1. Introduction
Prospective memory is defined as the capacity to remember to execute planned actions in the right situations (
Einstein & McDaniel, 1990;
Guo et al., 2023). For instance, remembering to submit homework when you see your teacher. The preparatory attention processing and memory processing (PAM) theory suggests that prospective memory involves two types of processing processes: preparatory attention processing and memory processing. Among them, preparatory attention processing involves top-down attention monitoring, while memory processing involves bottom-up spontaneous retrieval (
Rummel & Kvavilashvili, 2023). Prospective memory comprises two processing components: a prospective component and a retrospective component. According to the PAM theory, the prospective component involves cue monitoring, while the retrospective component involves memory processing (
Guo et al., 2024). Successful cue monitoring and intention retrieval are both indispensable for the successful execution of prospective memory tasks. Prospective memory undergoes rapid development during the school-age period, but it remains immature in early primary school children (
Yang et al., 2011). Prospective memory tasks frequently appear in children’s daily routines, and children’s performance on these tasks can affect their quality of life. However, studies from multiple countries around the world have found that the prospective memory ability of school-age children is significantly lower than that of adults (
Cottini, 2023;
Zuber & Kliegel, 2020). The prospective memory performance of Chinese primary school children also shows significant deficiencies (
Yang et al., 2011). Therefore, to support reliable performance of prospective memory in children, some effective strategies can be implemented. Among these, reminders constitute a potent intervention.
Existing studies generally find that reminders can enhance children’s prospective memory (
Cheie et al., 2014;
Chen et al., 2017;
Kliegel & Jäger, 2007). However, the reminders employed in these studies have invariably been presented in the form of written language or pictures. In addition to the reminder of the content, the reminder of the context in which the cues of prospective memory appear may also significantly improve children’s performance in prospective memory. The effects of content and situational reminders on prospective memory are not identical. For example, writing the details of the intended action on a sticky note constitutes a content reminder, which can provide help when an individual forgets the content of the prospective memory task. Setting a prominent alarm before the appearance of prospective memory cues is a reminder of the situation, which can help individuals know when prospective memory cues appear. According to the PAM theory, situational reminders can significantly reduce the dependence of cue monitoring on attention. The development of the prefrontal cortex in children is not yet fully mature (
Fogarasi et al., 2001), and they have difficulty in flexibly allocating their attention among multiple tasks (
Wilson et al., 2011). Successfully carrying out prospective memory tasks usually demands considerable attentional resources, especially during cue monitoring (
Scullin et al., 2010). When attentional resources are insufficient, children may have a hard time giving enough attention to cue monitoring and therefore have difficulty completing prospective memory tasks successfully. Reminders that specify the situational context in which prospective memory cues will appear supply external information about the cue situation. When such reminders are available, individuals tend to rely on the provided external information to confirm cues, rather than allocating substantial attention resources to cue monitoring (
Gilbert et al., 2020). Therefore, when reminders about the situation for prospective memory cues are given, children can successfully monitor those cues by mostly relying on reminders, thereby reducing the demand on their own attentional resources. This may compensate for their insufficient ability in cue monitoring.
Currently, many studies have found that reminders of contextual cues can significantly improve adults’ prospective memory performance (
Black & McBride, 2024;
Henry et al., 2019), and this promoting effect can reduce their reliance on internal attention (
Guo et al., 2022). However, currently only a small amount of research has focused on whether school-age children can effectively utilize the contextual information provided by reminders to improve prospective memory performance. For example, one study has examined how reminders for cue situation affect children’s prospective memory.
Redshaw et al. (
2018) instructed children in the reminder group to move the stimulus containing the prospective memory cues to the adjacent area of the response region in advance, thus facilitating task execution when the cues appeared. The results revealed that reminders enhanced children’s prospective memory performance. However, no cognitive offloading was observed in the reminder group, instead children’s task response speed even slowed down. This may be because the reminders placed by
Redshaw et al. (
2018) were positioned in a highly salient location of the task environment. Repeated exposure to reminder content may have progressively increased the activation level of the prospective memory representation, thereby enhancing children’s strategic monitoring for prospective memory cues. Moreover, the reminders employed by
Redshaw et al. (
2018) incorporated the specific content of the prospective memory task, thereby constituting reminders of the content itself. Consequently, whether reminders that solely specify the contextual circumstances in which prospective memory cues appear can significantly enhance children’s prospective memory performance remains to be empirically verified.
The facilitative effect of reminders on prospective memory may be influenced by attention loads. Under low attention load conditions, children possess sufficient available attention resources and can still allocate ample attention to cue monitoring of prospective memory. Consequently, the external information provided by reminders is unlikely to yield additional facilitation to children’s cue monitoring. However, under high attention load conditions, children have limited available attention resources and are unable to allocate sufficient attention toward monitoring prospective memory cues (
Peper et al., 2023). Reminders that specify the situation in which prospective memory cues will appear provide external information about cue situations. Children can rely on this external information to confirm the presence of the cues, thereby compensating for their limited monitoring capacity. Therefore, the facilitative effect of reminders on prospective memory is more pronounced under high attention load conditions.
The distance between a reminder and the prospective memory cue may affect the facilitative impact of the reminder on prospective memory performance. When the reminder is presented in close temporal or spatial proximity to the prospective memory cue, participants may treat the reminder itself as an additional salient cue for the prospective memory tasks. Consequently, they are not required to engage in extensive cue monitoring, and they can rely primarily on the external information to determine the appropriate situation for executing prospective memory tasks (
Scarampi & Gilbert, 2020). Thus, when reminders are situated near the prospective memory cue, children can successfully monitor the context where the prospective memory task is to be performed by relying on the external information supplied by the reminder, thereby significantly enhancing their prospective memory performance. However, when reminders are temporally or spatially distant from the prospective memory cue, it can only specify the approximate interval or region within which the cue is likely to appear, affording participants merely an expectation of the cue’s location. Under such conditions, individuals will sustain heightened cue monitoring specifically within the anticipated range where the prospective memory target may occur (
Scullin et al., 2013). Although reminders that are closer to prospective memory cues can significantly reduce their attentional expenditure on cue monitoring, the promoting effect of recent reminders on prospective memory may be influenced by attentional load. Under low load conditions, children may still retain sufficient attention to monitor prospective memory cues, so recent reminders may not be sufficient to significantly improve their prospective memory performance. However, under high attentional load conditions, children retain less attention for cue monitoring, and recent reminders may largely compensate for their lack of attention on cue monitoring and improve their prospective memory performance.
This study focused on the effects of reminders of the situations in which prospective memory cues appear on children’s prospective memory under different attention load conditions. We refer to reminders that occur relatively close to prospective memory cues as recent reminders and those that occur relatively far from prospective memory cues as distant reminders. Our specific objective was to examine whether only recent reminders could improve children’s prospective memory performance, and whether the promoting effect of recent reminders on children’s prospective memory was influenced by attentional load. Given that
Redshaw et al. (
2018) discovered the promoting effect of reminders on children’s prospective memory in the complex ongoing tasks, we hypothesized that only recent reminders under high attention load conditions could significantly improve children’s prospective memory accuracy. There was evidence to suggest that recent reminders could significantly reduce internal attention consumption, which reserved enough attention to process ongoing tasks (
Guo et al., 2022). Therefore, we speculated that recent reminders would improve the accuracy of ongoing tasks under different attention load conditions. At present, there was no evidence indicating how reminders affect the response speed of prospective memory tasks and ongoing tasks, so we explored their relationship in an exploratory manner.
4. Discussion
Reminders regarding the context in which prospective memory cues occur reduce individuals’ reliance on internal attention. Instead, they come to rely more on the external information provided by the reminders to determine when to perform prospective memory tasks (
Guo et al., 2022). This may offset children’s inadequacies in attention monitoring, thereby enhancing their prospective memory performance. However, the facilitative effect of reminders on prospective memory may be influenced by the distance between the reminder and the cue, as well as the attention loads. This study aims to explore the influence of reminders at different distances from prospective memory cues on prospective memory under different attention loads. The results of this study found that the accuracy of prospective memory in children under low attention load conditions was higher than that under high attention load conditions, indicating that children’s prospective memory performance is easily influenced by attention load. According to the PAM theory, the preparatory attention processing of prospective memory relies heavily on attention, and preparatory attention processing is mainly used for monitoring cues. Therefore, the phenomenon that children’s prospective memory performance is easily affected by attention load indicates that it is difficult for children to effectively monitor cues when they have insufficient attention resources. In addition, the results revealed that the accuracy of prospective memory in the recent reminder condition was higher than that in both the distant reminder and the control condition, which was consistent with the results of a previous study (
Redshaw et al., 2018). This suggests that only recent reminders can promote children’s prospective memory, which is consistent with our predictions. The failure of distant reminders to promote children’s prospective memory may be that there is still a considerable distance between recent reminders and prospective memory cues, which requires children to engage in cue monitoring for a relatively long time. Children have deficiencies in attention allocation and sustained attention (
Wan & Sloutsky, 2024;
Yang et al., 2011), and their insufficient attention monitoring during intention maintenance may not be significantly compensated for under the condition of distant reminders. However, the recent reminder directly provides clear information about the location of the cue’s appearance. This enables children to successfully perform the prospective memory tasks by fully relying on external information (
Guo et al., 2022). Recent reminders largely compensate for children’s attention inadequacies (
Armitage & Gilbert, 2025), thus significantly improving their prospective memory performance. However, the present study found that the accuracy of prospective memory in the recent reminder condition was higher than that in the control group under different attention loads. This shows that attention load does not influence the facilitative effect of recent reminders on children’s prospective memory. And this results conflict with our predictions. This might be because the prospective memory task used in this study was set at a comparatively high difficulty level, resulting in poor performance even under low attention load conditions. Meanwhile, the recent reminders we set were placed near the prospective memory cues, which substantially reduced the attention demands of prospective memory task. Therefore, children’s prospective memory performance remained influenced by the recent reminders even under the low attention load condition.
It is worth further exploration to examine how recent reminders promote prospective memory performance. Experimental studies on prospective memory tasks typically use a dual-task paradigm, in which the prospective memory task and the ongoing task need to be performed simultaneously (
Boag et al., 2019). Thus, performance of the ongoing task can indirectly reflect the attention demands of the prospective memory task. The results of this study showed that the accuracy of the ongoing task in the recent reminder condition was higher than that in both the distant reminder condition and the control condition, which were consistent with the results of some previous studies (
Chen et al., 2017). The results indicate that recent reminders reduce the attention demands of children’s prospective memory. Reminders provide information about the occurrence of prospective memory cues, enabling individuals to monitor these cues effectively while reducing attention demands (
Chen et al., 2017). When performing ongoing tasks, prospective memory cues had not yet emerged. However, the results of the ongoing tasks in this study confirmed that children reduced their attention search for prospective memory cues in the presence of recent reminders. When reminders are present, individuals tend to rely more on the external information provided by the reminders, which causes them to reduce the internal attention’s monitoring of the cues (
Gilbert et al., 2020). Thus, the finding in this study that attention demands are lower under the recent reminder condition reflects children have become more dependent on reminders. It remains to be examined whether the explicit information about the occurrence of prospective memory cues provided by recent reminders leads children in the reminder condition to respond promptly to prospective memory cues when they encounter the cues. Results regarding response speed of prospective memory showed no differences across different reminder conditions, indicating that even with reminders, children spent the same amount of time processing prospective memory tasks. When prospective memory cues emerge, individuals still need to go through processes such as cue identification, task switching, intention retrieval, and execution (
Henry, 2021;
Matos & Albuquerque, 2021). Even under the recent reminder condition, children still engage in multiple subsequent cognitive processes. Consequently, when prospective memory cues appear, children do not exhibit the phenomenon of cognitive offloading. In conclusion, recent reminders prompt children to increase their reliance on external information to judge the context in which prospective memory cues appear, reducing their attention demands associated with cue monitoring during intention maintenance. Nevertheless, reminders fail to diminish the attention demands involved in retrieval of prospective memory intentions.
This study primarily examines the effects of reminders regarding the context in which prospective memory tasks occur on children’s prospective memory under varying attention load conditions. However, this study had several limitations. First, ongoing task performance was used as an indicator to reflect changes in attention, but it was only an indirect indicator. Eye-tracking technology, by contrast, can dynamically and in real time capture changes in an individual’s attention level as well as the specific processes involved in attention search, thereby functioning as a more direct and effective measure of attention (
West et al., 2007). Second, children across different age groups exhibit varying abilities to utilize information provided by reminders (
Cottini, 2023). Although the present study focused exclusively on how children’s prospective memory is influenced by reminders, it did not explore whether such facilitative effects vary with age. Future research could build on this by employing eye-tracking technology to examine the impact of reminders on prospective memory among children of different ages. Future research can use eye tracking technology to investigate the relationship between the promoting effect of reminders on children’s prospective memory and attention, as well as whether the impact of reminders on children’s prospective memory changes with age. The results of this study indicate that only recent reminders can significantly improve children’s prospective memory performance, which has certain practical implications. When setting reminders for children in real life, we must ensure they are relatively close to the prospective memory cues. Only in this way can we increase the possibility of children successfully performing prospective memory tasks.