of 12, Intervention: none, Comparators: (a) different countries, (b) Western vs. Non-Western cultures, (c) age and sex comparisons, (d) differences according to measurement tools, and (e) time period, Outcomes: determination of the prevalence rate of the three levels of nomophobia symptoms.

2. Method

Before registering our protocol, we did a detailed analysis of PROSPERO and other evidence networks to prevent duplication. The protocol was then registered in the PROSPERO International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (PROSPERO) database (Registration number: CRD42022355657). Our protocol followed the PRISMA2020 protocol for systematic reviews and meta-analyses [11].
Two members of our team performed all independent electronic searches for pertinent studies published between the origin of each database and 15 September 2022. The following databases were searched: AccessMedicine, BIOSIS Citation Index, CINAHL, ClinicalKey, Cochrane Library (via Ovid), EMBASE, Health and Wellness (GALE), PROQUEST Research Library (including ABI/INFORM), Psychiatry Online, PsycINFO, PubMed (including MEDLINE), ScienceDirect, Scopus, and the Web of Science. There were no restrictions on language use. The search syntax, search methodology, and thorough search translations are displayed in Supplemental File S1.
To identify important grey literature [12], two digital preservation services, CLOCKSS and Swiss National Library (Helveticat), and three content aggregator services, Google Scholar, Scilit, and WorldCat (OCLC), were screened for best match hits.
Based on PICO, key terms and PubMed Medical Subjects Headings (MeSH) were used as search terms. The (All Fields) search was constructed using the Boolean logic operators (OR, AND, NOT). The search was conducted using the following keywords: “nomophobia*”, (OR) “no-mo-phobia, (OR “ “no mobile* phobia, (OR) “ “mobile* phobia,” (OR) “mobile* addiction,” (AND) “prevalence”. The reference lists of the identified studies were examined to ensure that all pertinent publications had been covered. The final search results were transformed into a Microsoft Office (Excel spreadsheet 365 *.xlsx) file to filter and remove duplicates. The citations employing (Research Information Systems *.RIS) or integrated files were managed using EndNote 20.4.1.
Inclusion criteria were (1) original English-language papers about nomophobia published prior to 15 September 2022, (2) participants over the age of 12 (3) all participants completed a nomophobia screening test, and (4) participant responses to each test were scored and reported so that the percentages of participants falling above and below predetermined cut-off points could be calculated.
Exclusion criteria were (1) research targeting something other than the prevalence of nomophobia and (2) studies for which, despite contacting the authors, we were unable to obtain the information we needed The PRISMA 2020 study selection flowchart is depicted in Figure 1.

2.1. Screening, Data Extraction, Quality Assessment, and Data Analysis

The studies chosen for the systematic review were screened and coded using ASReview [13], a free online tool that integrates digital technologies (such as natural language processing) with artificial intelligence and machine learning. The accuracy of abstract screening was improved using the semi-automated Abstrackr [14], an abstract screening tool for systematic reviews. Whenever required, data were extracted from plot images using the free and open-source web application WebPlotDigitizer v4.5 [15]. Quality check was maintained by manual cross-checking of the integrity of the data by a member of the extraction team.
To standardize data extraction, three members of the study team (OB, AFA, and AH). independently extracted the following variables in addition to the primary finding of the event rate of nomophobia (by severity). Data extraction explicitly included the following information: author names, publication year, country of data collection, sample size, mean age (years), sex (male: female ratio), and the test used to assess if nomophobia was present or not.
Disagreements as to what should be included/excluded were resolved through consensus among the aforementioned three reviewers. If an agreement could not be reached, a fourth author (ZS or HJ) was brought in to resolve the matter through discussion. If important information was lacking from a publication, the author of the article was contacted.
A pair of authors (any of OB, AFA, or AH), working independently, used the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) to assess the caliber of the included studies [16]. We used the NOS checklist created for cross-sectional investigations [17]. There are several components to it: participant selection, comparability, outcome, and statistics. Each item in the NOS is given one to three-quarters of a star on a rating scale [16]. As a result, cross-sectional studies can only receive a maximum score of nine. A study that receives an eight is considered of good quality with a low risk of bias, a study that receives a five to seven is considered of moderate quality with a low risk of bias, and a study that receives a zero to four is considered of poor quality with a high risk of bias.
Using the random-effects model, a classic frequentist meta-analysis was conducted, assuming that actual effects will differ over time between samples [17]. The DerSimonian–Laird method was used to estimate and adjust for the variance of the effect between studies using the untransformed proportions and the general inverse variance method, with a continuity correction of 0.5 in studies with zero cell frequencies [18]. In random-effects modeling, the assumption is that different sets of studies estimate different, yet conceptually related, effects by using different measures. In each study, the pooled prevalence is reported along with the 95% confidence interval. Meta-analysis data were visualized using a forest plot [19]. Statistical analyses were conducted and presented according to the Meta-analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (MOOSE) protocol [20].
An I2 value between 75–100% indicates a high degree of heterogeneity between studies [21]. A Cochran’s Q statistic [22] was also used to evaluate heterogeneity, as well as tau2 (τ2) and tau (τ) [21]. The Jackson method was used for the confidence interval of tau2 and tau. The H statistic [23] is equal to Cochran’s χ2 heterogeneity statistic divided by the degree of freedom. The initial visual tool used to investigate publication bias was a funnel plot [24]. An inverse Galbraith radial [25] plot was used to visualize heterogeneity by plotting observed effect sizes against their corresponding standard errors (horizontal axis). An arc shows the effect size or outcome on the right-hand side of a full-scale Galbraith plot [26,27]. The Doi plot [28] substitutes a folded normal quantile (Z-score) vs. effect plot for the traditional scatter (the funnel) plot [29] of precision versus effect. Studies make up the limbs of this plot; if there is asymmetry, one or more studies may make up one limb more than the other [30], causing an unequal divergence of both limbs from the midpoint [31]. In the absence of asymmetry, it would be anticipated that the Doi plot would be divided into two zones with comparable areas by a line drawn perpendicular to the X-axis from its tip. The gold standard for detecting publication bias was also used by employing rank correlations by Begg and Mazumdar [32] and Egger’s regression [33].
When outliers are included in meta-analyses, their validity and robustness may be compromised [34]. Studies classified as outliers, when their confidence interval did not match the pooled effects, were addressed by sensitivity analysis [34]. To ensure no inordinate influence was coming from a single study, we used the Jackknife sensitivity analysis. In this analysis, the main meta-analysis is repeated as many times as there are studies included, removing one study at a time [34].
The odds of a research paper being published are affected when its results encounter publication bias. An adjusted point estimate was generated using the trim and fill approach to correct funnel plot asymmetry due to publication bias [35].
For investigating heterogeneous outcomes and answering specific queries regarding distinct populations or study characteristics, subgroup meta-analyses [36] and meta-regression models [37] were used. Categorical variables, including country and culture (Western vs. non-Western), population (general adults vs. university students vs. adolescents), and measures/scales, were used in subgroup analyses. United Nations regional groups of member states were used to categorize Western and non-Western countries [38]. Study subgroups were based on the year of publication (year of data collection) to investigate the effect of time as a confounder. Forest plots were then used to present results for each subgroup meta-analysis.
A meta-regression involves predicting the outcome variable based on one or more explanatory factors [37]. The regression coefficient of a meta-regression will show how the outcome variable changes as the explanatory variable (maybe a moderator or effect modifier or confounding variable) increase by one unit [37]. Meta-regression was performed on univariate analysis using age and sex. A sex-age interaction term was tested. In statistically significant meta-regression models, effect sizes were reported using R2. A small effect size was defined as 1–8%, a medium effect size as 9–24%, and a large effect size was defined as 25% [39].
All data were analyzed using R software version 4.1.3 for statistical computing [40]. A p-value < 0.05 was considered statistically significant. To perform the classical meta-analysis, the packages ‘meta’ [41] and ‘metafor’ [42] were used. Using the package ‘forester’, a summary forest plot was generated based on the combined effect sizes of multiple forest plots, omitting the results of individual studies. For example, the combined results (omitting results from individual studies) from mild, moderate, and severe nomophobia were all presented in one plot instead of three separate forest plots.
For quality assessment, risk-of-bias plots were generated using the package ‘robvis’ [43]. A summary plot (weighted) shows the proportion of information inside each judgment. A detailed risk of bias assessment of all studies, displayed using a traffic light plot, depicts the bias risk in each domain and the overall risk.

2.2. Role of the Funding Source

This systematic review and meta-analysis have received no funding from the government, private sector, or non-profit sector.

3. Results

3.1. The Characteristics of the Included Studies

The search included the time frame from the inception of the databases until 15 September 2022. A total of 791 records were located using various sources, including electronic database searches. After duplicate records were eliminated, 459 records remained. The title, abstract and full content of all potential articles were examined. A PRISMA2020 flowchart is used to represent the search process in Figure 1.
A total of 52 studies [7,8,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66,67,68,69,70,71,72,73,74,75,76,77,78,79,80,81,82,83,84,85,86,87,88,89,90,91,92,93] (53 data points) involving 47,399 participants from 20 countries were involved in this meta-analysis. The number of studies (Ks) and corresponding overall sample size per country (Ns) are as follows in alphabetical order: Australia (K = 3, N = 6601), Bahrain (K = 3, N = 1752), Bosnia and Herzegovina (K = 1, N = 1083), Canada (K = 3, N = 2481), China (K = 1, N = 473), Croatia (K = 1, N = 257), Ghana (K = 1, N = 345), India (K = 5, N = 2262), Iran (K = 1, N = 320), Italy (K = 4, N = 5719), Kuwait (K = 1, N = 512), Lebanon (K = 1, N = 2260), Oman (K = 1, N = 740), Pakistan (K = 4, N = 940), Peru (K = 1, N = 3139), the Philippines (K = 1, N = 3374), Saudi Arabia (K = 3, N = 6314), Spain (K = 1, N = 850), Thailand (K = 1, N = 638), and Turkey (K = 16, N = 7339). A summary of the studies included in this systematic review and meta-analysis is in Table 1.
It was interesting to observe that although the term nomophobia was first described in 2008, the first measurement tool (i.e., NMP-Q) was published in 2015–2016, and all prevalence studies were published in the year 2018 or afterward.
The mean prevalence sample size was 895 (95%CI 613; 1175) participants. Males accounted for 37% (95%CI 33; 42%), and the mean age was 22 (95%CI 21; 25) years. A total of 48 (91%) of the studies used the NMP-Q and mostly adopted a cross-sectional approach for data collection. Online surveying was used in 50 (95%) of the studies. The studies were generally robust with a mean quality score of 7.3 (95% 7.0; 7.6). A detailed quality assessment of each of the included studies is available in a traffic light plot format in Supplemental File S1. The risk of bias was moderate in 16 (30%) of the studies and the remaining 37 (70%) were of a low risk of bias. Most of the risk of bias was in the sample selection. Detailed results are in a summary plot format in Figure 2.
Out of the 53 data points, 53 (100%) provided a global estimate of all nomophobia symptoms and 42 (80%) provided an estimate of nomophobia by severity (mild, moderate, and severe forms of the condition).

3.2. Prevalence of Nomophobia by Severity

Figure 3 provides a summary of the entire results of this meta-analysis on the prevalence of nomophobia by severity, country, culture, population, measurement tool, and year of data. The following section provides a detailed examination of each element at a micro level.

3.2.1. All Symptoms (Cumulative or All Severities)

A random-effects meta-analysis evaluated the prevalence of nomophobia in all populations (K = 53, N = 47,399) and generated a pooled prevalence rate of 93.92% (93.19; 94.66%), 95%PI (88.56; 99.29%), τ2 = 0.007 (0.0038; 0.0101); τ = 0.0265 (0.0614; 0.1006), I2 = 99.6% (99.5%; 99.6%); H = 15.38 (14.82; 15.95), Q = 12,293.72 (df = 52) p < 0.001. Detailed results are presented in Table 2. The Forest plot of all nomophobia symptoms is in Supplemental File S2.
A (leave-one-out) sensitivity analysis found that no study had a greater than 2% impact on the global prevalence estimate. Some outliers were detected but deleting them did not result in a major change in the estimates (within 3%) of all nomophobia symptoms.
Visual inspection of the funnel plot (Supplemental File S3), Galbraith plot (Supplemental File S4), and DOI plot (Supplemental File S5) indicated a publication bias. A linear regression test of funnel plot asymmetry showed a test result: t = −4.55, df = 51, p-value < 0.001, suggesting publication bias. Similarly, the rank correlation test of funnel plot asymmetry showed a test result of z = −7.00, p-value < 0.001, suggesting the presence of publication bias. The adjusted meta-analysis for all nomophobia symptoms (using the trim and fill approach) yielded an estimate of 99.78% (98.86%; 100.00%). Detailed results are presented in Table 3, Part 1.
For all nomophobia symptoms, a statistically significant difference was observed based on country, culture, population, measurement tool, and year of data collection, all p < 0.05. Detailed results are presented in Table 3, Part 1.
Bahrain and Canada had the highest rates of all nomophobia symptoms with prevalence rates of 100.00% (99.86%; 100.00%) and 100.00% (99.91%; 100.00%), respectively. India and Saudi Arabia had the lowest rates of 85.74% (80.36%; 91.11%) and 83.49% (64.23%; 100.00%), respectively.
Western cultures had a higher prevalence rate of all nomophobia symptoms with a rate of 95.30% (94.04%; 96.56%) vs. Non-Western cultures of 93.38% (92.44%; 94.32%), p = 0.02. Detailed results are presented in Table 3, Part 1.
University students appeared to have the highest prevalence of all nomophobia symptoms with a rate of 97.38% (96.72%; 98.04%), followed by the general adult population at 95.15% (93.06%; 97.25%), and high school students and community adolescents 84.17% (82.11%; 86.22%). The difference between the three population groups was statistically significant, p < 0.001. Detailed results are presented in Table 3, Part 1.
The NMP-Q captured a larger prevalence rate compared to other tools, with estimates of 97.59% (97.13%; 98.05%) vs. 66.52% (53.66%; 79.37%), respectively. The difference between the two groups was statistically significant, p < 0.001. Detailed results are presented in Table 3, Part 1.
Analysis of all nomophobia symptoms by year showed an uptrend between 2018–2021 (peak 2021), followed by a slight downtrend trend to date. Prevalence rates for 2018, 2019, 2020, 2021, and 2022 were: 82.30% (76.20%; 88.40%), 85.35% (81.71%; 89.00%), 94.77% (92.13%; 97.42%), 99.46% (99.12%; 99.79%), and 93.01% (91.55%; 94.47%). The difference in all nomophobia symptoms between the years was statistically significant, p < 0.001. Detailed results are presented in Table 3, Part 1.
Subgroup meta-analyses for all nomophobia symptoms by country, culture, population measurement tool, and year of data collection are shown in Supplemental Files S6–S10.
Meta-regression models showed that both age (in years) and sex (proportion of male participants) are statistically significant predictors for all nomophobia symptoms. Age showed: p < 0.001; τ2 (estimated amount of residual heterogeneity): 0.007 (SE = 0.003); τ (square root of estimated τ2 value): 0.0270; I2 (residual heterogeneity/unaccounted variability): 99.59%; H2 (unaccounted variability/sampling variability): 240.99, and R2 (amount of heterogeneity accounted for): 0.00%. Sex showed: p < 0.001; τ2 (estimated amount of residual heterogeneity): 0.007 (SE = 0.003); τ (square root of estimated τ2 value): 0.0272; I2 (residual heterogeneity/unaccounted variability): 99.58%; H2 (unaccounted variability/sampling variability): 240.74; and R2 (amount of heterogeneity accounted for): 0.00%. The age-sex interaction term was not a significant predictor p = 0.6095. Detailed results are presented in Table 3, Part 1.

3.2.2. Mild Symptoms

Mild nomophobia was prevalent in 25.80% (19.83; 31.78%), 95%PI (00.00; 65.99%), τ2 = 0.0386 (0.0182; 0.0776); τ = 0.1965 (0.1348; 0.2785), I2 = 99.8%; H = 20.81 (20.13; 21.51), Q = 17,747.07 (df 41) p < 0.001. Detailed results are presented in Table 2. A Forest plot of mild nomophobia symptoms is in Supplemental File S11.
A (leave-one-out) sensitivity analysis found that no study had a greater than 2% impact on the global prevalence estimate. Some outliers were detected but deleting them did not result in a major change in the estimates (within 3%) of mild nomophobia symptoms.
Visual inspection of the funnel plot, Galbraith plot, and DOI plot indicated a publication bias. A linear regression test of funnel plot asymmetry showed a test result: t = 7.05, df = 40, p-value < 0.001, suggesting publication bias. However, the rank correlation test of funnel plot asymmetry showed a test result of z = −1.22, p-value = 0.2206 showing the absence of publication bias. The adjusted meta-analysis for mild nomophobia symptoms (using a trim and fill approach) yielded an estimate of 4.17% (1.00%; 9.97%). Detailed results are presented in Table 3, Part 2.
For mild nomophobia symptoms, a statistically significant difference was observed based on the country only, p < 0.001. The difference in mild nomophobia symptoms based on culture, population, measurement tool, and year of data collection did not reach a statistical significance of p > 0.05. Detailed results are presented in Table 3, Part 2. Subgroup meta-analyses for mild nomophobia symptoms by country, culture, population measurement tool, and year of data collection are shown in Supplemental Files S12–S16.
Meta-regression models showed that both age (in years) and sex (proportion of male participants) were not significant predictors for mild nomophobia symptoms. Age showed: p = 0.4722; τ2 (estimated amount of residual heterogeneity): 0.0396 (SE = 0.0297); τ (square root of estimated τ2 value): 0.1989; I2 (residual heterogeneity/unaccounted variability): 99.77%; H2 (unaccounted variability/sampling variability): 443.55; R2 (amount of heterogeneity accounted for): 0.00%. Sex showed: p = 0.8009; τ2 (estimated amount of residual heterogeneity): 0.0342 (SE = 0.0197); τ (square root of estimated τ2 value): 0.1850; I2 (residual heterogeneity/unaccounted variability): 99.65%; H2 (unaccounted variability/sampling variability): 287.95; R2 (amount of heterogeneity accounted for): 11.34%. The age-sex interaction term was not a significant predictor, p = 0.9404. Detailed results are presented in Table 3, Part 2.

3.2.3. Moderate Symptoms

Moderate nomophobia was prevalent in 52.40% (44.21; 60.60%), 95%PI (00.00; 100.00%), τ2 = 0.0728 (0.0288; 0.0787); τ = 0.2698 (0.1697; 0.2806), I2 = 99.7%; H = 17.86 (17.21; 18.53), Q = 13,080.70 (df = 41) p < 0.001. Detailed results are presented in Table 2. The Forest plot of all nomophobia symptoms is presented in Supplemental File S17.
A (leave-one-out) sensitivity analysis found that no study had a greater than 2% impact on the global prevalence estimate. Some outliers were detected but deleting them did not result in a major change in the estimates (within 4%) of moderate nomophobia symptoms.
Visual inspection of the funnel plot, Galbraith plot, and DOI plot indicated a publication bias. A linear regression test of funnel plot asymmetry showed a test result of t = −2.74, df = 40, and p-value = 0.0092, suggesting publication bias. However, the rank correlation test of funnel plot asymmetry showed a test result of z = 1.54, p-value = 0.1238, showing the absence of publication bias. The adjusted meta-analysis for moderate nomophobia symptoms (using a trim and fill approach) yielded an estimate of 74.66 % (65.59%; 83.73%).
For moderate nomophobia symptoms, a statistically significant difference was observed based on the country and the measurement tool used, p < 0.001. The difference in moderate nomophobia symptoms based on culture, population, and year of data collection did not reach a statistical significance of p > 0.05. Detailed results are presented in Table 3, Part 3. Subgroup meta-analyses for all nomophobia symptoms by country, culture, population measurement tool, and year of data collection are shown in Supplemental Files S18–S22.
Meta-regression models showed that both age (in years) and sex (proportion of male participants) were not significant predictors for mild nomophobia symptoms. Age showed: p = 0.8940; τ2 (estimated amount of residual heterogeneity): 0.0745 (SE = 0.0294); τ (square root of estimated τ2 value): 0.2730; I2 (residual heterogeneity/unaccounted variability): 99.69%; H2 (unaccounted variability/sampling variability): 323.37; R2 (amount of heterogeneity accounted for): 0.00%. Sex showed: p = 0.8198; τ2 (estimated amount of residual heterogeneity): 0.0700 (SE = 0.0261); τ (square root of estimated τ2 value): 0.2646; I2 (residual heterogeneity/unaccounted variability): 99.66%; H2 (unaccounted variability/sampling variability): 292.81; R2 (amount of heterogeneity accounted for): 3.86%. The age-sex interaction term was not a significant predictor, p = 0.7915. Detailed results are presented in Table 3, Part 3.

3.2.4. Severe Symptoms

Severe nomophobia was prevalent in 20.35% (16.51; 24.20%), 95%PI (00.00; 46.07%), τ2 = 0158 (0.0124; 0.0468); τ = 0.1257 (0.1116; 0.2163), I2 = 99.6%; H = 15.75 (15.13; 16.40), Q = 10,176.69 (df = 41) p < 0.001. Detailed results are presented in Table 2. The Forest plot of all nomophobia symptoms is presented in Supplemental File S23.
A (leave-one-out) sensitivity analysis found that no study had a greater than 1% impact on the global prevalence estimate. Some outliers were detected but deleting them did not result in a major change in the estimates (within 2%) of severe nomophobia symptoms.
Visual inspection of the funnel plot, Galbraith plot, and DOI plot indicated a publication bias. Linear regression test of funnel plot asymmetry showed a test result of t = 7.35, df = 40, and p-value < 0.001, suggesting publication bias. However, the rank correlation test of funnel plot asymmetry showed a test result of z = −0.98, p-value = 0.3293, showing the absence of publication bias. The adjusted meta-analysis for severe nomophobia symptoms (using a trim and fill approach) yielded an estimate of 4.62 % (0.77%; 8.47%). Detailed results are presented in Table 3, Part 4.
For severe nomophobia symptoms, a statistically significant difference was observed based on country, culture, measurement tool, and year of data collection with p < 0.001. The difference in moderate nomophobia symptoms based on population did not reach a statistical significance of p > 0.05. Detailed results are presented in Table 3, Part 4. Subgroup meta-analyses for all nomophobia symptoms by country, culture, population measurement tool, and year of data collection are shown in Supplemental Files S24–S28.
Meta-regression models showed that both age (in years) and sex (proportion of male participants) were not significant predictors for mild nomophobia symptoms. Age showed: p = 0.3732; τ2 (estimated amount of residual heterogeneity): 0.0187 (SE = 0.0124); τ (square root of estimated τ2 value): 0.1366; I2 (residual heterogeneity/unaccounted variability): 99.61%; H2 (unaccounted variability/sampling variability): 253.91; R2 (amount of heterogeneity accounted for): 0.00%.
Sex showed: p = 0.9139; τ2 (estimated amount of residual heterogeneity): 0.0203 (SE = 0.0104); τ (square root of estimated τ2 value): 0.1426; I2 (residual heterogeneity/unaccounted variability): 99.56%; H2 (unaccounted variability/sampling variability): 227.25; R2 (amount of heterogeneity accounted for): 0.001%. The age-sex interaction term was not a significant predictor, p = 0.6594. Detailed results are presented in Table 3, Part 4.

4. Discussion

Prevalence rates are core indicators of the healthcare needs of a population. These rates are also essential inputs for the burden of disease studies and simulation models that make projections about population health in the future. It is for this reason that population nomophobia rates are needed. There may be a question as to why severity rates are required in clinical settings. It is important to know the proportion of severe nomophobia because, in all probability, only severe cases will need clinical intervention.
The main findings of this systematic review and meta-analysis show that nomophobia symptoms are very common in all studied populations. Most individuals experience symptoms that are mild or moderate; mild, moderate, and severe symptoms are present in about 25%, 50%, and 20% of the individuals. Our results suggest that university students, from non-Western countries and cultures, are the ones most affected by severe symptoms. A wave of increased prevalence of nomophobia was observed in the year 2021, potentially related to COVID-19. The NMP-Q appeared to be the most sensitive tool for the detection of moderate and severe nomophobia.
The findings of the present systematic review and meta-analysis on the prevalence rate of nomophobia are consistent with the results of two previous meta-analyses [2,94]. Thus, the results of mild, moderate, and severe symptoms of about 25%, 50%, and 20%, respectively, are very robust.
Due to its serious impact on health, severe nomophobia needs to be the focus of the discussion, since it can lead to mental disorders [50], with symptoms of depression [95], anger [96], nervousness, anxiety, and stress [72,97,98], aggression [83], and insomnia and other sleep issues [70]. Recent studies also showed that excessive and intensive use of smartphones can lead to musculoskeletal problems [99,100]. The main problems identified have been neck [99] and thumb [100] injuries. Severe nomophobia is associated with personal safety issues, including an increased risk of road traffic accidents [74,101].
Special situations may trigger increased nomophobia symptoms. For example, a recent study looked at how a social media outage affects nomophobia [102]. Results revealed that symptoms of nomophobia, notable anxieties about access and connectivity, increased dramatically during outages [102]. Resultant insomnia also increases the severity of nomophobia [102].
Our results suggest that neither age nor sex is a significant predictor of nomophobia. According to a systematic review published in 2021, however, there has been a recent significant increase in nomophobia among women and those aged under 35 [94]. However, it is difficult to be certain because of methodological differences among studies [94]. The proportion of “at-risk” individuals and those suffering from nomophobia varies greatly from study to study, ranging from 13 to 79% and 6 to 73%, respectively [94]. Similar to our findings, the review previously mentioned supports the greater frequency of moderate cases relative to severe cases [94].
It has been reported that adolescents who experience sleep loss and ongoing sleep deprivation due to nomophobia may experience behavioral and academic issues (e.g., possible aggression, failing grades, and absenteeism) [70]. A greater likelihood of accidents has also been predicted. Concern has been expressed that the lifestyle and quality of life of teenagers will be severely compromised if nomophobia is left unattended by physicians and psychologists [2].

Limitations and Directions for Future Research

This systematic review and meta-analysis has a limitation inherited from the original studies included in the analysis and related to the fact that most of the participants were limited to a narrow age range. To better estimate nomophobia’s distribution among different age groups, future studies should include older adults and the elderly. Nevertheless, as the world moves toward a more digital lifestyle, this systematic review provides an understanding of the global prevalence of this newly emerging condition, which may well increase with time. For this reason, efforts at prevention and timely intervention are in order.
There are a few other issues with this meta-analysis. First, heterogeneity was high. In a large epidemiological meta-analysis, this is to be expected. The use of random-effects modeling was expected to address concerns linked to the consequences of reviewing a large number of studies that do not all follow the same pattern, but follow a distribution. To mitigate this, we used 95% prediction intervals. Individual patient data (IPD) meta-analyses are useful and should be promoted in future studies to work out, assess, and discuss different elements of nomophobia. Second, we only found a few moderators. Future evaluations should broaden this investigation to include additional lifestyle variables such as physical activity, smoking, and substance use, with a focus on controlling for pre-existing stress-related illnesses such as posttraumatic stress disorder, adjustment disorders, anxiety, and depression. Risk factors would be important to determine.
Human beings are increasingly living a socially isolated digital lifestyle, which impacts behavior and undermines coping mechanisms. Preventive measures for this increasingly global issue are needed. Due to its rising prevalence, it is suggested that this disorder be included in forthcoming editions of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM), the International Classification of Diseases (ICD), and the International Classification of Sleep Disorders (ICSD).
Future studies are encouraged to adopt a two-phase design for improved prevalence estimation. This would mean screening a population sample, then interviewing varying proportions of screened individuals that have been stratified according to their probability of fulfilling the criteria for nomophobia. Prevalence rates can then be calculated by weighting back to the original population.

5. Conclusions

The prevalence rates of mild, moderate, and severe nomophobia, as here reviewed, are approximately 25%, 50%, and 20%, respectively, across the world regions represented in the available studies. University students appear to be the most impacted by the disorder. For any medical condition, and this includes nomophobia, knowing prevalence rates helps in the determination of risk factors, which then permits the development of a program of prevention, early intervention, and effective therapeutic management.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/bs13010035/s1, Supplemental S1: Traffic light plot of the risk of bias assessment; Supplemental S2: Forest plot of the prevalence of all nomophobia symptoms; Supplemental S3: Funnel plot of the prevalence of all nomophobia symptoms; Supplemental S4: Galbraith plot of the prevalence of all nomophobia symptoms; Supplemental S5: DOI plot of the prevalence of all nomophobia symptoms; Supplemental S6: Subgroup forest plot of the prevalence of all nomophobia symptoms by country; Supplemental S7: Subgroup forest plot of the prevalence of all nomophobia symptoms by culture; Supplemental S8: Subgroup forest plot of the prevalence of all nomophobia symptoms by population; Supplemental S9: Subgroup forest plot of the prevalence of all nomophobia symptoms by tool; Supplemental S10: Subgroup forest plot of the prevalence of all nomophobia symptoms by year of data collection; Supplemental S11: Forest plot of the prevalence of mild nomophobia symptoms; Supplemental S12: Subgroup forest plot of the prevalence of mild nomophobia symptoms by country; Supplemental S13: Subgroup forest plot of the prevalence of mild nomophobia symptoms by culture; Supplemental S14: Subgroup forest plot of the prevalence of mild nomophobia symptoms by population; Supplemental S15: Subgroup forest plot of the prevalence of mild nomophobia symptoms by tool; Supplemental S16: Subgroup forest plot of the prevalence of mild nomophobia symptoms by year of data collection; Supplemental S17: Forest plot of the prevalence of moderate nomophobia symptoms; Supplemental S18: Subgroup forest plot of the prevalence of moderate nomophobia symptoms by country; Supplemental S19: Subgroup forest plot of the prevalence of moderate nomophobia symptoms by culture; Supplemental S20: Subgroup forest plot of the prevalence of moderate nomophobia symptoms by population; Supplemental S21: Subgroup forest plot of the prevalence of moderate nomophobia symptoms by tool; Supplemental S22: Subgroup forest plot of the prevalence of moderate nomophobia symptoms by year of data collection; Supplemental S23: Forest plot of the prevalence of severe nomophobia symptoms; Supplemental S24: Subgroup forest plot of the prevalence of severe nomophobia symptoms by country; Supplemental S25: Subgroup forest plot of the prevalence of severe nomophobia symptoms by culture; Supplemental S26: Subgroup forest plot of the prevalence of severe nomophobia symptoms by population; Supplemental S27: Subgroup forest plot of the prevalence of severe nomophobia symptoms by tool; Supplemental S28: Subgroup forest plot of the prevalence of severe nomophobia symptoms by year of data collection.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, H.J.; methodology, H.J.; software, H.J.; validation, H.J.; formal analysis, H.J.; investigation, H.J.; resources, H.J.; data curation, H.J., K.T., O.B., J.H.H., A.F.A., A.H., Z.S. and S.R.P.-P.; writing—original draft, H.J., K.T., O.B., J.H.H., A.F.A., A.H., Z.S., S.R.P.-P. and M.V.S.; writing—review and editing, H.J., K.T. and M.V.S.; visualization, H.J.; supervision, H.J.; project administration, H.J.; funding acquisition, H.J. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This systematic review and meta-analysis have received no funding from the government, private sector, or non-profit sector.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Ethical approval/Institutional Review Board was not required because this study retrieved and synthesized data from already published studies.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Derived data (and analysis codes) supporting the findings of this review are available from the corresponding author based on request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. King, A.L.; Valença, A.M.; Nardi, A.E. Nomophobia: The mobile phone in panic disorder with agoraphobia: Reducing phobias or worsening of dependence? Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 2010, 23, 52–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Humood, A.; Altooq, N.; Altamimi, A.; Almoosawi, H.; Alzafiri, M.; Bragazzi, N.L.; Husni, M.; Jahrami, H. The Prevalence of Nomophobia by Population and by Research Tool: A Systematic Review, Meta-Analysis, and Meta-Regression. Psych 2021, 3, 249–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Ratan, Z.A.; Parrish, A.-M.; Alotaibi, M.S.; Hosseinzadeh, H. Prevalence of Smartphone Addiction and Its Association with Sociodemographic, Physical and Mental Well-Being: A Cross-Sectional Study among the Young Adults of Bangladesh. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 16583. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Yildirim, C. Exploring the Dimensions of Nomophobia: Developing and Validating a Questionnaire Using Mixed Methods Research. Ph.D. Thesis, Iowa State University, Ames, IA, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  5. Yildirim, C.; Correia, A.-P. Exploring the dimensions of nomophobia: Development and validation of a self-reported questionnaire. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2015, 49, 130–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Cochrane. Why Perform a Meta-Analysis in a Review? Available online: https://handbook-5-1.cochrane.org/chapter_9/9_1_3_why_perform_a_meta_analysis_in_a_review.htm (accessed on 12 August 2022).
  7. Prasad, M.; Patthi, B.; Singla, A.; Gupta, R.; Saha, S.; Kumar, J.K.; Malhi, R.; Pandita, V. Nomophobia: A cross-sectional study to assess mobile phone usage among dental students. J. Clin. Diagn. Res. JCDR 2017, 11, ZC34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Sui, A.; Sui, W.; Irwin, J. Exploring the prevalence of nomophobia in a Canadian university: An environmental scan. J. Am. Coll. Health 2022, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Budd, J.; Miller, B.S.; Manning, E.M.; Lampos, V.; Zhuang, M.; Edelstein, M.; Rees, G.; Emery, V.C.; Stevens, M.M.; Keegan, N.; et al. Digital technologies in the public-health response to COVID-19. Nat. Med. 2020, 26, 1183–1192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Nishikawa-Pacher, A. Research Questions with PICO: A Universal Mnemonic. Publications 2022, 10, 21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Page, M.J.; McKenzie, J.E.; Bossuyt, P.M.; Boutron, I.; Hoffmann, T.C.; Mulrow, C.D.; Shamseer, L.; Tetzlaff, J.M.; Akl, E.A.; Brennan, S.E.; et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 2021, 372, n71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Conn, V.S.; Valentine, J.C.; Cooper, H.M.; Rantz, M.J. Grey literature in meta-analyses. Nurs. Res. 2003, 52, 256–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. van de Schoot, R.; de Bruin, J.; Schram, R.; Zahedi, P.; de Boer, J.; Weijdema, F.; Kramer, B.; Huijts, M.; Hoogerwerf, M.; Ferdinands, G.; et al. An open source machine learning framework for efficient and transparent systematic reviews. Nat. Mach. Intell. 2021, 3, 125–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Rathbone, J.; Hoffmann, T.; Glasziou, P. Faster title and abstract screening? Evaluating Abstrackr, a semi-automated online screening program for systematic reviewers. Syst. Rev. 2015, 4, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. PLOTCON. WebPlotDigitizer. Available online: https://automeris.io/WebPlotDigitizer/ (accessed on 12 August 2022).
  16. Luchini, C.; Stubbs, B.; Solmi, M.; Veronese, N. Assessing the quality of studies in meta-analyses: Advantages and limitations of the Newcastle Ottawa Scale. World J. Meta-Anal. 2017, 5, 80–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Shuster, J.J. Empirical vs. natural weighting in random effects meta-analysis. Stat. Med. 2010, 29, 1259–1265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Borenstein, M.; Hedges, L.V.; Higgins, J.P.; Rothstein, H.R. A basic introduction to fixed-effect and random-effects models for meta-analysis. Res. Synth. Methods 2010, 1, 97–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Lewis, S.; Clarke, M. Forest plots: Trying to see the wood and the trees. BMJ 2001, 322, 1479–1480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Stroup, D.F.; Berlin, J.A.; Morton, S.C.; Olkin, I.; Williamson, G.D.; Rennie, D.; Moher, D.; Becker, B.J.; Sipe, T.A.; Thacker, S.B. Meta-analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology: A Proposal for Reporting. JAMA 2000, 283, 2008–2012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Higgins, J.P.; Thompson, S.G. Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Stat. Med. 2002, 21, 1539–1558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Huedo-Medina, T.B.; Sánchez-Meca, J.; Marin-Martinez, F.; Botella, J. Assessing heterogeneity in meta-analysis: Q statistic or I2 index? Psychol. Methods 2006, 11, 193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  23. Lin, L.; Chu, H.; Hodges, J.S. Alternative measures of between-study heterogeneity in meta-analysis: Reducing the impact of outlying studies. Biometrics 2017, 73, 156–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Mathur, M.B.; VanderWeele, T.J. Sensitivity analysis for publication bias in meta-analyses. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. C (Appl. Stat.) 2020, 69, 1091–1119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Galbraith, R.F. Some applications of radial plots. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 1994, 89, 1232–1242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Galbraith, R. Graphical display of estimates having differing standard errors. Technometrics 1988, 30, 271–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Galbraith, R. A note on graphical presentation of estimated odds ratios from several clinical trials. Stat. Med. 1988, 7, 889–894. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Doi, S.A. Rendering the Doi plot properly in meta-analysis. JBI Evid. Implement. 2018, 16, 242–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Egger, M.; Smith, G.D.; Schneider, M.; Minder, C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ 1997, 315, 629–634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  30. Patsopoulos, N.A.; Evangelou, E.; Ioannidis, J.P. Sensitivity of between-study heterogeneity in meta-analysis: Proposed metrics and empirical evaluation. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2008, 37, 1148–1157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Viechtbauer, W.; Cheung, M.W. Outlier and influence diagnostics for meta-analysis. Res. Synth. Methods 2010, 1, 112–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Gjerdevik, M.; Heuch, I. Improving the error rates of the Begg and Mazumdar test for publication bias in fixed effects meta-analysis. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 2014, 14, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Sterne, J.A.; Egger, M. Regression methods to detect publication and other bias in meta-analysis. Publ. Bias Meta-Anal. Prev. Assess. Adjust. 2005, 1, 99–110. [Google Scholar]
  34. Copas, J.; Shi, J.Q. Meta-analysis, funnel plots and sensitivity analysis. Biostatistics 2000, 1, 247–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Duval, S.; Tweedie, R. A nonparametric “trim and fill” method of accounting for publication bias in meta-analysis. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 2000, 95, 89–98. [Google Scholar]
  36. Sedgwick, P. Meta-analyses: Heterogeneity and subgroup analysis. BMJ 2013, 346, F4040. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Thompson, S.G.; Higgins, J.P. How should meta-regression analyses be undertaken and interpreted? Stat. Med. 2002, 21, 1559–1573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. UN. Available online: https://www.un.org/dgacm/en/content/regional-groups (accessed on 12 August 2022).
  39. OSC. Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science 2015, 349, aac4716. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. R. 4.1.1. Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 5 May 2020).
  41. Schwarzer, G.; Schwarzer, M.G. Package ‘meta’. R Found. Stat. Comput. 2012, 9, 1–94. [Google Scholar]
  42. Viechtbauer, W. Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J. Stat. Softw. 2010, 36, 1–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. McGuinness, L.A.; Higgins, J.P. Risk-of-bias VISualization (robvis): An R package and Shiny web app for visualizing risk-of-bias assessments. Res. Synth. Methods 2021, 12, 55–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Al-Balhan, E.M.; Khabbache, H.; Watfa, A.; Re, T.S.; Zerbetto, R.; Bragazzi, N.L. Psychometric evaluation of the Arabic version of the nomophobia questionnaire: Confirmatory and exploratory factor analysis—Implications from a pilot study in Kuwait among university students. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manag. 2018, 11, 471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. AlMarzooqi, M.A.; Alhaj, O.A.; Alrasheed, M.M.; Helmy, M.; Trabelsi, K.; Ebrahim, A.; Hattab, S.; Jahrami, H.A.; Ben Saad, H. Symptoms of Nomophobia, Psychological Aspects, Insomnia and Physical Activity: A Cross-Sectional Study of ESports Players in Saudi Arabia. Healthcare 2022, 10, 257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Alwafi, H.; Naser, A.Y.; Aldhahir, A.M.; Fatani, A.I.; Alharbi, R.A.; Alharbi, K.G.; Almutwakkil, B.A.; Salawati, E.; Ekram, R.; Samannodi, M. Prevalence and predictors of nomophobia among the general population in two middle eastern countries. BMC Psychiatry 2022, 22, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Ayar, D.; Gerçeker, G.Ö.; Özdemir, E.Z.; Bektas, M. The effect of problematic internet use, social appearance anxiety, and social media use on nursing students’ nomophobia levels. CIN Comput. Inform. Nurs. 2018, 36, 589–595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Bano, N.; Khan, M.A.; Asif, U.; de Beer, J.; Rawass, H. Effects of nomophobia on anxiety, stress and depression among Saudi medical students in Jeddah, Saudi Arabia. J. Pak. Med. Assoc. 2020, 11, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Bartwal, J.; Nath, B. Evaluation of nomophobia among medical students using smartphone in north India. Med. J. Armed Forces India 2020, 76, 451–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Bragazzi, N.L.; Re, T.S.; Zerbetto, R. The relationship between nomophobia and maladaptive coping styles in a sample of Italian young adults: Insights and implications from a cross-sectional study. JMIR Ment. Health 2019, 6, e13154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Buctot, D.B.; Kim, N.; Kim, S.-H. Personal profiles, family environment, patterns of smartphone use, nomophobia, and smartphone addiction across low, average, and high perceived academic performance levels among high school students in The Philippines. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 5219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Catone, G.; Senese, V.P.; Pisano, S.; Siciliano, M.; Russo, K.; Muratori, P.; Marotta, R.; Pascotto, A.; Broome, M.R. The drawbacks of Information and Communication Technologies: Interplay and psychopathological risk of nomophobia and cyber-bullying, results from the bullying and youth mental health Naples study (BYMHNS). Comput. Hum. Behav. 2020, 113, 106496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Çelik İnce, S. Relationship between nomophobia of nursing students and their obesity and self-esteem. Perspect. Psychiatr. Care 2021, 57, 753–760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Çevik-Durmaz, Y.; Yalçinkaya-Önder, E.; Timur, S. Preservice teachers’ nomophobia levels, sense of loneliness and adjustment to college life. Perspect. Psychiatr. Care 2021, 57, 1052–1072. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. ÇIRAK, M.; Dost, M.T. Nomophobia in University Students: The Roles of Digital Addiction, Social Connectedness, and Life Satisfaction. Turk. Psychol. Couns. Guid. J. 2022, 12, 35–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Copaja-Corzo, C.; Aragón-Ayala, C.J.; Taype-Rondan, A.; Nomotest-Group. Nomophobia and Its Associated Factors in Peruvian Medical Students. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 5006. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Karademir Coskun, T.; Kaya, O. The Distribution of Variables That Affect Nomophobia in Adults’ Profiles. Int. J. Res. Educ. Sci. 2020, 6, 534–550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Daei, A.; Ashrafi-Rizi, H.; Soleymani, M.R. Nomophobia and health hazards: Smartphone use and addiction among university students. Int. J. Prev. Med. 2019, 10, 202. [Google Scholar]
  59. Denprechavong, V.; Ngamchaliew, P.; Buathong, N. Prevalence of Nomophobia and Relationship with Anxiety and Depression among University Students in Southern Thailand. J. Med. Assoc. Thail. 2022, 105, 359–367. [Google Scholar]
  60. Essel, H.B.; Vlachopoulos, D.; Tachie-Menson, A.; Nunoo, F.K.N.; Johnson, E.E. Nomophobia among Preservice Teachers: A descriptive correlational study at Ghanaian Colleges of Education. Educ. Inf. Technol. 2022, 27, 9541–9561. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Farchakh, Y.; Hallit, R.; Akel, M.; Chalhoub, C.; Hachem, M.; Hallit, S.; Obeid, S. Nomophobia in Lebanon: Scale validation and association with psychological aspects. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0249890. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Farooq, M.; Rizvi, M.A.; Wajid, W.A.; Ashraf, M.; Farooq, M.; Javed, H.; Sadiq, M.A.; Jafar, H.M.; Hameed, F.; Rizvi, M.A. Prevalence of Nomophobia and an Analysis of Its Contributing Factors in the Undergraduate Students of Pakistan. Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw. 2022, 25, 147–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Farooqui, I.A.; Pore, P.; Gothankar, J. Nomophobia: An emerging issue in medical institutions? J. Ment. Health 2018, 27, 438–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Fidancı, İ.; Aksoy, H.; Ayhan Başer, D.; Yengil Taci, D.; Cankurtaran, M. Evaluation of Nomophobia and Smartphone Addiction Levels among University Students in terms of Alcohol and Drug Abuse Risk. Anatol. J. Fam. Med. 2021, 4, 159–164. [Google Scholar]
  65. Gurbuz, I.B.; Ozkan, G. What is your level of nomophobia? An investigation of prevalence and level of nomophobia among young people in Turkey. Community Ment. Health J. 2020, 56, 814–822. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Hoşgör, H.; Coşkun, F.; Çalişkan, F. Relationship between nomophobia, fear of missing out, and perceived work overload in nurses in Turkey. Perspect. Psychiatr. Care 2020, 57, 1026–1033. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Işcan, G.; Yildirim Baş, F.; Özcan, Y.; Özdoğanci, C. Relationship between “nomophobia” and material addiction “cigarette” and factors affecting them. Int. J. Clin. Pract. 2021, 75, e13816. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Jahrami, H.; Rashed, M.; AlRasheed, M.M.; Bragazzi, N.L.; Saif, Z.; Alhaj, O.; BaHammam, A.S.; Vitiello, M.V. Nomophobia is associated with insomnia but not with age, sex, bmi, or mobile phone screen size in young adults. Nat. Sci. Sleep 2021, 13, 1931. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Jahrami, H.; Abdelaziz, A.; Binsanad, L.; Alhaj, O.A.; Buheji, M.; Bragazzi, N.L.; Saif, Z.; BaHammam, A.S.; Vitiello, M.V. The association between symptoms of nomophobia, insomnia and food addiction among young adults: Findings of an exploratory cross-sectional survey. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 711. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Jahrami, H.A.; Fekih-Romdhane, F.; Saif, Z.Q.; Alhaj, O.A.; AlRasheed, M.M.; Pandi-Perumal, S.R.; BaHammam, A.S.; Vitiello, M.V. Sleep dissatisfaction is a potential marker for nomophobia in adults. Sleep Med. 2022, 98, 152–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Jilisha, G.; Venkatachalam, J.; Menon, V.; Olickal, J.J. Nomophobia: A mixed-methods study on prevalence, associated factors, and perception among college students in Puducherry, India. Indian J. Psychol. Med. 2019, 41, 541–548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Kaur, A.; Ani, A.; Sharma, A.; Kumari, V. Nomophobia and social interaction anxiety among university students. Int. J. Afr. Nurs. Sci. 2021, 15, 100352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Kaviani, F.; Robards, B.; Young, K.L.; Koppel, S. Nomophobia: Is the fear of being without a smartphone associated with problematic use? Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 6024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Kaviani, F.; Young, K.; Koppel, S. Using nomophobia severity to predict illegal smartphone use while driving. Comput. Hum. Behav. Rep. 2022, 6, 100190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Koppel, S.; Stephens, A.N.; Kaviani, F.; Peiris, S.; Young, K.L.; Chambers, R.; Hassed, C. It’s all in the mind: The relationship between mindfulness and nomophobia on technology engagement while driving and aberrant driving behaviours. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2022, 86, 252–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Kundu, A.; Patnaik, L.; Pattanaik, S.; Sahu, T. Prevalence of nomophobia and use of social networking sites and applications—A cross-sectional study among undergraduate students in a medical college of Eastern India. J. Datta Meghe Inst. Med. Sci. Univ. 2022, 17, 69. [Google Scholar]
  77. Lupo, R.; Zacchino, S.; Caldararo, C.; Calabrò, A.; Carriero, M.C.; Santoro, P.; Carvello, M.; Conte, L. The use of electronical devices and relative levels of Nomophobia within a group of Italian nurses: An Observational Study. Epidemiol. Biostat. Public Health 2020, 17, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Ma, J.; Liu, C. Evaluation of the factor structure of the Chinese version of the nomophobia questionnaire. Curr. Psychol. 2021, 40, 1367–1373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Polat, F.; Delibas, L.; Bilir, İ. The relationship between nomophobia level and worry severity in future healthcare professional candidates. Perspect. Psychiatr. Care 2021, 58, 339–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Qutishat, M.; Lazarus, E.R.; Razmy, A.M.; Packianathan, S. University students’ nomophobia prevalence, sociodemographic factors and relationship with academic performance at a University in Oman. Int. J. Afr. Nurs. Sci. 2020, 13, 100206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Ramos-Soler, I.; López-Sánchez, C.; Quiles-Soler, C. Nomophobia in teenagers: Digital lifestyle, social networking and smartphone abuse. Commun. Soc. 2021, 34, 17–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Santl, L.; Brajkovic, L.; Kopilaš, V. Relationship between nomophobia, various emotional difficulties, and distress factors among students. Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2022, 12, 716–730. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Schwaiger, E.; Tahir, R. Nomophobia and its predictors in undergraduate students of Lahore, Pakistan. Heliyon 2020, 6, e04837. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Schwaiger, E.; Tahir, R. The impact of nomophobia and smartphone presence on fluid intelligence and attention. Cyberpsychology J. Psychosoc. Res. Cyberspace 2022, 16, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Sevim-Cirak, N.; Islim, O.F. Investigation into nomophobia amongst Turkish pre-service teachers. Educ. Inf. Technol. 2021, 26, 1877–1895. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Sui, W.; Sui, A.; Munn, J.; Irwin, J.D. Comparing the prevalence of nomophobia and smartphone addiction among university students pre-COVID-19 and during COVID-19. J. Am. Coll. Health 2022, 6, 1–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Sui, W.; Munn, J.; Irwin, J.D. Exploring and predicting Canadian university students’ trait anxiety and nomophobia during COVID-19. Int. J. Health Promot. Educ. 2022, 4, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Tomczyk, Ł.; Lizde, E.S. Nomophobia and Phubbing: Wellbeing and new media education in the family among adolescents in Bosnia and Herzegovina. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 2022, 137, 106489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Torpil, B.; Ünsal, E.; Yıldız, E.; Pekçetin, S. Relationship between nomophobia and occupational performance among university students. Br. J. Occup. Ther. 2021, 84, 441–445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Torpil, B.; Pekçetin, S. The effectiveness of two different occupational therapy interventions on time management and on perceived occupational performance and satisfaction in university students with severe Nomophobia: A Single-blind, randomized controlled trial. Occup. Ther. Ment. Health 2022, 38, 86–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Torpil, B.; Bahadir, Z.; Yilmaz, G.G.; Pekçetin, S. Comparison of sleep quality and sleepiness in university students with different levels of nomophobia. Int. J. Disabil. Sport. Health Sci. 2022, 5, 9–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Yavuz, M.; Altan, B.; Bayrak, B.; Gündüz, M.; Bolat, N. The relationships between nomophobia, alexithymia and metacognitive problems in an adolescent population. Turk. J. Pediatr. 2019, 61, 345–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  93. Durak, H.Y. Investigation of nomophobia and smartphone addiction predictors among adolescents in Turkey: Demographic variables and academic performance. Soc. Sci. J. 2019, 56, 492–517. [Google Scholar]
  94. León-Mejía, A.C.; Gutiérrez-Ortega, M.; Serrano-Pintado, I.; González-Cabrera, J. A systematic review on nomophobia prevalence: Surfacing results and standard guidelines for future research. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0250509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Sharma, M.; Amandeep; Mathur, D.M.; Jeenger, J. Nomophobia and its relationship with depression, anxiety, and quality of life in adolescents. Ind. Psychiatry J. 2019, 28, 231–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Darvishi, M.; Noori, M.; Nazer, M.R.; Sheikholeslami, S.; Karimi, E. Investigating Different Dimensions of Nomophobia among Medical Students: A Cross-Sectional Study. Open Access Maced. J. Med. Sci. 2019, 7, 573–578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  97. Liu, W.; Chen, J.-S.; Gan, W.Y.; Poon, W.C.; Tung, S.E.H.; Lee, L.J.; Xu, P.; Chen, I.-H.; Griffiths, M.D.; Lin, C.-Y. Associations of Problematic Internet Use, Weight-Related Self-Stigma, and Nomophobia with Physical Activity: Findings from Mainland China, Taiwan, and Malaysia. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 12135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  98. Güneş, N.A.; Özdemir, Ç. The relationship between nomophobia and anxiety levels in healthy young individuals. J. Psychosoc. Nurs. Ment. Health Serv. 2021, 59, 23–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  99. Khan, F.A.; Waqar, A.; Niazi, S.N.K. Text neck syndrome among students of a medical and dental college in Lahore. J. Sharif. Med. Dent. Coll. 2020, 6, 5–8. [Google Scholar]
  100. Ahmed, S.; Akter, R.; Pokhrel, N.; Samuel, A.J. Prevalence of text neck syndrome and SMS thumb among smartphone users in college-going students: A cross-sectional survey study. J. Public Health 2021, 29, 411–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Kaviani, F.; Young, K.L.; Robards, B.; Koppel, S. Nomophobia and self-reported smartphone use while driving: An investigation into whether nomophobia can increase the likelihood of illegal smartphone use while driving. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2020, 74, 212–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Jahrami, H.; Fekih-Romdhane, F.; Saif, Z.; Bragazzi, N.L.; Pandi-Perumal, S.R.; BaHammam, A.S.; Vitiello, M.V. A social media outage was associated with a surge in nomophobia, and the magnitude of change in nomophobia during the outage was associated with baseline insomnia. Clocks Sleep 2022, 4, 508–519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. PRISMA 2020 flow diagram for study selection.
Figure 1. PRISMA 2020 flow diagram for study selection.
Behavsci 13 00035 g001
Figure 2. A summary plot of the risk of bias assessment.
Figure 2. A summary plot of the risk of bias assessment.
Behavsci 13 00035 g002
Figure 3. Summary forest plot of nomophobia.
Figure 3. Summary forest plot of nomophobia.
Behavsci 13 00035 g003
Table 1. Summary of studies included in the systematic review and meta-analysis about the prevalence of nomophobia symptoms.
Table 1. Summary of studies included in the systematic review and meta-analysis about the prevalence of nomophobia symptoms.
SNRefStudyCountryPopulationSample (N)Male:FemaleAge (Years) ToolNomophobia (%)NOS (Stars)
1[44]Al-Balhan, 2018KuwaitUniversity Students51250:50%20NMP-Q100.08
2[45]Almarzooqi, 2022Saudi ArabiaGeneral Population89374:26%24NMP-Q99.48
3[46]Alwafi, 2022Saudi ArabiaGeneral Population519131:69%24Others51.07
4[47]Ayar, 2018TurkeyUniversity Students75517:83%21NMP-Q99.78
5[48]Bano, 2021Saudi ArabiaAdolescents23047:53%22NMP-Q100.06
6[49]Bartwal, 2020IndiaUniversity Students45138:62%21NMP-Q100.08
7[50]Bragazzi, 2019ItalyUniversity Students40340:60%28NMP-Q100.08
8[51]Buctot, 2021PhilippinesAdolescents337442:58%15NMP-Q99.58
9[52]Catone, 2020ItalyAdolescents295952:48%15Others69.07
10[53]Çelik İnce, 2021TurkeyUniversity Students60725:75%21NMP-Q99.78
11[54]Çevik-Durmaz, 2021TurkeyUniversity Students23418:82%22NMP-Q100.06
12[55]Çırak, 2022TurkeyUniversity Students45133:67%20NMP-Q100.08
13[56]Copaja-Corzo, 2022PeruUniversity Students313939:61%22NMP-Q96.08
14[57]Coskun, 2020TurkeyGeneral Population21051:49%33NMP-Q98.16
15[58]Daei, 2019IranUniversity Students32041:59%23Others100.05
16[59]Denprechavong, 2022ThailandUniversity Students63882:18%20NMP-Q76.28
17[60]Essel, 2022GhanaGeneral Population34543:57%20NMP-Q100.06
18[61]Farchakh, 2021LebanonGeneral Population22600:100%28NMP-Q97.78
19[62]Farooq, 2022PakistanUniversity Students45531:69%22NMP-Q100.08
20[63]Farooqui, 2018IndiaUniversity Students14546:54%19NMP-Q100.06
21[64]Fidanci, 2021TurkeyUniversity Students38651:49%22NMP-Q96.68
22[65]Gurbuz, 2020TurkeyGeneral Population40042:58%28Others100.07
23[66]Hoşgör, 2021TurkeyGeneral Population17810:90%31NMP-Q96.16
24[67]Işcan, 2021TurkeyUniversity Students64127:73%21NMP-Q99.78
25[68]Jahrami, 2021BahrainGeneral Population54946:54%27NMP-Q100.08
26[69]Jahrami, 2021BahrainGeneral Population65446:54%27NMP-Q100.08
27[70]Jahrami, 2022BahrainGeneral Population54949:51%27NMP-Q100.08
28[71]Jilisha, 2019IndiaUniversity Students77441:59%19NMP-Q98.88
29[72]Kaur, 2021PakistanUniversity Students20952:48%21NMP-Q100.06
30[73]Kaviani, 2020AustraliaGeneral Population283847:53%25NMP-Q99.28
31[74]Kaviani, 2022AustraliaGeneral Population277347:53%20NMP-Q99.28
32[75]Koppel, 2022AustraliaGeneral Population99030:70%51NMP-Q98.98
33[76]Kundu, 2022IndiaUniversity Students33850:50%21NMP-Q100.06
34[77]Lupo, 2020ItalyGeneral Population54027:73%33NMP-Q91.38
35[78]Ma, 2021ChinaUniversity Students47332:68%19NMP-Q82.98
36[79]Polat, 2022TurkeyAdolescents74524:76%21NMP-Q100.08
37[7]Prasad, 2017IndiaUniversity Students55447:53%22Others24.97
38[80]Qutishat, 2020OmanUniversity Students74034:66%33NMP-Q99.38
39[81]Ramos-Soler, 2021SpainAdolescents85052:48%15NMP-Q100.08
40[82]Santl, 2022CroatiaAdolescents25714:86%22NMP-Q100.06
41[83]Schwaiger, 2020PakistanUniversity Students13833:67%20NMP-Q100.06
42[84]Schwaiger, 2022PakistanUniversity Students13833:67%20NMP-Q97.16
43[85]Sevim-Cirak, 2021TurkeyAdolescents106632:68%20NMP-Q100.08
44[86]Sui, 2022CanadaUniversity Students25820:80%22NMP-Q100.06
45[8]Sui, 2022CanadaUniversity Students100221:79%23NMP-Q100.08
46[87]Sui, 2022CanadaUniversity Students122128:72%23NMP-Q100.08
47[88]Tomczyk, 2022Bosnia and HerzegovinaAdolescents108340:60%15NMP-Q29.58
48[89]Torpil, 2021TurkeyUniversity Students18115:85%20NMP-Q100.06
49[90]Torpil, 2022TurkeyUniversity Students4633:67%21NMP-Q100.06
50[91]Torpil, 2022TurkeyUniversity Students21510:90%23NMP-Q100.06
51[92]Yavuz, 2019ItalyAdolescents181746:54%15NMP-Q99.28
52[93]Yildiz Durak, 2019TurkeyAdolescents61252:48%13Others, NMP-Q53.47
Notes: NMP-Q = Nomophobia Questionnaire; NOS = Newcastle-Ottawa Scale.
Table 2. Results of the random-effects meta-analysis models of the prevalence of nomophobia symptoms.
Table 2. Results of the random-effects meta-analysis models of the prevalence of nomophobia symptoms.
AnalysisDescriptiveRandom-Effects Meta-AnalysisAdjusted Meta-AnalysisHeterogeneity Publication BiasModerators
KNPooled Results (95%CI) I2τ2τHQ pEgger’s TestRank TestAgeSexInt
Prevalence of all nomophobia symptoms (all severity)5347,39993.92% (93.19%; 94.66%)99.78 % (98.86%; 100.00%)99.6%0.0010.0315.3812,293.72 (df = 52)0.0010.0010.0010.0010.0010.61
Prevalence of all nomophobia symptoms (mild symptoms only)4233,78025.80% (19.83%; 31.78%)04.17 % (01.00%; 09.97%)99.8%0.040.2020.8117,747.07 (df = 41)0.0010.0010.220.470.800.94
Prevalence of all nomophobia symptoms (moderate symptoms only)4233,78052.40% (44.21%; 60.60%)74.66 % (65.59%; 83.73%)99.7%0.070.2717.8613,080.70 (df = 41)0.0010.010.120.890.820.79
Prevalence of all nomophobia symptoms (severe symptoms only)4233,78020.35% (16.51%; 24.20%)04.62 % (00.77%; 08.47%)99.6%0.020.1315.7510,176.69 (df = 41)0.0010.0010.330.370.910.66
Notes: K: Represents the number of included studies. N: Represents the number of included samples of the included studies. Rank-based nonparametric data augmentation was done using the Duval and Tweedie trim and fill approach. The technique was used to calculate the number of studies that a meta-analysis was missing since the most extreme results were suppressed on one side of the funnel plot. I2: Refers to the percentage of variation across samples due to heterogeneity rather than chance. τ2: Describes the extent of variation among the effects observed in different samples (between-sample variance). τ: Under the presumption that these genuine effect sizes are normally distributed, tau is an estimate of the standard deviation of the distribution of true effect sizes. The prediction interval is computed using tau. H: Describes confidence intervals of heterogeneity. It is more broadly characterized by the method of moments. As an inherited technique from meta-analysis, it is utilized in meta-regression.
Table 3. Results of the random-effects subgroup meta-analysis models of the prevalence of nomophobia symptoms by country, culture, population, tool, and year.
Table 3. Results of the random-effects subgroup meta-analysis models of the prevalence of nomophobia symptoms by country, culture, population, tool, and year.
Part 1—All Symptoms
AnalysisDescriptiveRandom-Effects Meta-AnalysisHeterogeneity
KNPooled Results
(95%CI)
I2τ2τQ p
By Country
  • Australia
3660199.18% (98.96%; 99.40%)0.0%0.0010.0010.860.001
  • Bahrain
31752100.00% (99.86%; 100.00%)0.0%0.0010.0010.0010.001
  • Canada
32481100.00% (99.91%; 100.00%)0.0%0.0010.0010.0010.001
  • India
5276085.74% (80.36%; 91.11%)99.8%0.0010.061667.120.001
  • Italy
4571989.96% (83.49%; 96.42%)99.8%0.0010.071317.430.001
  • Pakistan
494099.94% (99.53%; 100.00%)26.5%0.0010.0014.080.001
  • Saudi Arabia
3631483.49% (64.23%; 100.00%)100.0%0.030.174568.080.001
  • Turkey
16733996.29% (95.35%; 97.24%)98.7%0.0010.021195.410.001
By Culture
  • Western
810,37295.30% (94.04%; 96.56%)99.5%0.0010.021501.000.02
  • Non-Western
4537,02793.38% (92.44%; 94.32%)99.6%0.0010.0310,771.870.02
By Population
  • General adults
1418,37095.15% (93.06%; 97.25%)99.7%0.0010.045048.440.001
  • University students
2815,42497.38% (96.72%; 98.04%)98.7%0.0010.022121.920.001
  • High school students and community adolescents
1113,60584.17% (82.11%; 86.22%)99.8%0.0010.035071.850.001
By Tool
  • NMP-Q
4737,97597.59% (97.13%; 98.05%)98.8%0.0010.023844.290.001
  • Others
6942466.52% (53.66%; 79.37%)99.9%0.030.167961.510.001
By Year
  • 2018
4196682.30% (76.20%; 88.40%)99.8%0.0010.061661.680.001
  • 2019
6453885.35% (81.71%; 89.00%)99.6%0.0010.041163.340.001
  • 2020
8827694.77% (92.13%; 97.42%)99.5%0.0010.041352.490.001
  • 2021
1511,89299.46% (99.12%; 99.79%)92.4%0.0010.01184.090.001
  • 2022
2020,72793.01% (91.55%; 94.47%)99.8%0.0010.037886.390.001
Part 2—Mild Symptoms
AnalysisDescriptiveRandom-effects meta-analysisHeterogeneity
KNPooled results (95%CI)I2τ2τQp
By Country
  • Australia
3660136.24% (33.83%; 38.66%)74.6%0.0010.027.880.001
  • Bahrain
317526.45% (5.30%; 7.60%)0.0%0.0010.0010.030.001
  • India
4170819.13% (15.94%; 22.33%)61.4%0.0010.037.770.001
  • Italy
3276051.79% (36.42%; 67.16%)98.1%0.020.13107.810.001
  • Pakistan
494010.17% (8.24%; 12.10%)0.0%0.0010.0011.780.001
  • Saudi Arabia
2112317.45% (15.23%; 19.67%)0.0%0.0010.0010.050.001
  • Turkey
13588128.68% (18.03%; 39.33%)99.3%0.040.191809.780.001
By Culture
  • Western
5593446.70% (8.91%; 84.48%)99.9%0.190.437683.160.22
  • Non-Western
3727,84622.95% (18.24%; 27.66%)99.2%0.020.144341.570.22
By Population
  • General adults
1313,17928.57% (19.58%; 37.55%)99.4%0.030.161988.470.82
  • University students
2312,51924.36% (14.11%; 34.60%)99.7%0.060.258590.290.82
  • High school students and community adolescents
6808225.30% (11.04%; 39.55%)99.7%0.030.181854.420.82
By Tool
  • NMP-Q
4033,06026.10% (19.95%; 32.25%)99.8%0.040.2017,607.120.07
  • Others
272019.86% (16.95%; 22.77%)0.0%0.0010.0010.010.07
By Year
  • 2018
3141223.47% (11.72%; 35.23%)96.0%0.010.1050.330.65
  • 2019
4331432.91% (19.43%; 46.38%)98.5%0.020.14197.640.65
  • 2020
7531731.79% (18.36%; 45.22%)99.1%0.030.18693.360.65
  • 2021
1411,65824.20% (17.33%; 31.07%)98.9%0.020.131168.320.65
  • 2022
1412,07922.86% (12.23%; 33.49%)99.9%0.040.208692.360.65
Part 3—Moderate symptoms
AnalysisDescriptiveRandom-effects meta-analysisHeterogeneity
KNPooled results (95%CI)I2τ2τQp
By Country
  • Australia
3660148.69% (47.48%; 49.90%)0.0%0.0010.0010.080.001
  • Bahrain
3175272.95% (70.87%; 75.03%)0.0%0.0010.0010.010.001
  • India
4170860.78% (54.40%; 67.15%)85.2%0.0010.0620.260.001
  • Italy
3276036.31% (18.72%; 53.90%)98.8%0.020.15164.490.001
  • Pakistan
494056.13% (49.52%; 62.75%)74.2%0.0010.0611.630.001
  • Saudi Arabia
2112351.47% (48.55%; 54.39%)0.0%0.0010.0010.630.001
  • Turkey
13588147.95% (25.86%; 70.04%)99.8%0.160.417057.220.001
By Culture
  • Western
5593440.44% (19.85%; 61.04%)99.6%0.050.231080.700.23
  • Non-Western
3727,84654.03% (45.66%; 62.39%)99.6%0.070.269897.150.23
By Population
  • General adults
1313,17953.01% (45.73%; 60.30%)98.6%0.020.13871.240.80
  • University students
2312,51950.48% (41.23%; 59.72%)99.2%0.050.222849.630.80
  • High school students and community adolescents
6808258.49% (35.16%; 81.81%)99.9%0.080.293811.990.80
By Tool
  • NMP-Q
4033,06051.40% (42.93%; 59.87%)99.7%0.070.2713,028.810.001
  • Others
272072.38% (69.12%; 75.65%)0.0%0.0010.0010.340.001
By Year
  • 2018
3141255.11% (50.89%; 59.34%)55.2%0.0010.034.460.95
  • 2019
4331454.06% (42.90%; 65.22%)97.3%0.010.11112.580.95
  • 2020
7531752.88% (39.56%; 66.20%)98.9%0.030.18530.860.95
  • 2021
1411,65852.19% (45.97%; 58.42%)97.8%0.010.12585.360.95
  • 2022
1412,07951.20% (31.59%; 70.81%)99.9%0.140.3710,854.130.95
Part 4—Severe symptoms
AnalysisDescriptiveRandom-effects meta-analysisHeterogeneity
KNPooled results (95%CI)I2τ2τQp
By Country
  • Australia
3660114.08% (12.23%; 15.94%)77.4%0.0010.018.840.001
  • Bahrain
3175220.60% (18.71%; 22.50%)0.0%0.0010.0010.050.001
  • India
4170819.67% (15.77%; 23.57%)73.7%0.0010.0311.420.001
  • Italy
327608.77% (5.11%; 12.42%)89.8%0.0010.0319.650.001
  • Pakistan
494031.68% (23.14%; 40.22%)86.7%0.010.0822.630.001
  • Saudi Arabia
2112330.95% (27.31%; 34.59%)29.0%0.0010.021.410.001
  • Turkey
13588122.45% (14.12%; 30.78%)99.8%0.020.155522.010.001
By Culture
  • Western
5593410.22% (5.19%; 15.25%)97.7%0.0010.06172.290.001
  • Non-Western
3727,84621.75% (17.25%; 26.26%)99.6%0.020.149784.910.001
By Population
  • General adults
1313,17916.66% (11.43%; 21.88%)99.1%0.010.091312.050.33
  • University students
2312,51923.65% (15.37%; 31.93%)99.4%0.040.203877.840.33
  • High school students and community adolescents
6808215.81% (6.35%; 25.27%)99.7%0.010.121639.160.33
By Tool
  • NMP-Q
4033,06020.99% (17.01%; 24.98%)99.6%0.020.1310,153.290.001
  • Others
27207.70% (5.75%; 9.65%)0.0%0.0010.0010.670.001
By Year
  • 2018
3141220.34% (11.53%; 29.16%)93.3%0.010.0729.630.04
  • 2019
4331412.41% (6.23%; 18.58%)96.4%0.0010.0682.290.04
  • 2020
7531712.78% (6.09%; 19.47%)99.0%0.010.09575.380.04
  • 2021
1411,65821.29% (17.05%; 25.54%)97.2%0.010.08464.880.04
  • 2022
1412,07925.22% (15.70%; 34.75%)99.8%0.030.186590.380.04
Notes: K: Represents the number of included studies. N: Represents the number of included samples of the included studies. Rank-based nonparametric data augmentation was done using the Duval and Tweedie trim and fill approach. The technique was used to calculate the number of studies that a meta-analysis was missing since the most extreme results were suppressed on one side of the funnel plot. I2: Refers to the percentage of variation across samples due to heterogeneity rather than chance. τ2: Describes the extent of variation among the effects observed in different samples (between-sample variance). τ: Under the presumption that these genuine effect sizes are normally distributed, tau is an estimate of the standard deviation of the distribution of true effect sizes. The prediction interval is computed using tau. H: Describes confidence intervals of heterogeneity. It is more broadly characterized by the method of moments. As an inherited technique from meta-analysis, it is utilized in meta-regression.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Jahrami, H.; Trabelsi, K.; Boukhris, O.; Hussain, J.H.; Alenezi, A.F.; Humood, A.; Saif, Z.; Pandi-Perumal, S.R.; Seeman, M.V. The Prevalence of Mild, Moderate, and Severe Nomophobia Symptoms: A Systematic Review, Meta-Analysis, and Meta-Regression. Behav. Sci. 2023, 13, 35. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13010035

AMA Style

Jahrami H, Trabelsi K, Boukhris O, Hussain JH, Alenezi AF, Humood A, Saif Z, Pandi-Perumal SR, Seeman MV. The Prevalence of Mild, Moderate, and Severe Nomophobia Symptoms: A Systematic Review, Meta-Analysis, and Meta-Regression. Behavioral Sciences. 2023; 13(1):35. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13010035

Chicago/Turabian Style

Jahrami, Haitham, Khaled Trabelsi, Omar Boukhris, Jumana Hasan Hussain, Ahmad F. Alenezi, Ali Humood, Zahra Saif, Seithikurippu R. Pandi-Perumal, and Mary V. Seeman. 2023. "The Prevalence of Mild, Moderate, and Severe Nomophobia Symptoms: A Systematic Review, Meta-Analysis, and Meta-Regression" Behavioral Sciences 13, no. 1: 35. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13010035

APA Style

Jahrami, H., Trabelsi, K., Boukhris, O., Hussain, J. H., Alenezi, A. F., Humood, A., Saif, Z., Pandi-Perumal, S. R., & Seeman, M. V. (2023). The Prevalence of Mild, Moderate, and Severe Nomophobia Symptoms: A Systematic Review, Meta-Analysis, and Meta-Regression. Behavioral Sciences, 13(1), 35. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13010035

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop