To Take a Risk or Not? The Effect of Perceived Scarcity on Risky Choices
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Methods and Materials
2.1. Method
2.1.1. Participants
2.1.2. Procedure
2.2. Manipulation and Measures
3. Results
3.1. The Manipulating Effect
3.2. The Scarcity Effect
3.3. Psychological Variables and Risky Choices
4. General Discussion
4.1. Conclusions
4.2. Discussion
4.3. Contribution
4.4. Future Research Direction
4.5. Limitations
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Mullainathan, S.; Shafir, E. Scarcity: Why Having too Little Means so Much, 1st ed.; Zhejiang People’s Publishing House: Hangzhou, China, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Liang, S.J.; Fan, P.; Yang, G.Y. The effect of perceived scarcity: Experiencing scarcity increases risk taking. J. Psychol. 2021, 155, 59–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Shah, A.K.; Mullainathan, S.; Shafir, E. Some Consequences of Having Too Little. Science 2012, 338, 682–685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Huijsmans, I.; Ma, I.; Micheli, L.; Civai, C.; Stallen, M.; Sanfey, A.G. A scarcity mindset alters neural processing underlying consumer decision making. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2019, 116, 11699–11704. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Williams, C.C.; Saffer, B.Y.; Mcculloch, R.B.; Krigolson, O.E. The scarcity heuristic impacts reward processing within the medial-frontal cortex. Neuro Rep. 2016, 27, 522–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaplan, H.S.; Gangestad, S.W. Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology; Wiley Online Library: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Griskevicius, V.; Tybur, J.M.; Delton, A.W.; Robertson, T.E. The influence of mortality and socioeconomic status on risk and delayed rewards: A life history theory approach. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 100, 1015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Griskevicius, V.; Ackerman, J.M.; Cantu, S.M.; Delton, A.W.; Robertson, T.E.; Simpson, J.A.; Thompson, M.E.; Tybur, J.M. When the Economy Falters, Do People Spend or Save? Responses to Resource Scarcity Depend on Childhood Environments. Psychol. Sci. 2013, 24, 197–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Caraco, T.; Martindale, S.; Whittam, S.T. An empirical demonstration of risk-sensitive foraging preferences. Anim. Behav. 1980, 28, 820–830. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mishra, S.; Fiddick, L. Beyond gains and losses: The effect of need on risky choice in framed decisions. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2012, 102, 1136–1147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mishra, S.; Lalumière, M.L. You can’t always get what you want: The motivational effect of need on risk-sensitive decision-making. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2010, 46, 605–611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pietras, C.J.; Hackenberc, T.D. Risk-sensitive choice in humans as a function of an earnings budget. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 2001, 76, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gonzales, J.; Mishra, S.; Camp, R.D. For the win: Risk-sensitive decision-making in teams. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2016, 30, 462–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Song, Y.; Xu, R.; Xing, C. Risk-sensitivity theory: Need motivates risky decision-making. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 2017, 25, 486–499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Payne, B.K.; Brown-Iannuzzi, J.B.; Hannay, J.W. Economic inequality increases risk taking. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2017, 114, 4643–4648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kugler, T.; Connolly, T.; Ordonez, L.D. Emotion, Decision, and Risk: Betting on Gambles versus Betting on People. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2012, 25, 123–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Soane, E.; Dewberry, C.; Narendran, S. The role of perceived costs and perceived benefits in the relationship between personality and risk-related choices. J. Risk. Res. 2010, 13, 303–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mishra, S.; Barclay, P.; Sparks, A.M. The Relative State Model Integrating Need-Based and Ability-Based Pathways to Risk-Taking. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2017, 21, 176–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dou, K.; Nie, Y.; Wang, Y.; Li, J. Ego depletion promotes risk- taking behavior. J. Psychol. Sci. 2014, 37, 150–155. [Google Scholar]
- Roux, C.; Goldsmith, K.; Bonezzi, A. On the psychology of scarcity: When reminders of resource scarcity promote selfish (and generous) behavior. J. Consum. Res. 2015, 42, 615–631. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mishra, S.; Hing, L.S.; Lalumiere, M.L. Inequality and Risk-Taking. Evol. Psychol. 2015, 13, 1474704915596295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clark, L.A.; Watson, D. Mood and the mundane: Relations between daily life events and self-reported mood. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1988, 54, 296–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huang, L.; Yang, T.; Ji, Z. Applicability of the Positive and the Negative Affect Scale in China. Chin. Ment. Health J. 2003, 17, 54–56. [Google Scholar]
- Zhang, D.C.; Highhouse, S.; Nye, C.D. Development and validation of the General Risk Propensity Scale (GRiPS). J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 2019, 32, 152–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gosling, S.D.; Rentfrow, P.J.; Swann, W.B. A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. J. Res. Personal. 2003, 37, 504–528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Whiteside, S.P.; Lynam, D.R. The Five Factor Model and impulsivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2001, 30, 669–689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tangney, J.P.; Baumeister, R.F.; Boone, A.L. High Self-control Predicts Good Adjustment, Less Pathology, Better Grades, and Interpersonal Success. J. Pers. 2004, 72, 271–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Unger, A.; Bi, C.; Xiao, Y.Y.; Ybarra, O. The revising of the Tangney Self-Control Scale for Chinese students. Psych J. 2016, 5, 101–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peng, Y.; Wang, X.; Wu, S.; Jin, S.; Sun, R. A brief introduction of Life History Theory and its combination with social psychology: Moral behaviors as an example. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 2016, 24, 464–474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Higgins, E.T. Promotion and Prevention: Regulatory Focus as A Motivational Principle. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 1998, 30, 1–46. [Google Scholar]
- Haushofer, J.; Fehr, E. On the psychology of poverty. Science 2014, 344, 862–867. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Allen, M.W.; Bettinger, R.L.; Codding, B.F. Resource scarcity drives lethal aggression among prehistoric hunter-gatherers in central California. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2016, 113, 12120–12125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weber, E.; Hsee, C. Culture and individual judgment and decision making. Appl. Psychol. 2000, 49, 32–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Measure | M | SD | Cronbach’s α | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Perceived scarcity | 4.94 | 0.85 | 0.667 | |
Emotion | Positive emotion | 28.99 | 5.06 | 0.855 |
Negative emotion | 19.37 | 4.76 | 0.821 | |
Risk attitude | GRiPS | 3.70 | 1.22 | 0.941 |
Personality | Extroversion | 4.60 | 1.33 | 0.661 |
Agreeableness | 5.31 | 0.96 | 0.102 | |
Conscientiousness | 4.35 | 1.12 | 0.518 | |
Emotional stability | 4.61 | 1.20 | 0.589 | |
Openness to experiences | 4.46 | 0.85 | 0.622 | |
Impulsivity | Premeditation | 4.67 | 0.70 | 0.815 |
Urgency | 3.55 | 0.92 | 0.880 | |
Sensation seeking | 4.11 | 1.06 | 0.867 | |
Perseverance | 4.52 | 0.63 | 0.752 | |
Self-control | Self-discipline | 4.07 | 0.56 | 0.531 |
Deliberate action | 4.32 | 0.92 | 0.830 | |
Healthy habits | 5.05 | 0.92 | 0.689 | |
Work ethics | 3.92 | 0.94 | 0.647 | |
Reliability | 5.01 | 0.74 | 0.492 |
Description | Risk of Receiving CNY 0 | |
---|---|---|
Safe Option | Receiving CNY 20 (USD 2.86) directly | 0.00 |
Risky Option | 80% probability of receiving CNY 25 (USD 0) | 0.20 |
66% probability of receiving CNY 33 (USD 4.73) | 0.40 | |
40% probability of receiving CNY 50 (USD 7.16) | 0.60 | |
30% probability of receiving CNY 67 (USD 9.59) | 0.70 | |
20% probability of receiving CNY 100 (USD 14.32) | 0.80 | |
10% probability of receiving CNY 200 (USD 28.64) | 0.90 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Liang, S.; Fan, P.; Yang, G. To Take a Risk or Not? The Effect of Perceived Scarcity on Risky Choices. Behav. Sci. 2023, 13, 743. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13090743
Liang S, Fan P, Yang G. To Take a Risk or Not? The Effect of Perceived Scarcity on Risky Choices. Behavioral Sciences. 2023; 13(9):743. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13090743
Chicago/Turabian StyleLiang, Shujing, Ping Fan, and Guangyong Yang. 2023. "To Take a Risk or Not? The Effect of Perceived Scarcity on Risky Choices" Behavioral Sciences 13, no. 9: 743. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13090743
APA StyleLiang, S., Fan, P., & Yang, G. (2023). To Take a Risk or Not? The Effect of Perceived Scarcity on Risky Choices. Behavioral Sciences, 13(9), 743. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13090743