Next Article in Journal
Exploring the Transformational Role of Regular Nature-Based Adventure Activity Engagement in Mental Health and Long-Term Eudaimonic Well-Being
Previous Article in Journal
The Impact of Background Music on Flow, Work Engagement and Task Performance: A Randomized Controlled Study
Previous Article in Special Issue
Participants’ Roles in Bullying Among 7–11 Year Olds: Results from a UK-Wide Randomized Control Trial of the KiVa School-Based Program
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Editorial

Promoting Behavioral Change to Improve Health Outcomes

by
John A. Parkinson
Wales Centre for Behaviour Change, Department of Psychology, Bangor University, Gwynedd LL57 2AS, UK
Behav. Sci. 2025, 15(4), 417; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15040417
Submission received: 6 March 2025 / Accepted: 10 March 2025 / Published: 25 March 2025
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Promoting Behavioral Change to Improve Health Outcomes)

1. Introduction

The key to long-term mental and physical health, including a high quality of life without the burden of disease, is adopting and sustaining behaviors that prevent illness and promote resilient health. However, the prevalence of non-communicable diseases is increasing across the world, including obesity, type 2 diabetes, cardiovascular disease, mental health disorders, etc. Such a burden is unsustainable and reduces quality of life, productivity, and wellbeing across the board (Edwards, 2024; Olshansky, 2018). In the UK, where healthcare is free at the point of delivery, the National Health Service is struggling to manage this increased burden of disease across citizens (DHSC, 2018). Increasingly, arguments are being made for an emphasis on prevention and an understanding of the potential return on investment of acting early to prevent or reduce illness and promote wellbeing (Edwards, 2024; Masters et al., 2017; Musich et al., 2016).
By zooming out and viewing this health crisis from a distance, it seems clear that individuals, groups, and even entire populations need to change their behavior in order to promote better health outcomes. Adopting preventative habits, such as eating a healthy diet, exercising regularly, developing a healthy sleeping habit, and reducing or cutting out drug use (tobacco, alcohol, etc.), the burden of disease would be diminished, and the number of years a person lives disease-free (healthspan) would increase significantly. Currently, the opposite trend is true for many societies (Davoud¡-Monfared et al., 2024; DHSC, 2018; Wagner & Brath, 2012).
Sustained behavior change is challenging, not only because our good intentions are not always followed by good actions (Allan et al., 2008; Gould et al., 2024; Verplanken & Orbell, 2022). We have seemingly evolved with multiple brain mechanisms that make decisions and control our behavior in ways that are not always consistent or outwardly rational (Khalil & Amin, 2023; Lalani et al., 2023). As such, the particular value of behavioral science is in recognizing and understanding that non-conscious behavioral drivers can sometimes override deliberate intentions. Factors such as social norms, habits, and contextual elements—including physical architecture—play a crucial role in shaping behavior, often leading individuals to act contrary to their explicit intentions (Kahneman, 2013; Thaler & Sunstein, 2009).
This discrepancy between intention and action, commonly referred to as the “intention–action gap”, is well-documented in the literature and can be traced back at least as far as Plato’s Republic. A dual-process framework provides a theoretical lens through which this gap can be understood, suggesting that behavior is governed by two interacting pathways: one intuitive and automatic, and the other deliberative and controlled (Evans & Stanovich, 2013). Typically, these systems operate in concert to guide behavior, balancing automatic responses with intentional regulation. However, in certain contexts, these systems may come into conflict, and in such cases, Type 1 processes—characterized as evolutionarily older and more directly linked to behavioral execution at a neural level—are likely to dominate decision-making and action (Levant & Parkinson, 2014). Furthermore, with this insight, contemporary approaches to behavior change are more nuanced and cleverly designed to target the right systems for the right type of behavior. For example, the COM-B model, developed from Michie’s Behavior Change Wheel (Cane et al., 2012; Michie et al., 2011) considers behavior through the lenses of capability, opportunity and motivation, acknowledging the myriad influences across contexts. Building upon this complexity, there is a growing recognition that system approaches are required to give a holistic view of how behavior is determined by the larger system within which it sits. This recognizes the importance of not only intervention at different levels (Bioethics, 2007; Chater & Loewenstein, 2023) but also understanding unintended consequences of interventions, as well as the need to map actors and behaviors within larger systems to fully understand the drivers and influencing factors (Gould et al., 2024; Parkinson et al., 2025; Reynolds, 2024).
Within this context, this Special Issue sought to bring together research that explored health-related behavior change, underpinned by contemporary theory, with the aim of promoting longer-term wellbeing and health across a number of domains and with a diversity of methodologies.

2. Research by Health Domain

The papers in this Special Issue span a diversity of domains and behaviors, demonstrating the far-reaching value of a behavioral science approach. For example, three studies focused on substance use. One used a brief motivational intervention to reduce excessive drinking in students (contribution 1) incorporating motivational interviewing and systematic motivational counseling. The intervention was successful, though there were gender differences around the specificity of efficacy for the interventions. The second study explored users’ perceptions of therapeutic services for substance abuse to inform better experiences and outcomes for those enrolled (contribution 2). Users perceived the therapeutic context as distinct from the ‘real world’, and the authors emphasized the need for agency and empowerment to support the recovery process. The third study evaluated a novel acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) intervention for substance users to better understand the process of change and recovery (contribution 3). The intervention was co-designed by people in recovery, and success was related to core ACT mechanisms at play.
In the context of physical exercise, one study focused on exercise intentions from the perspective of job-related factors (contribution 4), and identified certain high-risk groups. Recommendations for comprehensive work-based support were made. A second study considered exercise and its relationship to sleep quality with the added perspectives of self-control and mindfulness (contribution 5). As expected, exercise predicted sleep quality in college students, and the authors went on to explore the mediation and moderation of this relationship with self-control and mindfulness. Two studies had diet and weight management as a health focus. Shourche et al. (contribution 6) tested the Life Advancement and Enhancement Program (LEAP) in a group of overweight females. The results showed that adding LEAP to a weight management practice had a significant effect on improving outcomes. Exploring a different avenue, Halabi et al. (contribution 7) considered non-dairy alternatives in coffee drinkers from the perspective of health awareness and consumer’s beliefs about the consequences of their actions. Based on the findings, the researchers made recommendations within the context of the Behavior Change Wheel framework, relating to fostering inclusivity, health awareness, and supporting environmental sustainability efforts.
Several studies considered medical adherence and/or the use of augmented medical devices. For example, one study sought to better understand home medication management practices in order to improve medical adherence (contribution 8) and proposed personalized guidance to patients from their healthcare providers. A second study explored attitudes and behaviors, including negative emotions such as fear and anxiety, towards vaccinations using a multicultural network analysis (contribution 9). A clear relationship was found between vaccination status and cognitive symptoms of fear and anxiety, suggesting novel routes for intervention to promote vaccine acceptance. Another study considered user intentions related to wearing augmented hearing aid devices (contribution 10) and found that functionality quality, perceived interaction speed, and perceived usability related to confidence in use of the novel devices.
Three studies focused on mental health. One looked at the role of guilt in mental health ‘burnout’ (contribution 11) and found that feelings of guilt appeared to drive psychosomatic symptoms during the burnout process. They recommend that future studies of burnout should encompass emotions including guilt as potentially important elements of the process. Two other studies focused on COVID-19. The first used a longitudinal design to track the mental health of university students over a two-year period during and after the initial COVID-19 epidemic (contribution 12). They found that distress and generalized anxiety remained high following the pandemic and argue that student mental health ‘is still in crisis’ with a need for further support for this cohort who are transitioning into the world of work. Another study looked at the potential role of ‘behavioral fatigue’ during the acute phase of COVID-19 (contribution 13) using a COM-B analysis and concluded that in order to maintain prolonged behavior change, capabilities, opportunities and motivation are all important for target behaviors. Continuing in the context of COVID-19, one experimental study explored risk behaviors from the perspective of psychological distance, testing an intervention designed to make COVID-19 feel more concrete and real (contribution 14). The authors demonstrated that participant risk perceptions were influenced by psychological distance (whether something is close/concrete or far/abstract) and were able to reduce the willingness of participants to engage in risky COVID-19 behaviors by making the concept of the COVID-19 virus more concrete.
To round off the diversity of behavioral domains, Hutchings et al. (contribution 15) evaluated data from an RCT trial of the KiVa anti-bullying school-based program in the UK. The results supported a social architecture model of bullying behavior and suggested further work to refine the model in addressing bullying as a public health challenge. Finally, Wetton et al. (contribution 16) used behavioral systems mapping to understand sustainability and waste management behaviors in Kenya. They developed a map which demonstrated key actors, sub-systems, and feedback loops that helped explain poor solid waste management practices and could be used to create systemic behavior change interventions in the future.

3. Research by Methodology and Approach

Across this Special Issue, two papers focused explicitly on prevention: one testing an intervention to reduce bullying, and the other exploring ways to promote COVID-19-safe behaviors (contribution 14; contribution 15). Additionally, eleven papers sought to use contemporary analyses in order to better understand a problem situation or behavior (contribution 2; contribution 4; contribution 5; contribution 7; contribution 8; contribution 9; contribution 10; contribution 11; contribution 12; contribution 13; contribution 16). Finally, three focused specifically on secondary prevention (Simeonsson, 1991) for either substance use or weight management (contribution 1; contribution 3; contribution 6).
Furthermore, of the approaches above, three papers based their study on the COM-B framework (contribution 7; contribution 13; contribution 16), whilst seven used questionnaires followed by a variety of modeling techniques, such as structural equation modeling (contribution 4; contribution 5; contribution 8; contribution 9; contribution 10; contribution 11; contribution 12). Finally, four employed intervention designs that were based on specific psychological (contribution 6; contribution 14; contribution 15) or therapeutic theories (contribution 1). Of these, only one paper explicitly incorporated dual-process theory (contribution 14).

4. Conclusions

This Special Issue showcases novel and impactful original research within the domain of promoting healthy behavior change. First and foremost, the research demonstrates the value of an approach based on behavioral science, particularly in demonstrating the value of integrating psychological theory, such as the dual-process approach (Evans & Stanovich, 2013), in developing interventions. The work herein also demonstrates the value of employing contemporary frameworks, such as COM-B (Michie et al., 2011), and testing them in the real world (Atkins et al., 2017).
Recently, we developed a high-level tool based on an integration of COM-B and dual-process theory ((Evans & Stanovich, 2013; Michie et al., 2011) to support health and wellbeing during the COVID-19 pandemic. The aim was to help those designing interventions to consider whether to target automatic (Type 1) or cognitive (Type 2) processes, and whether to focus on capability, opportunity, or motivation (see Figure 1, taken from (Gould et al., 2024). It also emphasizes that the moment of decision-making is often when behavior is most susceptible to influence. We hope that such an approach can improve the efficacy of behavioral interventions to promote positive and long-lasting health outcomes.
Ultimately, the combination of knowledge dissemination, targeted behavior change interventions, supportive environments, and strategic policies can lead to sustainable behavior change. The benefits of these interventions are twofold: individuals lead healthier, longer lives; and the societal costs associated with treating preventable diseases are considerably reduced. The field of health economics is becoming increasingly important in this regard, identifying the financial (and other) return on investment of implementing preventative and health-promoting interventions to build more human and social resilience to health challenges (Edwards, 2024; Whiteley et al., 2024). There is currently a paucity of research focused on identifying and utilizing dual-process theory in order to leverage the most effective behavior change techniques (Gould et al., 2024; Parkinson et al., 2014), particularly with regard to actually measuring behavioral outcomes rather than proxies such as attitudes or intentions (Murphy et al., 2023). Finally, given the complex nature of health challenges, future research will need to consider whole-system approaches, including system thinking, as well as considering at what level within a system an intervention is likely to have the largest benefit (Chater & Loewenstein, 2023; Parkinson et al., 2025; Reynolds, 2024).

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflicts of interest.

List of Contributions

  • Hogan, L. M., & Cox, W. M. (2024). Evaluating two brief motivational interventions for excessive-drinking university students. Behavioral Sciences, 14(5), 381. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14050381.
  • Iudici, A., Berardelli, T., Fenini, D., Subissi, E., & Neri, J. (2024). The discursive configuration of the therapeutic community for substance users: Positioning and ethnopsychological processes concerning entry. Behavioral Sciences, 14(10), 951. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14100951.
  • Shepley, E. L., Jackson, M. C., & Hogan, L. M. (2024). A qualitative exploration of the process and experience of change in moving on in my recovery: An acceptance and commitment therapy based recovery group for substance use disorder. Behavioral Sciences, 14(12), 1237. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14121237.
  • Wang, W.-H., Hsu, W.-T., Cheng, H.-I., Li, R.-H., Huang, S.-L., & Tang, F.-C. (2024). Exploring job-related factors and exercise intentions in relation to overall physical activity and its subdivisions. Behavioral Sciences, 14(10), 912. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14100912.
  • Li, X., Xu, C., Chen, W., & Tian, J. (2025). Physical exercise and sleep quality among chinese college students: The mediating role of self-control and the moderating role of mindfulness. Behavioral Sciences, 15(2), 232. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15020232.
  • Shourche, R. M., Nematy, M., Cox, W. M., & Fadardi, J. S. (2024). Effectiveness of the life enhancement and advancement program for weight management in overweight and obese females. Behavioral Sciences, 14(8), 724. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14080724.
  • Halabi, N., Hristova, V., & Vlaev, I. (2024). Milking the alternatives: Understanding coffee consumers’ preferences for non-dairy milk. Behavioral Sciences, 14(7), 569. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14070569.
  • Gualtieri, L., Steinfeldt, M., Shaveet, E., Estime, B., & Singhal, M. (2024). The impact of home medication management practices on medication adherence. Behavioral Sciences, 14(9), 804. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14090804.
  • Malas, O., Boustani, N. M., Duradoni, M., Omotoso, D., Avşar, A. Ş., Shyroka, A., Colombini, G., & Tolsá, M. D. (2024). Links between vaccination fear-, anxiety-, alexithymia-, and type D personality-related vaccination decisions: A network analysis in a multicultural sample. Behavioral Sciences, 14(9), 761. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14090761.
  • Deng, L., Chen, J., & Li, D. (2024). Factors influencing intentions of people with hearing impairments to use augmented reality glasses as hearing aids. Behavioral Sciences, 14(8), 728. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14080728.
  • Gil-LaOrden, P., Carlotto, M. S., & Gil-Monte, P. R. (2024). The role of guilt feelings in the development of the burnout process: The influence on psychosomatic problems. Behavioral Sciences, 14(12), 1196. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14121196.
  • Allen, R. E., Hochard, K. D., Kannangara, C., & Carson, J. (2024). The road to recovery: A two-year longitudinal analysis of mental health among university students during and after the COVID-19 pandemic. Behavioral Sciences, 14(12), 1146. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14121146.
  • Khanal, S., Schmidtke, K. A., Haralampiev, K., & Vlaev, I. (2024). Considering the role of behavioural fatigue in the COVID-19 lockdown debates in Great Britain: A retrospective analysis of a national survey assessing the relationships between capability, opportunity, motivation, and behaviour. Behavioral Sciences, 14(9), 852. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14090852.
  • Williams, C., Rauwolf, P., Boulter, M., & Parkinson, J. A. (2024). Closing the gap: How psychological distance influences willingness to engage in risky COVID behavior. Behavioral Sciences, 14(6), 449. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14060449.
  • Hutchings, J., Pearson, R., Babu, M., Clarkson, S., Williams, M. E., Badger, J. R., Cannings-John, R., Hastings, R. P., Hayes, R., & Bowes, L. (2025). Participants’ roles in bullying among 7–11 year olds: Results from a UK-wide randomized control trial of the KiVa school-based program. Behavioral Sciences, 15(2), 236. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15020236.
  • Wetton, J. D., Santilli, M., Gitau, H., Muindi, K., Zimmermann, N., Michie, S., & Davies, M. (2025). Behavioural systems mapping of solid waste management in kisumu, kenya, to understand the role of behaviour in a health and sustainability problem. Behavioral Sciences, 15(2), 133. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15020133.

References

  1. Allan, J. L., Johnston, M., & Campbell, N. (2008). Why do people fail to turn good intentions into action? The role of executive control processes in the translation of healthy eating intentions into action in young Scottish adults. BMC Public Health, 8, 123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Atkins, L., Francis, J., Islam, R., O’Connor, D., Patey, A., Ivers, N., Foy, R., Duncan, E. M., Colquhoun, H., Grimshaw, J. M., Lawton, R., & Michie, S. (2017). A guide to using the theoretical domains framework of behaviour change to investigate implementation problems. Implementation Science, 12(1), 77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Bioethics, N. C. O. (2007). Public health: Ethical issues. C. P. Ltd. Available online: https://www.nuffieldbioethics.org/publications/public-health (accessed on 5 March 2025).
  4. Cane, J., O’Connor, D., & Michie, S. (2012). Validation of the theoretical domains framework for use in behaviour change and implementation research. Implementation Science, 7(1), 37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Chater, N., & Loewenstein, G. (2023). The i-frame and the s-frame: How focusing on individual-level solutions has led behavioral public policy astray. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 46, e147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Davoud¡-Monfared, E., Mozafari, J. K., Keramatinia, A., Naimi, E., Amiri, P., Rahimpour, E., Ganji, S., Vejdani, M., & Mohseny, M. (2024). A review on lifestyle before and after COVID-19 pandemic: Four levels of prevention [short survey]. International Journal of Body, Mind and Culture, 11(4), 258–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. DHSC. (2018). Prevention is better than cure: Our vision to help you live well for longer. Available online: https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/media/5be00437e5274a6e174bdac1/Prevention_is_better_than_cure_5-11.pdf (accessed on 5 March 2025).
  8. Edwards, R. T. (2024). Health economics of well-being and well-becoming across the life-course. Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Evans, J. S. T., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition: Advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3), 223–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Gould, A., Lewis, L., Evans, L., Greening, L., Howe-Davies, H., West, J., Roberts, C., & Parkinson, J. A. (2024). COVID-19 personal protective behaviors during large social events: The value of behavioral observations. Behavioral Sciences, 14(1), 63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Kahneman, D. (2013). Thinking, fast and slow (1st ed.). Farrar, Straus and Giroux. [Google Scholar]
  12. Khalil, E. L., & Amin, A. (2023). The parallelism of cognitive economy and physiological economy: A rationality-based dual process theory. Current Psychology, 42(28), 24148–24162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Lalani, B., Gray, S., & Mitra-Ganguli, T. (2023). Systems Thinking in an era of climate change: Does cognitive neuroscience hold the key to improving environmental decision making? A perspective on climate-smart agriculture [perspective]. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 17, 1145744. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Levant, B., & Parkinson, J. (2014). Positive emotions and reward: Appetitive systems–Amygdala and striatum. Elsevier. [Google Scholar]
  15. Masters, R., Anwar, E., Collins, B., Cookson, R., & Capewell, S. (2017). Return on investment of public health interventions: A systematic review. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 71(8), 827–834. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Michie, S., van Stralen, M. M., & West, R. (2011). The behaviour change wheel: A new method for characterising and designing behaviour change interventions. Implementation Science, 6(1), 42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Murphy, C., Lim, W. W., Mills, C., Wong, J. Y., Chen, D. X., Xie, Y., Li, M. W., Gould, S., Xin, H. L., Cheung, J. K., Bhatt, S., Cowling, B. J., & Donnelly, C. A. (2023). Effectiveness of social distancing measures and lockdowns for reducing transmission of COVID-19 in non-healthcare, community-based settings. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society A-Mathematical Physical and Engineering Sciences, 381(2257), 20230132. [Google Scholar]
  18. Musich, S., Wang, S., Hawkins, K., & Klemes, A. (2016). The impact of personalized preventive care on health care quality, utilization, and expenditures. Population Health Management, 19(6), 389–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Olshansky, S. J. (2018). From lifespan to healthspan. JAMA, 320(13), 1323–1324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Parkinson, J. A., Eccles, K. E., & Goodman, A. (2014). Positive impact by design: The wales centre for behaviour change. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 9(6), 517–522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Parkinson, J. A., Gould, A., Knowles, N., West, J., & Goodman, A. (2025). Integrating systems thinking and behavioral science. Behavioral Sciences, 15(4), 403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Reynolds, M. (2024). Systems thinking principles for making change. Systems, 12(10), 437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Simeonsson, R. J. (1991). Primary, secondary, and tertiary prevention in early intervention. Journal of Early Intervention, 15(2), 124–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Thaler, R. H., & Sunstein, C. R. (2009). Nudge: Improving decisions about health, wealth, and happiness. Penguin Books. [Google Scholar]
  25. Verplanken, B., & Orbell, S. (2022). Attitudes, habits, and behavior change. Annual Review of Psychology, 73(1), 327–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Wagner, K.-H., & Brath, H. (2012). A global view on the development of non communicable diseases. Preventive Medicine, 54, S38–S41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Whiteley, H., Parkinson, J., Hartfiel, N., Makanjuola, A., Lloyd-Williams, H., Lawrence, C., & Edwards, R. T. (2024). Evaluating the well-being benefits and social value of volunteer gardening: Health economics meets behavioral science. Behavioral Sciences, 14(12), 1233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Taken from (Gould et al., 2024).
Figure 1. Taken from (Gould et al., 2024).
Behavsci 15 00417 g001
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Parkinson, J.A. Promoting Behavioral Change to Improve Health Outcomes. Behav. Sci. 2025, 15, 417. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15040417

AMA Style

Parkinson JA. Promoting Behavioral Change to Improve Health Outcomes. Behavioral Sciences. 2025; 15(4):417. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15040417

Chicago/Turabian Style

Parkinson, John A. 2025. "Promoting Behavioral Change to Improve Health Outcomes" Behavioral Sciences 15, no. 4: 417. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15040417

APA Style

Parkinson, J. A. (2025). Promoting Behavioral Change to Improve Health Outcomes. Behavioral Sciences, 15(4), 417. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15040417

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop