Next Article in Journal
Flexibility Retained: Unimpaired Updating of Expectations in Schizophrenia
Next Article in Special Issue
Relationships between Self-Talk, Inner Speech, Mind Wandering, Mindfulness, Self-Concept Clarity, and Self-Regulation in University Students
Previous Article in Journal
Enforcement of Fairness Norms by Punishment: A Comparison of Gains and Losses
Previous Article in Special Issue
Is Anosognosia for Left-Sided Hemiplegia Due to a Specific Self-Awareness Defect or to a Poorly Conscious Working Mode Typical of the Right Hemisphere?
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Academic Self-Concept Wins the Race: The Prediction of Achievements in Three Major School Subjects by Five Subject-Specific Self-Related Variables

1
Center for Mental Health Education, School of Psychology, Southwest University Chongqing, 2 Tiansheng Road, Beibei, Chongqing 400715, China
2
Faculty of Psychology, Philipps-University Marburg, 35032 Marburg, Germany
3
Research Center for Modern Linguistics and Foreign Language Education, College of International Studies, Southwest University Chongqing, 2 Tiansheng Road, Beibei, Chongqing 400715, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2024, 14(1), 40; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14010040
Submission received: 15 October 2023 / Revised: 14 December 2023 / Accepted: 27 December 2023 / Published: 5 January 2024
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Conceptual and Empirical Connections between Self-Processes)

Abstract

:
The importance of self-related constructs in predicting academic achievement has been increasingly emphasized in recent decades. Typically, bivariate associations of self-related variables with achievements have been reported. Research quantifying the combined predictive power of more than two self-variables has been scarce. Moreover, except for the academic self-concept, these variables have almost always been measured across domains, i.e., without considering the specifics of individual school subjects. The current study aimed to statistically predict academic achievement (operationalized via school grades) in three major subjects (Chinese (native language), mathematics, and English (foreign language)) by using subject-tied scales, namely academic self-concept, conscientiousness, need for cognition, perseverance of effort, and consistency of interest. The sample comprised 791 Chinese adolescents. Each scale was related separately to each of the three school subjects. Hierarchical linear regression analyses were run. The control variable, biological sex, accounted for 2% of Chinese grades and 8% of English grades, but not of mathematics grades. Adding subject-specific self-concept scales increased the explained variance to 7% (Chinese), 16% (mathematics), and 32% (English). Further additions to the other four self-related scales did not increase the variances that were accounted for. The discussion underlines the relevance of subject-specific academic self-concepts as predictors for subject-tied academic achievements.

1. Introduction

In addition to cognitive abilities, self-related variables have been discussed as important determinants of (academic) achievement for the last three decades. Morin refers to “self” as an umbrella term that comprises “all imaginable physical and psychological (e.g., cognitive, affective, motivational, social) characteristics that make a person unique and different from others” [1] (p. 3). Among the Big Five personality traits, conscientiousness is more closely related to academic achievement than agreeableness, extraversion, openness, or neuroticism [2,3,4,5]. Conscientiousness is a “robust predictor of student performance” and “accounts for performance above and beyond cognitive ability” [6] (p. 245). However, motivational variables are generally considered better predictors than general personality traits [7]. Against this background, the present study investigated the factorial structures and psychometric properties of five self-related school subject-specific scales (hereafter in short: self-scales), namely academic self-concept, conscientiousness, need for cognition, perseverance of effort, and consistency of interest. Each scale was administered in three versions, each addressing one of the three main school subjects (native language Chinese, mathematics, and foreign language English). Subsequently, their intercorrelations and relationships with academic achievement were calculated for the three school subjects. Hierarchical linear regression analyses were subsequently used to quantify the joint power of these five self scales in predicting school grades. In addition, the incremental validity of each of the five variables above and beyond the other four variables was quantified.
These five variables were selected for the following two reasons: first, they are among the most frequently discussed non-cognitive determinants of scholastic performance, and second, most of the relevant primary studies have only reported bivariate correlations with (academic) achievement but have failed to analyze the combined predictive power of these variables for academic achievement.

1.1. Self-Related Constructs and Achievement

1.1.1. Academic Self-Concept (ASC)

The self-concept comprises the knowledge, beliefs, and attitudes of a person about himself or herself. In other words, it is “a person’s mental model of his or her abilities and characteristics” [8] (p. 750) and predicts a variety of behaviors in everyday life [9].
An early meta-analysis that was conducted by Hansford and Hattie [10] revealed an average correlation of rØ = 0.18 between overall (domain-general) self-concept and achievement, whereas self-concept of ability was more closely associated with achievement (rØ = 0.42). The ASC, i.e., the way a person describes and evaluates his or her academic performance, is one of the most important motivational factors in education [11,12,13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20,21]. Until the 1970s, the ASC was often defined and measured without taking into account the differences between school subjects. However, students’ different experiences in diverse school subjects lead to different perceptions of educational settings and scholastic successes and thus to the development of a multidimensional ASC (i.e., consisting of different factors). It has now been widely agreed that there are many ASCs [22,23,24], as “students align their self-concepts closely to the curriculum they encounter” [25] (p. 491). As school experience increases, the domain specificity of the ASC increases [26,27], and a reciprocal relationship with performance develops [28,29,30,31,32,33,34]. Today, most operationalizations of the ASC are contextual, referring directly to single school subjects [12,13,22,30,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43]. There is ample evidence that performance in specific school subjects (mathematics and language subjects have been studied most often) can be well predicted via their associated (i.e., subject specific) ASCs [12,13,22,30,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49], even across different performance levels and even after controlling for intelligence and previous scholastic achievement [16,50,51]. As with many other motivational variables, ASCs are more strongly linked to the grades given by the teacher than to the results of standardized achievement tests [19,52]. ASCs in mathematics and German were found to be good predictors of matched grades (r = 0.69 and r = 0.48, respectively) and even outperformed intelligence (r = 0.33 and r = 0.28, respectively) [53]. A meta-analysis [19] reported averaged correlations of verbal and mathematic self-concepts with corresponding achievements of rØ = 0.35 and rØ = 0.43, respectively. Huang’s analytical review [28] yielded slightly lower coefficients for longitudinal predictions of academic achievement via subject-specific ASCs.
In addition, it has been found that subject-linked ASCs are more closely associated with assigned academic performance than a global self-score or a cross-subject general ASC [30,46,54]. These findings were confirmed with a series of studies that examined the relationships of six subject-specific ASCs (biology, English, German, history, mathematics, and physics) with adolescents’ scholastic achievement in eleven samples [39]. The medians of the general (i.e., cross domain) ASC achievement correlations ranged from rMd = 0.33 (biology) to rMd = 0.41 (history). In contrast, the lowest median of the subject-matched (convergent) correlations was rMd = 0.49 (biology), and the maximal was rMd = 0.62 (mathematics). The medians of the divergent coefficients (ASCs and grades addressed to different subjects) were markedly lower, varying from rMd = 0.10 for the English ASC to rMd = 0.23 for the History ASC. A meta-analysis that was conducted by Möller et al. [19] revealed similar results for the relationships between mathematics and verbal self-concepts and mathematics and verbal achievements. The Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study 2019 [55] has once again proven that it is particularly useful to focus ASCs and academic achievements on the same topic. In large samples from Japan, Norway, and the United States, the science-specific self-concept emerged as “the strongest predictor of science achievement among motivational constructs” [56] (p. 22).
At this point, it should be noted that academic self-efficacy (ASE) is also a strongly performance-related construct [57,58,59]. Both of these self-variables, ASC and ASE, are strongly correlated with each other and with academic performance, but they are not interchangeable (i.e., identical) constructs that are merely labeled differently. They are anchored in distinct psychological theories, can be separated through factor analytical methods, refer to different frames of reference, develop differently, and differ in their unique power to predict academic performance [59,60]. ASE is almost exclusively defined in domain-specific terms. It therefore generally predicts domain-specific performance better than a cross-domain construct (such as a general ASC). However, if ASC and ASE are measured at the same aggregate level (both are tied to the same subject), then subject-specific ASCs are equivalent or even superior to subject-specific ASEs in predicting students’ subject-specific academic achievements [12].

1.1.2. Conscientiousness (CSN)

Of the Big Five personality dimensions, the content-unspecific (general) trait CSN displays the strongest relationship with academic performance [61,62,63,64]. Individuals scoring higher on CSN questionnaires are more responsible, reliable, and hardworking than those with lower scores. They take their tasks seriously and carry them out carefully. As a result, conscientious students tend to perform better than less conscientious students [63,64]. A comprehensive review [64] and several meta-analyses [2,65,66] have summarized the relationship between CSN and academic performance at school and college. Their reported effects ranged from ρ = 0.17 to ρ = 0.28 and were largely independent of intelligence [2]. A second-order meta-analysis confirmed these findings [67]. Across domains, the overall relationship between CSN and achievement tests was ρ = 0.22, and that between CSN and the grade point average (GPA) was ρ = 0.28. Meyer et al. [68] also reported stronger correlations with school grades than with standardized scholastic achievement tests (ρ = 0.28 vs. ρ = 0.13, respectively).
To what extent can CSN statistically predict academic achievement in conjunction with intelligence, the other four Big Five factors (openness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism), self discipline, and self efficacy? A hierarchical linear regression analysis by Dumfart and Neubauer [7] addressed this question. The dependent variables were GPA as well as grades in science and language subjects. Of the non-cognitive predictors, only CSN emerged as relevant after controlling for age, gender, and cognitive ability. The authors emphasized that CSN is “the crucial noncognitive predictor for school achievement” [7] (p. 14), thus substantiating previous research results [69,70]. The results of a recent regression analysis to predict the examination results of young British people confirmed this finding. Among several independent variables (adolescents’ intelligence, maternal cognitive ability, Big Five personality factors, and indicators of socioeconomic status), students’ intelligence was the best predictor of the General Certificate of Secondary Education (GCSE), followed by CSN. In contrast, the predictive power of each of the other variables was much lower [71]. This outcome was replicated with an extremely large sample of German adolescents [72].

1.1.3. Need for Cognition (NFC)

Like for CSN, the NFC construct has been conceptualized as a stable personality trait that cuts across domains [73,74]. It refers to individuals’ general preference for mentally processing complex intellectual problems and solving them through cognitive effort. The NFC construct is not an ability construct but reflects an intrinsic cognitive motivation “to engage in and enjoy thinking” [73] (p. 116). This domain-general variable has often been measured with questionnaires developed by Cacioppo et al. [74] or modifications of their scales.
The NFC construct has been positively related to learning outcomes, final grade expectations in mathematics and language subjects, and general scholastic achievement (GPA) [75,76,77,78]. Meta-analyses have reported correlations of r = 0.17 and r = 0.26 between the NFC construct and academic achievement and between the NFC construct and college entrance test scores, respectively [2,79,80].
We only found one study that examined the extent to which the relationship between NFC and academic achievement is moderated via the particular ability-based learning environment. For secondary school students attending the highest (i.e., most demanding) track, the mean correlation between overall NFC and academic achievement in three majors was rØ = 0.18. For students attending the middle track (i.e., moderately demanding), it was rØ = 0.11. For students attending the low track (i.e., least demanding), it was rØ = 0.01 [81].
Psychometric test performances in French and German were exclusively predictable via subject-tied NFC (γ = −19 and γ = 0.08, respectively), but not by general NFC (both subjects: γ = 0.00). For mathematics, both its general and subject-tied NFC values were predictive, with the subject-specific factor having a significantly greater relevance [82].

1.1.4. Perseverance of Effort (POE) and Consistency of Interest (COI)

The personality disposition “grit” refers to a person’s ability to pursue long-term goals with passion and persistence [83,84]. It has been conceptualized as a hierarchically structured “domain-general trait” [85] (p. 11), composed of two correlated primary factors, POE and COI, which are said to form the secondary factor grit. The POE construct encompasses the ability to sustain efforts for short- and long-term goals in the face of challenges, obstacles, and setbacks. The COI construct characterizes the ability to consistently focus on a goal or content over a long period of time. Based on the assumed hierarchical structure, Duckworth and colleagues suggested amalgamating POE and COI into one global (i.e., across domain) variable [83,84,85,86,87]. They claimed that (excellent) academic performance could be better predicted via grit than via intelligence [86,87], a statement that contradicts the results of decades of research showing that general intelligence g is by far the best single predictor of achievement [81,82,83,84,85,86,87,88,89,90,91,92,93,94,95,96]. Duckworth’s book on grit [86] and her grit scales [83,84] quickly gained a wide level of popularity [87,97,98,99] and were translated into many languages.
The global grit score has come under heavy fire. Criticisms have referred to its factorial structure, the lack of validity of the global grit score for achievement, and the alleged “novelty” of the construct. The hierarchical factor structure of grit that was postulated by Duckworth and colleagues [83,84] and other authors [100,101,102,103,104,105] cannot be conformed empirically, as the fit indexes of a hierarchical model with only two primary factors are always identical to those of a non-hierarchical model with two primary factors [106,107,108,109,110].
Moreover, a meta-analysis that was carried out by Credé et al. [106] found that merging the POE and COI constructs into one global grit score significantly decreased the predictive power of academic performance (GPA) compared to the predictive power of POE alone (ρ = 17 vs. ρ = 26, respectively). The COI construct was even less related to GPA (ρ = 10). Subsequent studies also showed that the POE construct was significantly more strongly correlated with academic achievement than the COI construct, although the magnitude of the correlation coefficients varied substantially across samples. Therefore, the global cross-domain grit construct, and thus its global score, was predominantly abandoned in favor of using the two primary factors, POE and COI, separately [111,112,113,114,115,116,117,118,119,120].
As far as the COI construct is concerned, a cross-domain individual interest contradicts the theory of interest. There has always been a consensus that a general academic interest, detached from specific content, does not exist [121,122,123,124,125]. In other words: “Individuals are not ‘interested’ in the same way that they may be ‘extraverted’… They are interested in something” [126] (p. 98). In the school setting, subjects moderate the relationship between interest and achievement. The only meta-analysis in this regard [127] reported average correlations between interest and achievement of rØ = 0.31, ranging from rØ = 0.17 for biology to rØ = 0.35 for science. In a large-scale study, the average correlation between interest and grades (five subjects: biology, chemistry, German, mathematics, and physics) was of a similar size [128].
Finally, it has been repeatedly shown that the POE construct overlaps extremely highly with the Big Five factor CSN [105,106,112,129,130]. This raises the question of whether the POE construct can be used to predict academic achievement beyond CSN.

1.2. Redundancies

The preceding remarks have shown that the five self-related constructs discussed are positively correlated with academic achievement. However, their variances overlap to a great extent in some cases, and they form a complex nomological network. A few examples of the many redundancies include the meta-analysis that was conducted by Credé et al. [106], which revealed strong associations of CSN with POE and COI (r = 0.83 and r = 0.61, respectively) and of POE with COI (r = 0.60). These high correlations were subsequently confirmed through several studies [111,112,120,130,131]. Medium-to-low correlations have been reported for the association between ASC with CSN and NFC, but only small correlations have been uncovered between NFC and CSN [14,75,132,133,134]. It has been shown that interests in different school subjects share many common elements with corresponding ASCs, up to a correlation of rØ = 0.77 [13,27,135,136,137].

1.3. Domain Specifities

Different school subjects are perceived differently by students and require different types of engagement [138,139]. Therefore, when interpreting the predictive powers of self-variables for academic achievement, it is useful to take subject-specific dependencies into account. This is because subject-specific measurements are usually more closely related to the corresponding subject-specific performance than are general (i.e., cross subject) measurements: they are symmetric concerning their degrees of generality. In other words, predictors and criteria should be operationalized under the same level of abstraction [9,140,141,142,143,144,145,146,147,148,149].
In the case of the ASC, its measurement has been predominantly linked to school subjects, often mathematics, science, and language subjects [26,30,37,38,42,49], but also to minor subjects such as sports and physical education [45,150,151], music [152,153,154,155], dancing [156], and arts [157]. However, for the other four self-concepts discussed so far, little or no subject-specific measurements have been performed to date.
Even with CSN, which has traditionally been conceptualized and measured as a cross-domain construct [73,74,158,159,160,161,162,163,164], it is not inconceivable, indeed likely, that it varies across school subjects. Everyday experiences have shown that a student may be diligent and conscientious in one subject but less industrious and more careless in another. However, we could not find any study that assessed CSN in a domain-specific way.
The COI construct has been designed as a trait-like characteristic that applies to all possible interests, regardless of a specific object. However, “there is no such thing as objectless interest” [165] (p. 137). Concerning scholastic interests, we have already noted that academic interests obviously and indisputably develop from a student’s engagement with subject-tied content. Thus, for many years, interest theory and interest research have contradicted the underlying assumption of the COI construct in that individual interests are object unspecific and can be validly measured across domains [13,121,122,123,124,125,126,127,128,166,167,168,169,170]. Such subject-linked relationships with academic achievement may also be relevant for the POE construct. Students’ propensity to stick with a subject and continue to strive despite setbacks and obstacles is likely to depend on the perceived difficulty of a school subject and on prior learning experiences, which may vary from subject to subject. For grit with POE and COI, this domain specificity was only addressed a few years ago, mainly concerning language subjects and mathematics [102,171,172,173,174,175,176,177], but also sports [178,179].
We only identified two papers that addressed a possible domain specificity of NFC. Pechtl [180] studied business students and concluded that NFC should be measured in a domain-specific manner, as is common for many school-related motivational and volitional variables. In line with this study, another study measured subject-specific NFC (four subjects) among high school students [82].

1.4. Recent Study

The ASC, CSN, NFC, PER, and COI constructs may be partially redundant in terms of their power to predict academic achievement. Therefore, the present study is the first in which these five self-variables were used simultaneously to jointly predict academic achievement in three main school subjects, namely Chinese, mathematics, and English.
To maximize their potential predictive power, the predictors and criteria should address the same subject. Therefore, the ASC was measured in a subject-specific manner. Unlike most of the previous studies, the other four predictors (CSN, NFC, POE, and COI) were also measured on a subject-specific basis. Thus, the Brunswick symmetry [144,145], i.e., the specificity-matching principle [9], was taken into account. This means that a cross-domain criterion (such as GPA) can be better predicted using a cross-domain (i.e., general) ASC than via a domain-specific one. On the other hand, in the case of a domain-specific criterion, such as a grade in a specific school subject, a subject-specific ASC can predict school performance better than a cross-subject (i.e., general) one.
The objective of the current study was to provide an answer to the following questions:
(1)
Within each of the five self-variables (ASC, CSN, NFC, POE, and COI), is it possible to separate three main school subjects (Chinese, mathematics, and English) via factor analyses (with the self-variable held constant in each case)?
(2)
Within each of the three main school subjects (Chinese, mathematics, and English), is it possible to separate the five self-variables (ASC, CSN, NFC, POE, and COI) via factor analyses (with the school subject held constant in each case)?
(3)
What is the joint explanatory power of the five subject-specific self-scales (ASC, CSN, NFC, POE, and COI) in predicting the linked academic achievement in three main school subjects (Chinese, mathematics, and English)?
(4)
What is the incremental (i.e., unique) validity of each of the five subject-specific self-variables (ASC, CSN, NFC, POE, and COI) in predicting their corresponding academic achievement in three main school subjects (Chinese, mathematics, and English) when the other four variables have been previously controlled? Or, to put it in another way, what is the extent to which each of the five variables, by itself and not in conjunction with the other predictors, statistically predicts school grades?
In the first step, we checked whether the assumed one-dimensionality of each of the fifteen subject-specific self-constructs (five variables × three subjects) was given. Based on these results, we formed 15 self-scales and calculated their psychometric properties (second step). In the third step, the intercorrelations of all the variables were established. In the fourth step, a series of hierarchical linear regression analyses were run to statistically predict the variance in school grades. Of particular interest was the amount of the subject-tied incremental validity of each self-scale after controlling for the remaining four.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Sample and Procedure

The initial sample comprised 807 adolescents from 20 middle school classes of 3 schools from a Chinese megacity. The smallest class comprised 30 students, and the largest comprised 47 pupils. A freely given informed consent was obtained from the parents, headmasters, and teachers. Parents and students were informed that their participation was completely voluntary, that all personal data would remain anonymous, and that non-participation would not result in any personal disadvantage. The survey took place in the classrooms during regular school hours and lasted less than 15 min. It was administered by an experienced psychologist. Six students did not want to participate. Due to not carefully filling the questionnaire in (16 questionnaires with missing responses or systematic ticking patterns could not be used), the statistical analyses of motivational variables were based on an effective sample of N = 791 (age: M = 15.76 years; SD = 0.78; boys: 48%). School grades were not available for 12 participants. Thus, analyses including academic achievement were based on N = 779. Once the file was completed, the assignment list (names associated with numbers) was given to the schools.

2.2. Variables

2.2.1. Age, Sex, and Academic Achievement

Participants noted their age and sex. School grades for Chinese (native language), mathematics, and English (foreign language) were taken from the most recent school report card. Higher scores corresponded to better grades. Grades were chosen as performance criteria, as they are a central factor in the formation of students’ achievement-related self-cognition and determine their school career [181,182,183]. They are extremely significant for their future life trajectories and are real-life criteria. Therefore, they are ecologically valid by definition.

2.2.2. Self-Variables

Five self-constructs, one personality variable (CSN), as well as four motivation variables (ASC, NFC, NFC, POE, and COI), were measured, each of which was specifically operationalized for three school subjects. The items, which focused on one of three school subjects (Chinese, mathematics, or English), were sequenced at random. The order was the same for all of the classes. Each item was presented in three versions: the wording of the stem was always identical. Only the subject specification (Chinese, mathematics, or English) differed.
The questionnaire was designed in the form of a table (grid layout, see Figure 1). The first column contained one item per row. Each item had a placeholder (…) for the respective domain. The headline labels of the next three columns indicated the respective subject. In the cells (item × topic), students marked the extent to which each statement personally applied to them. The response options were identical for all of the items and ranged from “1 = does not apply at all” to “6 = applies exactly”. Thus, the same item stem was the basis for three items formulated in parallel, each relating to a different school subject. The usefulness of a grid design for the fast and efficient measurement of self-variables has been demonstrated in many studies [12,13,38,39,40,41,50,111,112,132,142,169,184,185,186,187].
To measure the ASC, CSN, POE, and COI variables, we used items that had already been shown to be appropriate for Chinese adolescents. [12,13,111,112]. The NFC items were translated into Chinese. With the help of two bilingual psychologists, the accuracy of the translations was checked via back-and-forth transcription [188,189].
The six ASC items were taken from the differential self-concept grid [39,40,190]. For an example, see the first item of Figure 1. CSN was measured with eight items from the Big Five Personality test (B5T) [164]. Figure 1, second item, shows an example. The six NFC items were adopted from Preckel [191]. An example is shown in Figure 1, third item. In addition, we administered the eight items of the short grit questionnaire [84]. However, in line with many factor analytic studies, no total grit score was formed in favor of the two separate self-scales POE (four items; example: Figure 1, fourth item) and COI (four items; example: Figure 1, fifth item). As the wording of the COI items reflects a lack of interest, their scores were reversed.

2.3. Statistical Data Treatment

Statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS 25 and IBM AMOS 25. All variables, except for age and biological sex, were class-wise z-standardized (M = 0.00; SD = 1.00). Such cluster-centered standardizations eliminate any intraclass correlations. Indeed, our study focused on individual effects, i.e., on level 1 associations [192]. Level 2 or level 3 correlations (class or school effects) were not of interest. Product-moment correlations were calculated. Correlations were averaged (rØ) via Fisher’s z-transformation [193,194]. The sample was sufficiently large for the statistical methods that were applied [195,196,197,198,199,200,201]. Due to the large sample size, the significance level was set to 1% [202].
Confirmatory factor analyses (CFAs, maximum likelihood estimation) were run to test the appropriateness of a model with three subject-specific factors within each of the five self-constructs on the one hand and a model with five self-factors within each of the three subjects on the other hand. Correlated measurement errors were only accepted for items with identical wording stems (just the name of the school subject differed). For larger samples, the global fit index (χ2) usually reaches statistical significance whether a model fits the data or not. For this reason, and as different fit indexes respond differently to misspecification and model mismatches, we used the ratio χ2/df and a combination of three fit indexes, namely the incremental index CFI and the absolute indexes SRMR and RMSEA [203,204,205,206]. The frequently cited cut-off values for fit indexes recommended by Hu and Bentler [204] seem to be too strict for questionnaire data. Therefore, it has been recommended to apply less strict standards for questionnaire studies and to tolerate moderate deviations [207,208,209]. Hence, we applied the following simple cut-off criteria: RMSEA ≤ 0.08 and SRMR ≤ 0.08 (“badness-of-fit” indexes), and CFI ≥ 0.90 (“goodness-of-fit” index). We prioritized theoretical fit over adherence to the stated rules of thumb, as they “are rather arbitrary [and] should not be taken too seriously” [210] (p. 52). The accepted model should perform better in competition with a general factor model. For model comparison, we additionally used the entropy-based information criterion AIC [211,212,213]. A smaller AIC stands for a better fit.
Psychometric scale properties and variable intercorrelations were calculated. Subsequently, the relative relevance of each subject-specific self-scale for predicting its corresponding academic achievement was identified through running three hierarchical linear regression analyses, one for each criterion (Chinese grade, mathematics grade, and English grade). All regressions were structured in the same way. The two control variables sex and age were always entered first (step 1). Based on the hypothesis that among the measured constructs, the ASC is the best predictor of academic achievement, it was added as step 2. In step 3, the remaining four self-scales (CSN, NFC, POE, and COI) were added. Finally, we determined the unique contribution of each variable to the regression effect (i.e., the amount of the predicted variance not shared with the other four self-scales, step 4). To avoid interpretation problems due to high multicollinearity, the maximum predictor–predictor correlation was limited to r = 0.85, and no variance inflation factor (VIF) was allowed to exceed the limit of 3.5 [188,189].

3. Results

3.1. Factor Analyses

Table 1 shows the fit indexes of the measurement models when three distinguishable school subject factors were postulated within each of the five self-constructs, i.e., when the self-variable was always held constant. None of the five motivational constructs was adequately described by a general (i.e., content unspecific) factor. Instead, a structure with the subject factors Chinese, mathematics, and English fitted the data in each case. Each item marked “its” motivational factor according to theory. The averaged factor loadings ranged from λØ = 0.51 (English COI) to λØ = 0.78 (Chinese ASC).
The fit indexes of the models with five distinguishable self-factors within each of the three school subjects are shown in Table 2. In each case, the school subject was held constant. Unsurprisingly, a general factor model was again proven to be inconsistent with the data. The theoretically coherent models with five self-factors (ASC, CSN, NFC, POE, and COI) within each of the three school subjects had sufficient fit indexes. Again, each item indicated its respective self-factor. The averaged factor loadings ranged from λØ = 0.51 (English COI) to λØ = 0.75 (English ASC).

3.2. Psychometric Scale Properties

Based on the results of the CFAs, fifteen subject-specific self-scales were formed (three subjects × five self-constructs). All 15 scales were unimodally distributed. Their psychometric properties are listed in Table 3. Skewness and kurtosis values were well below ǀ1ǀ. Thus, univariate normality was not seriously violated. The internal consistencies of the ASC, CSN, and NFC scales proved to be satisfactory (ranging from α = 0.80 to α = 0.88). The POE scales declined in this respect (from α = 0.71 to α = 0.73). The internal consistencies of the COI scales were hardly sufficient (from α = 0.58 to α = 0.66). This was only partly due to their shortness, as the POE scales, each also only comprising four items, had significantly better internal consistencies. If the COI scales were extended by two equivalent items from four to six (i.e., on the length of the ASC and NFC scales), the internal consistencies would only increase to α = 0.74 (Chinese), α = 0.69 (mathematics), and α = 0.67 (English).

3.3. Intercorrelations

All correlations are presented in Table 4. Age did not correlate with any of the other variables. The variances of the five self-scales showed a high degree of overlap in many cases. For example, within each of the four self-scales, the averaged intercorrelations of the three subject-tied scales were quite high (COI: rØ = 0.70, CSN: rØ = 0.59, NFC: rØ = 0.53, and POE: rØ = 0.42), while the ASC scales were significantly less related to each other (rØ = 0.29). Mirroring this, four of the five self-sales (ASC, CSN, NFC, and POE) were strongly correlated within the same subject (Chinese: rØ = 0.65, mathematics: rØ = 0.68, and English: rØ = 0.70). However, COI made an exception with rØ = 0.22 (Chinese), rØ = 0.28 (mathematics), and rØ = 0.27 (English).
For the control variable biological sex (coding: 0 = boys and 1 = girls), there were statistically significant associations with grades in Chinese and English (r = 0.15 and r = 0.28, respectively). The highest correlation between a subject-tied scale and the corresponding grade was found for the English ASC (r = 0.53). The average association of the ASC scales with the matched grades was rØ = 0.39. The corresponding averaged coefficients for CSN, NFC, and POE were uniformly smaller (rØ = 0.23, rØ = 0.27, and rØ = 0.23, respectively). By far, the smallest mean correlation with grades was observed for COI (rØ = 0.10).

3.4. Statistical Prediction of Academic Achievement

Hierarchical linear multiple regression analyses were run to determine the predictive validity of the subject-tied self-scales in statistically explaining the corresponding school grades. There were no significant multicollinearities that could threaten the interpretation of the regression analyses (maximal predictor–predictor correlation r = 0.78; maximal VIF = 3.03). Table 5 shows the results of the regressions.
Of the two control variables biological sex and age, only the first (sex) was a statistically significant predictor in model 1 for Chinese (β = 0.15) and English (β = 0.38), but not for mathematics (β = −0.01). The variance that was accounted for amounted to 2% (Chinese) and 8% (English). Model 2 added the ASC, which was an additional statistically significant and relevant predictor for each grade (Chinese: β = 0.22; mathematics: β = 0.40; and English: β = 0.50). This second step accounted for 7% (Chinese), 16% (mathematics), and as much as 32% (English) of the achievement variance. The addition of CSN, NFC, POE, and COI (model 3) had little effect on the results. None of the four additional self-scales had a statistically significant β weight. The increment in explained variance was negligible for the English grade (∆R2 = 0.01), and the explained criterion variance did not increase at all for the Chinese grade and mathematics grade (∆R2 < 0.01). Hence, when calculating the unique (i.e., nonshared) significance of each scale in explaining the scores statistically, relevant increments (after controlling for the other four variables) were exclusively obtained from the ASCs. The increase was 5.0% for Chinese, 16% for mathematics, and as much as 24% for English.

4. Discussion

Based on a sample of Chinese adolescents, the present study aimed to shed light on the extent to which subject-specific academic achievements in three major school subjects (criteria) can be statistically predicted using five subject-tied self-constructs. The school subjects were Chinese, mathematics, and English. Two control variables (sex and age) and five subject-focusing self-variables served as predictors: academic self-concept (ASC), conscientiousness (CSN), need for cognition (NFC), perseverance of effort (POE), and consistency of interest (COI). The school grades from the last report card were used as ecologically valid operationalizations of subject-tied academic achievements (i.e., real-life criteria).
To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first to test the joint power of five subject-specific self-constructs for statistically predicting academic achievement in three school subjects. The questions that were posed at the beginning (1.4) of this study can be answered as follows:
(1)
Within each of the five self-variables (ASC, CSN, NFC, POE, and COI), the three subjects (Chinese, mathematics, and English) were confirmed to be separate factors.
(2)
Similarly, within each of the three school subjects (Chinese, mathematics, and English), the five self-variables (ASC, CSN, NFC, POE, and COI) were confirmed to be separate factors.
(3)
Based on the results of the CFAs, fifteen approximately normally distributed scales (three subjects × five self-constructs) were formed, each with a good (ASC, CSN, and NFC), satisfactory (POE), or sufficient (COI) internal consistency. These self-scales served as predictors of achievement variance within each of the three school subjects. All five variables together statistically explained 7%, (Chinese), 16% (mathematics), and 33% (English) of the grade variances.
(4)
Hierarchical linear multiple regression analyses evidenced that CSN, NFC, POE, and COI, in addition to the ASC, contributed virtually nothing independently to the statistical explanation of the subject-specific grade variances (the increments were only 1% in English and less than 1% in the other two subjects, Chinese and mathematics). In other words, the increments were minuscule and completely negligible in all three subjects. For the ASC, on the other hand, the proportion of explained variance in academic performance that was not shared with the other scales was substantial and varied by school subject (Chinese: 5%, mathematics: 16%, and English: 24%).
The results are clear and easy to grasp; therefore, the discussion can be brief. The bivariate correlations of the one-dimensional subject-specific self-scales ASC, CSN, and NFC with the assigned academic achievement are roughly within the range of what has been reported in the literature. However, we found extremely low correlations between the subject-linked interest scales (COI) and school grades in mathematics and English, which were clearly below the meta-analytic interest–achievement relationship of r = 0.31 [127]. For Chinese, we even observed a zero correlation. However, this meta-analysis integrated research findings from Western cultures. One explanation for our divergent results may be that the achievement motivations of Chinese adolescents differ from those of Western students [214,215]. Chinese youths “study not only for themselves but primarily for the status and prestige of their families” [215] (p. 135) and are under strong parental pressure to perform well at school. They are therefore mainly extrinsically motivated. However, individual interest, by contrast, is mainly an intrinsic phenomenon [216,217,218,219,220,221].
The correlation pattern, confirmatory factor analyses, and regression analyses underscore that it makes sense to measure school subject-related self-constructs in a specific way, tied to individual school subjects. The three school subject-specific ASC scales were proven to be overwhelming winners in the race against the other self-scales in predicting subject-specific academic achievements. They were the only ones that had notably unique powers to account for school grade variances. Noteworthy are the different proportions of variances in school grades, which were accounted for via the ASCs. For English, this corresponded to r = 0.49; for mathematics, it corresponded to r = 0.40; and for Chinese, it corresponded to r = 0.22. These divergent coefficients underline the relevance of subject-specific analyses. For the sake of comparison, it should be noted that in unselected WIRED samples (drawn from populations that are white, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic), correlations between IQ and academic performance in individual subjects are usually in the range from r ≈ 0.40 to r ≈ 0.60 [88,90,91,92,93,94,95,96,222].
The remaining four self-scales were deemed to be irrelevant for predicting academic achievement after controlling for the associated ASCs. Thus, if the self-variables used in this study are taken to predict school grades, the measurement of subject-tied CSN, NFC, POE, and COI seems to be redundant if one also measures their corresponding ASCs. The reverse is not true. Measuring these four self-variables by no means eliminates the need to measure the ASC, at least for Chinese adolescents. In other words, our study provides additional insights into which of the five self-constructs surveyed should be prioritized in educational and psychological counseling and research. Subject-specific ASCs appear to be the key non-cognitive predictors of academic achievement.
Every research project has its limitations, and so does the present study. Our results were based on the administered questionnaires and school grades. Different operationalizations of predictors (e.g., other self-questionnaires) and different criteria (e.g., standardized achievement tests instead of grades) could lead to different outcomes. Methodologically, all the predictors were measured via self-reports. This may have slightly increased their intercorrelations due to possible common method variance (self-report bias) [223,224], but it did not affect their associations with school grades as the predictors and criteria were obtained from different sources.
Future research could use complementary alternative methods to assess the variables under study, for example, ratings by peers, teachers, and parents. Furthermore, since (achievement related) self-constructs develop and differentiate over the course of schooling, it is unknown whether these results are even valid for younger students. In addition, the sample consisted of Chinese adolescents. Studies comparing students from individualistic (Western) and collectivistic (Eastern) countries could explore whether and to what extent subjects’ cultural background is a relevant moderator variable in predicting academic achievement.
The present results are limited to two subjects from the verbal domain (Chinese as the native language; English as the first foreign language) and one from the numerical domain (mathematics). Future studies should also include other majors, such as science and history, as well as minors, such as music, sports, and arts.
The present study is a cross-sectional one. Research has repeatedly shown that ASCs and scholastic achievements are reciprocally connected [28,29,30,31,32,33]. It cannot be ruled out that this also applies to the other self-constructs that were measured. Longitudinal studies could reveal developmental changes in their complex nomological network and its relationship with academic achievement.
As with all correlational studies, causal conclusions are not possible. Randomized controlled training experiments are needed to figure out whether a training-induced improvement in one self-variable leads to an increase in other self-variables on the one hand and to an improvement in the associated academic performance on the other hand.
In summary, school subject-linked ASCs very much outperformed the school subject-linked self-variables CSN, NFC, POE, and CO in terms of statistically predicting assigned grades in two verbal subjects (Chinese and English) and in mathematics, at least among Chinese adolescents. Thus, ASCs are non-cognitive key predictors of academic achievement. For other purposes, i.e., beyond the performance predictions, CSN, NFC, POE, and COI may nonetheless be of great importance both in and out of school. Last but not least, assiduousness and diligence, desire for intellectual activities, willingness to learn continuously, and cultivation of individual interests should be valued as educational and psychological objectives and outcomes in their own right.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: D.H.R. and X.F.; Methodology: D.H.R.; Formal Analysis: D.H.R.; Writing—original draft: D.H.R.; Writing—Review and Editing: D.H.R.; Supervision: D.H.R. and X.F.; Investigation: X.F.; Resources: X.F.; Data Curation: X.F.; Project administration: X.F.; Funding acquisition: X.F. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

National Social Science Foundation of China (no. 18XYY015).

Institutional Review Board Statement

Ethics committee approval was not required for data collection. This research project was approved and funded by the National Social Science Foundation of China. No medical, psychiatric, or psychological treatment or intervention took place. Only a short questionnaire was filled out, in which a few simple and completely harmless items were presented regarding students’ interest and engagement in three school subjects (item example: “In the subject Maths, I enjoy a task that involves finding new solutions to problems”). Informed consent was obtained from the school headmasters, teachers, and parents. Participation was voluntary. Parents and adolescent students were informed that all data would remain anonymous and that non-participation would not result in any personal disadvantages.

Informed Consent Statement

All participants agreed and signed the informed consent form.

Data Availability Statement

Restrictions apply to the availability of these data. The data are only available upon reasonable request to the authors.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all the people involved for their good cooperation.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Morin, A. Toward a glossary of self-related terms. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Poropat, A.E. A meta-analysis of the five-factor model of personality and academic performance. Psychol. Bull. 2009, 135, 322–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Richardson, M.; Abraham, C.; Bond, R. Psychological correlates of university students’ academic performance: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 2012, 138, 353–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Vedel, A. The big five and tertiary academic performance: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2014, 71, 66–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Dumfart, B.; Neubauer, A.C. Conscientiousness is the most powerful noncognitive predictor of school achievement in adolescents. J. Individ. Differ. 2016, 37, 8–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Mammadov, S. Big five personality traits and academic performance: A meta-analysis. J. Personal. 2022, 90, 222–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Steinmayr, R.; Spinath, B. Predicting school achievement from motivation and personality. Z. Für Pädagogische Psychol. 2007, 21, 207–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Moschner, B.; Dickhäuser, O. Selbstkonzept [Self-concept]. In Handwörterbuch Pädagogische Psychologie, 5th ed.; Rost, D.H., Sparfeldt, J.R., Buch, S.R., Eds.; Beltz: Weinheim, Germany, 2018; pp. 750–756. [Google Scholar]
  9. Swann, W.B., Jr.; Chang-Schneider, C.; Larsen McClarty, K. Do people’s self-views matter? Self-concept and self-esteem in everyday life. Am. Psychol. 2007, 62, 84–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Hansford, B.C.; Hattie, J.A. The relationship between self and achievement/performance measures. Rev. Educ. Res. 1982, 52, 123–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Basarkod, G.; Marsh, H.W. Academic self-concept: A central motivational construct. In Motivation Science: Controversies and Insights; Bong, M., Reeve, F.S.-I., Kim, D.-S., Eds.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2023; p. 59–C1.10.P22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Feng, X.; Wang, J.-L.; Rost, D.H. Akademische Selbstkonzepte und akademische Selbstwirksamkeiten: Interdependenzen und Beziehungen zu schulischen Leistungen [Academic self-concepts and academic self-efficacies: Their interdependencies and relationships to scholastic achievement]. Z. Pädagogische Psychol. 2018, 33, 23–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Feng, X.; Wang, J.-L.; Rost, D.H. Subject-specific interests and subject-specific self-concepts. Z. Pädagogische Psychol. 2023, 37, 322–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Hausen, J.E.; Möller, J.; Greiff, S.; Niepel, C. Students’ personality and state academic self-concept: Predicting differences in mean level and within-person variability in everyday school life. J. Educ. Psychol. 2022, 114, 1394–1411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Kadir, M.S.; Yeung, A.S. Academic self-concept. In Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences; Zeigler-Hill, V., Shackelford, T., Eds.; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2016; pp. 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Kriegbaum, K.; Becker, N.; Spinath, B. The relative importance of intelligence and motivation as predictors of school achievement: A meta-analysis. Educ. Res. Rev. 2018, 25, 120–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Marsh, H.W.; Pekrun, R.; Murayama, K.; Arens, A.K.; Parker, P.D.; Guo, J.; Dicke, T. An integrated model of academic self-concept development: Academic self-concept, grades, test scores, and tracking over 6 years. Dev. Psychol. 2018, 54, 263–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Marsh, H.W.; Seaton, M.; Dicke, T.; Parker, P.D.; Horwood, M.S. The centrality of academic self-concept to motivation and learning. In The Cambridge Handbook of Motivation and Learning; Renninger, K.A., Hidi, S.E., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, NY, USA, 2019; pp. 36–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Möller, J.; Pohlmann, B.; Köller, O.; Marsh, H.W. A meta-analytic path analysis of the internal/external frame of reference model of academic achievement and academic self-concept. Rev. Educ. Res. 2009, 79, 1129–1167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Möller, J.; Zitzmann, S.; Helm, F.; Machts, N.; Wolff, F. A meta-analysis of relations between achievement and self-concept. Rev. Educ. Res. 2020, 90, 376–419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Hattie, J. Visible Learning: A Synthesis of Over 800 Meta-Analyses Related to Achievement; Routledge: London, UK, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  22. Marsh, H.W. Content specificity of relations between academic achievement and academic self-concept. J. Educ. Psychol. 1992, 84, 35–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Marsh, H.W. Self-Concept Theory, Measurement and Research into Practice: The Role of Self-Concept in Educational Psychology; British Psychological Society: Leicester, UK, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  24. Orth, U.; Dapp, L.C.; Erol, R.Y.; Krauss, S.; Luciano, E.C. Development of domain-specific self-evaluations: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2021, 120, 145–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Gorges, J.; Hollmann, J. The structure of academic self-concept when facing novel learning content: Multidimensionality, hierarchy, and change. Eur. J. Psychol. 2019, 15, 491–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Wan, S.; Lauermann, F.; Bailey, D.H.; Eccles, J.S. Developmental changes in students’ use of dimensional comparisons to form ability self-concepts in math and verbal domains. Child Dev. 2023, 94, 272–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Denissen, J.J.; Zarrett, N.R.; Eccles, J.S. I like to do it, I’m able, and I know I am: Longitudinal couplings between domain-specific achievement, self-concept, and interest. Child Dev. 2007, 78, 430–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Huang, C. Self-Concept and academic achievement: A meta-analysis of longitudinal relations. J. Sch. Psychol. 2011, 49, 505–528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Hübner, N.; Wagner, W.; Zitzmann, S.; Nagengast, B. How strong is the evidence for a causal reciprocal effect? Contrasting traditional and new methods to investigate the reciprocal effects model of self-concept and achievement. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2023, 35, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Marsh, H.W.; Craven, R.G. Reciprocal effects of self-concept and performance from a multidimensional perspective: Beyond seductive pleasure and unidimensional perspectives. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 1, 133–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Marsh, H.W.; Martin, A.J. Academic self-concept and academic achievement: Relations and causal ordering. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 2011, 81, 59–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Wu, H.; Guo, Y.; Yang, Y.; Zhao, L.; Guo, C. A meta-analysis of the longitudinal relationship between academic self-concept and academic achievement. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2021, 33, 1749–1778. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Niepel, C.; Brunner, M.; Preckel, F. The longitudinal interplay of students’ academic self-concepts and achievements within and across domains: Replicating and extending the reciprocal internal/external frame of reference model. J. Educ. Psychol. 2014, 106, 1170–1191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Möller, J.; Köller, O. Die Genese akademischer Selbstkonzepte. Effekte dimensionaler und sozialer Vergleiche [On the development of academic self-concepts. The impact of social and dimensional comparisons]. Psychol. Rundsch. 2004, 55, 19–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Arens, A.K.; Jansen, M.; Preckel, F.; Schmidt, I.; Brunner, M. The structure of academic self-concept: A methodological review and empirical illustration of central models. Rev. Educ. Res. 2021, 91, 34–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Fang, J.; Huang, X.; Zhang, M.; Huang, F.; Li, Z.; Yuan, Q. The big-fish-little-pond effect on academic self-concept: A meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Jansen, M.; Schroeders, U.; Lüdtke, O. Academic self-concept in science: Multidimensionality, relations to achievement measures, and gender differences. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2014, 30, 11–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Rost, D.H.; Sparfeldt, J.R.; Dickhäuser, O.; Schilling, S.R. Dimensional comparisons in subject-specific academic self-concepts and achievements: A quasi-experimental approach. Learn. Instr. 2005, 15, 557–570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Rost, D.H.; Sparfeldt, J.R.; Buch, S.R. DISK-Gitter Mit SKSLF-8 [DISK Grid with SKSLF-8]; Hogrefe: Göttingen, Germany, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  40. Schilling, S.R.; Sparfeldt, J.R.; Rost, D.H.; Nickels, G. Schulische Selbstkonzepte. Zur Validität einer erweiterten Version des Differentiellen Selbstkonzept Gitters (DISK-Gitter) [Academic self-concepts. On the validity of an extended version of the Differential Self-Concept Grid (DISK-grid). Diagnostica 2005, 51, 21–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Schilling, S.R.; Sparfeldt, J.R.; Rost, D.H. Facetten schulischen Selbstkonzepts: Welchen Unterschied macht das Geschlecht? [Facets of academic self-concept. What difference does the sex make?]. Z. Pädagogische Psychol. 2006, 20, 9–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Lohbeck, A.; Möller, J. Social and dimensional comparison effects on math and reading self-concepts of elementary school children. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2017, 54, 73–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Habók, A.; Magyar, A.; Németh, M.; Csapo, B. Motivation and self-related beliefs as predictors of academic achievement in reading and mathematics: Structural equation models of longitudinal data. Int. J. Educ. Res. 2020, 103, 101634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Schwabe, F.; Schlitter, T.; Igler, J.; Ohle-Peters, A.; Teerling, A.; Köller, O.; McElvany, N. Lesemotivation, Leseselbstkonzept und Leseverhalten am Ende der Grundschulzeit—Wirksamkeit und differenzielle Effekte der schulischen Teilnahme an einer bundesweiten Förderinitiative [Reading motivation, reading self-concept, and reading behavior at the end of elementary school-effectiveness and differential effects of school participation in a nationwide training initiative]. Z. Pädagogische Psychol. 2020, 35, 53–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Sarrazin, P.; Boiché, J.; Guay, F.; Chanal, J. Verbal, mathematics, and physical education self-concepts and achievements: An extension and test of the internal/external frame of reference model. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2009, 10, 61–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Torppa, M.; Aro, T.; Eklund, K.; Psarilla, R.; Eloranta, A.-K.; Ahonen, T. Adolescent reading and math skills and self-concept beliefs as predictors of age 20 emotional well-being. Read. Writ. 2023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Wolff, F. A longitudinal experiment examining the joint effects of social, dimensional, and temporal comparisons on self- and external assessments of students’ abilities. Soc. Psychol. Educ. 2023, 26, 1723–1758. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Wolff, F.; Möller, J. An individual participant data meta-analysis of the joint effects of social, dimensional, and temporal comparisons on students’ academic self-concepts. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2022, 34, 2569–2608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Van der Westhuizen, L.; Arens, A.K.; Greiff, S.; Fischbach, A. The generalized internal/external frame of reference model with academic self-concepts, interests, and anxieties in students from different language backgrounds. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2022, 68, 102037. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Steinmayr, R.; Spinath, B. The importance of motivation as a predictor of school achievement. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2009, 19, 80–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Susperreguy, M.I.; Davis-Kean, P.E.; Duckworth, K.; Chen, M. Self-concept predicts academic achievement across levels of the achievement distribution: Domain specificity for math and reading. Child Dev. 2018, 89, 2196–2214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Lotz, C.; Schneider, R.; Sparfeldt, J.R. Differential relevance of intelligence and motivation for grades and competence tests in mathematics. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2018, 65, 30–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Lauermann, F.; Meißner, A.; Steinmayr, R. Relative importance of intelligence and ability self-concept in predicting test performance and school grades in the math and language arts domains. J. Educ. Psychol. 2020, 112, 364–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Marsh, H.W. Academic self-concept: Theory, measurement, and research. In The Self in Social Perspective; Suls, J.M., Ed.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1993; pp. 59–98. [Google Scholar]
  55. LaRoche, S.; Foy, P. Sample implementation in TIMSS 2019. In Methods and Procedures: TIMSS 2019 Technical Report; Martin, M.O., von Davier, I., Mullis, V.S., Eds.; TIMMS & PIRLS International Study Center, Boston College and International Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement (IEA): Chestnut Hill, MA, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  56. Ustun, U. How well does self-concept predict science achievement across cultures? The mediating effect of autonomous motivation. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2023, 45, 541–570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Bandura, A. Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol. Rev. 1977, 84, 191–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Bandura, A. Self-Efficacy. The Exercise of Control; Freeman & Company: New York, NY, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  59. Bong, M.; Skaalvik, E.M. Academic self-concept and self-efficacy: How different are they really? Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2003, 15, 1–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Köller, O.; Möller, J. Selbstwirksamkeit [Self-efficacy]. In Handwörterbuch Pädagogische Psychologie, 5th ed.; Rost, D.H., Sparfeldt, J.R., Buch, S.R., Eds.; Beltz: Weinheim, Germany, 1998; pp. 757–763. [Google Scholar]
  61. McCrae, R.R.; Costa, P.T., Jr. Personality in Adulthood: A Five-Factor Theory Perspective, 2nd ed.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. McCrae, R.R.; Costa, P.T., Jr. The five-factor theory of personality. In Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research; John, O.P., Robins, R.W., Pervin, A., Eds.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 159–181. [Google Scholar]
  63. Kim, L.E.; Poropat, A.E.; MacCann, C. Conscientiousness in education: Its conceptualization, assessment, and utility. In Psychosocial Skills and School Systems in the 21st Century: Theory, Research, and Practice; Lipnevich, A.A., Preckel, F., Roberts, R.D., Eds.; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2016; pp. 155–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Spielmann, J.; Yoon, H.J.R.; Ayoub, M.; Chen, Y.; Eckland, N.S.; Trautwein, U.; Zheng, A.; Roberts, B.W. An in-depth review of conscientiousness and educational issues. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2022, 34, 2745–2781. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Hessen, R.P.; Kuncel, N.R. Beyond grades: A meta-analysis of personality predictors of academic behavior in middle school and high school. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2022, 199, 111809. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. O’Connor, M.C.; Paunonen, S.V. Big five personality predictors of post-secondary academic performance. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2007, 43, 971–990. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Zell, E.; Lesick, T.L. Big five personality traits and performance: A quantitative synthesis of 50+ meta-analyses. J. Personal. 2022, 90, 559–573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Meyer, J.; Jansen, T.; Hübner, N.; Lüdtke, O. Disentangling the association between the big five personality traits and student achievement: Meta-analytic evidence on the role of domain specificity and achievement measures. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2023, 35, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Ivcevic, Z.; Brackett, M. Predicting school success: Comparing conscientiousness, grit, and emotion regulation ability. J. Res. Personal. 2014, 52, 29–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Noftle, E.E.; Robins, R.W. Personality predictors of academic outcomes: Big five c of GPA and SAT Scores. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2007, 93, 116–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. O’Connell, M.; Grades, G.N. Cognitive ability and conscientiousness are more important than SES for educational attainment: An analysis of the UK millennium cohort study. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2022, 188, 111471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Bittmann, F. Are cognitive ability and conscientiousness really more important for educational attainment than SES? A replication and extension of O’Connell and Grades. Collabra Psychol. 2022, 8, 37460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Cacioppo, J.T.; Petty, R.E. The need for cognition. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1982, 42, 116–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Cacioppo, J.T.; Petty, R.E.; Kao, C.F. The efficient assessment of need for cognition. J. Personal. Assess. 1984, 48, 306–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Dickhäuser, O.; Reinhard, M.-A. How students build their performance expectancies: The importance of need for cognition. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 2010, 25, 399–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Petty, R.E.; Jarvis, W.B.G. An individual differences perspective on assessing cognitive processes. In Answering Questions: Methodology for Determining Cognitive and Communicative Processes in Survey Research; Schwarz, N., Sudman, S., Eds.; Jossey-Bass: San Francisco, CA, USA, 1996; pp. 221–257. [Google Scholar]
  77. Stenlund, T.; Jönsson, F.U.; Jonsson, B. Group discussions and test-enhanced learning: Individual learning outcomes and personality characteristics. Educ. Psychol. 2017, 37, 145–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Weissgerber, S.C.; Reinhard, M.-A.; Schindler, S. Learning the hard way: Need for cognition influences attitudes toward and self-reported use of desirable learning difficulties. Educ. Psychol. 2018, 38, 176–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Liu, Q.; Nesbit, J.C. The relation between need for cognition and academic achievement: A meta-analysis. Rev. Educ. Res. 2023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Von Stumm, S.; Ackerman, P.L. Investment and intellect: A review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 2013, 139, 841–869. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Colling, J.; Wollschläger, R.; Keller, U.; Preckel, F.; Fischbach, A. Need for Cognition and its relation to academic achievement in different learning environments. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2022, 93, 102110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Keller, U.; Strobel, A.; Martin, R.; Preckel, F. Domain-specificity of need for cognition among high school students. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2019, 35, 607–616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Duckworth, A.L.; Peterson, C.; Matthews, M.D.; Kelley, D.R. Grit: Perseverance and passion for long term goals. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2007, 92, 1087–1101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Duckworth, A.L.; Quinn, P.D. Development and validation of the Short Grit Scale (GRIT–S). J. Personal. Assess. 2009, 91, 166–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Eskreis-Winkler, L.; Shulman, E.P.; Beal, S.A.; Duckwrth, A.L. The grit effect: Predicting retention in the military, the workplace, school and marriage. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Duckworth, A.L. Grit. The Power of Passion and Perseverance; Scribner: New York, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  87. Duckworth, A.L. Grit–the Power of Passion and Perseverance. TED Talks Education 2013. Available online: https://www.ted.com/talks/angela_lee_duckworth_grit_the_power_of_passion_and_perseverance (accessed on 9 September 2023).
  88. Deary, I.J.; Strand, S.; Smith, P.; Fernandes, C. Intelligence and educational achievement. Intelligence 2007, 35, 13–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Gottfredson, L.S. Where and why g matters: Not a mystery. Hum. Perform. 2002, 15, 25–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Jensen, A.R. The G Factor: The Science of Mental Ability; Praeger: Westport, CT, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  91. Lozano-Blasco, R.; Quílez-Robres, A.; Usán, P.; Salavera, C.; Casanovas-López, R. Types of intelligence and academic performance: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Intell. 2022, 10, 123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  92. Neubauer, A.C.; Opriessnig, S. The development of talent and excellence. Do not dismiss psychometric intelligence, the (potentially) most powerful predictor. Talent. Dev. Excell. 2014, 6, 1–15. [Google Scholar]
  93. Rost, D.H. Intelligenz: Fakten und Mythen [Intelligence: Facts and Myths]; Beltz: Weinheim, Germany, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  94. Rost, D.H. Handbuch Intelligenz [Handbook Intelligence]; Beltz: Weinheim, Germany, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  95. Roth, B.; Becker, N.; Romeyke, S.; Schäfer, S.; Domnick, F.; Spinath, F.M. Intelligence and school grades: A meta-analysis. Intelligence 2015, 53, 118–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Süß, H.-M. Prädiktive Validität der Intelligenz im schulischen und außerschulischen Bereich [Predictive validity of intelligence in and out of school]. In Perspektiven der Intelligenzforschung; Stern, E., Guthke, J., Eds.; Pabst: Lengerich, Germany, 2001; pp. 109–135. [Google Scholar]
  97. Miller, M.S. Book Review: Grit by Angela Duckworth. Tabletalk. 1 August 2018. Available online: https://tabletalkmagazine.com/posts/book-review-grit-by-angela-duckworth/ (accessed on 8 September 2023).
  98. Shechtman, N.; deBarger, A.H.; Dornsife, C.; Rosier, S.; Yarnall, L. Promoting Grit, Tenacity, and Perseverance: Critical Factors for Success in the 21st Century; U.S. Department of Education, Office of Educational Technology: Menlo Park, CA, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  99. Van Zyl, L.E.; Olckers, C.; van der Vaart, L. (Eds.) Multidisciplinary Perspectives on Grit; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Fleckenstein, J.; Schmidt, F.T.C.; Möller, J. Wer hat Biss? Beharrlichkeit und beständiges Interesse von Lehramtsstudierenden. Eine deutsche Adaptation der 12-ltem Grit Scale [Who’s got grit? Perseverance and consistency of interest in pre-service teachers. A German adaptation of the 12-item grit scale]. Psychol. Erzieh. Unterr. 2014, 61, 281–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Li, J.; Zhao, Y.; Kong, F.; Du, S.; Yang, S.; Wang, S. Psychometric assessment of the Short Grit Scale among Chinese adolescents. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 2018, 36, 291–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Schmidt, F.T.C.; Fleckenstein, J.; Retelsdorf, J.; Eskreis-Winkler, L.; Möller, J. Measuring grit: A German validation and a domain-specific approach to grit. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2019, 35, 436–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Schmidt, F.T.C.; Sudzina, F.; Botek, M. Psychometric assessment of the Short Grit Scale among Czech young adults. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 2021, 39, 508–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Sordia, N.; Martskvishvili, K. Psychometric properties of the Georgian version of the Grit Scale. Prizren Soc. Sci. J. 2020, 4, 8–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Sudina, E.; Brown, J.; Datzman, B.; Oki, Y.; Song, K.; Cavanaugh, R.; Thiruchelvam, B.; Plonsky, L. Language-specific grit: Exploring psychometric properties, predictive validity, and differences across contexts. Innov. Lang. Learn. Teach. 2021, 15, 334–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Credé, M.; Tynan, M.C.; Harms, P.D. Much ado about grit: A meta-analytic synthesis of the grit literature. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2017, 113, 492–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  107. Credé, M. What shall we do about grit? A critical review of what we know and what we don’t know. Educ. Res. 2018, 47, 606–611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Credé, M.; Harms, P.D. 25 years of higher order confirmatory factor analysis in the organizational sciences: A critical review and development of reporting recommendations. J. Organ. Behav. 2015, 36, 845–872. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Credé, M.; Tynan, M.C. Should language acquisition researchers study “grit”? A cautionary note and some suggestions. J. Psychol. Lang. Learn. 2021, 3, 37–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Duckworth, A.L.; Quinn, P.D.; Tsukayama, E. Revisiting the factor structure of grit: A commentary on Duckworth and Quinn (2009). J. Personal. Assess. 2021, 103, 573–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Feng, X. Academic Self-Cognitions and Achievements: A Study of Students of German Studies; Science Press: Beijing, China, 2019. (In Chinese) [Google Scholar]
  112. Feng, X.; Rost, D.H. Grit bei Adoleszenten: Eine „neue“ leistungsthematisch-motivationale Eigenschaft? [Adolescents’ grit: A “new” achievement-related motivational trait?]. Z. Pädagogische Psychol. 2019, 33, 241–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Larkin, P.; Cocić, D.; Hendry, D.T.; Williams, A.M.; O’Connor, D.; Bilalić, M. Gritting one’s way to success—Grit explains skill in elite youth soccer players beyond (deliberate) practice. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2023, 64, 102328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Midkiff, B.; Langer, M.; Demetriou, C.; Panter, A.T. Measuring grit among first-generation college students: A psychometric analysis. In Quantitative Psychology, Proceedings of the 81st Annual Meeting of the Psychometric Society, Asheville, NC, USA, 11–17 July 2016; van der Ark, A.L., Wiberg, M., Culpepper, S.A., Douglas, J.A., Wang, W.-C., Eds.; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2017; pp. 407–420. [Google Scholar]
  115. Muenks, K.; Wigfield, A.; Yang, J.S.; O’Neal, C.R. How true is grit? Assessing its relations to high school and college students’ personality characteristics, self-regulation, engagement, and achievement. J. Educ. Psychol. 2017, 109, 599–620. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Steinmayr, R.; Weidinger, A.F.; Wigfield, A. Does students’ grit predict their school achievement above and beyond their personality, motivation, and engagement? Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2018, 53, 106–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Tyumeneva, Y.; Kardanova, E.; Kuzmina, J. Grit: Two related but independent constructs instead of one. Evidence from item response theory. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2019, 35, 469–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Zhao, X.; Wang, D. Grit, emotions, and their effects on ethnic minority students’ English language learning achievements: A structural equation modeling analysis. System 2023, 13, 102979. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Zhong, C.; Wang, M.-C.; Shou, Y.; Ren, F.; Zhang, X.; Li, M.; Yang, W. Assessing construct validity of the Grit-S in Chinese employees. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0209319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  120. Teuber, Z.; Nussbeck, F.; Wild, E. The bright side of grit in burnout-prevention: Exploring grit in the context of demands-resources model among Chinese high school studente. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 2021, 55, 464–476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  121. Krapp, A. An educational-psychological conception of interest. Int. J. Educ. Vocat. Guid. 2007, 7, 5–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Krapp, A. Interesse [Interest]. In Handwörterbuch Pädagogische Psychologie, 5th ed.; Rost, D.H., Sparfeldt, J.R., Buch, S.R., Eds.; Beltz: Weinheim, Germany, 2018; pp. 750–756. [Google Scholar]
  123. Krapp, A.; Prenzel, M. Research on interest in science: Theories, methods, and findings. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2011, 33, 27–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  124. O’Keefe, P.A.; Harackiewicz, J.M. (Eds.) The Science of Interest; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  125. Lettau, W.D. Schulisches Interesse als Forschungsgegenstand. Über den theoretischen Anspruch und die empirische Umsetzung von Studien am Beispiel des Interesses [Academic Interest as an Object of Research. About the Theoretical Claim and the Empirical Implementation of Studies Using the Example of Interest]. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Rostock, Rostock, Germany, 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  126. Rounds, J.; Su, R. The nature and power of interests. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2014, 23, 98–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  127. Schiefele, U.; Krapp, A.; Winteler, A. Interest as a predictor of academic achievement: A meta-analysis of research. In The Role of Interest in Learning and Development; Renninger, K.A., Hidi, S., Krapp, A., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1992; pp. 183–212. [Google Scholar]
  128. Jansen, M.; Lüdtke, O.; Schroeders, U. Evidence for a positive relation between interest and achievement: Examining between-person and within-person variation in five domains. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2016, 46, 116–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Ponnock, A.; Muenks, K.; Morell, M.; Seung Yang, J.; Gladstone, J.R.; Wigfield, A. Grit and conscientiousness: Another jangle fallacy. J. Res. Personal. 2020, 89, 104021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  130. Schmidt, F.T.C.; Nagy, G.; Fleckenstein, J.; Möller, J.; Retelsdorf, J. Same same, but different? Relations between facets of conscientiousness and grit. Eur. J. Personal. 2018, 32, 705–720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  131. Wang, R.; Shirvan, M.E.; Taherian, T. Perseverance of effort and consistency of interest: A longitudinal perspective. Front. Psychol. 2023, 12, 743414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  132. Andronie, M.; Wildemann, A. Need for Cognition, subjektives Wohlbefinden und akademisches Selbstkonzept bei Grundschulkindern [Need for cognition, subjective well-being, and academic self-concept in elementary school children]. In Grundschulpädagogik zwischen Wissenschaft und Transfer; Donie, C., Foerster, F.F., Obermeyer, M., Deckwerth, A., Kammermeyer, K., Lenske, G., Leuchter, M., Wildeman, A., Eds.; Springer VS.: Wiesbaden, Germany, 2019; pp. 219–226. [Google Scholar]
  133. Fleischhauer, M.; Enge, S.; Brocke, B.; Ullrich, J.; Strobel, A.; Strobel, A. Same or different? Clarifying the relationship of need for cognition to personality and intelligence. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2009, 36, 82–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  134. Hu, H. The need for cognition as it relates to personality traits of openness to experience and conscientiousness. BCP Educ. Psychol. 2022, 7, 241244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  135. Korhonen, J.; Tapola, A.; Linnanmäki, K.; Aunio, P. Gendered pathways to educational aspirations: The role of academic self-concept, school burnout, achievement and interest in mathematics and reading. Learn. Instr. 2016, 46, 21–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  136. Lohbeck, A.; Nitkowski, D.; Petermann, F. A control-value theory approach: Relationships between academic self-concept, interest, and test anxiety in elementary school children. Child Youth Care Forum 2016, 45, 887–904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  137. Rösler, L.; Retelsdorf, J. Entwicklung und Determinanten von Interesse und Selbstkonzept im Studienverlauf [Development and determinants of interest and self-concept in the course of study]. In PaLea: Professionelle Kompetenzen und Studienstrukturen im Lehramtsstudium; Kauper, T., Bernholt, A., Möller, J., Köller, O., Eds.; Waxmann: Münster, Germany, 2023; pp. 205–226. [Google Scholar]
  138. Haag, L.; Götz, T. Mathe ist schwierig und Deutsch aktuell: Vergleichende Studie zur Charakterisierung von Schulfächern aus Schülersicht [Maths is difficult and German up to date: A study on the characterization of subject domains from students’ perspective]. Psychol. Erzieh. Unterr. 2012, 59, 32–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  139. Stevens, R.; Wineburg, S.; Herrenkohl, L.R.; Bell, P. Comparative understanding of school subjects: Past, present, and future. Rev. Educ. Res. 2005, 75, 125–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  140. Bong, M. Between- and within-domain relations of academic motivation among middle and high school students: Self-efficacy, task value, and achievement goals. J. Educ. Psychol. 2001, 93, 23–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  141. Green, J.; Martin, A.J.; Marsh, H.W. Motivation and engagement in English, mathematics and science high school subjects: Towards an understanding of multidimensional domain specificity. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2007, 17, 269–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  142. Sparfeldt, J.R.; Brunnemann, N.; Wirthwein, L.; Buch, S.R.; Schult, J.; Rost, D.H. General versus specific achievement goals: A re-examination. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2015, 43, 170–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  143. Tett, R.P.; Steele, J.R.; Beauregard, R.S. Broad and narrow measures on both sides of the personality-job performance relationship. J. Organ. Behav. 2003, 24, 335–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  144. Wittmann, W.W. Multivariate reliability theory: Principles of symmetry and successful validation strategies. In Handbook of Multivariate Experimental Psychology, 2nd ed.; Nesselroade, J.R., Cattell, R.B., Eds.; Plenum Press: New York, NY, USA, 1988; pp. 505–560. [Google Scholar]
  145. Wittmann, W.W. Brunswik-Symmetrie: Ein Schlüsselkonzept für erfolgreiche psychologische Forschung [Brunswik-symmetry: A key concept for successful psychological research]. In Die Person im Biologischen und Sozialen Kontext; Myrtek, M., Ed.; Hogrefe: Göttingen, Germany, 2002; pp. 163–186. [Google Scholar]
  146. Sticca, F.; Goetz, T.; Möller, J.; Eberle, F.; Murayama, K.; Shavelson, R. Same same but different: The role of subjective domain similarity in the longitudinal interplay among achievement and self-concept in multiple academic domains. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2023, 102, 102270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  147. Sparfeldt, J.R.; Rost, D.H. Content-specific achievement motives. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2011, 50, 496–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  148. Mächel, L.; Steinmayr, R.; Christiansen, H.; Wirthwein, L. On the association between students’ (domain-specific) subjective well-being and academic achievement–disentangling mixed findings. Curr. Psychol. 2022, 42, 30825–30839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  149. Talić, I.; Sparfeldt, J.R.; Möller, J.; Renner, K.-H.; Greiff, S.; Niepel, C. Social and dimensional comparison effects in general and domain-specific test anxiety: A nested factor modeling approach. Curr. Psychol. 2023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  150. Slutzky, C.B.; Simpkins, S.D. The link between children’s sport Participation and self-esteem: Exploring the mediating role of sport self-concept. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2009, 10, 381–389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  151. Tietjens, M. Physisches Selbstkonzept im Sport [Physical Self-Concept in Sport]; Czwalina: Hamburg, Germany, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  152. Pfeiffer, W. Musikalisches Selbstkonzept. Eine empirische Untersuchung [Musical self-concept. An empirical study]. Diskuss. Musik. 2006, 29, 53–57. [Google Scholar]
  153. Pfeiffer, W. Das musikalische Selbstkonzept. Effekte und Wirkungen [The Musical Self-concept. Effects and Impacts]. Diskuss. Musik. 2007, 33, 40–44. [Google Scholar]
  154. Fiedler, D.; Spychiger, M. Measuring “musical self-concept” throughout the years of adolescence with MUSCI_youth: Validation and adjustment of the Musical Self-Concept Inquiry (MUSCI) by investigating samples of students at secondary education schools. Psychomusicol. Music. Mind Brain 2017, 27, 167–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  155. Spychiger, M. From musical experience to musical identity: Musical self-concept as a mediating psychological structure. In Handbook of Musical Identities; MacDonald, R., Hargreaves, D.J., Miell, D., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2017; pp. 267–287. [Google Scholar]
  156. Rudi, H.; Steinberg, C. Das tänzerische Selbstkonzept bei Kindern. Empirische Annäherungen an ein bisher vernachlässigtes Konstrukt [The Dance Self-concept in Children. Empirical Approaches to a Previously Neglected Construct]. In Bewegung, Spiel und Sport im Kindesalter; Schwier, J., Seyda, M., Eds.; Transcript: Bielefeld, Germany, 2022; pp. 157–168. [Google Scholar]
  157. Burleson, K.; Leach, C.W.; Harrington, D.M. Upward social comparison and self-concept: Inspiration and inferiority among art students in an advanced programme. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2005, 44, 109–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  158. Asendorpf, J.B.; van Aken, M.A.G. Validity of big five personality judgements in childhood: A 9 year longitudinal study. Eur. J. Personal. 2003, 17, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  159. DeYoung, C.G.; Quilty, L.C.; Peterson, J.B. Between facets and domains: 10 aspects of the big five. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2007, 93, 880–896. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  160. Franzen, P.; Arens, A.K.; Greiff, S.; van der Westhuizen, L.; Fischbach, A.; Wollschläger, R.; Niepel, C. Developing and validating a short-form questionnaire for the assessment of seven facets of conscientiousness in large-scale assessments. J. Personal. Assess. 2022, 104, 759–773. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  161. Laros, J.A.; Peres, A.J.S.; de Andrade, J.M.; Passos, M.F.D. Validity evidence of two short scales measuring the big five personality factors. Psicol. Reflex. Crít. 2018, 31, 32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  162. MacCann, C.; Duckworth, A.L.; Roberts, R.D. Empirical identification of the major facets of conscientiousness. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2009, 19, 451–458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  163. Rammstedt, B.; John, O.P. Kurzversion des Big Five Inventory (BFI-K): Entwicklung und Validierung eines ökonomischen Inventars zur Erfassung der fünf Faktoren der Persönlichkeit [Short-version of the Big Five Inventory (BFI-K): Development and validation of an economic measure to assess the big five personality dimensions]. Diagnostica 2005, 51, 195–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  164. Satow, L. Big-Five-Persönlichkeitstest (B5T). Test-und Skalendokumentation 2012 [Big-Five-Personality Test (B5T). Test and Scale Documentation 2012]. Available online: https://www.drsatow.de (accessed on 18 November 2023).
  165. Rubinstein, S. Das Interesse [The interest]. In Die Motivation Menschlichen Handelns, 6th ed.; Thomae, H., Ed.; Kiepenheuer & Witsch: Köln, Germany, 1970; pp. 137–144. [Google Scholar]
  166. Gogol, K.; Brunner, M.; Martin, R.; Preckel, F.; Goetz, T. Affect and motivation within and between school subjects: Development and validation of an integrative structural model of academic self-concept, interest, and anxiety. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2017, 49, 46–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  167. Katz, M.R.; Norris, L.; Halpern, G. The Measurement of Academic Interests, Part 1. Characteristics of the Academic Interests Measures; Educational Testing Service: Princeton, NJ, USA, 1970; Available online: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdf/10.1002/j.2333-8504.1970.tb00786.x (accessed on 10 September 2023).
  168. Lu, Z.; Dang, Y.; Xu, W. Academic interest scale for adolescents: Development, validation, and measurement invariance with Chinese students. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 2301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  169. Sparfeldt, J.R.; Rost, D.H.; Schilling, S.R. Schulfachspezifische Interessen—Ökonomisch gemessen [Subject-specific interests–measured economically]. Psychol. Erzieh. Unterr. 2004, 51, 213–220. [Google Scholar]
  170. Todt, E. Das Interesse [The Interest]; Huber: Bern, Switzerland, 1978. [Google Scholar]
  171. Botes, E.; Azari Noughabi, M.; Amirian, S.M.R.; Greiff, S. New wine in new bottles: L2 grit in comparison to domain-general grit, conscientiousness, and cognitive ability as a predictor of language learning. PsyArXiv 2023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  172. Mikami, H. Revalidation of the L2-Grit Scale: A conceptual replication of Teimouri, Y., Plonsky, L., & Tabandeh, F. L2 grit: Passion and perseverance for second-language learning. Lang. Teach. 2023, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  173. Oxford, R.; Khajavy, G.H. Exploring grit: “Grit linguistics” and research on domain-general grit and L2 grit. J. Psychol. Lang. Learn. 2021, 3, 7–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  174. Paradowski, M.B.; Jelińska, M. The predictors of L2 grit and their complex interactions in online foreign language earning: Motivation, self-directed learning, autonomy, curiosity, and language mindsets. Comput. Assist. Lang. Learn. 2023, 1–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  175. Teimouri, Y.; Plonsky, L.; Tabandeh, F. L2 grit: Passion and perseverance for second language learning. Lang. Teach. Res. 2022, 26, 893–918. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  176. Teimouri, Y.; Sudina, E.; Plonsky, L. On domain-specific conceptualization and measurement of grit in L2 learning. J. Psychol. Lang. Learn. 2021, 3, 156–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  177. Yu, Y.; Hua, L.; Feng, X.; Wang, Y.; Yu, Z.; Zi, T.; Zhao, Y.; Li, J. True grit in learning math: The math anxiety-achievement link is mediated by math-specific grit. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 645793. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  178. Mosewich, A.D.; Dunn, J.G.H.; Causgrove Dunn, J.; Wright, K.S. Domain-specific grit, identity, and self-compassion in intercollegiate athletes. Sport Exerc. Perform. Psychol. 2021, 10, 257–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  179. Rumbold, J.L.; Dunn, J.G.H.; Olusoga, P. Examining the predictive validity of the Grit Scale-Short (Grit-S) using domain-general and domain-specific approaches with student-athletes. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 837321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  180. Pechtl, H. Anmerkungen zur Operationalisierung und Messung des Konstrukts ‘Need for Cognition’ [Notes on the Operationalization and Measurement of the Construct ‘Need for Cognition’]; Wirtschaftswissenschaftliche Diskussionspapiere; Universität Greifswald: Greifswald, Germany, 2009; Available online: https://www.econstor.eu/handle/10419/41073 (accessed on 7 September 2023).
  181. Schuler, H.; Funke, U.; Baron-Boldt, J. Predictive validity of school grades. A meta-analysis. Appl. Psychol. 1990, 39, 89–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  182. Schuler, H.; Schult, J. Prädiktoren von Studien- und Berufserfolg [Predictors of academic and career success]. In Handwörterbuch Pädagogische Psychologie, 5th ed.; Rost, D.H., Sparfeldt, J.R., Buch, S.R., Eds.; Beltz: Weinheim, Germany, 2018; pp. 645–653. [Google Scholar]
  183. Tent, L.; Birkel, P. Zensuren [School grades]. In Handwörterbuch Pädagogische Psychologie, 4th ed.; Rost, D.H., Ed.; Beltz: Weinheim, Germany, 2010; pp. 949–958. [Google Scholar]
  184. Sparfeldt, J.R.; Buch, S.R.; Wirthwein, L.; Rost, D.H. Zielorientierungen: Zur Relevanz der Schulfächer [Goal orientations: On the relevance of school subjects]. Z. Entwicklungspsychol. Pädagogische Psychol. 2007, 39, 165–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  185. Sparfeldt, J.R.; Schilling, S.R.; Rost, D.H.; Stelzl, I.; Peipert, D. Leistungsängstlichkeit: Facetten, Fächer, Fachfacetten? Zur Trennbarkeit nach Angstfacette und Inhaltsbereich [Test anxiety: The relevance of anxiety facets as well as school subjects]. Z. Pädagogische Psychol. 2005, 19, 225–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  186. Baudson, T.G.; Jung, N.; Freund, P.A. Measurement invariance in a grid-based measure of academic self-concept. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2017, 33, 467–470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  187. Schneider, R.; Sparfeldt, J.R.; Niepel, C.; Buch, S.R.; Rost, D.H. Measurement invariance of test anxiety across four school subjects. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2022, 38, 356–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  188. Brislin, R.W. Back-translation for cross-cultural research. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 1970, 1, 185–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  189. Jones, P.S.; Lee, J.W.; Phillips, L.R.; Zhang, X.E.; Jaceldo, K.B. An adaptation of Brislin’s translation model for cross-cultural research. Nurs. Res. 2001, 50, 300–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  190. Buch, S.R.; Sparfeldt, J.R.; Rost, D.H. Diagnostik schulfachspezifischer Selbstkonzepte. Differentielles Schulisches Selbstkonzept-Gitter (DISK-Gitter) [Diagnostics of school subject-specific self-concepts. Differential Academic Self-Concept Grid (DISK Grid)]. In Diagnostik und Förderung von Motivation und Volition; Gaspard, H., Trautwein, U., Hasselhorn, M., Eds.; Hogrefe: Göttingen, Germany, 2019; pp. 69–86. [Google Scholar]
  191. Preckel, F. Assessing need for cognition in early adolescence: Validation of a German adaption of the Cacioppo/Petty scale. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2014, 30, 65–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  192. Enders, C.K.; Tofighi, D. Centering predictor variables in cross-sectional multilevel models: A new look at an old issue. Psychol. Methods 2007, 12, 121–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  193. Fisher, R. The correlation between relatives on the supposition of mendelian inheritance. Earth Environ. Sci. Trans. R. Soc. Edinb. 1919, 52, 399–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  194. Silver, N.C.; Dunlap, W.P. Averaging correlation coefficients: Should Fisher’s z transformation be used? J. Appl. Psychol. 1987, 72, 146–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  195. Koran, J. Preliminary proactive sample size determination for confirmatory factor analysis models. Meas. Eval. Couns. Dev. 2016, 49, 296–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  196. Koran, J. Indicators per factor in confirmatory factor analysis: More is not always better. Struct. Equ. Model. A Multidiscip. J. 2020, 27, 765–772. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  197. Mat Roni, S.; Djajadikerta, H.G. Data Analysis with SPSS for Survey-Based Research; Springer: Singapore, 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  198. Knofczynski, G.T.; Mundfrom, D. Sample sizes when using multiple linear regression for prediction. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 2008, 68, 431–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  199. Kelley, K.; Anderson, S.F.; Maxwell, S.E. Sample-size planning. In APA Handbook of Research Methods in Psychology: Foundations, Planning, Measures, and Psychometrics; Cooper, H., Coutanche, M.N., McMullen, L.M., Panter, A.T., Rindskopf, D., Sher, K.J., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2023; pp. 189–209. [Google Scholar]
  200. Jobst, L.J.; Bader, M.; Moshagen, M. A tutorial on assessing statistical power and determining sample size for structural equation models. Psychol. Methods 2023, 28, 207–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  201. Urban, D.; Mayerl, J. Angewandte Regressionsanalyse: Theorie, Technik und Praxis [Applied Regression Analysis: Theory, Technique, and Practice], 4th ed.; Springer: Wiesbaden, Germany, 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  202. Rost, D.H. Interpretation und Bewertung Pädagogischer und Psychologischer Studien. Eine Einführung [Interpretation and Evaluation of Educational and Psychological Studies. An Introduction], 4th ed.; Klinkhardt: Bad Heilbrunn, Germany, 2023. [Google Scholar]
  203. Hooper, D.; Coughlan, J.; Mullen, M.R. Structural equation modelling: Guidelines for determining model fit. Electron. J. Bus. Res. Methods 2008, 6, 53–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  204. Hu, L.-t.; Bentler, P.M. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. 1999, 6, 1–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  205. Kline, R.B. Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling, 5th ed.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2023. [Google Scholar]
  206. Goodboy, A.K.; Kline, R.B. Statistical and practical concerns with published communication research featuring structural equation modeling. Commun. Res. Rep. 2017, 34, 68–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  207. Marsh, H.W.; Hau, K.-T.; Grayson, D. Goodness of fit evaluation in structural equation modeling. In Contemporary Psychometrics. A Festschrift for Roderick P. McDonald; Maydeu-Olivares, A., McArdle, J., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2005; pp. 275–340. [Google Scholar]
  208. Marsh, H.W.; Hau, K.-T.; Wen, Z. In search of golden rules: Comment on hypothesis-testing approaches to setting cutoff values for fit indexes and dangers in overgeneralizing Hu and Bentler’s (1999) findings. Struct. Equ. Model. 2004, 11, 320–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  209. Bühner, M. Einführung in Die Test- und Fragebogenkonstruktion [Introduction to Test and Questionnaire Construction], 4th ed.; Pearson: München, Germany, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  210. Schermelleh-Engel, K.; Moosbrugger, H.; Müller, H. Evaluating the fit of structural equation models: Tests of significance and descriptive goodness-of-fit measures. Methods Psychol. Res. Online 2003, 8, 23–74. [Google Scholar]
  211. Akaike, H. A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Trans. Autom. Control. 1974, 19, 716–723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  212. Akaike, H. Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika 1987, 52, 317–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  213. Cavanaugh, J.E.; Neah, A.A. The Akaike information criterion: Background, derivation, propertis, application, interpretation, and refinements. WIREs Comput. Stat. 2019, 11, e1460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  214. Hau, K.-T.; Ho, I.T. Chinese students’ motivation and achievement. In The Oxford Handbook of Chinese Psychology; Bond, M.H., Ed.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2010; pp. 187–204. [Google Scholar]
  215. Baron, S.; Yin-Baron, G. Die Chinesen. Programm einer Weltmacht [The Chinese. Program of a World Power]; Ullstein: München, Germany, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  216. Reeve, J. The interest-enjoyment distinction in intrinsic motivation. Motiv. Emot. 1989, 13, 83–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  217. Krapp, A. Intrinsische Lernmotivation und Interesse. Forschungsansätze und konzeptuelle Überlegungen [Intrinsic learning motivation and interest. Research approaches and conceptual thoughts]. Z. Pädagogik 1999, 45, 387–406. [Google Scholar]
  218. Weber, K. The relationship of interest to internal and external motivation. Commun. Res. Rep. 2003, 20, 376–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  219. Renninger, K.A. Individual interest and its implications for understanding intrinsic motivation. In Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation: The Search for Optimal Motivation and Performance; Sansone, C., Harackiewicz, J.M., Eds.; Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 2000; pp. 373–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  220. Schiefele, U.; Köller, O.; Schaffner, E. Intrinsische und extrinsische Motivation [Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation]. In Handwörterbuch Pädagogische Psychologie, 5th ed.; Rost, D.H., Sparfeldt, J.R., Buch, S.R., Eds.; Beltz: Weinheim, Germany, 2018; pp. 309–319. [Google Scholar]
  221. Schiefele, U.; Schreyer, I. Intrinsische Lernmotivation und Lernen. Ein Überblick zu Ergebnissen der Forschung [Intrinsic motivation to learn and learning: A review of recent research findings]. Z. Pädagogische Psychol. 1994, 8, 1–13. [Google Scholar]
  222. Klauer, K.J.; Sparfeldt, J.R. Intelligenz und Begabung. [Intelligence and giftedness]. In Handwörterbuch Pädagogische Psychologie, 5th ed.; Rost, D.H., Sparfeldt, J.R., Buch, S.R., Eds.; Beltz: Weinheim, Germany, 2018; pp. 278–286. [Google Scholar]
  223. Podsakoff, P.M.; MacKenzie, S.B.; Lee, J.-Y.; Podsakoff, N.P. Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 2003, 88, 879–903. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  224. Brannick, M.T.; Chan, D.; Conway, J.M.; Lance, C.E.; Spector, P.E. What is method variance and how can we cope with it? A panel discussion. Organ. Res. Methods 2010, 13, 407–420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Layout of the questionnaire.
Figure 1. Layout of the questionnaire.
Behavsci 14 00040 g001
Table 1. Factorial structures (three subject factors within each of the five self-related variables).
Table 1. Factorial structures (three subject factors within each of the five self-related variables).
χ2 (df)CFISRMRRMSEA [90% CI]AIC
Academic self-concept (ASC)
(18 items, 6 per subject)
  General factor5567.58 (135)0.350.190.23 [0.22, 0.23]5675
  Three subject factors:
  Chinese, mathematics, and English397.83 (114)0.970.060.06 [0.05, 0.06]547
Conscientiousness (CSN)
(24 items, 8 per subject)
  General factor7750.12 (252)0.380.130.19 [0.18, 0.19]7594
  Three subject factors:
  Chinese, mathematics, and English472.28 (225)0.980.050.04 [0.03, 0.04]670
Need for cognition (NFC)
(24 items, 8 per subject)
  General factor6938.08 (252)0.390.140.18 [0.18, 0.19]7082
Three subject factors:
  Chinese, mathematics, and English516.81 (225)0.970.050.05 [0.04, 0.05]714
Perseverance of effort (POE)
(12 items, 4 per subject)
  General factor2186.30 (54)0.530.130.22 [0.21, 0.23]2258
  Three subject factors:
  Chinese, mathematics, and English109.01 (39)0.990.040.05 [0.04, 0.06]212
Consistency of interest (COI)
(12 items, 4 per subject)
  General factor2129,49 (54)0.530.130.22 [0.21, 0.23]2201
  Three subject factors:
  Chinese, mathematics, and English75.45 (39)0.990.030.03 [0.02, 0.05]177
Notes. N = 791. All χ2: p < 0.001. Models with a general factor: maximal χ2/df = 41.24 (ASC); and minimal χ2/df = 27.53 (NFC). Models with three subject factors: maximal χ2/df = 3.49 (ASC); and minimal χ2/df = 1.93 (COI).
Table 2. Factorial structures (comprising five self-variables within each of three school subjects).
Table 2. Factorial structures (comprising five self-variables within each of three school subjects).
χ2 (df)CFISRMRRMSEA [90% CI]AIC
Subject: Chinese
  General factor2326.85 (405)0.780.070.08 [0.07, 0.09]2507
  Five factors: ASC, CSN, NFC, PER, and COI1254.36 (395)0.900.050.05 [0.05, 0.06]1454
Subject: Mathematics
  General factor2356.23 (405)0.790.060.08 [0.06, 0.08]2545
  Five factors: ASC, CSN, NFC, PER, and COI1236.28 (395)0.910.050.05 [0.05, 0.06]1483
Subject: English
  General factor2322.57 (405)0.810.060.08 [0.07, 0.08]2503
  Five factors: ASC, CSN, NFC, PER, and COI1377.68 (395)0.900.050.06 [0.05, 0.06]1578
Notes. N = 791. ASC = academic self-concept (6 items), CSN = conscientiousness (8 items), NFC = need for cognition (8 items), PER = perseverance of effort (4 items), and COI = consistency of interest (4 items). All χ2: p < 0.001. Models with a general factor: maximal χ2/df = 5.82 (mathematics); and minimal χ2/df = 5.73 (English). Models with three subject factors: maximal χ2/df = 3.49 (English); and minimal χ2/df = 3.13 (mathematics).
Table 3. Psychometric properties of five subject-specific self-scales (school subjects: Chinese, mathematics, and English).
Table 3. Psychometric properties of five subject-specific self-scales (school subjects: Chinese, mathematics, and English).
Min. Score aMax. Score aM aSD aMean rit bSkewness bKurtosis bα b
Academic self-concept c
  Chinese63619.355.970.640.24−0.340.84
  Mathematics63617.997.000.690.48−0.400.88
  English63817.617.160.690.30−0.590.88
Conscientiousness d
  Chinese84830.477.230.53−0.07−0.310.80
  Mathematics84830.667.980.52−0.11−0.510.80
  English84828.648.360.55−0.17−0.440.82
Need for cognition d
  Chinese84830.627.580.57−0.18−0.240.84
  Mathematics84831.448.340.58−0.15−0.470.84
  English84828.388.720.60−0.09−0.500.85
Perseverance of effort e
  Chinese42414.883.920.510.07−0.340.71
  Mathematics42414.664.530.52−0.05−0.610.73
  English42413.764.560.530.07−0.590.73
Consistency of interest e
  Chinese42414.304.260.45−0.10−0.300.66
  Mathematics42414.324.390.41−0.04−0.460.62
  English42414.114.340.36−0.18−0.410.58
Note. N = 791. rit = item discrimination index. a Raw scores, b items class-wise class converted into z-scores, c six items per scale, d eight items per scale, and e four items per scale.
Table 4. Intercorrelations (only decimals are given) of age, sex, subject-specific grades, and subject-specific self-scales (school subjects: Chinese, mathematics, and English).
Table 4. Intercorrelations (only decimals are given) of age, sex, subject-specific grades, and subject-specific self-scales (school subjects: Chinese, mathematics, and English).
1234567891011121314151617181920
1Age0400−01−04−0100−03040403−0101−01020302−01−03−02
2Sex 15−042811−161707−091115−112008−081203−0604
3Grade-C 152024−040413−000215−040214−000202−03−03
4Grade-M 12−0040−01−0225−01−0428−040128000013−01
5Grade-E 05−1153−04−133000−1337−02−112703−0414
6ASC-C 2834532125661828592227180810
7ASC-M 16165911207012246414082706
8ASC-E 160661200272230863070324
9CSN-C 6262682835764445241819
10CSN-M 50366724497734152913
11CSN-E 361772483478141528
12NFC-C 4651683533201215
13NFC-M 30326919072304
14NFC-E 382473090827
15POE-C 5858242124
16POE-M 42143214
17POE-E 131528
18COI-C 7369
19COI-M 68
20COI-E
Notes. N = 791 (correlations with grades: N = 779); scoring of sex: 0 = male and 1 = female. C = Chinese, E = English, and M = mathematics; ASC = academic self-concept, CSN = conscientiousness, COI = consistency of interest, NFC = need for cognition, and POE = perseverance of E = effort. ǀrǀ > 09: p < 0.01 and ǀrǀ > 12: p < 0.001 (two tailed).
Table 5. Hierarchical linear regression analyses. Statistical prediction of subject-specific academic achievements (grades in Chinese, mathematics, and English) by sex and age (model 1), by sex, age, and subject-specific academic self-concept (model 2), and by sex, age, subject-specific academic self-concept, subject-specific need for cognition, subject-specific perseverance of effort, and subject-specific consistency of interest (model 3).
Table 5. Hierarchical linear regression analyses. Statistical prediction of subject-specific academic achievements (grades in Chinese, mathematics, and English) by sex and age (model 1), by sex, age, and subject-specific academic self-concept (model 2), and by sex, age, subject-specific academic self-concept, subject-specific need for cognition, subject-specific perseverance of effort, and subject-specific consistency of interest (model 3).
Chinese GradeMathematics GradeEnglish Grade
Model 1βPR2R2p-ValueβpR2R2p-Valueβp-Value R2p-Value
Sex0.15<0.001 −0.040.283 0.28<0.001
Age−0.010.761 −0.010.862 −0.030.434
0.020.02<0.001 <0.01<0.010.556 0.080.08<0.001
Model 2
Sex0.13<0.001 0.030.402 0.20<0.001
Age−0.010.772 −0.010.783 −0.020.529
ASC0.22<0.001 0.40<0.001 0.50<0.001
0.070.05<0.001 0.160.16<0.001 0.320.24<0.001
Model 3
Sex0.13<0.001 0.030.413 0.20<0.001
Age−0.010.731 −0.010.777 −0.020.619
ASC0.24<0.001 0.38<0.001 0.56<0.001
CSN0.030.622 0.010.878 0.060.253
NFC−0.040.480 −0.020.687 −0.030.601
POE0.010.912 0.040.528 −0.130.011
COI−0.030.373 0.020.583 0.020.479
0.07<0.010.854 0.16<0.01 0.330.010.060
Notes. N = 779. ASC = academic self-concept, CSN = conscientiousness, NFC = need for cognition, POE = perseverance of effort, and COI = consistency of interest.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Rost, D.H.; Feng, X. Academic Self-Concept Wins the Race: The Prediction of Achievements in Three Major School Subjects by Five Subject-Specific Self-Related Variables. Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 40. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14010040

AMA Style

Rost DH, Feng X. Academic Self-Concept Wins the Race: The Prediction of Achievements in Three Major School Subjects by Five Subject-Specific Self-Related Variables. Behavioral Sciences. 2024; 14(1):40. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14010040

Chicago/Turabian Style

Rost, Detlef H., and Xiaoli Feng. 2024. "Academic Self-Concept Wins the Race: The Prediction of Achievements in Three Major School Subjects by Five Subject-Specific Self-Related Variables" Behavioral Sciences 14, no. 1: 40. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14010040

APA Style

Rost, D. H., & Feng, X. (2024). Academic Self-Concept Wins the Race: The Prediction of Achievements in Three Major School Subjects by Five Subject-Specific Self-Related Variables. Behavioral Sciences, 14(1), 40. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14010040

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop