Next Article in Journal
Self-Esteem and Risk Behaviours in Adolescents: A Systematic Review
Previous Article in Journal
Familiarity Determines Whether Accent Affects Attitudes and Behaviors of the Listener
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Effects of Developmental Timing on Cognitive and Behavioral Profiles in Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorder: Considerations for Education

by
Nerea Felgueras
*,
José María López-Díaz
* and
Inmaculada Garrote
High-Performance Research Group on Inclusive Education, People with Disabilities and Universal Accessibility (DIVERSIA), Department of Educational Sciences, Faculty of Education and Sport and Interdisciplinary Studies, King Juan Carlos University, 28933 Fuenlabrada, Spain
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2024, 14(6), 431; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14060431
Submission received: 1 May 2024 / Revised: 20 May 2024 / Accepted: 21 May 2024 / Published: 22 May 2024

Abstract

:
Associations and families demand the need to raise awareness of the implications in the cognitive and behavioral development of children with Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorder (FASD) that affect their learning and school participation. This study aims to generate a profile of executive and behavioral functioning in children and adolescents diagnosed with FASD. A probabilistic sampling by clusters (associations for individuals with FASD) is applied. The sample is composed of 66 families from three associations. The BRIEF-2 and SENA tests were administered to assess executive and behavioral functioning domains. Data analysis found that the executive and behavioral functioning profile of individuals with FASD varies with age, with greater impairment in middle and late adolescence. Likewise, the domain of executive functioning most affected in any of the developmental stages is working memory. Finally, cognitive impairment in the executive functioning domains has a direct impact on the social and adaptive functioning of people with FASD.

1. Introduction

Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorder (FASD) brings together a heterogeneity of conditions related to prenatal alcohol exposure associated with facial abnormalities; unusual growth and weight (considering established age parameters); and irregularities in brain structures and neurocognitive difficulties, including impaired intellectual capacity, learning difficulties, memory impairment, and deficits in visuospatial ability, executive functioning, and self-regulatory capacity. It is considered the leading non-genetic cause of completely preventable intellectual disability [1].
The literature surrounding this disorder calls for an accurate and universal detection and diagnostic system that enables early identification of its core manifestations [2,3]. Based on these arguments and considering the prevalence data in Spain, estimated at 22.2% cases of FASD per 10,000 inhabitants [4], as well as the lack of knowledge among healthcare and educational professionals regarding this disorder [5,6,7,8,9,10,11], it is essential to have a behavioral and cognitive profile that considers the developmental stage.
Therefore, research on FASD needs to progress towards evaluating the cognitive and behavioral domains affected by prenatal alcohol exposure, considering not only the clinical perspective but also the viewpoint of caregivers of individuals affected by this disorder [12]. Available evidence indicates that families and teachers are the best informants of the observable behaviors of children and adolescents with this diagnosis [13,14].
Regarding executive functions, these are defined as a multidimensional construct referring to interrelated abilities that enable a person to direct their behavior towards a goal [15]. The most representative impairments in executive functions in individuals with FASD are difficulties in organizational and planning abilities, concrete thinking, inhibition problems, understanding cause–effect relationships, following instructions, developing a plan of action with a predefined objective, making informed judgments, generalizing, and understanding abstract concepts [2]. At the same time, behavioral impairments are understood as a set of maladaptive behaviors depending on the specific circumstances, where the common element lies in difficulties with behavior, self-control, and emotion regulation [16]. The most probable behavioral impairments in FASD include difficulties with emotion regulation, behavioral manifestations of executive function dysregulation, attention deficit, hyperactivity, impulsivity, irritability, sleep disorders, deficits in social skills, and difficulties in adaptive behavior and social communication [2]. Likewise, the influence of gender on the expression of difficulties in executive and behavioral functioning in cases of comorbidity with ADHD should be considered [17]. In this sense, gender plays an important role in the profile of cognitive and behavioral functioning, with a greater deterioration in males [18].
The most significant implications of behavioral disorders are the transgression of people’s rights, as well as of established social norms, which often leads to repeated problems with justice and serious difficulties in adapting to the surrounding environment. Social ignorance about the implications of the behavioral alterations of this disorder in the autonomous life of the affected persons favors limitations in participation in society [19,20,21].
This study aims to create a profile of executive and behavioral functioning of the people with FASD according to developmental stage, gender, and diagnosis through the contributions of the family member who has the greatest opportunities for observation of the affected person.
The following working hypotheses are considered:
  • It is observed that the contribution of families through the BRIEF-2 and SENA assessments allows for the development of a cognitive and behavioral functioning pattern for children and adolescents with FASD.
  • It is contemplated that age is a factor that modulates significant differences in the neurocognitive and behavioral profiles of individuals affected by FASD.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Design

The Research Ethics Committee of the Universidad Rey Juan Carlos issued a favorable opinion in September 2020 on the adequacy of the planning and research design of this study, meeting the necessary ethical requirements in relation to the objectives of the research project. Furthermore, all participants in this study agreed to the requirements of this study and consented to participate in this study.
It is a quasi-experimental design, being the only viable alternative in the impossibility of conducting a random assignment research, which could hinder the control of all variables involved in families’ responses. Additionally, it is identified with a quantitative methodology. It is characterized by its objectivity and the use of tools that allow the precise measurement of specific psychological constructs, enabling the statistical treatment of the data and the generalization of the results to the population.
The method followed in the study is shown in the flow chart below (Figure 1).

2.2. Participants

The attainment of the primary research unit was carried out following a probabilistic cluster sampling approach, resulting in five conglomerate groups. Each group corresponds to one of the registered associations nationwide related to FASD and Fetal Alcohol Syndrome (FAS), regulated under the provisions of the Organic Law 1/2002, dated 22 March, regulating the Right of Association. The sample size for each of the conglomerates is distributed as follows: 250 cases registered in VISUAL TEAF (stands for FASD), 305 cases in AFASAF (stands for Association of Families Affected by Fetal Alcohol Syndrome), 80 cases in SAFGROUP (stands for FAS), 60 cases in Tolerancia-Cero, and 40 cases in Zero FAS (stands for FAS). Subsequently, three groups were selected according to random sampling rules: VISUAL TEAF, SAFGROUP, and AFASAF. The sample consists of a total of 66 families of children with FASD. Regarding the association of origin, 40.9% belong to AFASAF (n = 27), 27.3% to SAFGROUP (n = 18), and 31.8% (n = 21) are associated with VISUAL TEAF. The distribution of participants includes 57.6% men (n = 38) and 42.4% women (n = 28). The breakdown by age groups is as follows: 19.7% (n = 13) in the child group, 25.8% (n = 17) in the early adolescence group, 21.2% (n = 14) in the middle adolescence group, and 33.3% (n = 22) in the late adolescence group. There are 49 cases with a complete Fetal Alcohol Syndrome (FAS) diagnosis (n = 49; 74.24%), 11 cases of partial FAS (n = 11; 16.66%), and 6 cases of Alcohol-Related Neurodevelopmental Disorder (ARND) (n = 6; 9.09%). If attention is paid to the co-morbidity situation, there are six cases presenting a joint diagnosis with Attention Deficit and/or Hyperactivity Disorder (n = 6; 9.09%); four cases showing Specific Learning Disorder (n = 4; 6. 06%); four cases showing Conduct Disorder (n = 4; 6.06%); two cases presenting hypoacusis (n = 2; 3.03%); a single case presenting Specific Language Disorder (n = 1; 1.51%); and, finally, two cases presenting comorbidity with Autism Spectrum Disorder (n = 2; 3.03%). The rest of the co-participants did not present comorbidity with other disorders (n = 47; 71.21%). The diagnosis of all cases was made based on the criteria established by Hoyme [1]. Finally, 78.8% had a disability (n = 52), 48.5% were in a situation of dependency, and, lastly, all participants were adopted (n = 66; 100%).

2.3. Variables

On the one hand, the developmental stage or age, gender, and the diagnosis of FASD are considered independent variables. The developmental stage is organized into childhood (7–10 years), early adolescence (11–14 years), middle adolescence (15–17 years), and late adolescence (18–19 years). The gender variable distinguishes between men and women. The variable diagnosis of FASD includes complete FAS, partial FAS, and ARND.
On the other hand, the dependent variables are identified with each of the executive functioning domains of the BRIEF-2 scale: inhibition (INH), self-monitoring (SMO), flexibility (FLE), emotional control (EMC), initiative (INI), working memory (WM), planning and organization (PLA), task monitoring (TAS), and materials organization (ORG). Finally, the dependent variables are also associated with each of the domains assessed in the SENA test: depression (DEP), anxiety (ANS), social anxiety (SCA), somatic complaints (SOM), attention problems (ATE), hyperactivity–impulsivity (HIP), anger control problems (ANG), aggression (AGG), challenging behavior (CHA), antisocial behavior (ANT), substance use (SUB), eating behavior problems (EAT), unusual behavior (UNU), emotional regulation problems (REG), rigidity (RIG), isolation (ISO), social integration and competence (SOC), emotional intelligence (EMI), and study disposition (STU).

2.4. Measurement

In this study, the standardized test Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, Second Edition, hereinafter BRIEF-2, in its Spanish adaptation, was applied. In addition, the standardized neuropsychological test ‘Sistema de Evaluación de Niños y Adolescentes’ (SENA) is applied for the Spanish-speaking population. This test is applied because of its close parallelism with the diagnostic criteria included in the DSM-V [16] and its adequate psychometric properties. The administration of both tests uses a self-administered version aimed at families of children and adolescents with FASD.
The BRIEF-2 and SENA assessments are structured according to three age levels, which primarily correspond to the educational stages of Early Childhood Education, from 3 to 6 years old; Primary Education, from 6 to 12 years old; and finally, Compulsory Secondary Education, from 12 to 18 years old. However, they allow for some flexibility to adapt to the specific assessment needs based on the stage of the developmental cycle.
The administration of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function, Second Edition (BRIEF-2) provides a profile of impairment in different areas of executive function in children and adolescents aged 5 to 18 years, with an approximate administration time of 10 min. The BRIEF-2 consists of 63 items that are assessed on a Likert-type rating scale with three response options: never, sometimes, and frequently. Additionally, the System for Evaluating Children and Adolescents (SENA) is also administered. It evaluates various psychological problems frequently encountered in children and adolescents from a multidimensional perspective. The SENA family version for adolescents contains 154 items, using a Likert-type rating scale with five response options, where respondents are asked to select the frequency of a behavior. The SENA family version for the child population (6 to 12 years old) includes 129 items, utilizing the same rating scale. Both versions have an approximate administration time of 30 min. One aspect to highlight is that there is consistent evidence of the validity and reliability of the results provided by both tests. In addition, there is correlation between these two tests in studies of populations with neurodevelopmental disorders [13,14].

3. Statistical Methods and Results

To create the executive and behavioral functioning profile, the typical scores (T) obtained from the BRIEF-2 and SENA tests will be used for each variable. Additionally, data analysis is performed using the SPSS V27 software for Windows.
The normality of the independent variables (developmental stage and diagnosis) and the dependent variables of executive and behavioral functioning are assessed using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test (D) for the total sample (n ≥ 50) and the Shapiro–Wilk test (W) for groups with n ≤ 50. Regarding the sociodemographic variable of the developmental stage, it is determined that all subgroups of this variable do not follow the assumption of normality, as the significance level is ≤0.05 (D = 0.170, ptotal < 0.000; W = 0.821, pchildhood < 0.012; W = 0.874, pearly-adolescence < 0.025; W = 0.862, pmiddle-adolescence < 0.032; W = 0.628, plate-adolescence < 0.000). The diagnostic variable does not meet the assumption of normality in its distribution, with p < 0.000 (D = 0.448). Table 1 shows the normality study for the dependent variables.
To ascertain if there are significant differences in the variables of executive and behavioral functioning between the general population and the clinical population (FASD), Student’s t-test for independent samples is applied in cases where the assumption of normality is met. It is important to note that the general population mean is 50, with a standard deviation of 10, in both the BRIEF-2 test [14] and the SENA test [13]. Student’s test of executive and behavioral functioning is presented in Table 2.
If p < 0.05, the data are inconsistent with the hypothesis that the population mean value is as proposed. If the lower and upper bounds include the value zero, the sample data are compatible with the proposed population value, so the H0 is accepted, and vice versa.
Bivariate correlation analysis is used to determine whether there are differences according to age and diagnosis for each of the variables. It will allow us to find out whether there is an association between the variable age or diagnosis and each of the variables, as well as the strength and directionality of this correlation (bilateral probability) (Table 3 and Table 4). The program automatically calculates the level of significance from the 99% and 95% confidence interval, so that significant correlations at the 0.01 level will be identified with two asterisks (**) and significant correlations at the 0.05 level with one (*).
It is noted that age is a factor that intervenes in almost all the variables of executive control and behavioral functioning.
In addition, the diagnosis does not determine the level of executive functioning. However, it does correlate with the behavioral functioning variables attention problems, hyperactivity, unusual behavior, integration problems, and social competence and willingness to study.
This analysis is not sufficient to explain the nature of this correlation, so one-factor analysis of variance (ANOVA) is applied. It determines whether the population means of the independent variable (IV) and the dependent variable (DV) are equal. This test assumes the principle of homoscedasticity, or equality of variances, and the normality of distribution. The higher the F-statistic, the more the variables are related, i.e., DV values differ between VI groups. If the p is less than 0.05, the null hypothesis (H0) of equal means is rejected, concluding that not all compared population means are equal (Table 5 and Table 6).
In turn, the Kruskal–Wallis H-test is applied to determine whether there are significant differences in the executive and behavioral functioning variables according to the type of diagnosis (Table 7 and Table 8). If the critical level is less than 0.05, the null hypothesis of equality of population means is rejected and it can be concluded that diagnostic types differ on the cognitive and behavioral functioning variables.
Therefore, in the sample assessed there are no statistically significant differences in cognitive and behavioral functioning according to diagnosis.
Finally, differences in gender in the variable of executive and behavioral functioning will be tested by applying the non-parametric Kolmogorov–Smirnov test for independent samples (n ≤ 30) (Table 9).
In the executive functioning variables, no statistically significant differences were observed between men and women. However, this is not true for the variables working memory (p < 0.001) and task monitoring (p < 0.013), as they have a significance level of less than 0.05. On this occasion, the null hypothesis is retained in which the behavioral functioning variables belong to the same population, regardless of gender, except for the variables ATE (attention problems (p < 0.005)) and ISO (isolation (p < 0.011)), which reflect significant differences according to male or female sex.

4. Discussion

In the first instance, the BRIEF-2 neuropsychological test provides a measure of executive function assessment based on an ecological perspective [14]. There are three previous studies that apply the BRIEF-2 test for the assessment of all executive functions in the population affected by FASD in the United Kingdom and Canada, showing significant alterations in most of the domains assessed by the test, except in the domain “organization of materials” [12,18,22]. The results of this study follow a similar pattern to those obtained in different works [12,18,23]. That is, participants affected by FASD show significant differences in the executive functioning domains assessed (inhibition, self-monitoring, flexibility, emotional control, initiative, working memory, planning and organization, task monitoring, and organization of materials), with working memory being the most impaired variable and organization of materials the least impaired, although in some cases they continue to belong to the clinical population. However, we find that the interaction of the age variable is a moderating factor for executive functioning [23]. Thus, school-aged children, which in this study comprises ages seven to ten, show less marked impairment in the executive functions of inhibition, self-monitoring, cognitive flexibility, emotional control, working memory, planning and organization, and task monitoring compared to their peers in higher developmental stages (middle and late adolescence). The results of this study coincide with those obtained in the studies by [23], in which age is identified as a factor that determines the deterioration of executive functioning, which is more notable at higher ages, but which stabilizes in the adolescent period. This not only confirms the hypothesis that the age variable is a determinant of the level of functioning of the different domains of executive functions at a general level, but also achieves one of the objectives of this study: the verification of significant differences in EF motivated by the evolutionary moment of the person affected by FASD. Finally, studies by several authors ([19,24,25], among others) analyze the comorbidity between FASD and ADHD and reveal that cases with a comorbid diagnosis with ADHD compared to a single diagnosis of FASD show higher scores in all domains of executive functions assessed by the BRIEF test, with the variables working memory and inhibition being the most affected. Thus, the presence of comorbid ADHD is associated with an exacerbation of impaired executive functioning [18,26].
Secondly, in the literature reviewed, there is no evidence of the application of the SENA test in the population affected by FASD. The impairment of neurocognitive domains affected by FASD, namely intellectual functioning, memory, attention, executive functions, or communication skills, directly impacts the person’s social and adaptive functioning. In other words, impaired executive functioning domains self-regulation, information processing, inhibition, working memory, self-monitoring, and planning and organization impact the ability to make decisions and regulate behavior effectively and adaptively [27]. Thus, executive functioning will condition behavioral functioning. Therefore, individuals exposed to alcohol in the prenatal period with impaired inhibition and emotional control experience serious behavioral problems, such as illogical thinking, difficulties in understanding cause–effect relationships, aggressive and antisocial behavior, or other maladaptive behaviors, irrespective of IQ. In this sense, most of the child-age population with severe adaptive behavioral disturbances also show them in adulthood, often associated with problems with justice [28]. This study shows the presence of alterations in all of the domains of adaptive behavior assessed by means of the SENA test in the clinical population affected by FASD in comparison with the general population. Specifically, alterations coinciding with the clinical population were observed in the domains of depression, anxiety, social anxiety, attention problems, hyperactivity and impulsivity, anger control problems, aggression, defiant behavior, antisocial behavior, substance use, problems with eating behavior, unusual behavior, rigidity, isolation, integration and social competence, emotional intelligence, and willingness to study. As can be seen, the impairment of behavioral functioning in the diagnosis of FASD is compatible with the impairments in the domains, as can be similarly reflected in the studies of several authors ([1,2,19,20,21,22,27], among others). Depression poses severe life-threatening risks for adolescents affected by FASD, such as suicidal ideation, self-harming behaviors, or suicide attempts [29]. Deficits in behavioral control are explained by deficits in behavioral self-regulation, behavioral inhibition, and adaptive functioning [21]. Problems with anger control, aggression, or defiant behavior towards authority figures are common in the FASD population, especially with advancing age [21]. These adaptive dysfunctions, together with behavioral inhibition and impulsive behaviors, are consistent with impaired social functioning, intensifying at older ages [20]. All these findings are confirmed by the results obtained in this study. Furthermore, all these impairments in adaptive behavior and social competence lead to difficulties in community participation. In addition, social deterioration with advancing age is also motivated by alterations in the social and communication domain [19], revealing a halt in the progression of social competence. Thus, since social and communication demands increase in adolescence and adulthood, social deterioration is indeed confirmed as age increases [21].
The presence or absence of dysmorphic traits with prenatal alcohol exposure does not necessarily imply significant differences in executive functioning [19,30]. These results are supported by those obtained in the same study. Therefore, considering that the association between neurocognitive impairment and dysmorphic traits is not statistically significant, it is suggested that the diagnosis of FASD should prioritize central nervous system deficits [31]. Other studies show differences in terms of which behavioral disturbances manifested according to the diagnostic entity [32,33], with the genetic component, the environment, and early adversity experiences becoming conditioning elements [21].
From another point of view, considering the gender variable as a moderating factor in the impairment of executive and behavioral functioning, in this study, no significant differences were observed in either the executive functioning or behavioral functioning variables as a function of gender. These results are confirmed by those obtained in the work of Herman and colleagues [17] or Rai and colleagues [18], which reveal no statistically significant differences between boys and girls on measures of impaired executive functioning using the BRIEF-2 test. Likewise, these results are consistent with those obtained in the study by Sakano and colleagues [21], where possible differences in behavioral domains could be due to other factors unrelated to FASD.

5. Conclusions

Firstly, it is considered that the contribution of the family through the BRIEF-2 and SENA standardized assessment tests for the Spanish-speaking population allows for the elaboration of a defined executive and behavioral profile of the child and adolescent with a diagnosis of Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorder, thus fulfilling the first working hypothesis of this study.
The domains of executive functioning that are affected in each of the profiles that are formed from the three classifying factors can be found in Appendix A, expressed through the qualitative indicators proposed by the BRIEF-2 test: typical scores between 0–59 are qualified as “no clinical significance”, T scores between 60–64 as “mild elevation”, T scores between 65–69 as “potentially clinical elevations” and, finally, T scores equal to or above 70 as “clinically significant elevations”. In contrast, it was determined that both the gender variable and the diagnostic entity did not become conditioning factors for the level of cognitive functioning or the level of executive function. Ultimately, the pattern of executive functioning becomes very similar between the diagnostic entities considered in this study, which allows us to confirm that the hypothesis about affinity between patterns of functioning of the diagnostic entities is fulfilled.
It is also noted that the behavioral functioning of people with FASD is significantly altered in comparison with their age and gender counterparts because of alterations in executive functioning. This altered behavioral functioning mainly responds to problems of self-regulation, evidencing problems in anger control or emotional regulation, hyperactivity and impulsivity, problems in maintaining attention, cognitive rigidity, and alterations in social interaction and competence related to unusual behavior, which on many occasions leads to situations of marked social isolation. In addition, anxiety, depression, defiant behavior, antisocial behavior, and aggressiveness may be observed. In all cases, the willingness to study has been affected, remaining stable at higher ages. This fact highlights the absence or insufficient support to offer experiences of academic success to people affected by FASD within the educational system. In turn, the effect on behavioral functioning worsens with increasing age in some domains of behavioral functioning: anxiety, attention problems, challenging behavior, antisocial behavior, unusual behavior, problems in emotional regulation, cognitive rigidity, and social isolation. This may be due to a multifactorial origin, such as the person’s support network, experiences of academic success, or perceived social support, among others (See Appendix B).
The scope of this research study in the field of research is promising. The development of a neurocognitive and behavioral profile of the adopted person with a diagnosis of FASD has shown that developmental timing is an element that modulates the expression of alterations in executive and behavioral functioning. However, this finding is subject to a more complex assessment that includes not only the analysis of the affected person’s environment, but also its comparison with a clinical group of the biological population. In any case, having a defined profile in terms of cognitive and behavioral functioning is a very valuable resource for professionals in the educational community. It serves on the one hand, as a guide to understanding how these students function and learn and, on the other hand, as a guideline for the implementation of inclusive educational practices. The main methodological limitations identified during the development of the research are the sample size, the lack of research, and the biases associated with the administration of questionnaires. Considering that there are no real data describing the prevalence and incidence data of this disorder in Spain, rather than the mere fact of an estimate of it [4], it is difficult to define a representative sample size of the clinical population. Although it is true that, considering the estimated data (22 people affected per 10,000 inhabitants), the sample size is small (n = 66). In addition, the establishment of the correlation between the alterations in executive and behavioral functioning and the age variable has been carried out based on the available sample. However, the ideal would be to design a longitudinal study to analyze the development of executive functions and the establishment of a behavioral pattern in the same population, which would undoubtedly require an increase in economic resources and time. In this sense, the conclusions drawn are limited to the conditions of this study. Finally, the bias associated with the administration of questionnaires that use the Likert scale as a recording measure can lead to the loss of information, since it is limited to polarized categories and can even be affected by the statistical treatment carried out by the researcher himself [34].
As lines of future research, it is recommended to continue with the design of specific training on FASD that can be offered in educational centers, through associations, in congresses or seminars, where the profiles generated in this study can be provided.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, N.F.; methodology, N.F. and J.M.L.-D.; software, N.F. and J.M.L.-D.; formal analysis, N.F. and J.M.L.-D.; investigation, N.F., J.M.L.-D. and I.G.; data curation, N.F. and I.G.; writing—original draft preparation, N.F. and I.G.; writing—review and editing, I.G.; project administration, N.F.; funding acquisition, N.F. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research project has received funding from the Institutional Chair on People with Disabilities, Universal Accessibility and Inclusion of the Universidad Rey Juan Carlos for the purchase of standardized assessment tests and correction pins, amounting to €615.22.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The research project was carried out in accordance with the Research Ethics Committee of the Universidad Rey Juan Carlos with internal registration number 0403202007620, obtaining a favorable opinion for the study to be carried out on 16 September 2020.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study. Written informed consent has been obtained from the patient(s) to publish this paper.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available upon request to the corresponding author due to the preservation of the health data protection of the study participants.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank all family and associations who participated in the research.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Appendix A

PROFILE OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING BY AGE
CHILDHOOD (7–10 years)
Inhibition: PCE
Self-monitoring: ME
Flexibility: PCE
Emotional control: ME
Initiative: CSE
Working memory: CSE
Planning and organization: PCE
Task monitoring: PCE
Material organization: EL.
EARLY ADOLESCENCE (11–14 years)
Inhibition: PCE
Self-monitoring: ME
Flexibility: PCE
Emotional control: ME
Initiative: CSE
Working memory: CSE
Planning and organization: PCE
Task monitoring: PCE
Material organization: ME
MIDDLE ADOLESCENCE (15–17 years)
Inhibition: PCE
Self-monitoring: ME
Flexibility: PCE
Emotional control: ME
Initiative: CSE
Working memory: CSE
Planning and organization: PCE
Task monitoring: PCE
Material organization: ME
LATE ADOLESCENCE (18–19 years)
Inhibition: PCE
Self-monitoring: ME
Flexibility: EPC.
Emotional control: ME
Initiative: CSE
Working memory: CSE
Planning and organization: PCE
Task monitoring: PCE
Material organization: ME
PROFILE OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING BY GENDER
WOMEN
Inhibition: CSE
Self-monitoring: CSE
Flexibility: CSE
Emotional control: PCE
Initiative: CSE
Working memory: CSE
Planning and organization: CSE
Task monitoring: CSE
Material organization CSE
MEN
Inhibition: CSE
Self-monitoring: CSE
Flexibility: CSE
Emotional control: PCE
Initiative: CSE
Working memory: CSE
Planning and organization: CSE
Task monitoring: CSE
Material organization: CSE
PROFILE OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING BY DIAGNOSIS
Complete FAS
Inhibition: CSE
Self-monitoring: CSE
Flexibility: CSE
Emotional control: CSE
Initiative: CSE
Working memory: CSE
Planning and organization: PCE
Task monitoring: CSE
Material organization: CSE
Partial FAS
Inhibition: CSE
Self-monitoring: CSE
Flexibility: CSE
Emotional control: PCE
Initiative: CSE
Working memory: CSE
Planning and organization: CSE
Task monitoring: CSE
Material organization: CSE
ARND
Inhibition: CSE
Self-monitoring: CSE
Flexibility: CSE
Emotional control: CSE
Initiative: CSE
Working memory: CSE
Planning and organization: CSE
Task monitoring: PCE
Material organization: ME
NCS = no clinical significance (typical or T scores between 0–59); ME = mild elevation (T between 60–64); PCE = potentially clinical elevation (T between 65–69); CSE = clinically significant elevation (T ≥ 70).

Appendix B

PROFILE OF BEHAVIORAL FUNCTIONING BY AGE
CHILDHOOD (7–10 years)
Depression: 58.7
Anxiety: 56.9
Social anxiety: 53.9
Somatic complaints: 47
Attention problems: 67.7
Hyperactivity-impulsivity: 67.2
Anger control problems: 66.9
Aggression: 65.7
Challenging behavior: 66.5
Antisocial behavior: 0
Substance use: 0
Eating behavior problems: 0
Unusual behavior: 70.9
Emotional regulation problems: 59.5
Rigidity: 61.5
Isolation: 67.6
Social integration and competence: 29
Emotional intelligence: 38.1
Study disposition: 32.2
EARLY ADOLESCENCE (11–14 years)
Depression: 63.5
Anxiety: 65.6
Social anxiety: 62.4
Somatic complaints: 57.1
Attention problems: 72.7
Hyperactivity-impulsivity: 66.7
Anger control problems: 68.4
Aggression: 63.8
Challenging behavior: 63.8
Antisocial behavior: 50.7
Substance use: 36.1
Eating behavior problems: 41.9
Unusual behavior: 80.1
Emotional regulation problems: 66.7
Rigidity: 69.4
Isolation: 78.7
Social integration and competence: 30.4
Emotional intelligence: 34.4
Study disposition: 28.1
MIDDLE ADOLESCENCE (15–17 years)
Depression: 66.3
Anxiety: 75.8
Social anxiety: 66.5
Somatic complaints: 60.7
Attention problems: 78.6
Hyperactivity-impulsivity: 76.8
Anger control problems: 71.7
Aggression: 76.3
Challenging behavior: 69.1
Antisocial behavior: 86.5
Substance use: 51.1
Eating behavior problems: 59.9
Unusual behavior: 93.2
Emotional regulation problems: 72.9
Rigidity: 74.6
Isolation: 85.4
Social integration and competence: 24.3
Emotional intelligence: 35
Study disposition: 28.3
LATE ADOLESCENCE (18–19 years)
Depression: 63.4
Anxiety: 66.5
Social anxiety: 56.1
Somatic complaints: 49.9
Attention problems: 74.6
Hyperactivity-impulsivity: 73.6
Anger control problems: 75.6
Aggression: 80
Challenging behavior: 76.8
Antisocial behavior: 99.5
Substance use: 79
Eating behavior problems: 59.2
Unusual behavior: 96
Emotional regulation problems: 73.3
Rigidity: 73.2
Isolation: 82.3
Social integration and competence: 27.2
Emotional intelligence: 32.8
Study disposition: 28.1
PROFILE OF BEHAVIORAL FUNCTIONING BY GENDER
WOMEN
Depression: 60.5
Anxiety: 65.5
Social anxiety: 63.4
Somatic complaints: 50.1
Attention problems: 75.9
Hyperactivity-impulsivity: 68.9
Anger control problems: 69.9
Aggression: 73
Challenging behavior: 71.3
Antisocial behavior: 68.6
Substance use: 46.2
Eating behavior problems: 41.4
Unusual behavior: 82.8
Emotional regulation problems: 68.3
Rigidity: 68.4
Isolation: 85.5
Social integration and competence: 25.5
Emotional intelligence: 33.9
Study disposition: 26.3
MEN
Depression: 65.1
Anxiety: 66.9
Social anxiety: 56.6
Somatic complaints: 56
Attention problems: 71.9
Hyperactivity-impulsivity: 73
Anger control problems: 72.2
Aggression: 71.6
Challenging behavior: 68.7
Antisocial behavior: 61.6
Substance use: 46.7
Eating behavior problems: 44.6
Unusual behavior: 89
Emotional regulation problems: 69.2
Rigidity: 71.5
Isolation: 74.5
Social integration and competence: 29.4
Emotional intelligence: 35.3
Study disposition: 31
PROFILE OF BEHAVIORAL FUNCTIONING BY DIAGNOSIS
Complete FAS
Depression: 62.6
Anxiety: 67.6
Social anxiety: 59.1
Somatic complaints: 54.2
Attention problems: 75
Hyperactivity-impulsivity: 73.3
Anger control problems: 69.8
Aggression: 71.5
Challenging behavior: 69.9
Antisocial behavior: 67.1
Substance use: 44.4
Eating behavior problems: 44.5
Unusual behavior: 90.3
Emotional regulation problems: 68.6
Rigidity: 71
Isolation: 81.9
Social integration and competence: 27.2
Emotional intelligence: 35.4
Study disposition: 28
Partial FAS
Depression: 67.3
Anxiety: 61.7
Social anxiety: 60.8
Somatic complaints: 52.6
Attention problems: 70.8
Hyperactivity-impulsivity: 65
Anger control problems: 69.7
Aggression: 67.3
Challenging behavior: 66.8
Antisocial behavior: 57.5
Substance use: 52.9
Eating behavior problems: 37.5
Unusual behavior: 76.6
Emotional regulation problems: 66
Rigidity: 66.7
Isolation: 74.2
Social integration and competence: 30.4
Emotional intelligence: 31.8
Study disposition: 29.7
ARND
Depression: 60.3
Anxiety: 64.7
Social anxiety: 60.3
Somatic complaints: 48.7
Attention problems: 67.7
Hyperactivity-impulsivity: 65.7
Anger control problems: 85.3
Aggression: 86.7
Challenging behavior: 74.3
Antisocial behavior: 57.3
Substance use: 51.7
Eating behavior problems: 43.7
Unusual behavior: 72.3
Emotional regulation problems: 76.3
Rigidity: 69.7
Isolation: 66
Social integration and competence: 27
Emotional intelligence: 34.7
Study disposition: 36
T-scores equal to or above 60 or equal to and below 39 should be considered with caution as they are relatively infrequent in the population, indicating possible difficulties in that area; T-scores equal to or above 70 or equal to and below 29 should be considered clinically significant as they imply a high level of impairment in that area. Finally, T-scores equal to or above 80 or equal to and below 19 are truly extreme scores and should be given priority attention.

References

  1. Hoyme, E.H.; Kalberg, W.; Elliott, A.; Blankenship, J.; Buckley, D.; Marais, A.S.; Nursing, C.; Manning, M.; Robinson, L.; Adam, M.; et al. Updated clinical guidelines for diagnosing fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Pediatrics 2016, 138, e20154256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Maya-Enero, S.; Ramis-Fernández, S.; Astals-Vizcaino, M.; García-Algar, O. Perfil neurocognitivo y conductual del trastorno del espectro alcohólico fetal. An. Pediatría 2021, 95, e1–e208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Popova, S.; Dozet, D.; Burd, L. Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorder: Can We Change the Future? Alcohol Clin. Exp. Res. 2020, 44, 815–819. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Popova, S.; Lange, S.; Probst, C.; Gmel, C.; Rehm, J. Estimation of national, regional, and global prevalence of alcohol use during pregnancy and fetal alcohol syndrome: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Glob. 2017, 5, e290–e299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Bombardelli, O. Inclusive Education and Its Implementation: International Practices. Educ. Self Dev. 2020, 15, 37–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Coates, J.; Harris, J.; Waring, M. The effectiveness of a special school experience for improving preservice teachers’ efficacy to teach children with special educational needs and disabilities. Br. Educ. Res. J. 2020, 46, 909–928. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Emmers, E.; Baeyens, D.; Petry, K. Attitudes and self-efficacy of teachers towards inclusion in higher education. Eur. J. Spec. Needs Educ. 2019, 35, 139–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Lees, B.; Riches, J.; Mewton, L.; Elliot, E.; Allsop, S.; Newton, N.; Thomas, S.; Rice, L.; Nepal, S.; Teeson, M.; et al. Fetal alcohol spectrum disorder resources for educators: A scoping review. Health Promot. J. Austr. 2022, 33, 797–809. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Taresh, S.; Aniza, N.; Roslan, A.S.; Ma’rof, A.M.; Zaid, S. Pre-School Teachers’ Knowledge, Belief, Identification Skills, and Self-Efficacy in Identifying Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD): A Conceptual Framework to Identify Children with ASD. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Taylor, T.; Enns, L.N. Factors predictive of a fetal alcohol spectrum disorder diagnosis: Parent and teacher ratings. Child Neuropsychol. 2018, 25, 507–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Reid, N.; White, C.; Hawkins, E.; Crawford, A.; Liu, W.; Shanley, D. Outcomes and needs of health and education professionals following fetal alcohol spectrum disorder-specific training. J. Paediatr. Child Health 2019, 56, 317–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Zameer, M.; Carlisle, A.C.S.; Livesey, A.C.; Mukherjee, R.A.S. Comparisons of the BRIEF parental report and neuropsychological clinical tests of executive function in Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorders: Data from the UK national specialist clinic. Child Neuropsychol. 2020, 25, 648–663. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Fernández-Pinto, I.; Santamaría, P.; Sánchez-Sánchez, F.; Carrasco, M.; del Barrio, V. SENA. Sistema de Evaluación de Niños y Adolescentes. Manual de Aplicación, Corrección e Interpretación; TEA Ediciones: Madrid, Spain, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  14. Gioia, G.; Isquith, P.; Guy, S.; Kenworthy, K. BRIEF-2. Evaluación Conductual de la Función Ejecutiva; Maldonado, M.J., Fournier, M.C., Martínez-Arias, R., González-Marqués, J., Santamaría, J.M.E.-S.Y.P., Eds.; TEA Ediciones: Madrid, Spain, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  15. Baddeley, A. Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2012, 63, 1–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  17. Herman, L.E.; Acosta, M.; Chang, P.N. Gender and attention deficits in children diagnosed with a fetal alcohol spectrum disorder. Can. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 2008, 15, e411–e419. Available online: https://jptcp.com/index.php/jptcp/article/view/234 (accessed on 21 September 2023). [PubMed]
  18. Rai, J.; Abecassis, M.; Casey, J.; Flaro, L.; Erdodi, L.; Roth, R. Parent rating of executive function in fetal alcohol spectrum disorder: A review of the literature and new data on Aboriginal Canadian children. Child Neuropsychol. 2017, 23, 713–732. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Mattson, S.; Bernes, G.; Doyle, L. Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorders: A review of the neurobehavioral deficits associated with prenatal alcohol exposure. Alcohol Clin. Exp. Res. 2019, 43, 1046–1062. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Rangmar, J.; Sandberg, A.D.; Aronson, M.; Fahlke, C. Cognitive and executive functions, social cognition, and sense of coherence in adults with fetal alcohol syndrome. Nord. J. Psychiatry 2015, 69, 1754–1760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Sakano, M.; Mukherjee, R.A.S.; Turk, J. Behaviour and adaptive functioning in children and young people with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders: A UK study. Adv. Dual Diagn. 2019, 12, 62–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Rasmussen, C.; Andrew, G.; Zwaigenbaum, L.; Tough, S. Neurobehavioral outcomes of children with Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorders: A Canadian perspective. Paediatr. Child Health 2008, 13, 185–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. McLachlan, K.; Paolozza, A.; Kully-Martens, K.; Portales-Casamar, E.; Pavlidis, P.; Andrew, G.; Hanlon-Dearman, A.; Loock, C.; McFarlane, A.; Nikkel, S.; et al. Unpacking the heterogeneity of cognitive functioning in children and adolescents with fetal alcohol spectrum disorder: Determining the role of moderators and strengths. Adv. Neurodev. Disord. 2017, 1, 271–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Rasmussen, C.; Wyper, K.; Talwar, V. The relation between theory of mind and executive functions in children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. J. Popl. Ther. Clin. Pharmacol. 2018, 16, e370–e380. Available online: https://jptcp.com/index.php/jptcp/article/view/561 (accessed on 30 January 2024).
  25. Rasmussen, C.; Tamana, S.; Baugh, L.; Andrew, G.; Tough, S.; Zwaigenbaum, L. Neuropsychological impairments on the NEPSYII among children with FASD. Child Neuropsichol. 2013, 19, 337–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Khoury, J.E.; Milligan, K. Comparing Executive Functioning in Children and Adolescents with Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorders and ADHD: A Meta-Analysis. J. Atten. Disord. 2019, 23, 1801–1815. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Brown, N.; Connor, P.; Adler, R. Conduct-disordered adolescents with fetal alcohol spectrum disorder. Intervention in secure treatment settings. Crim. Justice Behav. 2012, 39, 770–793. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Nash, K.; Stevens, S.; Clairman, H.; Rovet, J. Preliminary Findings that a Targeted Intervention Leads to Altered Brain Function in Children with Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorder. Brain Sci. 2018, 8, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Xiang, X.; Cho, J.; Sun, Y.; Wang, X. Childhood adversity and cognitive impairment in later life. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 935254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Kingdon, D.; Cardoso, C.; McGrath, J.J. Research Review: Executive function deficits in fetal alcohol spectrum disorders and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder—A meta-analysis. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2016, 57, 116–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Chudley, A.E.; Conry, J.; Cook, J.L.; Loock, C.; Rosales, T.; Le Blanc, N. Fetal alcohol spectrum disorder: Canadian guidelines for diagnosis. Can. Med. Assoc. J. 2005, 1, s1–s21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Kully-Martens, K.; Denys, K.; Treit, S.; Tamana, S.; Rasmussen, C. A Review of Social Skills Deficits in Individuals with Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorders and Prenatal Alcohol Exposure: Profiles, Mechanisms, and Interventions. Alcohol Clin. Exp. Res. 2012, 36, 568–576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Johnson, M.E.; Robinson, R.V.; Corey, S.; Dewane, S.L.; Brems, C.; Casto, L.D. Knowledge, attitudes, and behaviors of health, education, and service professionals as related to fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Int. J. Public Health 2010, 55, 627–635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Westland, J.C. Information loss and bias in Likert survey responses. PLoS ONE 2022, 17, e0271949. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Research Method Flowchart.
Figure 1. Research Method Flowchart.
Behavsci 14 00431 g001
Table 1. Normality test.
Table 1. Normality test.
Variable
EF
FE StatisticsVariable
BF
BF Statistics
DpDp
INH0.0760.435DEP0.8100.331
SMO0.1560.000ANS0.0890.212
FLE0.1180.021SCA0.1030.075
EMC0.1210.014SOM0.1610.000
INI0.0940.150ATE0.0790.376
WM0.0730.503HIP0.1020.079
PLA0.1810.000ANG0.0920.170
TAS0.1160.026AGG0.1230.013
ORG0.0940.151CHA0.1130.032
ANT0.1910.000
SUB0.3620.000
EAT0.1660.001
UNU0.1030.077
REG0.1240.011
RIG0.1100.043
ISO0.1060.062
SOC0.1330.005
EMI0.0910.191
STU0.1350.004
N = 66. D = Kolmogorov-Smirnov statistic with Lilliefors significance correction (n ≥ 50) based on 10,000 Monte Carlo samples with a starting seed of 2,000,000. If p ≥ 0.05 H0 is accepted so the observed values are distributed under the assumption of normality.
Table 2. Student’s t-test of EF and BF domains.
Table 2. Student’s t-test of EF and BF domains.
Variable
EF
t-Test Results EFVariable
BF
t-Test Results BF
TpTp
INH **15.220.000DEP **8.380.000
SMO *17.710.000ANS **11.370.000
FLE *17.520.000SCA **5.790.000
EMC *11.860.000SOM *2.020.047
INI **23.330.000ATE **20.220.000
WM **22.380.000HIP **13.040.000
PLA *26.810.000ANG **10.70.000
TAS *20.130.000AGG *8.450.000
ORG **12.870.000CHA *10.60.000
ANT *9.320.000
SUB *3.460.001
EAT **3.720.001
UNU **11.830.000
REG *14.050.000
RIG *16.770.000
ISO **13.660.000
SOC *−15.370.000
EMI **−13.790.000
STU *−19.360.000
** normal distribution; * non-normal distribution; N = 66; df = 65; test value = 50. p = bilateral significance.
Table 3. Analysis of bivariate correlations in EF and BF variables according to age.
Table 3. Analysis of bivariate correlations in EF and BF variables according to age.
Variable
EF
PearsonSpearman’s RhopEFVariable
BF
PearsonSpearman’s RhopBF
INH0.456 ** 0.000 **DEP0.105 0.401
SMO 0.529 **0.000 **ANS0.274 * 0.026 *
FLE 0.487 **0.000 **SCA0.019 0.877
EMC 0.473 **0.000 **SOM −0.0150.907
INI0.153 0.219ATE0.290 * 0.018 *
WM0.275 * 0.026 *HIP0.251 * 0.042 *
PLA 0.335 **0.006 **ANG0.263 * 0.033 *
TAS 0.427 **0.000 **AGG 0.379 **0.002 **
ORG0.163 0.183CHA 0.352 **0.004 **
ANT 0.566 **0.000 **
SUB 0.2580.070
EAT 0.1000.491
UNU0.340 ** 0.005 **
REG 0.434 **0.000 **
RIG 410 **0.001 **
ISO0.263 * 0.033 *
SOC 0.0040.976
EMI−243 * 0.049 *
STU −0.1520.222
** correlation is significant at the 0.01 level; * correlation is significant at the 0.05 level. N = 66. p = bilateral significance. If p ≥ 0.05, H0 is accepted, i.e., there are no differences in the executive/behavioral functioning variable as a function of age.
Table 4. Analysis of bivariate correlations in EF and BF variables according to diagnosis.
Table 4. Analysis of bivariate correlations in EF and BF variables according to diagnosis.
Variable
EF
PearsonSpearman’s RhopEFVariable
BF
PearsonSpearman’s RhopBF
INH−0.034 0.786DEP0.021 0.434
SMO 0.0300.813ANS−0.144 0.124
FLE −0.0850.497SCA0.045 0.359
EMC 0.0440.726SOM −0.0430.366
INI−0.122 0.329ATE−0.257 * 0.019
WM−0.090 0.471HIP −0.262 *0.017
PLA 0.0540.668ANG0.228 0.033
TAS 0.0010.996AGG 0.0970.220
ORG−0.240 0.052CHA −0.0100.468
ANT −0.0750.275
SUB 0.1100.190
EAT −0.0620.312
UNU−0.264 * 0.016
REG 0.1080.195
RIG −0.1880.066
ISO 0.0250.422
SOC−0.293 ** 0.008
EMI−0.085 0.249
STU 0.215 *0.041
** correlation is significant at the 0.01 level; * correlation is significant at the 0.05 level. N = 66. p = bilateral significance. If p ≥ 0.05, H0 is accepted, i.e., there are no differences in the executive/behavioral functioning variable as a function of diagnosis.
Table 5. One-factor ANOVA for the age variable and the EF and BF variables.
Table 5. One-factor ANOVA for the age variable and the EF and BF variables.
ANOVA EFANOVA BF
EFFpFpBF
INI5.7030.002ANS7.8450.000
SMO9.2740.000ATE3.5110.020
FLE7.5180.000HIP2.2780.088
EMC3.9070.013ANG1.0390.381
WM4.4300.007AGG2.6100.059
PLA3.5340.020CHA2.9110.041
TAS6.0750.001ANT5.6950.002
UNU3.9550.012
REG6.6930.001
RIG6.4340.001
SOC3.0450.035
EMI0.9540.420
Inter-group df = 3; intra-group df = 62; total df = 65.
Table 6. One-factor ANOVA for the diagnosis variable and the EF and BF variables.
Table 6. One-factor ANOVA for the diagnosis variable and the EF and BF variables.
ANOVA BF
FpBF
ATE2.2320.116
HIP2.4500.094
UNU2.5090.089
SOC.3220.726
STU2.3650.102
Inter-group df = 2; intra-group df = 63; total df = 65.
Table 7. Kruskal–Wallis H-test of executive functioning variables according to diagnosis.
Table 7. Kruskal–Wallis H-test of executive functioning variables according to diagnosis.
Variables EFAverage RangeZp
FASpFASARND
INH33.9734.41280.5480.760
SMO33.2832.2337.670.3410.843
FLE34.4331.3629.830.4740.789
EMC33.1831.2740.170.8890.641
INI35.1425.7334.332.1860.335
WM34.0735.8624.501.5360.464
PLA33.202944.172.5040.286
TAS3343.4519.336.3100.043
ORG36.0129.8619.674.3730.112
NFAS = 49; NpFAS = 11; NARND = 6; df = 2. Significance level is 0.05.
Table 8. Kruskal–Wallis H-test of behavioral functioning variables according to diagnosis.
Table 8. Kruskal–Wallis H-test of behavioral functioning variables according to diagnosis.
Variables BFAverage RangeZp
FASpFASARND
DEP32.9937.7729.830.8010.670
ANS35.4127.7328.501.8940.388
SCA32.9235.2735.000.1760.916
SOM33.9732.4131.670.1200.942
ATE36.3029.4118.175.3880.068
HIP36.5124.0026.334.7500.093
ANG31.7031.5051.836.0310.049
AGG32.7130.4545.502.7130.258
CHA33.7829.9137.830.7020.704
ANT34.3031.9529.830.3840.825
SUB32.3236.1438.330.7950.672
EAT34.3927.5537.171.4110.494
UNU36.4526.3622.504.6740.097
REG32.6529.5547.673.8450.146
RIG35.7125.4530.172.7920.248
ISO35.8131.5018.334.5810.101
SOC33.2034.6433.830.0520.974
EMI34.7128.8232.170.8840.643
STU31.3235.4147.834.1050.128
NFAS = 49; NpFAS = 11; NARND = 6; df = 2. Significance level is 0.05.
Table 9. Kolmogorov–Smirnov test for executive and behavioral functioning variables (grouping variable: gender).
Table 9. Kolmogorov–Smirnov test for executive and behavioral functioning variables (grouping variable: gender).
Variables EFZpVariables BFZp
INH1.1550.139DEP1.0790.194
SMO0.9360.345ANS0.9060.385
FLE0.9360.345SCA1.2600.083
EMC0.7620.607SOM1.2680.080
INI0.6940.721ATE1.7430.005
WM1.9770.001HIP1.0040.266
PLA0.7700.594ANG0.6870.733
TAS1.5920.013AGG0.6110.849
ORG0.8680.438CHA1.1620.134
ANT0.5740.897
SUB1.2530.087
EAT1.0040.266
UNU0.7320.657
REG0.6340.816
RIG1.2300.097
ISO1.6080.011
SOC0.8450.473
EMI0.9810.291
STU1.2080.108
Ntotal = 66; Nmen = 38; Nwomen = 28. Significance level is 0.05.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Felgueras, N.; López-Díaz, J.M.; Garrote, I. Effects of Developmental Timing on Cognitive and Behavioral Profiles in Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorder: Considerations for Education. Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 431. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14060431

AMA Style

Felgueras N, López-Díaz JM, Garrote I. Effects of Developmental Timing on Cognitive and Behavioral Profiles in Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorder: Considerations for Education. Behavioral Sciences. 2024; 14(6):431. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14060431

Chicago/Turabian Style

Felgueras, Nerea, José María López-Díaz, and Inmaculada Garrote. 2024. "Effects of Developmental Timing on Cognitive and Behavioral Profiles in Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorder: Considerations for Education" Behavioral Sciences 14, no. 6: 431. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14060431

APA Style

Felgueras, N., López-Díaz, J. M., & Garrote, I. (2024). Effects of Developmental Timing on Cognitive and Behavioral Profiles in Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorder: Considerations for Education. Behavioral Sciences, 14(6), 431. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14060431

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop