Next Article in Journal
Reciprocal Associations among Social–Emotional Competence, Interpersonal Relationships and Academic Achievements in Primary School
Next Article in Special Issue
Attachment Orientation and Preferences for Partners’ Emotional Responses in Stressful and Positive Situations
Previous Article in Journal
Benefits of Enacting and Observing Gestures on Foreign Language Vocabulary Learning: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Romantic Love and Behavioral Activation System Sensitivity to a Loved One

1
School of Archaeology and Anthropology, ANU College of Arts and Social Sciences, The Australian National University, Canberra, ACT 2601, Australia
2
Discipline of Psychology, Faculty of Health, University of Canberra, Bruce, ACT 2617, Australia
3
Justice and Society, University of South Australia, Magill, SA 5072, Australia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2023, 13(11), 921; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13110921
Submission received: 6 September 2023 / Revised: 31 October 2023 / Accepted: 7 November 2023 / Published: 10 November 2023

Abstract

:
Research investigating the mechanisms that contribute to romantic love is in its infancy. The behavioral activation system is one biopsychological system that has been demonstrated to play a role in several motivational outcomes. This study was the first to investigate romantic love and the behavioral activation system. In study 1, the Behavioral Activation System—Sensitivity to a Loved One (BAS-SLO) Scale was validated in a sample of 1556 partnered young adults experiencing romantic love. In study 2, hierarchical linear regression was used to identify BAS-SLO Scale associations with the intensity of romantic love in a subsample of 812 partnered young adults experiencing romantic love for two years or less. The BAS-SLO Scale explained 8.89% of the variance in the intensity of romantic love. Subject to further validation and testing, the BAS-SLO Scale may be useful in future neuroimaging and psychological studies. The findings are considered in terms of the mechanisms and evolutionary history of romantic love.

1. Introduction

Research investigating the mechanisms that contribute to romantic love is in its infancy. The behavioral activation system (BAS) is one biopsychological system that has been demonstrated to play a role in several motivational outcomes. To our knowledge, no studies have investigated the role the BAS may play in romantic love. Using a biological conceptualization of romantic love, we develop a means of assessing BAS sensitivity to a loved one and assess its association with the intensity of romantic love. The result is the formulation of a new means of assessing one biopsychological system that may contribute to the expression of romantic love.

1.1. Romantic Love

The topic of love in romantic relationships is riddled with definitional inconsistency and ambiguity. Sociological [1,2], anthropological [3], psychological [4,5], and biological [6] conceptions of love in romantic relationships all have their own terminology and formulations. While in many such disciplines, it is common to refer to all types of love within romantic relationships as “romantic love,” the biopsychological focus of this article leads us to choose a different approach. In the discipline of biology, “romantic love” tends to refer to the period of intense feelings that often accompanies the early stages of romantic relationships [6,7]. As such, we use the term “romantic love” to refer to a motivational state associated with a range of reproductive functions, including mate choice, courtship, sex, and pair bonding [6] (p. 21). It is the basis of long-term romantic relationships and family formation throughout much of the world. It is associated with a range of cognitive, emotional, and behavioral activities in both sexes. It is sometimes referred to as “passionate love” in certain areas of psychology [8]. The expression of romantic love is partly socially or culturally influenced, and differences in its presentation are found across cultures (e.g., [1,2,3,9,10,11,12]).
Cognitive activity of romantic love includes intrusive thinking or preoccupation with the partner, idealization of the other in the relationship, and desire to know the other and to be known. Emotional activity includes attraction to the other, especially sexual attraction, negative feelings when things go awry, longing for reciprocity, desire for complete union, and physiological arousal. Behavioral activity includes actions toward determining the other’s feelings, studying the other person, service to the other, and maintaining physical closeness.
Romantic love often happens at the early stages of a romantic relationship (referred to as early-stage romantic love) and usually lasts months or years (see [13,14]) but can sometimes last many years or decades (referred to as long-term romantic love) [15,16,17]. The psychological characteristics of both types of romantic love are similar, except that long-term romantic love is not characterized by intrusive thinking or preoccupation with the partner [15,16]. The neural mechanisms that cause each type of romantic love are similar but are not identical.
Romantic love is most strongly associated with neural activity in systems associated with reward and motivation (e.g., ventral tegmental area, nucleus accumbens, amygdala, and medial prefrontal cortex), emotions (e.g., amygdala, anterior cingulate cortex, and the insula), sexual desire and arousal (e.g., caudate, insula, putamen, and anterior cingulate cortex), and social cognition (e.g., amygdala, insula, and medial prefrontal cortex), as well as higher-order cortical brain areas that are involved in attention, memory, mental associations, and self-representation [6,18]. Functional connectivity is increased in people experiencing early-stage romantic love within the reward, motivation, and emotion regulation network (dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, insula, caudate, amygdala, and nucleus accumbens) as well as the social cognition network (temporo-parietal junction, posterior cingulate cortex, medial prefrontal cortex, inferior parietal, precuneus, and temporal lobe [19]. Early-stage romantic love is also associated with lower network segregation and altered connectivity degree [20] and with the endocrinal activity of sex hormones, serotonin, dopamine, cortisol, oxytocin, and nerve growth factor [6]. To our knowledge, no research has investigated the endocrinological correlates of long-term romantic love.

1.2. The Behavioral Activation System (BAS)

One biological mechanism that is thought to play a role in the promotion of behavior is the BAS. This system is believed to be associated with dopaminergic reward and motivation circuitry [21,22,23,24]. The BAS works as a system that involves both inputs and outputs. Inputs are stimuli that serve as cues for goal-directed behavior. They include life events involving goal salience or goal attainment. Behavioral activation system outputs include motor activity, energy, confidence, interest, pleasure in rewards, and, potentially, sociability and exploration. The general outputs of the BAS have been compared with symptoms of mania, including initiation to locomotor activity, activity and exploration, and anger (see [25]).

1.3. The BAS and Romantic Love

People experiencing romantic love display a range of cognitions, emotions, and behaviors suggestive of heightened BAS activity. These include increased reward valuation, willingness to expend effort to gain reward, heightened initial hedonic response to success in the form of learning deficits, and lack of satiety in response to success (see [25]).
People experiencing romantic love demonstrate an increased reward valuation of the loved one. The loved one takes on a “special meaning” [26] (p. 32). The perception of the loved one changes, and idealization ensues, as does the belief that the loved one is the “perfect romantic partner” [10] (p. 391) for them and that their loved one satisfies their preferred standards of physical attractiveness [27] (p. 395). The loved one becomes the most important person in their life.
People experiencing romantic love appear to demonstrate a willingness to expend effort to gain reward. Romantic lovers often engage in courtship (see [6] for a review of the costs and benefits of courtship among people experiencing romantic love), which involves a series of signals and behaviors that serve as a means of assessing potential partner quality and willingness to invest in a relationship [28,29]. People experiencing romantic love are also willing to reorder daily priorities, make themselves available to their loved one, and take steps to make themselves desirable to their loved one by changing their “clothing, mannerisms, habits, or values” [26] (p. 33).
Some people experiencing romantic love may demonstrate some aspects of heightened initial hedonic response to success in the form of learning deficits. The most cogent example of this is the instances of obsessive pursuit (usually committed by men), which occur in the absence of rewarding interaction from the loved one. Men, in particular, but not exclusively, have a tendency to misinterpret politeness or friendliness for sexual interest from potential sexual partners (see [30] for review). Such a false positive bias is potentially present in people experiencing romantic love and can result in repeated attempts by an individual to court a loved one despite there being obvious indications that such efforts will be fruitless. That both females and males can be subject to ineffective courting demonstrates the potent motivational effect romantic love can have on both sexes. This is one BAS sensitivity component that warrants further investigation in people experiencing romantic love.
People experiencing romantic love demonstrate a lack of satiety in response to success. For example, even when an individual in love feels emotionally close to their loved one, there can be a desire to be even closer. A sense of avolition and uncontrollability is a feature of romantic love [26] (p. 33). This is evidenced by an individual reordering their daily activities to spend increasingly long periods with their loved one and, in the modern environment, the obsessive monitoring of social media pages of the loved one. More generally, people experiencing romantic love experience prolonged affect, confidence, and increased energy over prolonged periods, as is indicated by the hypomanic symptoms found to be present in adolescents experiencing romantic love reported by Brand and colleagues [31].

1.4. Salience of Loved One-Related Stimuli and the BAS

There is evidence that when an individual is in love, the loved one takes on a special meaning [26]. This can be considered in terms of loved one-related stimuli having increased salience, probably as a result of oxytocin activity in one or more motivation pathways [32] (see also [33]). This has been demonstrated empirically in terms of memory and attention [34], as well as the heightened BAS sensitivity characteristics of romantic love detailed above. Because the BAS is situated within a motivational system, we believe that this salience of the loved one and loved one-related stimuli means the BAS probably responds in a particularly sensitive manner to loved one-related stimuli.
This heightened salience of loved one-related stimuli among individuals experiencing romantic love suggests that BAS sensitivity, somewhat analogous to anxiety (see [35]), may exist in a trait and state form. General BAS sensitivity may be relatively stable and influence behavior over the life course in a consistent manner. This is a type of trait BAS sensitivity. There are also periods when the BAS may become particularly sensitive, such as during a manic episode (see [25]), or in relation to a particular person, such as in circumstances of romantic love. This is a type of state of BAS sensitivity. The foci of the current studies are this state of BAS sensitivity that is characteristic of romantic love.

1.5. The BAS Scale

One common measure of BAS sensitivity is the BAS Scale [36], which includes three subscales: reward responsiveness, drive, and fun-seeking. The BAS Scale was originally validated by Carver and White (1994) in conjunction with items assessing the behavioral inhibition system (BIS) using exploratory factor analysis, Cronbach’s alpha (reliability), and correlation scores with other related measures (convergent validity). The analysis found four factors explained the BIS/BAS Scale: (i) BIS, (ii) BAS reward responsiveness, (iii) BAS drive, and (vi) BAS fun-seeking. More recently, efforts using confirmatory factor analysis have been undertaken to confirm the reliability of the BAS Scale (e.g., [37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44]) and a single factor (two-factor BIS/BAS Scale) has been suggested (e.g., [38,39,43]) with some degree of support [38,43].
The reward-responsiveness subscale assesses the tendency to respond to rewards with energy and enthusiasm, the drive subscale assesses motivation to pursue goals, and the fun-seeking subscale assesses the tendency to pursue positive experiences without regard to potential threats or costs [36] (see [25] for a summary of findings in relation to BAS Scale subscales and bipolar disorder). It seems feasible that all three subscales could contribute to aspects of romantic love, as the BAS responds to loved one-related stimuli.

1.6. The Current Studies

This is the first attempt to investigate the Behavioral Activation System and romantic love. As a result, we undertake preliminary work to shed light on the relationships between these two constructs. We amended the BAS Scale to assess BAS Sensitivity to a Loved One (BAS-SLO; described below). In Study 1, we validate the BAS-SLO Scale. This was a necessary step in developing an initial understanding of the relationship between the behavioral activation system and romantic love. We used confirmatory factor analysis to assess the suitability of three factor structures: (i) a one-factor model; (ii) a three-factor, 13-item structure; and (iii) a three-factor, 12-item structure. We determined that a three-factor, 12-item structure possessed the best goodness of fit. We calculated Cronbach’s alphas to test internal reliability and correlated subscales with a related measure to assess convergent validity for this structure. In Study 2, we tested the hypothesis that the BAS-SLO Scale will be positively associated with the intensity of romantic love. Findings are considered within an evolutionary framework, which helps elucidate the mechanisms and evolutionary history of romantic love.

2. Study 1: Validating the BAS-SLO Scale

2.1. Materials and Methods

2.1.1. Participants

Participants were 1556 English-speaking young adults who self-identified as being in love taken from the Romantic Love Survey 2022 [45]. Appendix A presents the characteristics of participants used in Study 1 and the country of residence of participants. We use the majority of the ideas for sample characteristics reporting from Bode and Kowal [7].

2.1.2. Measures

The Behavioral Activation System Sensitivity to a Loved One (BAS-SLO) Scale was created by amending each item of the BAS Scale to relate to an individual’s loved one or relationship with their loved one. Participants were asked, “Indicate how much the following applies to you”. Responses were scored on a four-point scale (1 = very true for me; 4 = very false for me). Scores for each item are reverse coded, and subscale scores are summed. Table 1 presents the original BAS Scale items and the BAS-SLO Scale items for each subscale.
We also used the Passionate Love Scale—30 (PLS-30) to assess the convergent validity of the BAS-SLO scale. The PLS-30 is a 30-item measure of the cognitive, emotional, and behavioral characteristics of romantic love. Each item records scores by assessing agreement with statements on a nine-point Likert scale (1 = not at all true; 9 = definitely true). It is the most commonly used measure of romantic love in biological studies of romantic love [7]. Cronbach’s alpha for the PLS-30 in this sample was 0.944.

2.1.3. Procedure

We conducted a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) on the 13 items of the BAS-SLO Scale using techniques/suggestions from a guidance paper [46]) and predicted a three-factor solution in line with the original BAS Scale factor structure [36]. A CFA using a one-factor solution was also conducted, as there is some literature suggesting that the BAS can be explained by a single factor [38,39,43]. At the suggestion of one reviewer, following an initial round of peer review, we then conducted another three-factor CFA of 11 items from the proposed BAS-SLO (removing two poorly loaded items; reward responsiveness item 5 and fun-seeking item 3).
A weighted least square mean and variance adjusted (WLSMV) method of confirmatory factor analysis was used as the data were ordinal [47]. The comparative fit index (CFI), standardized root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA), and standardized root-mean-square residual (SRMR) were used to assess the appropriateness of all three models in accordance with common practice (see [46]).
The following criteria, based on work by Hu and Bentler [48] and the model CFA example by Knetka and Runyon [46], were used to assess the adequacy of the model: CFI > 0.95 (although 0.90 is required to ensure mis-specified models are not deemed acceptable), RMSEA < 0.06, and SRMR < 0.08. Internal reliability was assessed by calculating Cronbach’s alpha for each BAS-SLO subscale. Values of >0.70 were considered acceptable (see [49]). We assessed convergent validity by correlating BAS-SLO Scale subscales (i.e., reward responsiveness, drive, and fun-seeking) with the PLS-30 and the amended HCL-32. Factor loadings, covariances, and goodness of fit indices were calculated using the Lavaan package for R version 4.2.2 in R Studio. The CFA diagram was created in AMOS version 26. Convergent validity analyses were conducted using SPSS version 27.

2.2. Results

No items from the BAS-SLO were missing data. Two cases were missing data for the PLS-30. These two cases were not included in the correlation analysis. Table 2 presents the means, standard deviations, skewness statistics, and kurtosis statistics for the 13 items of the BAS-SLO. Most of the data were moderately skewed, but this was deemed acceptable as the robust maximum likelihood method has been shown to be robust against violations of normality (see [47]).

2.2.1. Three-Factor, 13-Item Model

Results from the three-factor 13-item CFA indicated that, in our sample, the model had adequate but not good psychometric properties (see Appendix B for a summary table of goodness of fit statistics for all models). CFI was 0.944, indicating an acceptable (but not quite good) fit. RMSEA was 0.055, indicating good fit. SRMR was 0.041, indicating good fit. Factor loadings ranged from 0.44 to 0.80, with the majority above 0.60 (see Appendix C), suggesting that the factors explained most of the items reasonably (but not very) well. Factors correlated with each other from 0.40 (drive and fun-seeking) to 0.66 (reward responsiveness and fun-seeking), suggesting the discriminate validity was acceptable. Two items (R5 and F3) loaded poorly onto the reward responsiveness and fun-seeking factors (0.44 and 0.52), respectively. Appendix C presents the results of the three-factor, 13-item CFA.

2.2.2. One-Factor, 13-Item Model

Results from the one-factor, 13-item CFA indicated that, in our sample, the model had very poor psychometric properties. CFI was 0.709, indicating very poor fit. RMSEA was 0.121, indicating very poor fit. SRMR was 0.086, indicating poor fit. Because this model had very poor fit, we do not report further on the results.

2.2.3. Three-Factor, 11-Item Model

Because R5 and F3 loaded substantially lower than all the other items in the three-factor, 13-item CFA, we removed these items and ran another three-factor CFA, this time with 11 items. Results indicated that, in our sample, the three-factor, 11-item model had good psychometric properties, but loadings were not generally improved from the three-factor, 13-item CFA. CFI was 0.966, indicating good fit. RMSEA was 0.048, indicating good fit. SRMR was 0.037, indicating good fit. Factor loadings ranged from 0.55 to 0.80, with the majority above 0.60 (see Figure 1), suggesting that the factors explained most of the items reasonably (but not very) well. Factors correlated with each other from 0.40 (drive and fun-seeking) to 0.68 (reward responsiveness and fun-seeking), suggesting the discriminate validity was acceptable. Figure 1 presents the results of the three-factor, 11-item CFA.
Cronbach’s alpha for the three-factor, 11-item BAS-SLO Scale was 0.725 for reward responsiveness, indicating acceptable internal reliability; 0.786 for drive, indicating acceptable internal reliability; and 0.629 for fun-seeking, indicating marginally questionable internal reliability. Cronbach’s alphas for all subscales aligned closely with those of the original BAS subscales (reward responsiveness = 0.73, drive = 0.76, fun-seeking = 0.66; [36]) and with subsequent studies (e.g., [37,39]).
Convergent validity was assessed by correlating each of the BAS-SLO Scale subscales with the PLS-30. We anticipated that each BAS-SLO Scale subscale would correlate highly with the PLS-30. Table 3 presents the correlations between the BAS-SLO Scale subscales and the PLS-30. PLS-30 had a large association with reward responsiveness and a medium association with drive and fun-seeking. This suggests good convergent validity.

2.3. Discussion

Study 1 reported three CFAs of the BAS-SLO Scale. A three-factor model for the BAS-SLO Scale with 11 items that aligned with the three factors of the original BAS Scale (reward responsiveness, drive, and fun-seeking) was deemed to be an appropriate model by CFA, as well as the reliability and convergent validity analyses. This is especially the case when considered in light of the psychometric properties of the original BAS Scale and subsequent studies indicating a three-factor model of the BAS Scale utilizing confirmatory factor analysis (e.g., [42,43,44]). Indices of fit generally supported the notion of an acceptable model with good fit. Factor loadings were lower than would be ideal, suggesting the factors did not explain the data well. Correlations among the factors suggest the discriminate validity of the BAS-SLO is moderately low. Internal reliability of the subscales ranged from marginal to acceptable. This is in line with alphas for these three subscales in previous studies [42,43,44]. Correlations between subscales of the BAS-SLO Scale and the PLS-30 were roughly as expected, suggesting good convergent validity. In sum, we think the BAS-SLO is a measure that could be used in studies investigating BAS sensitivity to a loved one and romantic love, as well as a range of other related phenomena. Appendix D presents the final items of the proposed BAS-SLO Scale.

3. Study 2: The BAS and Romantic Love

3.1. Materials and Methods

3.1.1. Participants

Participants were a subsample of study 1 participants, 812 English-speaking young adults who self-identified as being in love from the Romantic Love Survey 2022 [45]. Participants who had been in love for 23 months or less and scored above 130 on the PLS were included in the analysis. Two years is a likely period of time in which individuals experience early-stage romantic love rather than long-term romantic love (see [6]). Two cases were missing one data point, and these cases were removed. One intersex participant was removed. Appendix E presents the characteristics of participants used in Study 2 and the country of residence of the participants. We use the majority of the ideas for sample characteristics reporting from Bode and Kowal [7].

3.1.2. Measures

Behavioral Activation System sensitivity to a loved one was measured using the three subscales of the 11-item BAS-SLO Scale validated in Study 1. Intensity of romantic love was measured using the Passionate Love Scale (PLS-30; [10]; described in Study 1). Sex was measured using a simple question asking, “What is your biological sex?” Data were coded as 1 (female) or 2 (male). Some studies have suggested that females experience romantic love marginally more intensely than males [50,51]. Love in romantic relationships has been thought to follow a specific trajectory of intensity related to intimacy, passion, and commitment [52]. As such, the length of time an individual has been in love may be associated with the waxing or waning intensity of romantic love. Months in love was assessed by asking participants how long they had been in love with their loved one. Obsessive thinking is definitive of early-stage romantic love (see [10,53,54]) and one proposed biological component of romantic love [33]. It therefore follows that it may have a direct influence on the intensity of romantic love. Percent of time thinking about a loved one (obsessive thinking) was measured by asking participants, “What percentage of your waking hours do you spend thinking about the person you love?” Responses were on a scale from 0% to 100%. Commitment was measured by using five items from the TLS-15 commitment subscale [55] but with a nine-point scoring approach. Each item records scores by assessing agreement with statements ranging from 1 (not at all) to 9 (extremely). Romantic love is believed to serve as a commitment device [56,57] and, therefore, may have a direct association with the intensity of romantic love.

3.1.3. Procedure

To test the hypothesis that BAS sensitivity to a loved one would predict romantic love, we undertook a hierarchical linear regression whereby the BAS-SLO Scale predicted PLS-30. Step one included controls. Step 2 included controls and each of the three BAS-SLO Scale subscales.

3.2. Results

Table 4 reports the correlations among all variables used in Study 2 analyses and their descriptive statistics.
Our hypothesis predicted that the BAS-SLO Scale would be positively associated with the intensity of romantic love. To test this hypothesis, we undertook a hierarchical linear regression whereby the BAS-SLO Scale predicted PLS-30 scores after controlling for sex, months in love, obsessive thinking, and commitment. All assumptions for linear regression were met. The hierarchical linear regression predicting the intensity of romantic love revealed that, at Step 1, control variables contributed significantly to the regression model, with F(6, 805) = 166.987 and p < 0.001, and accounted for 45.02% of the variance in intensity of romantic love. Adding the BAS-SLO Scale to the regression model (Step 2) explained an additional 8.89% of the variation in the intensity of romantic love, and this change in adjusted R2 was significant; F(3, 802) = 136.519 and p < 0.001. Each individual BAS-SLO Scale subscale contributed significantly to the model (reward responsiveness, p < 0.001; drive, p < 0.001; and fun-seeking, p = 0.017). Table 5 presents the regression statistics for this analysis.

3.3. Discussion

Study 2 used the BAS-SLO Scale to examine the associations between the BAS sensitivity to a loved one and the intensity of romantic love in young adults experiencing romantic love for less than two years. We hypothesized that the BAS sensitivity to a loved one would be positively associated with the intensity of romantic love. Our hypothesis was confirmed. The BAS-SLO Scale explained 8.89% of the variance in the intensity of romantic love (measured by the PLS-30), confirming our hypothesis. This amounts to a medium effect [58] of BAS sensitivity to a loved one on the intensity of romantic love. All three subscales contributed significantly to the model, and this suggests that the BAS plays a role in romantic love. That all three subscales contributed to the model raises the question as to whether each subscale contributes to specific components that characterize the intensity of romantic love in the PLS-30.
The findings of Study 2 are important because they demonstrate that the BAS-SLO Scale may be useful in investigating romantic love and provide the first evidence that the BAS plays a role in romantic love. The findings suggest that future studies may be able to identify the unique components of romantic love caused by the BAS and its state of sensitivity to a loved one. Future studies could use the BAS-SLO Scale to predict individual features of the intensity of romantic love. The use of the BAS-SLO Scale could also potentially be extended to investigate aspects of established pair bonds and relationships characterized by pair bond maintenance and not characterized by the presence of pair bond formation and romantic love (see [33]). Further, the BAS-SLO Scale’s use could be combined with fMRI analyses to identify the neurobiological components of the BAS and their contribution to romantic love.
This study is not without limitations, however. The sample is constituted entirely of young adults in the first two years of romantic love. As a result, the sample is neither representative of the entirety of the human population who experiences romantic love nor the entire spectrum of romantic love (see [7] for issues of generalizability). Further, the analysis was undertaken on a subsample of that used to validate the Scale in Study 1. This limits the implications of the findings, given that the Scale may possess different properties in a different sample. Nonetheless, the study has demonstrated the potential usefulness of the BAS-SLO Scale and provided the first evidence that the BAS plays a role in romantic love.

4. General Discussion

This article presents the first direct evidence of the relationships between BAS sensitivity and romantic love. Study 1 demonstrated that it is possible to measure BAS sensitivity to a loved one. Study 2 demonstrated that this means of measurement can be useful in empirical studies investigating the relationship between the BAS and romantic love. Combined, these two studies suggest that BAS sensitivity to a loved one is a real phenomenon and that the state of romantic love is probably associated with BAS sensitivity to a loved one. This has implications for understanding the mechanisms and evolutionary history of romantic love (see [59]).
The reason the BAS can be particularly sensitive to a loved one may relate to the concept of salience. Froemke and Young [32] have suggested that oxytocin acts on motivation pathways to increase the salience of specific social stimuli. In humans, this may take place in the ventral tegmental area (VTA). The VTA is consistently implicated in fMRI studies of people experiencing romantic love (see [6,60,61]). Although void of oxytocin receptors, the human VTA has been identified as the area in which oxytocin attaches salience to socially rewarding cues [62]. This increased salience probably results in further up-regulation of dopamine pathways, presumably including those that characterize the activity of the BAS. This supports Bode’s [33] contention that the bonding attraction system in romantic love is characterized by both oxytocin and dopamine activity, among other factors.
Several studies have shed light on the neural structures associated with BAS sensitivity (assessed with the BAS Scale) in normal samples. BAS sensitivity has been associated with activity in the VTA–nucleus accumbens pathway and the orbitofrontal cortex [21], and BAS reward responsiveness has been associated with lateral prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, and ventral striatum [22] in healthy samples. Interestingly, BAS drive has been associated with less activity in the putamen, caudate, and thalamus, and BAS reward responsiveness has been associated with increased activity in the left precentral gyrus in response to different intensities of infant cries among mothers [23]. Variation in regional gray matter volume in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and inferior parietal lobule has also been associated with BAS Scale scores [24]. There is also evidence that reward network glutamate levels contribute to individual differences in BAS reward responsiveness [63]. These structures generally overlap with those found in romantic love (see [6]).
Knowledge about the neural structures associated with BAS sensitivity, their overlap with the structures associated with romantic love, and now, a means of measuring BAS sensitivity to a loved one provide the means of measuring specific bio-psychological mechanisms that likely contribute to romantic love. Functional magnetic resonance imaging studies can begin to isolate the specific contribution of the BAS to the intensity of romantic love or specific features of romantic love. The implications of the studies reported in this article extend beyond a better understanding of the mechanisms of romantic love. They also provide insights into the evolutionary history of romantic love.
The findings support the notion that romantic love evolved by using pre-existing neural mechanisms (see [6]). The BAS is evolutionarily old, and romantic love made use of this system in a novel way. Instead of increasing general sensitivity, it generates a salience of a particular social stimulus (the loved one), which in turn increases sensitivity to the loved one. This increased salience is possibly the same mechanism that results in increased sensitivity among a plethora of other mechanisms. For example, evidence that lovers have an attentional and memory bias towards loved one-related stimuli [34] suggests that the stimuli possess greater importance or value than other stimuli. This is not the result of state changes to the attentional or memory system but rather the result of increased salience of loved one-related stimuli. This is a simple and elegant way of recruiting cognitive, emotional, and behavioral efforts in response to stimuli that have been identified at the input to be of great importance. This salience presumably required an internal schema of the loved one, an assessment of stimuli to identify their concordance, and then the application of increased value or weight to those stimuli. The concepts of salience and sensitivity are fundamental to a better understanding of the mechanisms of romantic love.
This process of increasing salience is probably at the core and very beginning of the evolution of romantic love and may be associated with the left VTA [61]. Mutation permitted the increased valuation of particular social stimuli, and that was possibly the first step in its evolutionary history. The particular features of romantic love, and perhaps some of its functions, such as pair bonding, may have evolved long after this initial step. Courtship attraction, which is also associated with an increased salience of social stimuli (see [33]), and sexual desire probably become intertwined with romantic love over the following generations. This is in line with previous suggestions that a precursor to contemporary romantic love emerged prior to the evolution of pair bonds [33,64].
The findings of these two studies also highlight the likelihood that BAS sensitivity exists in both a trait and state manner. The traditional BAS Scale assesses dispositional trait sensitivity, whereas the BAS-SLO Scale assesses what can be considered a type of state sensitivity. Parallels with anxiety may help to guide future researchers when elucidating these distinct but related phenomena. To better understand the similarities and differences between trait and state BAS sensitivity, it will be necessary to identify the role of trait sensitivity in romantic love.

5. Conclusions

This article reported two studies related to the behavioral activation system and romantic love. In Study 1, the BAS-SLO Scale was validated in a sample of 1556 partnered young adults experiencing romantic love. The validation determined that the characteristics of the BAS-SLO Scale were sufficient to justify its use in future psychological and imaging studies. In study 2, hierarchical linear regression was used to identify BAS-SLO Scale associations with the intensity of romantic love in a subsample of 812 partnered young adults experiencing romantic love for two years or less. The BAS-SLO Scale explained 8.89% of the variance in the intensity of romantic love. The findings shed light on one of the biopsychological mechanisms that contribute to romantic love and provide insights into the specific functions of regions associated with romantic love from fMRI studies. The BAS-SLO Scale should be used in future psychological and imaging studies.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.B.; methodology, A.B. and P.S.K.; formal analysis, A.B.; data curation, A.B.; writing—original draft preparation, A.B.; writing—review and editing, P.S.K.; visualization, A.B.; supervision, P.S.K.; project administration, A.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The collection of data used in these studies was approved by the Australian National University Human Research Ethics Committee (Protocol 2022/298).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data and code used this study are available form authors upon request.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Marta Kowal for providing useful comments on an earlier version of the manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Table A1. Study 1 sample characteristics.
Table A1. Study 1 sample characteristics.
RangeMeanSDMediann%
Sample 1556100.00
Female 81452.31
Heterosexual 114273.39
Age18.00, 25.0022.441.8323.00
PLS-30 (mean item; 1–9)1.93, 9.007.021.137.17
TLS-Commitment (mean item; 1–9)1.80, 9.007.571.347.80
Time thinking (%)1.00, 100.0049.6823.4550.00
Dating, not co-habiting 47930.78
Committed, not co-habiting 71545.95
Committed, co-habiting 31420.18
Married or de facto 483.08
Years in love (0–10 or more)0.00, 10.001.902.151.00
Relationship duration (years)0.00, 10.001.822.051.00
Relationship satisfaction (1–5)1.00, 5.004.250.784.00
HCL-32 (0–32)0.00, 28.0016.384.9117.00
Having sex (no/yes) 137788.62
Sex times per week0.00, 30.003.032.792.00
>13 years of education 135286.89
Current student (no/yes) 104667.22
Not working 62940.42
Less than full-time work 49131.56
Full-time work 43628.02
AQOL-4D (12–48)12.00, 35.0017.593.4817.00
PLS-30 = Passionate Love Scale (divided by number of items); TLS-Commitment = five items from the TLS-15 Commitment subscale [55] but using a nine-point response option (divided by number of items); Obsessive thinking = Percent of time thinking about loved one; HCL-32 = Amended Hypomanic Checklist—32; AQOL-4D = Assessment of Quality of Life—4D; A small number of data points are missing among these variables.
Table A2. Country of residence of Study 1 participants.
Table A2. Country of residence of Study 1 participants.
Countryn%
South Africa18211.70
Poland1368.74
Mexico1157.39
Portugal1117.13
Italy1107.07
UK and NI1097.01
Greece935.98
USA915.85
Spain905.78
Germany764.88
Hungary674.31
Netherlands674.31
Canada432.76
Chile362.31
France311.99
Czech Republic231.48
Slovenia231.48
Australia201.29
Belgium201.29
Estonia181.16
Latvia181.16
Rest of sample774.95
Rest of sample = Finland, Sweden, Austria, Ireland, Israel, New Zealand, Switzerland, Denmark, Japan, Norway, Luxembourg, South Korea.

Appendix B

Table A3. Fit indices for one-factor and three-factor models of the BAS-SLO Scale in a sample of 1556 young adults self-reporting romantic love.
Table A3. Fit indices for one-factor and three-factor models of the BAS-SLO Scale in a sample of 1556 young adults self-reporting romantic love.
CFIRMSEASRMR
Criteria>0.950 (0.900)<0.060<0.080
One-factor model0.7090.1210.086
Three-factor model (13 items)0.9440.0550.041
Three-factor model (11 items)0.9660.0480.037

Appendix C

Figure A1. Results of the three-factor, 13-item CFA model for the BAS-SLO Scale. Note: Survey items (for description, see Table 3) are represented by rectangles, and latent factors are represented by ovals. Reward = reward responsiveness; Drive = drive; Fun = fun-seeking. The numbers above the one-directional arrows connecting factors to items represent standardized factor loadings. The numbers to the right of the bi-directional arrows connecting factors represent correlations between the factors.
Figure A1. Results of the three-factor, 13-item CFA model for the BAS-SLO Scale. Note: Survey items (for description, see Table 3) are represented by rectangles, and latent factors are represented by ovals. Reward = reward responsiveness; Drive = drive; Fun = fun-seeking. The numbers above the one-directional arrows connecting factors to items represent standardized factor loadings. The numbers to the right of the bi-directional arrows connecting factors represent correlations between the factors.
Behavsci 13 00921 g0a1

Appendix D

Table A4. Final items of the proposed BAS-SLO Scale.
Table A4. Final items of the proposed BAS-SLO Scale.
ItemBAS Scale ItemsBAS-SLO Scale Items
Reward responsiveness subscale
1When I’m doing well at something, I love to keep at it.When I’m doing well at something my partner values, I love to keep at it
2When I get something I want, I feel excited and energizedWhen my partner tells me they love me, I feel excited and energized
3When I see an opportunity for something I like, I get excited right awayWhen I see an opportunity to spend time with my partner, I get excited right away
4When good things happen to me, it affects me strongly When good things happen to my partner, it affects me strongly
Drive subscale
1I go out of my way to get things I wantI go out of my way to maintain my relationship with my partner
2When I want something, I usually go all-out to get itWhen it comes to maintaining my relationship with my partner, I usually go all-out
3If I see a chance to get something I want, I move on it right awayIf I see a chance to strengthen my relationship with my partner, I move on it straight away
4When I go after something, I use a “no holds barred” approachWhen it comes to maintaining my relationship with my partner, I use a “no holds barred” approach
Fun-seeking subscale
1I’m always willing to try something new if I think it will be funI’m always willing to try new things with my partner if I think it will be fun
2I will often do things for no other reason than that they might be funI will often do things with my partner for no other reason than they are fun
3I crave excitement and new sensationsI crave excitement and new sensations with my partner

Appendix E

Table A5. Study 2 sample characteristics.
Table A5. Study 2 sample characteristics.
RangeMeanSDMediann%
Sample 812100.00
Female 38947.91
Heterosexual 59272.91
Age18.00, 25.0022.161.8622.00
PLS-30 (mean item; 1–9)4.37, 9.006.991.047.10
TLS-Commitment (mean item; 1–9)2.00, 9.001.221.377.60
Time thinking (%)2.00, 100.0049.1022.5849.00
Dating, not co-habiting 35844.09
Committed, not co-habiting 37345.94
Committed, co-habiting 759.24
Married or de facto 60.74
Months in love (0–23)
Relationship satisfaction (1–5)1.00, 5.004.180.784.00
HCL-32 (0–32)0.00, 28.0016.354.7917.00
Having sex (no/yes) 72289.04
Sex times per week0.00, 20.003.212.873.00
>13 years of education 69385.34
Current student (no/yes) 56970.07
Not working 35543.72
Less than full-time work 27533.87
Full-time work 18222.41
AQOL-4D (12–48)12.00, 35.0017.553.4717.00
PLS-30 = Passionate Love Scale (divided by number of items); TLS-Commitment = five items from the TLS-15 Commitment subscale [55] but using a nine-point response option (divided by number of items); Obsessive thinking = Percent of time thinking about loved one; HCL-32 = Amended Hypomanic Checklist—32; AQOL-4D = Assessment of Quality of Life—4D; A small number of data points are missing among these variables.
Table A6. Country of residence of Study 2 participants.
Table A6. Country of residence of Study 2 participants.
Countryn%
South Africa10112.44
Poland738.99
UK and NI597.27
Portugal577.02
Mexico566.90
Greece516.28
Germany506.16
Spain496.03
Italy475.79
USA394.80
Hungary384.68
The Netherlands313.82
Canada222.71
Chile222.71
France161.97
Slovenia141.72
Estonia111.35
Australia101.23
Czech Republic101.23
Latvia91.11
Austria80.99
Rest of sample394.80
Rest of sample = Finland, Ireland, Israel, New Zealand, Belgium, Sweden, Switzerland, Japan, South Korea, Denmark, Luxembourg, Norway.

References

  1. Rusu, M.S. Theorising love in sociological thought: Classical contributions to a sociology of love. J. Class. Sociol. 2017, 18, 3–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. de Rougemont, D. Love in the Western World; Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1983. [Google Scholar]
  3. Jankowiak, W.R. (Ed.) Romantic Passion: A Universal Experience? Columbia University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
  4. Hatfield, E.; Walster, G.W. A New Look at Love, 1985 ed.; University Press of America: Lanham, MD, USA, 1985. [Google Scholar]
  5. Karandashev, V. Cultural Typologies of Love; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Bode, A.; Kushnick, G. Proximate and Ultimate Perspectives on Romantic Love. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 1088. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Bode, A.; Kowal, M. Toward consistent reporting of sample characteristics in studies investigating the biological mechanisms of romantic love. Front. Psychol. 2023, 14, 983419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Feybesse, C.; Hatfield, E. Passionate Love. In The New Psychology of Love, 2nd ed.; Sternberg, R.J., Sternberg, K., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  9. Jankowiak, W.R. (Ed.) Intimacies: Love and Sex Across Cultures; Columbia University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  10. Hatfield, E.; Sprecher, S. Measuring passionate love in intimate relationships. J. Adolesc. 1986, 9, 383–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Karandashev, V. Romantic Love in Cultural Contexts; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  12. Karandashev, V. Cross-Cultural Perspectives on the Experience and Expression of Love; Springer Nature: Cham, Switzerland, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  13. Tennov, D. Love and Limerence: The Experience of Being in Love; Stein & Day: New York, NY, USA, 1979. [Google Scholar]
  14. Bringle, R.G.; Winnick, T.; Rydell, R.J. The Prevalence and Nature of Unrequited Love. SAGE Open 2013, 3, 2158244013492160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. O’Leary, K.D.; Acevedo, B.P.; Aron, A.; Huddy, L.; Mashek, D. Is Long-Term Love More Than A Rare Phenomenon? If So, What Are Its Correlates? Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 2011, 3, 241–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Acevedo, B.P.; Aron, A. Does a Long-Term Relationship Kill Romantic Love? Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2009, 13, 59–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Sheets, V.L. Passion for life: Self-expansion and passionate love across the life span. J. Soc. Pers. Relatsh. 2013, 31, 958–974. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Cacioppo, S.; Bianchi-Demicheli, F.; Hatfield, E.; Rapson, R.L. Social Neuroscience of Love. Clin. Neuropsychiatry 2012, 9, 3–13. [Google Scholar]
  19. Song, H.W.; Zou, Z.L.; Kou, J.; Liu, Y.; Yang, L.Z.; Zilverstand, A.; Uquillas, F.D.; Zhang, X.C. Love-related changes in the brain: A resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Wang, C.; Song, S.S.; Uquillas, F.D.; Zilverstand, A.; Song, H.W.; Chen, H.; Zou, Z.L. Altered brain network organization in romantic love as measured with resting-state fMRI and graph theory. Brain Imaging Behav. 2020, 14, 2771–2784. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Wang, J.X.; Zhuang, J.Y.; Fu, L.; Lei, Q.; Fan, M.; Zhang, W. How ovarian hormones influence the behavioral activation and inhibition system through the dopamine pathway. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0237032. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Fuentes-Claramonte, P.; Ávila, C.; Rodríguez-Pujadas, A.; Ventura-Campos, N.; Bustamante, J.C.; Costumero, V.; Rosell-Negre, P.; Barrós-Loscertales, A. Reward sensitivity modulates brain activity in the prefrontal cortex, ACC and striatum during task switching. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0123073. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Montoya, J.L.; Landi, N.; Kober, H.; Worhunsky, P.D.; Rutherford, H.J.; Mencl, W.E.; Mayes, L.C.; Potenza, M.N. Regional brain responses in nulliparous women to emotional infant stimuli. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e36270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Li, Y.; Qiao, L.; Sun, J.; Wei, D.; Li, W.; Qiu, J.; Zhang, Q.; Shi, H. Gender-specific neuroanatomical basis of behavioral inhibition/approach systems (BIS/BAS) in a large sample of young adults: A voxel-based morphometric investigation. Behav. Brain Res. 2014, 274, 400–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Johnson, S.L.; Edge, M.D.; Holmes, M.K.; Carver, C.S. The behavioral activation system and mania. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2012, 8, 243–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Fisher, H.E. Lust, attraction, and attachment in mammalian reproduction. Hum. Nat. -Interdiscip. Biosoc. Perspect. 1998, 9, 23–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Hendrick, C.; Hendrick, S. A theory and method of love. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1986, 50, 392–402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Trivers, R.L. Parental investment and sexual selection. In Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man; Campbell, B., Ed.; Aldine: Chicago, IL, USA, 1972; pp. 136–179. [Google Scholar]
  29. Wachtmeister, C.-A.; Enquist, M. The evolution of courtship rituals in monogamous species. Behav. Ecol. 2000, 11, 405–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Farris, C.; Treat, T.A.; Viken, R.J.; McFall, R.M. Sexual coercion and the misperception of sexual intent. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2008, 28, 48–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Brand, S.; Luethi, M.; von Planta, A.; Hatzinger, M.; Holsboer-Trachsler, E. Romantic love, hypomania, and sleep pattern in adolescents. J. Adolesc. Health 2007, 41, 69–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Froemke, R.; Young, L. Oxytocin, Neural Plasticity, and Social Behavior. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2021, 44, 359–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Bode, A. Romantic love evolved by co-opting mother-infant bonding. Front. Psychol. 2023, 14, 1176067. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Langeslag, S.; Olivier, J.; Köhlen, M.; Nijs, I.; Van Strien, J. Increased attention and memory for beloved-related information during infatuation: Behavioral and electrophysiological data. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2015, 10, 136–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Spielberger, C.D.; Gorsuch, R.L.; Lushene, R.E. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Self-Evaluation Questionanire); COnsulting Psychologists Press: Palo Alto, CA, USA, 1970. [Google Scholar]
  36. Carver, C.S.; White, T.L. Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1994, 67, 319–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Moreira, D.; Almeida, F.; Pinto, M.; Segarra, P.; Barbosa, F. Data concerning the psychometric properties of the Behavioral Inhibition/Behavioral Activation Scales for the Portuguese population. Psychol. Assess. 2015, 27, 1117–1122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Maack, D.J.; Ebesutani, C. A re-examination of the BIS/BAS scales: Evidence for BIS and BAS as unidimensional scales. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. 2018, 27, e1612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Cogswell, A.; Alloy, L.B.; van Dulmen, M.H.M.; Fresco, D.M. A psychometric evaluation of behavioral inhibition and approach self-report measures. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2006, 40, 1649–1658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Xu, M.; Wang, J.; Jin, Z.; Xia, L.; Lian, Q.; Huyang, S.; Wu, D. The Behavioral Inhibition System/Behavioral Activation System Scales: Measurement Invariance Across Gender in Chinese University Students. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 681753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Che, Q.; Yang, P.; Gao, H.; Liu, M.; Zhang, J.; Cai, T. Application of the Chinese Version of the BIS/BAS Scales in Participants With a Substance Use Disorder: An Analysis of Psychometric Properties and Comparison With Community Residents. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 912. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Campbell-Sills, L.; Liverant, G.I.; Brown, T.A. Psychometric evaluation of the behavioral inhibition/behavioral activation scales in a large sample of outpatients with anxiety and mood disorders. Psychol. Assess. 2004, 16, 244–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Jorm, A.F.; Christensen, H.; Henderson, A.S.; Jacomb, P.A.; Korten, A.E.; Rodgers, B. Using the BIS/BAS scales to measure behavioural inhibition and behavioural activation: Factor structure, validity and norms in a large community sample. Personal. Individ. Differ. 1999, 26, 49–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Heubeck, B.G.; Wilkinson, R.B.; Cologon, J. A second look at Carver and White’s (1994) BIS/BAS scales. Personal. Individ. Differ. 1998, 25, 785–800. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Bode, A.; Kavanagh, P.S. Romantic Love Survey 2022, 3rd ed.; UNC Dataverse: Chapel Hill, NC, USA, 2022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Knekta, E.; Runyon, C.; Eddy, S. One Size Doesn’t Fit All: Using Factor Analysis to Gather Validity Evidence When Using Surveys in Your Research. CBE Life Sci. Educ. 2019, 18, rm1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Li, C.-H. Confirmatory factor analysis with ordinal data: Comparing robust maximum likelihood and diagonally weighted least squares. Behav. Res. Methods 2016, 48, 936–949. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Hu, L.T.; Bentler, P.M. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. A Multidiscip. J. 1999, 6, 1–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Tavakol, M.; Dennick, R. Making sense of Cronbach’s alpha. Int. J. Med. Educ. 2011, 2, 53–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Cannas Aghedu, F.; Veneziani, C.A.; Manari, T.; Feybesse, C.; Bisiacchi, P.S. Assessing passionate love: Italian validation of the PLS (reduced version). Sex. Relatsh. Ther. 2018, 35, 77–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Hendrick, S.S.; Hendrick, C. Gender differences and similarities in sex and love. Pers. Relat. 1995, 2, 55–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Sternberg, R.J. A triangular theory of love. Psychol. Rev. 1986, 93, 119–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Brand, S.; Foell, S.; Bajoghli, H.; Keshavarzi, Z.; Kalak, N.; Gerber, M.; Schmidt, N.B.; Norton, P.J.; Holsboer-Trachsler, E. “Tell me, how bright your hypomania is, and I tell you, if you are happily in love!”-Among young adults in love, bright side hypomania is related to reduced depression and anxiety, and better sleep quality. Int. J. Psychiat. Clin. 2015, 19, 24–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Langeslag, S.J.E.; van der Veen, F.M.; Fekkes, D. Blood Levels of Serotonin Are Differentially Affected by Romantic Love in Men and Women. J. Psychophysiol. 2012, 26, 92–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Kowal, M.; Sorokowski, P.; Dinić, B.M.; Pisanski, K.; Gjoneska, B.; Frederick, D.A.; Pfuhl, G.; Milfont, T.L.; Bode, A.; Aguilar, L.; et al. Validation of the Short Version (TLS-15) of the Triangular Love Scale (TLS-45) across 37 Languages. Arch. Sex. Behav. 2023, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Frank, R.H. Passion within Reason: The Strategic Role of the Emotions; Norton: New York, NY, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
  57. Buss, D.M. The evolution of love in humans. In The New Psychology of Love, 2nd ed.; Sternberg, R.J., Sternberg, K., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  58. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  59. Zietsch, B.P.; Sidari, M.J.; Murphy, S.C.; Sherlock, J.M.; Lee, A.J. For the good of evolutionary psychology, let’s reunite proximate and ultimate explanations. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2020, 42, 76–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Xu, X.M.; Weng, X.C.; Aron, A. The mesolimbic dopamine pathway and romantic love. In Brain Mapping: An Encyclopedic Reference; Toga, A.W., Mesulam, M.M., Kastner, S., Eds.; Elsevier: Oxford, UK, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  61. Shih, H.-C.; Kuo, M.-E.; Wu, C.W.; Chao, Y.-P.; Huang, H.-W.; Huang, C.-M. The Neurobiological Basis of Love: A Meta-Analysis of Human Functional Neuroimaging Studies of Maternal and Passionate Love. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 830. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Groppe, S.E.; Gossen, A.; Rademacher, L.; Hahn, A.; Westphal, L.; Gründer, G.; Spreckelmeyer, K.N. Oxytocin influences processing of socially relevant cues in the ventral tegmental area of the human brain. Biol. Psychiatry 2013, 74, 172–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Sydnor, V.J.; Larsen, B.; Kohler, C.; Crow, A.J.D.; Rush, S.L.; Calkins, M.E.; Gur, R.C.; Gur, R.E.; Ruparel, K.; Kable, J.W.; et al. Diminished reward responsiveness is associated with lower reward network GluCEST: An ultra-high field glutamate imaging study. Mol. Psychiatry 2021, 26, 2137–2147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Fisher, H.E. Anatomy of Love: A Natural History of Mating, Marriage, and Why We Stray, 2nd ed.; W. W. Norton & Company: New York, NY, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Results of the three-factor, 11-item CFA model for the BAS-SLO Scale. Note: Survey items (for description, see Table 3) are represented by rectangles, and latent factors are represented by ovals. Reward = reward responsiveness; Drive = drive; Fun = fun-seeking. The numbers above the one-directional arrows connecting factors to items represent standardized factor loadings. The numbers to the right of the bi-directional arrows connecting factors represent correlations between the factors.
Figure 1. Results of the three-factor, 11-item CFA model for the BAS-SLO Scale. Note: Survey items (for description, see Table 3) are represented by rectangles, and latent factors are represented by ovals. Reward = reward responsiveness; Drive = drive; Fun = fun-seeking. The numbers above the one-directional arrows connecting factors to items represent standardized factor loadings. The numbers to the right of the bi-directional arrows connecting factors represent correlations between the factors.
Behavsci 13 00921 g001
Table 1. Original BAS Scale items and the equivalent BAS-SLO Scale items.
Table 1. Original BAS Scale items and the equivalent BAS-SLO Scale items.
ItemBAS Scale ItemsBAS-SLO Scale Items
Reward responsiveness subscale
1When I’m doing well at something, I love to keep at it.When I’m doing well at something my partner values, I love to keep at it
2When I get something I want, I feel excited and energizedWhen my partner tells me they love me, I feel excited and energized
3When I see an opportunity for something I like, I get excited right awayWhen I see an opportunity to spend time with my partner, I get excited right away
4When good things happen to me, it affects me strongly When good things happen to my partner, it affects me strongly
5It would excite me to win a contestIt would excite me for my partner and me to win a contest
Drive subscale
1I go out of my way to get things I wantI go out of my way to maintain my relationship with my partner
2When I want something, I usually go all-out to get itWhen it comes to maintaining my relationship with my partner, I usually go all-out
3If I see a chance to get something I want, I move on it right awayIf I see a chance to strengthen my relationship with my partner, I move on it straight away
4When I go after something, I use a “no holds barred” approachWhen it comes to maintaining my relationship with my partner, I use a “no holds barred” approach
Fun-seeking subscale
1I’m always willing to try something new if I think it will be funI’m always willing to try new things with my partner if I think it will be fun
2I will often do things for no other reason than that they might be funI will often do things with my partner for no other reason than they are fun
3I often act on the spur of the momentI often act on the spur of the moment with my partner
4I crave excitement and new sensationsI crave excitement and new sensations with my partner
Table 2. Means, standard deviations, skewness statistics, and kurtosis statistics for BAS-SLO Scale items in Study 1.
Table 2. Means, standard deviations, skewness statistics, and kurtosis statistics for BAS-SLO Scale items in Study 1.
MSDSkewnessKurtosis
R13.580.56−0.920.08
R23.670.55−1.592.21
R33.560.62−1.230.94
R43.570.64−1.421.86
R53.560.66−1.371.29
D13.210.77−0.800.30
D23.120.76−0.650.24
D33.370.71−0.980.74
D42.840.78−0.23−0.039
F13.620.57−1.331.55
F23.420.70−1.000.44
F33.060.73−0.36−0.34
F43.340.69−0.67−0.22
R = Reward responsiveness subscale; D = Drive subscale; F = Fun-seeking subscale.
Table 3. Correlations among each BAS-SLO Scale subscale and the PLS-30 in Study 1.
Table 3. Correlations among each BAS-SLO Scale subscale and the PLS-30 in Study 1.
BAS-SLO Scale SubscalesPLS-30
Reward0.560 ***
Drive0.418 ***
Fun-seeking0.358 ***
n = 1554; *** p < 0.001.
Table 4. Correlations among variables used in Study 2 and their descriptive statistics.
Table 4. Correlations among variables used in Study 2 and their descriptive statistics.
Variable12345678
1Reward responsiveness10.413 ***0.465 ***0.520 ***−0.106 **0.071 *0.207 ***0.450 ***
2Drive 10.327 ***0.468 ***−0.0480.099 **0.308 ***0.344 ***
3Fun-seeking 10.340 ***−0.087 *0.0350.121 ***0.262 ***
4PLS-30 1−0.112 **0.137 ***0.465 ***0.612 ***
5Sex (male) 1−0.064−0.209 ***−0.074 *
6Months in love 10.0630.232 ***
7Obsessive thinking 10.330 ***
8Commitment 1
n 423
% 52.09
M14.3312.4910.43209.64 8.1349.1536.55
SD1.672.341.4231.27 5.9722.586.85
* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001.
Table 5. Hierarchical regression model of intensity of romantic love.
Table 5. Hierarchical regression model of intensity of romantic love.
95% CI
R2Adjusted R2Adjusted R2 ChangebSEβtpLowerUpper
Step 10.453 ***0.450 ***
Sex −0.7721.668−0.012−0.4630.644−4.0462.502
Months in love −0.0130.140−0.002−0.0900.928−0.2880.263
Obsessive thinking 0.4050.0390.29210.392<0.0010.3280.481
Commitment 2.3530.1290.51618.212<0.0012.0992.607
Step 20.543 ***0.539 ***0.089 ***
Sex 0.1521.5340.0020.0990.921−2.8603.164
Months in love 0.0190.1290.0040.1470.883−0.2340.272
Obsessive thinking 0.3390.0370.2459.252<0.0010.2670.410
Commitment 1.6670.1310.36512.733<0.0011.4101.924
Reward responsiveness 3.8980.5610.2096.951<0.0012.7974.999
Drive 2.1310.3700.1595.753<0.0011.4042.858
Fun-seeking 1.4380.6010.0652.3910.0170.2572.618
n = 812; *** p < 0.001.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Bode, A.; Kavanagh, P.S. Romantic Love and Behavioral Activation System Sensitivity to a Loved One. Behav. Sci. 2023, 13, 921. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13110921

AMA Style

Bode A, Kavanagh PS. Romantic Love and Behavioral Activation System Sensitivity to a Loved One. Behavioral Sciences. 2023; 13(11):921. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13110921

Chicago/Turabian Style

Bode, Adam, and Phillip S. Kavanagh. 2023. "Romantic Love and Behavioral Activation System Sensitivity to a Loved One" Behavioral Sciences 13, no. 11: 921. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13110921

APA Style

Bode, A., & Kavanagh, P. S. (2023). Romantic Love and Behavioral Activation System Sensitivity to a Loved One. Behavioral Sciences, 13(11), 921. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13110921

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop