Neo-Piagetian Predictors of Students’ Performance in Science Learning: Evidence from Primary Education
Abstract
:1. Introduction
Neo-Piagetian Constructs
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Rationale and Research Hypotheses
- The three neo-Piagetian constructs logical thinking (LTH), field dependence/independence (DFDI), and divergent thinking (DIV) are positively correlated with students’ performance in Knowledge and Interpretations.
- The three neo-Piagetian constructs logical thinking (LTH), field dependence/independence (FDI), and divergent thinking (DIV) can concomitantly act as linear predictors of students’ performance in Knowledge and Interpretations.
- Knowledge recall acts as a mediator of the above three neo-Piagetian constructs/effects on Interpretations.
2.2. Procedures and Instruments
3. Results
3.1. Correlation Analysis
3.2. Multiple Regression Analysis
3.3. Path Analysis
3.4. Mediation Analysis
4. Discussion and Concluding Remarks
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Appendix B. Sample Items from the Three Psychometric Tests
- 1
- Logical thinking Test (LTH)
- 2
- Field dependence/independence (FDI)
- 3
- Divergent Thinking (DIV)
References
- Johnson, P.M. Children’s Understanding of Substances, Part 1: Recognizing Chemical Change. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2000, 22, 719–737. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson, P.M. Children’s Understanding of Substances, Part 2: Explaining Chemical Change. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2002, 24, 1037–1054. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Solsona, N.; Izquierdo, M.; De Jong, O. Exploring the Development of Students’ Conceptual Profiles of Chemical Change. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2003, 25, 3–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Çalik, M.; Ayas, A. A Comparison of Level of Understanding of Eighth-Grade Students and Science Student Teachers Related to Selected Chemistry Concepts. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 2005, 42, 638–667. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liu, X.; Lesniak, K. Progression in Children’s Understanding of the Matter Concept from Elementary to High School. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 2006, 43, 320–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cokelez, A.; Dumon, A.; Taber, K.S. Upper Secondary French Students, Chemical Transformations and the “Register of Models”: A Cross-sectional Study. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2008, 30, 807–836. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Papageorgiou, G.; Johnson, P.M. Do Particle Ideas Help or Hinder Pupils’ Understanding of Phenomena? Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2005, 27, 1299–1317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson, P.M. Children’s Understanding of Changes of State Involving the Gas State, Part 1: Boiling Water and the Particle Theory. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 1998, 20, 567–583. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson, P.M. Progression in Children’s Understanding of a ‘Basic’ Particle Theory: A Longitudinal Study. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 1998, 20, 393–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hadenfeldt, J.C.; Neumann, K.; Bernholt, S.; Liu, X.; Parchmann, I. Students’ Progression in Understanding the Matter Concept. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 2016, 53, 683–708. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson, P.M. Children’s Understanding of Changes of State Involving the Gas State, Part 2: Evaporation and Condensation below Boiling Point. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 1998, 20, 695–709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Andersson, B. Pupils’ Conceptions of Matter and Its Transformations (Age 12-16). Stud. Sci. Educ. 1990, 18, 53–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stavridou, H.; Solomonidou, C. Conceptual Reorganization and the Construction of the Chemical Reaction Concept during Secondary Education. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 1998, 20, 205–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tsitsipis, G.; Stamovlasis, D.; Papageorgiou, G. The Effect of Three Cognitive Variables on Students’ Understanding of the Particulate Nature of Matter and Its Changes of State. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2010, 32, 987–1016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vosniadou, S.; Brewer, W.F. Mental Models of the Earth: A Study of Conceptual Change in Childhood. Cogn. Psychol. 1992, 24, 535–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Di Sessa, A.A. A History of Conceptual Change Research. In The Cambridge Handbook of the Learning Sciences; Sawyer, R.K., Ed.; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2014; pp. 88–108. [Google Scholar]
- Panagiotaki, G.; Nobes, G.; Potton, A. Mental Models and Other Misconceptions in Children’s Understanding of the Earth. J. Exp. Child. Psychol. 2009, 104, 52–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nobes, G.; Panagiotaki, G. Mental models or methodological artefacts? Adults’ naïve responses to a test of children’s conceptions of the earth. Br. J. Psychol. 2009, 100, 347–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frède, V.; Nobes, G.; Frappart, S.; Panagiotaki, G.; Troadec, B.; Martin, A.E. The Acquisition of Scientific Knowledge: The Influence of Methods of Questioning and Analysis on the Interpretation of Children’s Conceptions of the Earth. Infant Child Dev. 2011, 20, 432–448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vaiopoulou, J.; Stamovlasis, D.; Papageorgiou, G. New Perspectives for Theory Development in Science Education: Rethinking Mental Models of Force in Primary School. In Progress in Education, Volume 47; Nata, R.v., Ed.; Nova Science Publishers, Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 2017; pp. 1–16. [Google Scholar]
- Vaiopoulou, J.; Papageorgiou, G. Primary Students’ Conceptions of the Earth: Re-Examining a Fundamental Research Hypothesis on Mental Models. Presch. Prim. Educ. 2018, 6, 23–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Nobes, G.; Frède, V.; Panagiotaki, G. Astronomers’ Representations of the Earth and Day / Night Cycle: Implications for Children’s Acquisition of Scientific Concepts. Curr. Psychol. 2022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gómez, E.J.; Benarroch, A.; Marín, N. Evaluation of the Degree of Coherence Found in Students’ Conceptions Concerning the Particulate Nature of Matter. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 2006, 43, 577–598. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Crespo, M.Á.G.; Pozo, J.I. Relationships between Everyday Knowledge and Scientific Knowledge: Understanding How Matter Changes. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2004, 26, 1325–1343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Adbo, K.; Taber, K.S. Learners’ Mental Models of the Particle Nature of Matter: A Study of 16-year-old Swedish Science Students. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2009, 31, 757–786. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Chang, H.Y. Students’ Representational Competence with Drawing Technology across Two Domains of Science. Sci. Educ. 2018, 102, 1129–1149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Härmälä-Braskén, A.S.; Hemmi, K.; Kurtén, B. Misconceptions in Chemistry among Finnish Prospective Primary School Teachers—A Long-Term Study. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2020, 42, 1447–1464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morell, L.; Collier, T.; Black, P.; Wilson, M. A Construct-Modeling Approach to Develop a Learning Progression of How Students Understand the Structure of Matter. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 2017, 54, 1024–1048. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pitjeng-Mosabala, P.; Rollnick, M. Exploring the Development of Novice Unqualified Graduate Teachers’ Topic-Specific PCK in Teaching the Particulate Nature of Matter in South Africa’s Classrooms. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2018, 40, 742–770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, J.; Buck, G. The Relationship Between Chinese Students’ Subject Matter Knowledge and Argumentation Pedagogy. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2015, 37, 340–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gunnarsson, R.; Hellquist, B.; Strömdahl, H.; Zelic, D. Secondary School Science Teachers’ Arguments for the Particulate Nature of Matter. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 2018, 55, 503–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Park, W.; Yang, S.; Song, J. Eliciting Students’ Understanding of Nature of Science with Text-Based Tasks: Insights from New Korean High School Textbooks. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2020, 42, 426–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Freire, M.; Talanquer, V.; Amaral, E. Conceptual Profile of Chemistry: A Framework for Enriching Thinking and Action in Chemistry Education. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2019, 41, 674–692. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zohar, A.R.; Levy, S.T. Students’ Reasoning about Chemical Bonding: The Lacuna of Repulsion. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 2019, 56, 881–904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tsaparlis, G.; Angelopoulos, V. A Model of Problem Solving: Its Operation, Validity, and Usefulness in the Case of Organic-Synthesis Problems. Sci. Educ. 2000, 84, 131–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tsaparlis, G. Non-algorithmic Quantitative Problem Solving in University Physical Chemistry: A Correlation Study of the Role of Selective Cognitive Factors. Res. Sci. Technol. Educ. 2005, 23, 125–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kang, S.; Scharmann, L.C.; Noh, T.; Koh, H. The Influence of Students’ Cognitive and Motivational Variables in Respect of Cognitive Conflict and Conceptual Change. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2005, 27, 1037–1058. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lawson, A.E. Formal Reasoning, Achievement, and Intelligence: An Issue of Importance. Sci. Educ. 1982, 66, 77–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lawson, A.E. Predicting Science Achievement: The Role of Developmental Level, Disembedding Ability, Mental Capacity, Prior Knowledge, and Beliefs. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 1983, 20, 117–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lawson, A.E. Classroom Test of Scientific Reasoning: Revised Paper–Pencil Edition; Arizona State University: Tempe, AZ, USA, 1993. [Google Scholar]
- BouJaoude, S.; Salloum, S.; Abd-El-Khalick, F. Relationships between Selective Cognitive Variables and Students’ Ability to Solve Chemistry Problems. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2004, 26, 63–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Niaz, M. Reasoning Strategies of Students in Solving Chemistry Problems as a Function of Developmental Level, Functional M-capacity and Disembedding Ability. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 1996, 18, 525–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Piaget, J. The Development of Thought: Equilibration of Cognitive Structures; Viking Press: New York, NY, USA, 1977. [Google Scholar]
- Pascual-Leone, J. A Mathematical Model for the Transition Rule in Piaget’s Developmental Stages. Acta Psychol. 1970, 32, 301–345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stamovlasis, D.; Tsaparlis, G. Applying Catastrophe Theory to an Information-Processing Model of Problem Solving in Science Education. Sci. Educ. 2012, 96, 392–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tsitsipis, G.; Stamovlasis, D.; Papageorgiou, G. A Probabilistic Model For Students’ Errors And Misconceptions On The Structure Of Matter In Relation To Three Cognitive Variables. Int. J. Sci. Math. Educ. 2012, 10, 777–802. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Witkin, H.A.; Moore, C.A.; Goodenough, D.R.; Cox, P.W. Field-Dependent and Field-Independent Cognitive Styles and Their Educational Implications. ETS Res. Bull. Ser. 1975, 1975, 1–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Witkin, H.A.; Goodenough, D.R. Cognitive Styles: Essence and Origins. Field Dependence and Field Independence; International University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1981. [Google Scholar]
- Johnstone, A.H.; Al-Naeme, F.F. Room for Scientific Thought? Int. J. Sci. Educ. 1991, 13, 187–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tinajero, C.; Páramo, M.F. Field Dependence-Independence Cognitive Style and Academic Achievement: A Review of Research and Theory. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 1998, 13, 227–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Danili, E.; Reid, N. Some Strategies to Improve Performance in School Chemistry, Based on Two Cognitive Factors. Res. Sci. Technol. Educ. 2004, 22, 203–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stamovlasis, D.; Tsaparlis, G. Cognitive Variables in Problem Solving: A Nonlinear Approach. Int. J. Sci. Math. Educ. 2005, 3, 7–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Child, D.; Smithers, A. An Attempted Validation of the Joyce-Hudson Scale of Convergence and Divergence. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 1973, 43, 57–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heller, K.A. Scientific Ability and Creativity. High Abil. Stud. 2007, 18, 209–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Danili, E.; Reid, N. Cognitive Factors That Can Potentially Affect Pupils’ Test Performance. Chem. Educ. Res. Pract. 2006, 7, 64–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tsikalas, T.; Stamovlasis, D.; Papageorgiou, G. Mental Representations of 12 Year-Old Children about Boiling and Evaporation: A Probabilistic Association with Convergent and Divergent Thinking. Presch. Prim. Educ. 2014, 2, 17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stamovlasis, D.; Tsitsipis, G.; Papageorgiou, G. Structural Equation Modeling in Assessing Students’ Understanding of the State Changes of Matter. Chem. Educ. Res. Pract. 2012, 13, 357–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lawson, A.E. The Development and Validation of a Classroom Test of Formal Reasoning. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 1978, 15, 11–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Witkin, H.A.; Oltman, P.K.; Raskin, E.; Karp, S.A. Embedded Figures Test, Children’s Embedded Figures Test, Group Embedded Figures Test: Manual; Consulting Psychologists Press: Palo Alto, CA, USA, 1971. [Google Scholar]
- Bahar, M. Investigation of Biology Students’ Cognitive Structure through Word Association Tests, Mind Maps and Structural Communication Grids; University of Glasgow: Glasgow, UK, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Jose, P.E. Doing Statistical Mediation and Moderation; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA; London, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Stamovlasis, D.; Papageorgiou, G. Understanding Chemical Change in Primary Education: The Effect of Two Cognitive Variables. J. Sci. Teacher Educ. 2012, 23, 177–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson, P.M.; Papageorgiou, G. Rethinking the Introduction of Particle Theory: A Substance-Based Framework. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 2010, 47, 130–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hudson, L. Contrary Imaginations: A Psychological Study of the English Schoolboy; Penguin Books: Great Britain, UK, 1966. [Google Scholar]
- Runco, M.A. Divergent Thinking and Creative Performance in Gifted and Nongifted Children. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 1986, 46, 375–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnstone, A.H.; Al-Naeme, F.F. Filling a Curriculum Gap in Chemistry. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 1995, 17, 219–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bahar, M.; Hansell, M.H. The Relationship Between Some Psychological Factors and Their Effect on the Performance of Grid Questions and Word Association Tests. Educ. Psychol. 2000, 20, 349–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stamovlasis, D. Methodological and Epistemological Issues on Linear Regression Applied to Psychometric Variables in Problem Solving: Rethinking Variance. Chem. Educ. Res. Pract. 2010, 11, 59–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stamovlasis, D. Nonlinear Dynamics and Neo-Piagetian Theories in Problem Solving: Perspectives on a New Epistemology and Theory Development. Nonlinear Dyn. Psychol. Life Sci. 2011, 15, 145–173. [Google Scholar]
- Van Geert, P.L.C.; de Ruiter, N. Toward a Process Approach in Psychology: Stepping into Heraclitus’ River; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2022. [Google Scholar]
- Friedenberg, J. Dynamical Psychology: Complexity, Self-Organization and Mind; ISCE Publishing: New York, NY, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Skamp, K. Are Atoms and Molecules Too Difficult for Primary Children? Sch. Sci. Rev. 1999, 81, 87–96. [Google Scholar]
- Wiser, M.; Smith, C.L. Learning and Teaching about Matter in Grades K-8: When Should the Atomic-Molecular Theory Be Introduced? In International Handbook of Research on Conceptual Change; Routledge: London, UK, 2008; pp. 233–267. [Google Scholar]
- Zeitoun, H.H. The Relationship between Abstract Concept Achievement and Prior Knowledge, Formal Reasoning Ability and Gender. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 1989, 11, 227–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Snir, J.; Smith, C.L.; Raz, G. Linking Phenomena with Competing Underlying Models: A Software Tool for Introducing Students to the Particulate Model of Matter. Sci. Educ. 2003, 87, 794–830. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Adey, P.; Shayer, M. Really Raising Standards: Cognitive Intervention and Academic Achievement; Routledge: London, UK, 1994; ISBN 9781134853694. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, R.J.; Grigorenko, E.L. Teaching for Successful Intelligence: Principles, Procedures, and Practices. J. Educ. Gift. 2003, 27, 207–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Maya, J.; Luesia, J.F.; Pérez-Padilla, J. The Relationship between Learning Styles and Academic Performance: Consistency among Multiple Assessment Methods in Psychology and Education Students. Sustainability 2021, 13, 3341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De S. Oliveira, K.K.; de Souza, R.A.C. Digital Transformation towards Education 4.0. Inform. Educ. 2022, 21, 283–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Farias-Gaytan, S.; Aguaded, I.; Ramirez-Montoya, M.-S. Transformation and Digital Literacy: Systematic Literature Mapping. Educ. Inf. Technol. 2022, 27, 1417–1437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Variable | Mean | SD | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. LTH | 13.648 | 6.657 | 1 | ||||
2. FDI | 6.024 | 4067 | 0.354 *** | 1 | |||
3. DIV | 42.143 | 15.356 | 0.425 *** | 0.443 *** | 1 | ||
4. Knowledge | 2.435 | 1.439 | 0.246 *** | 0.092 | 0.219 *** | 1 | |
5. Interpretations | 6.221 | 3.174 | 0.381 *** | 0.174 *** | 0.323 *** | 0.322 *** | 1 |
6. TotalScore | 8.656 | 3.884 | 0.403 *** | 0.176 *** | 0.346 *** | 0.634 *** | 0.936 *** |
R2 | b | seb | β | t | F | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Knowledge | 0.073 | 15.48 *** | |||||
LTH | 0.041 | 0.012 | 0.191 | 3.44 *** | |||
DIV | 0.013 | 0.005 | 0.138 | 2.49 * | |||
Interpretations | 0.218 | 33.98 *** | |||||
Knowledge | 0.470 | 0.107 | 0.215 | 4.46 *** | |||
LTH | 0.121 | 0.025 | 0.254 | 4.89 *** | |||
DIV | 0.035 | 0.011 | 0.168 | 3.26 *** |
Estimate | Std. Error | z-Value | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Indirect effects | ||||
LTH → Knowledge → Interpretations | 0.020 | 0.007 | 2.739 | 0.006 |
DIV → Knowledge → Interpretations | 0.006 | 0.003 | 2.184 | 0.029 |
Total effects | ||||
LTH | 0.141 | 0.025 | 5.650 | <0.001 |
DIV | 0.041 | 0.011 | 3.783 | <0.001 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Vaiopoulou, J.; Tsikalas, T.; Stamovlasis, D.; Papageorgiou, G. Neo-Piagetian Predictors of Students’ Performance in Science Learning: Evidence from Primary Education. Behav. Sci. 2023, 13, 64. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13010064
Vaiopoulou J, Tsikalas T, Stamovlasis D, Papageorgiou G. Neo-Piagetian Predictors of Students’ Performance in Science Learning: Evidence from Primary Education. Behavioral Sciences. 2023; 13(1):64. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13010064
Chicago/Turabian StyleVaiopoulou, Julie, Themistocles Tsikalas, Dimitrios Stamovlasis, and George Papageorgiou. 2023. "Neo-Piagetian Predictors of Students’ Performance in Science Learning: Evidence from Primary Education" Behavioral Sciences 13, no. 1: 64. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13010064